Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-rdph2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-06T19:50:34.155Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of breakfast consumption timing v. breakfast omission on post-lunch glycaemia and insulinaemia in adolescent girls: a randomised crossover trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2025

Sahar Afeef
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Nutrition, Faculty of Applied Medical Sciences, King Abdulaziz University, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough LE11 3TU, UK
Julia K. Zakrzewski-Fruer
Affiliation:
Institute for Sport and Physical Activity Research, University of Bedfordshire, Bedford MK41 9EA, UK
Alice E. Thackray
Affiliation:
School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough LE11 3TU, UK NIHR Leicester Biomedical Research Centre, University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust and University of Leicester, Leicester, UK
Laura A. Barrett
Affiliation:
School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough LE11 3TU, UK
Keith Tolfrey*
Affiliation:
School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough LE11 3TU, UK
*
Corresponding author: Keith Tolfrey; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Adolescent girls often skip breakfast due to time constraints and reduced morning appetite. This study examined the acute impact of breakfast consumption timing v. breakfast omission (BO) on glycaemic and insulinaemic responses to lunch in infrequent breakfast-consuming girls. Fifteen girls (13·1 (sd 0·8) years) completed three conditions in a randomised crossover design: early-morning breakfast consumption (EM-BC; 08.30), mid-morning breakfast consumption (MM-BC; 10.30) and BO. A standardised lunch was provided at 12.30, followed by a 2-h post-lunch observation period. Blood and expired gas samples were collected periodically. Linear mixed models with Cohen’s d effect sizes compared outcomes between conditions. Pre-lunch glucose and insulin incremental AUC (iAUC) were higher in the breakfast conditions v. BO (P ≤ 0·009), with no differences between breakfast conditions. MM-BC reduced post-lunch glucose iAUC by 36 % and 25 % compared with BO and EM-BC, respectively (P < 0·001, d = 0·92–1·44). A moderate, non-significant 15 % reduction in post-lunch glucose iAUC was seen with EM-BC v. BO (P = 0·077, d = 0·52). These reductions occurred without changes in post-lunch insulinemia (P ≥ 0·323) and were accompanied by increased post-lunch carbohydrate oxidation compared with BO (P ≤ 0·018, d = 0·58–0·75); with no differences between EM-BC and MM-BC. MM-BC lowered glycaemic response over the experimental period compared with BO (P = 0·033, d = 0·98) and EM-BC (P = 0·123, d = 0·93), with no difference between EM-BC and BO. Compared with BO, both breakfast conditions lowered post-lunch glycaemic responses with mid-morning breakfast eliciting a greater second-meal effect than early-morning breakfast. These findings indicate the breakfast-to-lunch meal interval may be a crucial factor affecting postprandial glycaemia in infrequent breakfast-consuming girls.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Elevated postprandial glycaemia has been implicated in cardiometabolic diseases by promoting oxidative stress, inflammation and endothelial dysfunction(Reference Heine, Balkau and Ceriello1,Reference Blaak, Antoine and Benton2) . Even in healthy individuals, postprandial hyperglycaemia is recognised as a significant risk factor for type 2 diabetes (T2D)(Reference Abdul-Ghani, Abdul-Ghani and Ali3) and CVD(Reference Ning, Zhang and Dekker4) development. The prevalence of T2D among young people aged < 17 years is on a concerning rise in the UK(Reference Candler, Mahmoud and Lynn5). Indeed, the risk of T2D increases during puberty(Reference Cheng, Day and Lakshman6), partly due to the physiological reduction in insulin sensitivity(Reference Moran, Jacobs and Steinberger7), which if combined with other risk factors such as an unhealthy diet(Reference Malik, Fung and Van Dam8), can pose challenges on glycaemic control. Therefore, moderating postprandial glycaemia through dietary interventions is vital for disease prevention(Reference Pasmans, Meex and van Loon9).

Breakfast consumption is considered an integral component of healthy eating habits. Observational studies demonstrate that frequent breakfast consumption was associated with lower fasting blood glucose and insulin concentrations in adolescents(Reference Hallström, Labayen and Ruiz10,Reference De Souza, Neves and Souza11) and a lower incidence of T2D in adults(Reference Ballon, Neuenschwander and Schlesinger12,Reference Bi, Gan and Yang13) . Supporting this association, acute intervention studies with adults show that breakfast consumption reduced postprandial glycaemia, with or without changes in insulin concentrations, in response to a second meal (i.e. lunch) compared with breakfast omission (BO)(Reference Jovanovic, Gerrard and Taylor14,Reference Jovanovic, Leverton and Solanky15) . This improvement has been referred to as the ‘second meal effect’ and has been studied mainly in adults. Very little information is available regarding the second-meal effect in adolescents. In this regard, a recent study demonstrated that breakfast consumption significantly reduced glycaemic and insulinaemic responses to lunch, while also increasing carbohydrate oxidation, when compared with BO in adolescent girls who habitually consume breakfast(Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer, Morari and Champion16). Such findings may not apply directly to adolescent girls classified as infrequent breakfast consumers because previous studies in adults suggest that habitual breakfast skippers may have a less pronounced second-meal effect than habitual breakfast consumers(Reference Thomas, Higgins and Bessesen17). In young women (mean age of 19 years) who habitually skipped breakfast, consuming breakfast of different protein contents did not change the glycaemic and insulinaemic responses to lunch when compared with BO(Reference Alwattar, Thyfault and Leidy18). However, these observations were derived from combined data of both adolescents and adults aged 13–20 years, making it difficult to isolate the unique response of adolescents who have different hormonal and metabolic profiles(Reference Boisseau and Delamarche19). While existing evidence is largely based on adults, this study focussed on examining the impact of breakfast skipping on glycaemic and insulinaemic responses in adolescent girls who infrequently consume breakfast. This is particularly important given the increased prevalence of breakfast skipping during adolescence, specifically among girls (∼20 %)(Reference Coulthard, Palla and Pot20,Reference Timlin, Pereira and Story21) .

Definitions of ‘breakfast’ provide recommendations on quantity, composition and timing(Reference Gibney, Barr and Bellisle22Reference Timlin and Pereira24), yet the latter has not been investigated among adolescents with little data on adults. Timing-wise definitions have suggested that breakfast should be eaten within 2–3 h of awakening(Reference O’Neil, Byrd-Bredbenner and Hayes23), often no later than 10.00(Reference Timlin and Pereira24). Nevertheless, conflicting definitions have been utilised across the literature, primarily due to a lack of data supporting an optimal time to consume breakfast. Common reasons for skipping breakfast in adolescent girls are lack of time and not being hungry in the morning(Reference Shaw25). Thus, exploring different breakfast timings, such as consuming breakfast later in the morning (10.30) during school breaks, could be an attractive option to facilitate frequent breakfast consumption. Research comparing different breakfast timings would also be valuable in informing the time-of-day aspect of an evidence-based definition of breakfast consumption for research and practical application. A recent study in adult men with overweight or obesity showed that delayed breakfast consumed at 10.00 with lunch consumed in closer proximity (3 h) resulted in lower glycaemic and insulinaemic responses to lunch compared with breakfast consumed earlier in the morning at 07.00 and lunch 5 h later(Reference Parr, Devlin and Radford26). Yet, such responses have not been examined in adolescents, and the effect of breakfast consumption at different times compared to BO is still not known. Hence, the primary aim of this study was to compare the effects of early-morning breakfast consumption (EM-BC, 08.30) v. mid-morning breakfast consumption (MM-BC, 10.30) breakfast consumption with BO on post-lunch glycaemia and insulinaemia in adolescent girls classified as infrequent breakfast consumers. The secondary aim was to examine the effect of breakfast timing v. BO on substrate oxidation since skipping breakfast is associated with higher postprandial insulinaemia and increased fat oxidation, which represent markers of metabolic inflexibility due to extended fasting(Reference Nas, Mirza and Hägele27). Understanding these changes is crucial, as they could impair glucose regulation over time, impacting metabolic health.

Methods

Participants

After gaining Loughborough University Ethics approval (HPSC reference No. 3129), twenty-three healthy adolescent girls from local secondary schools volunteered to participate in the study. Four participants were excluded from the study during the preliminary visit and another four participants dropped out after the first breakfast condition due to a lack of interest. Therefore, a total of fifteen apparently healthy adolescent girls completed the study, and their data were carried forward for analysis (Figure 1). Data collection took place from November 2021 to July 2022. Participants were screened for general health using a questionnaire and gave their assent, and their caregiver provided written informed consent. Girls were included if they were 11–14 years old, generally healthy, were not allergic to the breakfast ingredients or taking prescribed medicines, were free from any metabolic diseases and were classified as infrequent breakfast consumers during the preliminary visit. The study was registered at clinicaltrials.gov (identifier NCT05000944).

Figure 1. Participant flow chart of adolescent girls who participated in a randomised crossover study design involving three conditions.

Preliminary visit

Participants arrived at the research lab after an overnight fast. Stature was measured using a stadiometer (The Leicester height measure, Seca Ltd., Birmingham, UK) to the nearest 0·01 m. Body mass was measured, and percentage body fat was estimated using bioelectrical impedance (Tanita BC-418MA, Tokyo, Japan), while participants stood barefoot wearing light clothes, to the nearest 0·1 kg and 0·1 %, respectively. BMI was calculated by dividing body mass (kg) by stature squared (m2). Consequently, weight status was determined using age and sex-specific BMI cut-off points(Reference Cole and Lobstein28). Waist circumference was measured midway between the tenth rib and the iliac crest(Reference McCarthy, Jarrett and Emmett29). Subsequently, participants sat quietly for 10 min before being fitted with a suitably sized facemask (Hans Rudolph Inc., Shawnee, USA) to determine the RMR as recommended(Reference Compher, Frankenfield and Keim30). Gas samples were then collected continuously for 10 min using a breath-by-breath gas analysis system (Vyntus CPX, CareFusion, Germany). The oxygen consumption and carbon dioxide production data were averaged into 5-second intervals and screened for steady-state conditions, with values exceeding a respiratory exchange ratio of 1·00 being excluded, consistent with indirect calorimetry assumptions(Reference Frayn31). Energy expenditure was estimated using the equations by Frayn (1983)(Reference Frayn31) and the RMR that represented the lowest 5 min rolling average was used to reflect minimum individual energy needs. Participants completed a breakfast habits questionnaire, which included questions about breakfast consumption frequency (days per week), timing and location, foods/drinks choices and reasons for skipping breakfast(Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer, Seall and Tolfrey32). This study targeted adolescent girls classified as ‘infrequent breakfast consumers’, defined as those who consumed breakfast (> 50 kcal before 10.30) on ≤ 4 days per week. Volunteers who consumed breakfast frequently (i.e. 5–7 days per week) were excluded. The focus on infrequent consumers was based on previous findings that link infrequent breakfast consumption with poorer cardiometabolic health among adolescents(Reference Nurul-Fadhilah, Teo and Huybrechts33Reference Hassan, Cunha and Valeria da Veiga35). Participants tasted both breakfast and lunch meals to ensure acceptability. All participants reported finding the meals acceptable. Lastly, the participants were asked to self-assess their physical maturation at home with the assistance of a caregiver using validated secondary sexual characteristics scales(Reference Tanner36,Reference Morris and Udry37) .

Experimental design

This study employed a randomised crossover design. Participants completed three conditions assigned according to the Latin square method: EM-BC, MM-BC and BO. The allocation sequence was generated, and participants were enrolled and assigned to conditions by the first author, SA. The participants attended all sessions in friendship pairs, receiving the same treatment in the same order to maintain consistency across sessions and increase comfort and adherence. Initial attempts were made to control for the menstrual cycle phase, but due to study logistics (friendship pairs), other parental and participant commitments, irregular cycles and availability of research assistants and lab resources, this was not feasible. There was a 3-to-30-day washout period between conditions, with a longer washout period for those with regular menstrual cycles. The participants were unaware of the order of study visits until they were informed on arrival. The participants were asked to record their dietary intake using a weighed food diary for 48 h preceding the first condition and were asked to replicate their intakes for the subsequent conditions. In addition, participants were asked to minimise moderate-vigorous intensity physical activity 48 h before each condition to control for any residual effect on glycaemia and insulinaemia(Reference Cockcroft, Williams and Weaver38,Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer and Tolfrey39) , which was confirmed via accelerometry. A schematic representation of the study design is provided in Figure 2. The experimental conditions started after an overnight fast and a 10-min rest at 08.00. Expired gas samples were collected with participants seated for 5 min at 0 h (baseline) and every 0·5 h thereafter using a standard Douglas bag method. Participants remained seated throughout the experimental period (08.00–14.30) but used the mouthpiece and nose clip only during the gas collection intervals. Substrate oxidation was estimated using recognised stoichiometric equations(Reference Frayn31). Capillary blood samples for the measurement of plasma glucose and insulin were also taken at 0, 0·5, 1, 2, 2·5, 3 and 4 h in the pre-lunch period and at 0·25, 0·5, 1 and 2 h post-lunch. The standardised breakfast was provided immediately after baseline measures at 08.30 for EM-BC and 2 h later at 10.30 for the separate MM-BC condition, with no breakfast being provided for the BO condition. The standardised lunch was consumed at 12.30 for all conditions. Ad libitum water intake was allowed during the first experimental condition, with the quantity and timings replicated in the subsequent conditions.

Figure 2. Schematic of the three experimental conditions. BO, breakfast omission; EM-BC, early-morning breakfast consumption; MM-BC, mid-morning breakfast consumption.

Standardised test meals

The quantity, composition and timing of the breakfast were in accordance with the proposed definitions of ‘breakfast’(Reference Gibney, Barr and Bellisle22Reference Timlin and Pereira24). The breakfast meal consisted of All-Bran Original cereal (Kellogg’s, Trafford, UK), semi-skimmed milk (Tesco, Welwyn Garden City, UK), Royal Gala apple (Tesco, Welwyn Garden City, UK) and orange juice (Tesco, Welwyn Garden City, UK), which provided 364 (sd 79) kcal (1524 (sd 331) kJ; i.e. 18 (sd 1) % of total daily energy needs) with 69 % carbohydrate, 13·5 % fat and 17·5 % protein of total meal energy. The calculated GI of the breakfast was 41 based on the International Tables of GI and Glycaemic Load values(Reference Foster-Powell, Holt and Brand-Miller40,Reference Atkinson, Foster-Powell and Brand-Miller41) and the weighted means of the GI values for the component foods(Reference Wolever and Jenkins42). The provided breakfast contained 0·04 g of carbohydrate per kcal of individual measured RMR. A carbohydrate-rich breakfast was selected based on evidence that such meals can induce the second-meal effect(Reference Jovanovic, Gerrard and Taylor14,Reference Jovanovic, Leverton and Solanky15) , with low GI meals successfully lowering glycaemic responses in subsequent meals(Reference Fletcher, Perfield and Thyfault43,Reference Maki, Phillips-Eakley and Smith44) .

The inclusion of a high-fibre, low GI, cereal-based breakfast was designed to provide consistent metabolic responses across breakfast consumption conditions (EM-BC and MM-BC v. BO), ensuring that breakfast timing, the primary variable under investigation, could be examined without variability from meal composition. Additionally, this type of breakfast aligns with recommendations for promoting better cardiometabolic health in children and adolescents(Reference Tolfrey and Zakrzewski45,Reference Williams46) . The portion size aligns with the recommendations that breakfast should contribute 15–25 % of daily energy needs(Reference Gibney, Barr and Bellisle22,Reference O’Neil, Byrd-Bredbenner and Hayes23) , and 55–75 % of energy should come from carbohydrates(Reference Gibney, Barr and Bellisle22). The timing of EM-BC aligns with the mean habitual breakfast timings of 07.39 (sd 00.49) on weekdays and 09.32 (sd 00.56) on weekend days reported by adolescent girls previously(Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer, Wells and Crawford47). The timing of MM-BC challenges the proposed 10.00 cut-off for breakfast consumption and aligns with the typical school break time, enhancing ecological validity.

The lunch meal consisted of white bread without crust (Tesco, UK), margarine (Flora Buttery Spread, Tesco, UK), strawberry jam (Tesco, UK), salted crisps (Walkers, Reading, UK) and a glucose drink (Lucozade Energy Original, Coleford, UK), which provided 503 (sd 104) kcal (2105 (sd 436) kJ; i.e. 25 % of total daily energy needs) with ∼62 % carbohydrate, ∼33 % fat and ∼5 % protein of total meal energy. For the single vegan participant, vegan food options (Actileaf soya milk and Pure vegan sunflower spread) were provided. The provided lunch contained 0·05 g of carbohydrate per kcal of measured RMR, and the calculated GI was 73. The lunch was intentionally designed as a carbohydrate-rich, high-GI meal to challenge the glycaemic control of adolescent girls(Reference Zakrzewski, Stevenson and Tolfrey48) and enhance the likelihood of observing a second-meal effect. Both meals were consumed within 15 min. For participants who were unable to consume the entire breakfast or lunch, the quantities consumed in the first condition were replicated in subsequent conditions.

Blood sampling and analysis

The whole hand was immersed in 40°C water for 5 min, followed by thorough drying, before a capillary blood sample was obtained by pricking the fingertip with a lancet (Unistick 3 Extra, Owen Mumford, UK). The first drop of blood was wiped, and 300–600 μl of blood was drawn into microvette tubes (Sarstedt Ltd., Leicester, UK). The sample tubes were placed immediately into a centrifuge at 12 800 g for 15 min (Eppendorf 5415c, Hamburg, Germany) to allow collection and storage of the resulting plasma at −80°C for subsequent batch analysis. Plasma glucose concentrations were determined by a benchtop analyser using enzymatic, colorimetric methods (Pentra 400; HORIBA ABX Diagnostics, Montpellier, France). Plasma insulin concentrations were determined using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (Mercodia AB, Uppsala, Sweden). The intra-assay coefficient of variation was 0·6 % for plasma glucose and 3·7 % for plasma insulin. The homeostatic model assessment of insulin resistance (HOMA-IR, averaged across conditions) was calculated by dividing the result of multiplying fasting glucose and insulin by 22·5(Reference Matthews, Hosker and Rudenski49). Participants ‘at risk’ of cardiometabolic disease were identified using age-, sex- and BMI-specific HOMA-IR percentiles(Reference Shashaj, Luciano and Contoli50). Impaired fasting plasma glucose was determined according to the American Diabetes Association criteria (i.e. 5·6–6·9 mmol·l−1)(51).

Sample size estimation

A sample size of eleven adolescent girls was estimated to detect a statistically significant interaction (3 conditions × 2 block periods of pre- and post-lunch) in glucose incremental AUC (iAUC) at 80 % power with an α level of 0·05 and a Cohen’s d effect size of 1·47 based on a previous two-treatment crossover design study in adolescent girls(Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer, Morari and Champion16). At least fourteen participants were targeted for recruitment to allow for an anticipated 20 % dropout rate.

Statistical analyses

Data were analysed using SPSS statistics software (version 25·0; SPSS Inc.). Postprandial outcomes were calculated for pre-lunch (breakfast manipulation period, time 0 through 4 h) and post-lunch period (from 4 through 6 h) using a time series response analyser(Reference Narang, Atkinson and Gonzalez52), which uses the trapezium rule. The total AUC (tAUC) and iAUC responses were divided by the respective time course to present the values in mmol·l–1 for glucose, pmol·l–1 for insulin and mg·min–1 for estimated substrate oxidation. For iAUC, areas below fasting levels were excluded. Additionally, a supplementary iAUC for delta glucose and insulin was calculated to account for baseline variability, reflecting relative postprandial changes. Linear mixed models were used to examine the differences between the three conditions in postprandial outcomes over two block periods of pre- and post-lunch. The linear mixed models included participants as a random effect and were adjusted for the order effect. Breakfast consumption frequency was considered as an additional covariate but was not included in the final analyses as it did not change the results significantly. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were examined with the Holm–Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. The data residuals were checked for normality using Shapiro–Wilks tests. Normally distributed data are presented as mean (standard deviation (sd)). The skewed data of glucose tAUC and iAUC were naturally log-transformed before analyses. These data are presented as geometric mean (95 % CI), and analyses are based on ratios of geometric means and 95 % CI for ratios. Statistical significance was accepted at P < 0·05. Cohen’s effect size (d) was calculated to describe the absolute magnitude of difference according to the following thresholds: trivial (< 0·2), small (≥ 0·2 to < 0·5), moderate (≥ 0·5 to < 0·8) and large (≥ 0·8)(Reference Cohen53).

Results

Participant characteristics

Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. One participant was classified as overweight and another one with obesity according to age and sex-specific BMI cut-off points(Reference Cole and Lobstein28). Four girls, including those with overweight/obesity, were classified as ‘at risk’ according to age-, sex- and BMI-specific percentiles of HOMA-IR(Reference Shashaj, Luciano and Contoli50). Two participants, including the one with overweight, had impaired fasting plasma glucose according to the American Diabetes Association criteria(51). Four participants had not started their period.

Table 1. Participant characteristics (n 15) (Mean values and standard deviations; median values and interquartile ranges)

* Self-assessment – median (interquartile range).

Breakfast habit questionnaire

Participants reported varying breakfast consumption frequencies: 4 d (n 3), 3 d (n 4), 2 d (n 2), 1 d (n 4) and never (n 2), resulting in an average of 2 (sd 1) days per week. The main reasons for skipping breakfast were ‘not feeling hungry’ (n 8) and ‘lack of time’ (n 5) on weekdays and ‘not feeling hungry’ (n 8) and ‘no motivation to prepare breakfast’ (n 4) on weekends.

Meal composition

The average energy and macronutrient composition of breakfast and lunch meals consumed during the experimental conditions are presented in Table 2. The participants consumed, on average, 0·03 g carbohydrate per kcal of average RMR for breakfast, which is ∼0·01 g per kcal RMR less than the prescribed breakfast amount (i.e. 0·04 g carbohydrate per kcal RMR). This difference was most likely due to their infrequent breakfast consumption habit as some reported difficulty eating in the morning. Four participants consumed the prescribed breakfast quantity, while the rest (n 11) consumed at least 72 % of the prescribed breakfast. Seven participants consumed the full amount of the prescribed lunch (i.e. 0·05 g carbohydrate per kcal RMR), while the rest consumed at least 72 % of the prescribed lunch. Furthermore, participants who conducted the MM-BC first had a 5 % higher breakfast intake compared to those who started with the EM-BC condition. That said, all participants replicated the quantities consumed in the first condition in the subsequent conditions to minimise within-participant variability and thus isolate the possible effect of breakfast meal timing on the outcome variables.

Table 2. The average energy and macronutrient composition of breakfast and lunch meals consumed during the experimental conditions (n 15) (Mean values and standard deviations)

Plasma glucose responses

Plasma glucose concentrations over time and iAUC across the conditions are shown in Figure 3(a) and (b). Plasma glucose iAUC differed significantly between conditions (main effect: P = 0·033), with MM-BC showing a lower glucose iAUC than BO (–0·34 mmol·L–1; P = 0·033, d = 0·98) and EM-BC (–0·28 mmol·L–1; P = 0·123, d = 0·93), with no difference between EM-BC and BO (P = 1·00). Condition-by-time interactions were identified for glucose iAUC, tAUC and peak glucose (P < 0·001). During the pre-lunch period, both EM-BC and MM-BC resulted in significantly higher glucose iAUC, tAUC and peak glucose than BO (P ≤ 0·003, d = 1·31–5·03), with no differences between EM-BC and MM-BC (P ≥ 0·890, d = 0·28–0·41). During the post-lunch period, MM-BC reduced glucose iAUC, tAUC and peak glucose compared with BO (P ≤ 0·002, d = 0·68–1·44) and reduced iAUC and peak glucose compared with EM-BC (P ≤ 0·001, d = 0·74–0·92), with no significant differences in tAUC (P = 0·289, d = 0·32). Differences between EM-BC and BO post-lunch were moderate but non-significant for iAUC (P = 0·077, d = 0·52) and trivial to small for tAUC and peak glucose (P ≥ 0·195, d = 0·00–0·36). Mean glucose values for each condition and the mean differences between conditions during pre- and post-lunch periods are presented in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. Supplementary analysis of iAUC for delta glucose revealed similar results to those observed with raw glucose values. However, post-lunch iAUC for delta glucose for EM-BC became significant with a moderate effect compared with BO (P = 0·012, d = 0·55) as shown in online Supplementary Table S1.

Figure 3. Changes in plasma glucose (a), glucose incremental AUC (iAUC) (b), plasma insulin (c), and insulin iAUC (d) concentrations during early-morning breakfast condition (EM-BC, open circle), mid-morning breakfast condition (MM-BC, open triangle) and breakfast omission (BO, open square). Data are presented as means and standard deviation. Black rectangles on panels (a) and (c) indicate consumption of breakfast (at 0 h for EM-BC and 2·0 h for MM-BC) and lunch meals (at 4·0 h for all conditions). Circles on panels (b) and (d) represent individual data values during the pre- (0–4 h, 240 min) and post-lunch periods (4–6 h, 120 min) of each condition. The iAUC (b) and (d) were divided by the time course of pre-lunch (240 min) and post-lunch periods (120 min) to present the values in mmol·l–1 for glucose and pmol·l–1 for insulin.

Table 3. Mean glucose, insulin and substrates oxidation values for each condition and pairwise comparisons between conditions during pre-lunch period (0–4 h, 240 min) (Mean values and standard deviations; geometric mean, mean difference, percentage difference, and their corresponding 95% CI)

EM-BC, early-morning breakfast consumption; MM-BC, mid-morning breakfast consumption; BO, breakfast omission; d, Cohen’s effect size; iAUC, incremental AUC; tAUC, total AUC; CHO, carbohydrate.

For untransformed (normally distributed) data, values are expressed as mean (standard deviation (sd)) and pairwise comparisons are presented as mean absolute difference and corresponding 95 % CI.

The data were divided by the time course of pre-lunch period (0–4 h, 240 min) to present the values in mmol·l–1 for glucose, pmol·l–1 for insulin and mg·min–1 for estimated substrate oxidation.

* Indicates significant paired differences (P < 0·05). Mean differences are A compared with B (e.g. 10 % indicates EM-BC was higher on average than BO).

For log-transformed data, values are presented as geometric means and corresponding 95 % CI, and pairwise comparisons are presented as percentage difference (%) based on ratios of geometric means and corresponding 95 % CI (%).

Table 4. Mean glucose, insulin and substrates oxidation values for each condition and pairwise comparisons between conditions during post-lunch period (4–6 h, 120 min) (Mean values and standard deviations; geometric mean, mean difference, percentage difference, and their corresponding 95% CI)

EM-BC, early-morning breakfast consumption; MM-BC, mid-morning breakfast consumption; BO, breakfast omission; d, Cohen’s effect size; iAUC, incremental AUC; tAUC, total AUC; CHO, carbohydrate.

For untransformed (normally distributed) data, values are expressed as mean (standard deviation (sd)) and pairwise comparisons are presented as mean absolute difference and corresponding 95 % CI.

The data were divided by the time course of pre-lunch period (0–4 h, 240 min) to present the values in mmol·l–1 for glucose, pmol·l–1 for insulin and mg·min–1 for estimated substrate oxidation.

* Indicates significant paired differences (P < 0·05). Mean differences are A compared with B (e.g. –5 % indicates EM-BC was lower on average than BO).

For log-transformed data, values are presented as geometric means and corresponding 95 % CI, and pairwise comparisons are presented as percentage difference (%) based on ratios of geometric means and corresponding 95 % CI (%).

Plasma insulin responses

Plasma insulin concentrations over time and iAUC across the conditions are shown in Figure 3(c) and (d). Plasma insulin iAUC and tAUC showed no significant main effects for the condition (P ≥ 0·088). However, condition-by-time interactions were identified for insulin iAUC, tAUC and peak insulin (P ≤ 0·009). During the pre-lunch period, both EM-BC and MM-BC resulted in significantly higher insulin iAUC, tAUC, and peak insulin compared to BO (P ≤ 0·014, d = 1·18–3·19), with no differences between EM-BC and MM-BC (P = 1·00, d = 0·10–0·20). During the post-lunch period, differences in insulin iAUC, tAUC and peak insulin across conditions were trivial to small and non-significant (P ≥ 0·323, d = 0·01–0·31). Mean insulin values for each condition and the mean differences between conditions during pre- and post-lunch periods are presented in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. Supplementary analysis of iAUC for delta insulin revealed similar results to those observed with raw insulin values, as shown in online Supplementary Table S1.

Fat and carbohydrate oxidation

Changes in substrate oxidation over time and tAUC across the conditions are shown in Figure 4. Substrate oxidation tAUC showed significant main effects for condition (P < 0·001). Both EM-BC and MM-BC resulted in higher carbohydrate oxidation tAUC (56 and 47 mg·min–1; P < 0·001) and lower fat oxidation tAUC (–15 and –11 mg·min–1; P ≤ 0·001) compared with BO, with no differences between the breakfast consumption conditions (P ≤ 0·569). Condition-by-time interactions were identified for carbohydrate oxidation (P = 0·047) but not for fat oxidation (P = 0·105). During the pre-lunch period, carbohydrate oxidation tAUC was significantly higher in EM-BC (P < 0·001, d = 1·53) and MM-BC (P = 0·003, d = 0·92) compared with BO, with a moderate reduction in MM-BC compared to EM-BC (P = 0·073, d = 0·59). During the post-lunch period, carbohydrate oxidation tAUC remained moderately higher in EM-BC and MM-BC compared with BO (P ≤ 0·018, d = 0·58–0·75), with no meaningful differences between EM-BC and MM-BC (P = 1·00, d = 0·17). Mean substrate oxidation values for each condition and the differences between conditions during pre- and post- lunch periods are presented in Tables 3 and 4, respectively.

Figure 4. Changes in carbohydrate oxidation (a), carbohydrate oxidation total AUC (tAUC) (b), fat oxidation (c) and fat oxidation tAUC (d) during early-morning breakfast condition (EM-BC, open circle), mid-morning breakfast condition (MM-BC, open triangle) and breakfast omission (BO, open square). Data are presented as means and standard deviation. Black rectangles on panels (a) and (c) indicate consumption of breakfast (at 0 h for EM-BC and 2·0 h for MM-BC) and lunch meals (at 4·0 h for all conditions). Circles on panels (b) and (d) represent individual data values during the pre- (0–4 h, 240 min) and post-lunch periods (4–6 h, 120 min) of each condition. The tAUC (b) and (d) were divided by the time course of pre-lunch (240 min) and post-lunch periods (120 min) to present the values in mg·min–1.

Discussion

This study examined the acute effect of breakfast timing (i.e. early morning and mid-morning) compared with BO on post-lunch glycaemia, insulinaemia and substrate oxidation in adolescent girls who infrequently consume breakfast. Both breakfast conditions reduced post-lunch glycaemic responses compared with BO, with MM-BC showing a stronger effect. These glycaemic improvements occurred without changes in post-lunch insulinaemia, suggesting an acute improvement in insulin sensitivity. Both breakfast conditions increased post-lunch carbohydrate oxidation compared with BO. However, only MM-BC reduced glycaemic responses when considering the combined effects of breakfast and lunch throughout the experimental period. These findings highlight the glycaemic benefits of consuming breakfast over skipping it, particularly in the mid-morning, in this population.

The reduction in glycaemic response after lunch with prior breakfast consumption v. BO has been referred to as the second-meal effect. This phenomenon has been reported previously in several studies in adults with(Reference Jovanovic, Gerrard and Taylor14,Reference Jakubowicz, Wainstein and Ahren54,Reference Jakubowicz, Wainstein and Landau55) and without T2D(Reference Jovanovic, Leverton and Solanky15,Reference Thomas, Higgins and Bessesen17,Reference Jakubowicz, Wainstein and Landau55,Reference Ogata, Hatamoto and Goto56) , with fewer studies examining this effect in adolescents(Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer, Morari and Champion16,Reference Alwattar, Thyfault and Leidy18) . In one of these studies, the impact of consuming breakfast of varying protein content over 4 d on post-lunch glycaemic and insulinaemic responses was investigated in both breakfast skippers and consumers(Reference Alwattar, Thyfault and Leidy18). The breakfast skippers exclusively completed an additional BO condition, and the findings revealed that regardless of protein content, the inclusion of breakfast did not change glycaemic and insulinaemic responses to a lunch meal consumed 4 h later(Reference Alwattar, Thyfault and Leidy18). In contrast, the current study found a large reduction in post-lunch glucose iAUC with MM-BC v. BO, while EM-BC showed a moderate, non-significant reduction in post-lunch glucose iAUC (15 %). Supplementary analysis of delta glucose iAUC supported these findings and revealed a significant second-meal effect for EM-BC with a moderate effect size. The use of both raw and delta glucose iAUC in this study highlights the importance of considering different baseline adjustment approaches in metabolic research. While raw iAUC measures glucose responses above baseline, delta iAUC enhanced sensitivity by accounting for baseline variations. The differences in results between the current study and Alwattar et al.(Reference Alwattar, Thyfault and Leidy18) could be attributed to differences in participants’ characteristics such as age (mean 13 v. 19 years), weight status (mainly normal v. overweight/obese) and the breakfast habit (≤ 4 v. ≤ 2 d/week). Furthermore, while the energy and carbohydrate content of the breakfasts in the current study were more comparable to the normal-protein breakfast than to the high-protein breakfast provided by Alwattar et al.(Reference Alwattar, Thyfault and Leidy18), the GI of the breakfast meals were not specified, precluding direct comparisons.

Low GI meals are known to reduce immediate and subsequent (second meal) glycaemic responses(Reference Fletcher, Perfield and Thyfault43,Reference Maki, Phillips-Eakley and Smith44) , impacting overall glycaemia. In this study, providing a low GI breakfast with a 2-hour time difference between the early-morning (08.30) and mid-morning (10.30) breakfast conditions did not result in distinct morning glycaemic and insulinaemic responses, indicating consistent insulin sensitivity(Reference Saad, Man and Nandy57,Reference Yoshino, Almeda-Valdes and Patterson58) . However, a pronounced second-meal effect was observed with the shorter breakfast-to-lunch interval in MM-BC (2 h) compared with the longer interval in EM-BC (4 h) and BO (extended overnight fast), resulting in a lower glycaemic response over the experimental period. EM-BC showed a moderate second-meal effect compared with BO. These findings align with a recent study in habitual breakfast-consuming girls (≥ 4 d/week), which reported a large second-meal effect with a 3-h meal interval and a low GI breakfast compared with BO(Reference Zakrzewski-Fruer, Morari and Champion16). Both studies suggest that shorter intervals enhance the second-meal effect, despite differences in meal intervals and breakfast habits.

Another study in women with overweight/obesity using a 4-h meal interval found that breakfast consumption reduced post-lunch glycaemia compared with breakfast skipping, regardless of breakfast habit(Reference Thomas, Higgins and Bessesen17). However, the negative impact of breakfast skipping on post-lunch insulinaemic response was only observed in habitual breakfast consumers (≥ 5 d/week), while habitual breakfast skippers (≤ 2 d/week) showed similar insulinaemic responses regardless of breakfast consumption, suggesting potential adaptive responses(Reference Thomas, Higgins and Bessesen17). Similarly, in the current study of infrequent breakfast-consuming girls (≤ 4 d/week), breakfast consumption lowered post-lunch glycaemia compared with BO without meaningful changes in post-lunch insulinemia. Despite differences in participants’ characteristics and meal composition, these findings align with those of habitual skippers from the previous study(Reference Thomas, Higgins and Bessesen17), suggesting that infrequent breakfast consumers may share similar metabolic adaptations. These results highlight the important role of breakfast consumption, timing and habitual patterns in optimising metabolic responses in this population.

A previous study in adults with T2D demonstrated that a high-protein, low-carbohydrate pre-load consumed 2 h before breakfast reduced postprandial glucose by 40 %(Reference Chen, Jovanovic and Taylor59). In the current study, a similar effect was observed, with a 36 % reduction in post-lunch glucose iAUC when a breakfast meal was consumed 2 h before lunch but the effect was less pronounced when breakfast was consumed 4 h before lunch. The impact on postprandial glycaemia may vary based on prior meal composition and timing. More research is needed to determine the dietary (e.g. meal interval) and population-specific factors (e.g. breakfast habit) influencing the second-meal effect. Although the reduction achieved by MM-BC is smaller than the ∼45–50 % clinically relevant benchmark established for therapeutic interventions in individuals without diabetes(Reference Alssema, Ruijgrok and Blaak60), this reference was included to highlight the magnitude of the observed reduction rather than for direct comparison. This novel finding highlights the potential benefits of subtle dietary adjustments in breakfast timing, with mid-morning breakfast offering a practical alternative for girls who infrequently consume breakfast, where early breakfast may be less common. Furthermore, the mid-morning breakfast in this study could be framed as a mid-morning ‘snack’ due to the timing of food intake. While there is no standardised definition of a ‘snack’(Reference Hess, Jonnalagadda and Slavin61), this framing may align with adolescent eating habits and support practical recommendations for those who typically consume a mid-morning snack instead of breakfast. For these individuals, ensuring that a mid-morning snack is of sufficient quantity and quality could offer metabolic benefits similar to those observed in this study. If these responses are maintained over time, it could play a crucial role in moderating postprandial glycaemic responses and, thus, promote cardiometabolic health(Reference Blaak, Antoine and Benton2) in those who infrequently consume breakfast.

The underlying mechanism of the second-meal effect has been linked to the suppression of NEFA concentrations and increased rate of muscle glycogen synthesis(Reference Jovanovic, Gerrard and Taylor14,Reference Jovanovic, Leverton and Solanky15) . Elevated plasma NEFA concentrations demonstrate a dose-dependent inhibition of insulin-stimulated nonoxidative glucose disposal (through glycogen synthesis), with a lesser impact on muscle glucose oxidation(Reference Boden, Chen and Ruiz62). Omitting breakfast elevates NEFA concentrations due to an extended fast(Reference Jovanovic, Leverton and Solanky15,Reference Astbury, Taylor and Macdonald63) . However, consuming oral glucose reduces NEFA concentrations to a nadir within 2 h, and then rebounds above baseline values after 4 h in healthy adults(Reference Jenkins, Wolever and Ocana Vuksan64). Although not measured directly, it is likely that pre-lunch NEFA concentrations would be suppressed to a greater extent in the MM-BC than EM-BC condition compared with the BO condition. Therefore, the magnitude of the post-lunch glucose reduction was more pronounced in MM-BC than EM-BC when both were compared with BO, potentially influenced by the time interval between meals (i.e. 4 h in EM-BC and 2 h in MM-BC). Post-lunch carbohydrate oxidation was comparable between the breakfast consumption conditions, suggesting no significant differences in oxidative glucose utilisation. While measuring nonoxidative glucose disposal, including glycogen synthesis, would provide insight into the underlying mechanism, this is beyond the scope of the current study. Furthermore, such measurements in young people are limited due to invasiveness, posing practical and ethical challenges.

The strength of this study lies in its experimental design, which directly compared two breakfast timings against BO in infrequent breakfast-consuming girls. The purpose of the study was to acutely reduce postprandial glycaemia, as repeated reductions in postprandial glycaemia are known to help reduce cardiometabolic disease risk(Reference Heine, Balkau and Ceriello1,Reference Blaak, Antoine and Benton2) , suggesting a beneficial daily practice. Conducted in a typically underrepresented group – apparently healthy, infrequent breakfast-consuming girls – our study addresses a gap in preventive health measures. However, the single-day crossover design limits insights into long-term adaptations, and the exclusion of mid-morning snacks may reduce generalisability to girls with habitual snacking patterns. While the design isolated the effects of breakfast timing without additional energy intake, future research should explore how mid-morning snacks influence metabolic outcomes. Furthermore, while our findings are promising, they are not directly generalisable to populations at higher risk (e.g. obesity and/or T2D); where the benefits of reducing postprandial glycaemia may be even greater. It is worth noting that there is no consensus on defining breakfast consumption frequency(Reference Tolfrey and Zakrzewski45), and consuming breakfast infrequently – even as often as four days a week – has been linked to increased cardiometabolic risk among adolescents(Reference Nurul-Fadhilah, Teo and Huybrechts33Reference Hassan, Cunha and Valeria da Veiga35). Given the potential effects of habitual breakfast frequency on glucose metabolism(Reference Thomas, Higgins and Bessesen17) and its variability among participants, sensitivity analyses incorporating breakfast consumption frequency as a covariate demonstrated that our findings remained consistent. While participants found the meals acceptable during the preliminary session, most girls (n 11) were unable to consume the entire breakfast, likely due to the timing of the meal and their infrequent breakfast consumption habits. Future research may benefit from considering smaller breakfast portions, including acclimatisation days, and assessing appetite and satiety to better understand the factors influencing meal consumption and metabolic responses in infrequent breakfast consumers. It should be emphasised that the breakfast portion remained standardised among participants and adhered to the recommended range of daily energy intake(Reference O’Neil, Byrd-Bredbenner and Hayes23). Although we initially controlled for the menstrual cycle phase (n 4), this was not possible across the whole sample due to the study recruitment plan of pairing the participants with friends to increase retention and enjoyment during the long testing sessions and other challenges such as the caregivers/participants commitments, irregular cycles and availability of research assistants and lab resources. Yet, studies based on adult women have been inconsistent regarding the effects of the menstrual cycle phase on insulin sensitivity and substrate oxidation at rest(Reference Isacco, Duch and Boisseau65,Reference Yeung, Zhang and Mumford66) .

In conclusion, the current study revealed that consuming breakfast lowers post-lunch glycaemic responses compared with skipping breakfast, with mid-morning breakfast demonstrating greater benefits over early-morning breakfast. These glycaemic improvements were achieved without significant changes in post-lunch insulinaemic responses and were accompanied by increased post-lunch carbohydrate oxidation. Importantly, mid-morning breakfast resulted in lower cumulative glycaemic responses across the breakfast and lunch periods. These findings underscore the importance of meal timing, specifically the between-meal interval, as a key determinant of glycaemic control in infrequent breakfast-consuming girls.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114525000248

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Liu Hanyu, Kerys Broughton and Max Holgate for their assistance with data collection. Sahar Afeef acknowledges support from King Abdulaziz University. Alice E Thackray acknowledges support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Leicester Biomedical Research Centre. The supporters had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript or in the decision to publish the results.

No financial support was provided.

K. T. and J. Z-F. contributed to study design and conception, statistical analysis, interpretation of results and revised the manuscript; S. A. contributed to data collection, plasma analysis, statistical analysis, interpretation of results and wrote the original manuscript; A. E. T. contributed to plasma analysis, statistical analysis and revised the manuscript; L. A. B. revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

References

Heine, RJ, Balkau, B, Ceriello, A, et al. (2004) What does postprandial hyperglycaemia mean? Diabet Med 21, 208213. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-5491.2004.01149.x Google Scholar
Blaak, EE, Antoine, JM, Benton, D, et al. (2012) Impact of postprandial glycaemia on health and prevention of disease. Obes Rev 13, 923984. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01011.x Google Scholar
Abdul-Ghani, MA, Abdul-Ghani, T, Ali, N, et al. (2008) One-hour plasma glucose concentration and the metabolic syndrome identify subjects at high risk for future type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 31, 16501655. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc08-0225 Google Scholar
Ning, F, Zhang, L, Dekker, JM, et al. (2012) Development of coronary heart disease and ischemic stroke in relation to fasting and 2-hour plasma glucose levels in the normal range. Cardiovasc Diabetol 11, 76. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2840-11-76 Google Scholar
Candler, TP, Mahmoud, O, Lynn, RM, et al. (2018) Continuing rise of type 2 diabetes incidence in children and young people in the UK. Diabet Med 35, 737744. https://doi.org/10.1111/dme.13609 Google Scholar
Cheng, TS, Day, FR, Lakshman, R, et al. (2020) Association of puberty timing with type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med 17, e1003017. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003017 Google Scholar
Moran, A, Jacobs, DR, Steinberger, J, et al. (1999) Insulin resistance during puberty: results from clamp studies in 357 children. Diabetes 48, 20392044. https://doi.org/10.2337/diabetes.48.10.2039 Google Scholar
Malik, VS, Fung, TT, Van Dam, RM, et al. (2012) Dietary patterns during adolescence and risk of type 2 diabetes in middle-aged women. Diabetes Care 35, 1218. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc11-0386 Google Scholar
Pasmans, K, Meex, RCR, van Loon, LJC, et al. (2022) Nutritional strategies to attenuate postprandial glycemic response. Obes Rev 23, e13486. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.13486 Google Scholar
Hallström, L, Labayen, I, Ruiz, JR, et al. (2013) Breakfast consumption and CVD risk factors in European adolescents: the HELENA (Healthy Lifestyle in Europe by Nutrition in Adolescence) Study. Public Health Nutr 16, 12961305. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980012000973 Google Scholar
De Souza, MR, Neves, MEA, Souza, ADM, et al. (2021) Skipping breakfast is associated with the presence of cardiometabolic risk factors in adolescents: study of cardiovascular risks in adolescents-ERICA. Br J Nutr 126, 276284. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114520003992 Google Scholar
Ballon, A, Neuenschwander, M & Schlesinger, S (2019) Breakfast skipping is associated with increased risk of type 2 diabetes among adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. J Nutr 149, 106113. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/nxy194 Google Scholar
Bi, H, Gan, Y, Yang, C, et al. (2015) Breakfast skipping and the risk of type 2 diabetes: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Public Health Nutr 18, 30133019. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980015000257 Google Scholar
Jovanovic, A, Gerrard, J & Taylor, R (2009) The second-meal phenomenon in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 32, 11991201. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc08-2196 Google Scholar
Jovanovic, A, Leverton, E, Solanky, B, et al. (2009) The second-meal phenomenon is associated with enhanced muscle glycogen storage in humans. Clin Sci 117, 119127. https://doi.org/10.1042/CS20080542 Google Scholar
Zakrzewski-Fruer, JK, Morari, V, Champion, RB, et al. (2023) Acute cardiometabolic and exercise responses to breakfast omission v. breakfast consumption in adolescent girls: a randomised crossover trial. Nutrients 15, 3210. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15143210 Google Scholar
Thomas, EA, Higgins, J, Bessesen, DH, et al. (2015) Usual breakfast eating habits affect response to breakfast skipping in overweight women. Obesity 23, 750759. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.21049 Google Scholar
Alwattar, AY, Thyfault, JP & Leidy, HJ (2015) The effect of breakfast type and frequency of consumption on glycemic response in overweight/obese late adolescent girls. Eur J Clin Nutr 69, 885890. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2015.12 Google Scholar
Boisseau, N & Delamarche, P (2000) Metabolic and hormonal responses to exercise in children and adolescents. Sports Med 30, 405422. https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200030060-00003 Google Scholar
Coulthard, JD, Palla, L & Pot, GK (2017) Breakfast consumption and nutrient intakes in 4–18-year-olds: UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey Rolling Programme (2008–2012). Br J Nutr 118, 280290. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114517001714 Google Scholar
Timlin, MT, Pereira, MA, Story, M, et al. (2008) Breakfast eating and weight change in a 5-year prospective analysis of adolescents: project EAT (eating among teens). Pediatrics 121, e638e645. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-1035 Google Scholar
Gibney, MJ, Barr, SI, Bellisle, F, et al. (2018) Towards an evidence-based recommendation for a balanced breakfast—a proposal from the International Breakfast Research Initiative. Nutrients 10, 1540. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10101540 Google Scholar
O’Neil, CE, Byrd-Bredbenner, C, Hayes, D, et al. (2014) The role of breakfast in health: definition and criteria for a quality breakfast. J Acad Nutr Diet 114, S8S26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2014.08.022 Google Scholar
Timlin, MT & Pereira, MA (2008) Breakfast frequency and quality in the etiology of adult obesity and chronic diseases. Nutr Rev 65, 268281. https://doi.org/10.1301/nr.2007.jun.268-281 Google Scholar
Shaw, M (1998) Adolescent breakfast skipping: an Australian study. Adolescence 33, 851861.Google Scholar
Parr, EB, Devlin, BL, Radford, BE, et al. (2020) A delayed morning and earlier evening time-restricted feeding protocol for improving glycemic control and dietary adherence in men with overweight/obesity: a randomized controlled trial. Nutrients 12, 505. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12020505 Google Scholar
Nas, A, Mirza, N, Hägele, F, et al. (2017) Impact of breakfast skipping compared with dinner skipping on regulation of energy balance and metabolic risk. Am J Clin Nutr 105, 13511361. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.116.151332 Google Scholar
Cole, T & Lobstein, T (2012) Extended International (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight and obesity. Pediatr Obes 7, 284294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2047-6310.2012.00064.x Google Scholar
McCarthy, HD, Jarrett, KV, Emmett, PM, et al. (2005) Trends in waist circumferences in young British children: a comparative study. Int J Obes 29, 157162. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802849 Google Scholar
Compher, C, Frankenfield, D, Keim, N, et al. (2006) Best practice methods to apply to measurement of resting metabolic rate in adults: a systematic review. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 881903. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2006.02.009 Google Scholar
Frayn, KN (1983) Calculation of substrate oxidation rates in vivo from gaseous exchange. J Appl Physiol Respir Environ Exerc Physiol 55, 628634. https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1983.55.2.628 Google Scholar
Zakrzewski-Fruer, JK, Seall, C & Tolfrey, K (2021) Breakfast consumption suppresses appetite but does not increase daily energy intake or physical activity energy expenditure when compared with breakfast omission in adolescent girls who habitually skip breakfast: a 7-day randomised crossover trial. Nutrients 13, 4261. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124261 Google Scholar
Nurul-Fadhilah, A, Teo, PS, Huybrechts, I, et al. (2013) Infrequent breakfast consumption is associated with higher body adiposity and abdominal obesity in Malaysian school-aged adolescents. PLoS One 8, e75903. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059297 Google Scholar
Ahadi, Z, Qorbani, M, Kelishadi, R, et al. (2015) Association between breakfast intake with anthropometric measurements, blood pressure and food consumption behaviors among Iranian children and adolescents: the CASPIAN-IV study. Public Health 129, 740747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2015.03.019 Google Scholar
Hassan, BK, Cunha, DB, Valeria da Veiga, G, et al. (2018) Changes in breakfast frequency and composition during adolescence: the Adolescent Nutritional Assessment Longitudinal Study, a cohort from Brazil. PLoS One 13, e0198377. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200587 Google Scholar
Tanner, JM (1962) Growth at Adolescents, 2nd ed. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific.Google Scholar
Morris, NM & Udry, JR (1980) Validation of a self-administered instrument to assess stage of adolescent development. J Youth Adolesc 9, 271280.Google Scholar
Cockcroft, E, Williams, C, Weaver, H, et al. (2017) Acute exercise and insulin sensitivity in boys: a time-course study. Int J Sports Med 38, 967974. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0043-118007 Google Scholar
Zakrzewski-Fruer, JK & Tolfrey, K (2012) Acute effect of Fatmax exercise on metabolism in overweight and nonoverweight girls. Med Sci Sports Exerc 44, 16981705. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31825804cf Google Scholar
Foster-Powell, K, Holt, SHA & Brand-Miller, JC (2002) International table of glycemic index and glycemic load values: 2002. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 556. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/76.1.5 Google Scholar
Atkinson, FS, Foster-Powell, K & Brand-Miller, JC (2008) International tables of glycemic index and glycemic load values: 2008. Diabetes Care 31, 22812283. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc08-1239 Google Scholar
Wolever, TMS & Jenkins, DJA (1986) The use of the glycemic index in predicting the blood glucose response to mixed meals. Am J Clin Nutr 43, 167172. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/43.1.167 Google Scholar
Fletcher, JA, Perfield, JW II, Thyfault, JP, et al. (2012) The second meal effect and its influence on glycemia. J Nutr Disord Ther 2, 108. https://doi.org/10.4172/2161-0509.1000108 Google Scholar
Maki, KC, Phillips-Eakley, AK & Smith, KN (2016) The effects of breakfast consumption and composition on metabolic wellness with a focus on carbohydrate metabolism. Adv Nutr 7, 613S621S. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.115.010314 Google Scholar
Tolfrey, K & Zakrzewski, JK (2012) Breakfast, glycaemic index and health in young people. J Sport Health Sci 1, 149159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2012.09.001 Google Scholar
Williams, PG (2014) The benefits of breakfast cereal consumption: a systematic review of the evidence base. Adv Nutr 5, 636S673S. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.114.006247 Google Scholar
Zakrzewski-Fruer, JK, Wells, EK, Crawford, NSG, et al. (2018) Physical activity duration but not energy expenditure differs between daily and intermittent breakfast consumption in adolescent girls: a randomized crossover trial. J Nutr 148, 236244. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/nxx055 Google Scholar
Zakrzewski, JK, Stevenson, EJ & Tolfrey, K (2012) Effect of breakfast glycemic index on metabolic responses during rest and exercise in overweight and non-overweight adolescent girls. Eur J Clin Nutr 66, 436442. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2011.175 Google Scholar
Matthews, DR, Hosker, JP, Rudenski, AS, et al. (1985) Homeostasis model assessment: insulin resistance and β-cell function from fasting plasma glucose and insulin concentrations in man. Diabetologia 28, 412419. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00280883 Google Scholar
Shashaj, B, Luciano, R, Contoli, B, et al. (2016) Reference ranges of HOMA-IR in normal-weight and obese young Caucasians. Acta Diabetol 53, 251260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00592-015-0782-4 Google Scholar
American Diabetes Association (2014) Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care 37, S81S90. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc14-S081 Google Scholar
Narang, BJ, Atkinson, G, Gonzalez, JT, et al. (2020) A tool to explore discrete-time data: the time series response analyser. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab 30, 374381. https://doi.org/10.1123/ijsnem.2020-0150 Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, D, Wainstein, J, Ahren, B, et al. (2015) Fasting until noon triggers increased postprandial hyperglycemia and impaired insulin response after lunch and dinner in individuals with type 2 diabetes: a randomized clinical trial. Diabetes Care 38, 18201826. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc15-0761 Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, D, Wainstein, J, Landau, Z, et al. (2017) Influences of breakfast on clock gene expression and postprandial glycemia in healthy individuals and individuals with diabetes: a randomized clinical trial. Diabetes Care 40, 15731579. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc16-2753 Google Scholar
Ogata, H, Hatamoto, Y, Goto, Y, et al. (2019) Association between breakfast skipping and postprandial hyperglycaemia after lunch in healthy young individuals. Br J Nutr 122, 431440. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114519001235 Google Scholar
Saad, A, Man, CD, Nandy, DK, et al. (2012) Diurnal pattern to insulin secretion and insulin action in healthy individuals. Diabetes 61, 26912700. https://doi.org/10.2337/db11-1478 Google Scholar
Yoshino, J, Almeda-Valdes, P, Patterson, BW, et al. (2014) Diurnal variation in insulin sensitivity of glucose metabolism is associated with diurnal variations in whole-body and cellular fatty acid metabolism in metabolically normal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 99, E1666E1670. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2014-1579 Google Scholar
Chen, MJ, Jovanovic, A & Taylor, R (2010) Utilizing the second-meal effect in type 2 diabetes: practical use of a soya-yogurt snack. Diabetes Care 33, 2552. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc10-0552 Google Scholar
Alssema, M, Ruijgrok, C, Blaak, EE, et al. (2021) Effects of α-glucosidase-inhibiting drugs on acute postprandial glucose and insulin responses: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Diabetes 11, 11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41387-021-00152-5 Google Scholar
Hess, JM, Jonnalagadda, SS & Slavin, JL (2016) What is a snack, why do we snack, and how can we choose better snacks? A review of the definitions of snacking, motivations to snack, contributions to dietary intake, and recommendations for improvement. Adv Nutr 7, 466475. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.115.009571.Google Scholar
Boden, G, Chen, X, Ruiz, J, et al. (1994) Mechanisms of fatty acid-induced inhibition of glucose uptake. J Clin Invest 93, 24382446. https://doi.org/10.1172/JCI117252 Google Scholar
Astbury, NM, Taylor, MA & Macdonald, IA (2011) Breakfast consumption affects appetite, energy intake, and the metabolic and endocrine responses to foods consumed later in the day in male habitual breakfast eaters. J Nutr 141, 13811389. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.110.128645 Google Scholar
Jenkins, DJA, Wolever, TMS, Ocana Vuksan, AM, et al. (1990) Metabolic effects of reducing rate of glucose ingestion by single bolus v. continuous sipping. Diabetes 39, 775781. https://doi.org/10.2337/diab.39.7.775 Google Scholar
Isacco, L, Duch, P & Boisseau, N (2012) Influence of hormonal status on substrate utilization at rest and during exercise in the female population. Sports Med 42, 327342. https://doi.org/10.2165/11598900-000000000-00000 Google Scholar
Yeung, EH, Zhang, C, Mumford, SL, et al. (2010) Longitudinal study of insulin resistance and sex hormones over the menstrual cycle: the BioCycle Study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 95, 54355442. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2010-0702 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Participant flow chart of adolescent girls who participated in a randomised crossover study design involving three conditions.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Schematic of the three experimental conditions. BO, breakfast omission; EM-BC, early-morning breakfast consumption; MM-BC, mid-morning breakfast consumption.

Figure 2

Table 1. Participant characteristics (n 15) (Mean values and standard deviations; median values and interquartile ranges)

Figure 3

Table 2. The average energy and macronutrient composition of breakfast and lunch meals consumed during the experimental conditions (n 15) (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 4

Figure 3. Changes in plasma glucose (a), glucose incremental AUC (iAUC) (b), plasma insulin (c), and insulin iAUC (d) concentrations during early-morning breakfast condition (EM-BC, open circle), mid-morning breakfast condition (MM-BC, open triangle) and breakfast omission (BO, open square). Data are presented as means and standard deviation. Black rectangles on panels (a) and (c) indicate consumption of breakfast (at 0 h for EM-BC and 2·0 h for MM-BC) and lunch meals (at 4·0 h for all conditions). Circles on panels (b) and (d) represent individual data values during the pre- (0–4 h, 240 min) and post-lunch periods (4–6 h, 120 min) of each condition. The iAUC (b) and (d) were divided by the time course of pre-lunch (240 min) and post-lunch periods (120 min) to present the values in mmol·l–1 for glucose and pmol·l–1 for insulin.

Figure 5

Table 3. Mean glucose, insulin and substrates oxidation values for each condition and pairwise comparisons between conditions during pre-lunch period (0–4 h, 240 min) (Mean values and standard deviations; geometric mean, mean difference, percentage difference, and their corresponding 95% CI)

Figure 6

Table 4. Mean glucose, insulin and substrates oxidation values for each condition and pairwise comparisons between conditions during post-lunch period (4–6 h, 120 min) (Mean values and standard deviations; geometric mean, mean difference, percentage difference, and their corresponding 95% CI)

Figure 7

Figure 4. Changes in carbohydrate oxidation (a), carbohydrate oxidation total AUC (tAUC) (b), fat oxidation (c) and fat oxidation tAUC (d) during early-morning breakfast condition (EM-BC, open circle), mid-morning breakfast condition (MM-BC, open triangle) and breakfast omission (BO, open square). Data are presented as means and standard deviation. Black rectangles on panels (a) and (c) indicate consumption of breakfast (at 0 h for EM-BC and 2·0 h for MM-BC) and lunch meals (at 4·0 h for all conditions). Circles on panels (b) and (d) represent individual data values during the pre- (0–4 h, 240 min) and post-lunch periods (4–6 h, 120 min) of each condition. The tAUC (b) and (d) were divided by the time course of pre-lunch (240 min) and post-lunch periods (120 min) to present the values in mg·min–1.

Supplementary material: File

Afeef et al. supplementary material

Afeef et al. supplementary material
Download Afeef et al. supplementary material(File)
File 22.8 KB