INTRODUCTION
While we know that a number of far-right groups’ offline activities—including the Stop the Steal rally and subsequent insurrection at the US Capitol on January 6, 2021—have been organized, in part, online, it remains difficult to assess whether and in what manner routine, everyday online discussions among right-wing extremist groups’ adherents relate to their offline behavior. To better understand this relationship, we must overcome at least two interrelated challenges. The first challenge is technical. Analyzing the incredible volume of online communication has proven difficult (Vidgen et al. Reference Vidgen, Harris, Nguyen, Tromble, Hale and Margetts2019), even for the largest and best-resourced social media platforms. And yet, we argue, the more fundamental challenge is a theoretical one. To enhance our analytical capabilities, analyses of online content—even large-scale online content—must draw on and develop technical approaches that are grounded in the rich, nuanced, and more complex social scientific theories of political communication and mobilization. In other words, by viewing online content not as a set of individual posts containing discrete messaging, but rather as a complex and rich discursive environment, in which messages ebb and flow over time, we are more likely to uncover the mobilizing power and potential of online communication.
In this study, we examine the relationship between the online communication of one prominent right-wing extremist group—the Proud Boys—and the group’s offline activities using the long-standing and well-developed collective action framing literature as the core theoretical lens driving our analytical approach. The power of messages and speech to motivate and mobilize group members lies at the heart of collective action framing theory. According to the foundational work of Goffman (Reference Goffman1974) and Snow and Benford (Reference Snow and Benford1988), collective action frames denote “schemata of interpretation” that enable individuals “to locate, perceive, identify, and label” occurrences within their life space and the world at large (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000, 21). Frames help to render events or occurrences meaningful and thereby function to organize experience and guide action.
Collective action frames simplify and condense aspects of the “world out there,” but in ways that are “intended to mobilize potential adherents and constituents, to garner bystander support, and to demobilize antagonists” (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000, 198). This framework has been used by scholars to examine the communication patterns of a multitude of violent and nonviolent social movement organizations. In particular, research has explored the ways in which social movement actors deploy collective action frames to promote group identity and solidarity, motivate actions, and interpret and make sense of information that could challenge the group’s foundational beliefs (Bos et al. Reference Bos, Schemer, Corbu, Hameleers, Andreadis, Schulz and Schmuck2020; Goh and Pang Reference Goh and Pang2016; Oktavianus, Davidson, and Guan Reference Oktavianus, Davidson and Guan2021).
Following this tradition, our analysis focuses on the degree to which members of the Proud Boys deploy three high-level categories of collective action frames—diagnostic, prognostic, and motivational—and two subcategories of diagnostic frames—injustice and othering—in their everyday conversations on the social media platform Telegram (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000; Hunt, Benford, and Snow Reference Hunt, Benford and Snow1994; Snow and Benford Reference Snow and Benford1988). We then examine the temporal correlation of these frames with both violent and nonviolent offline activities by members of this extremist group.
Carefully operationalizing these concepts, we fine-tune a state-of-the-art supervised machine learning model that allows us to identify each type of frame and subframe at scale and analyze their variation over time. Applying the models to more than 500,000 messages posted on 92 Proud Boys-affiliated Telegram channels between January 1, 2020, and July 26, 2022, our empirical analysis reveals that diagnostic messages—those that identify a problem and/or assign attribution for a problem perceived to afflict the group—are effectively omnipresent in online discussions. However, increases in the proportion of these messages predict subsequent offline violence. Motivational messages—those that boost in-group morale, pride, or solidarity—on the other hand, prove both responsive to and predictive of offline activities. As our analysis suggests, these solidarity-building messages tend to increase in proportion after nonviolent offline protests. In turn, upticks in the proportion and frequency of motivational frames prove a strong predictor of offline violence. We refer to these reciprocal dynamics as the online messaging–offline action cycle.
These findings empirically support the capacity for collective action frames to mobilize group members, as posited by the social movement literature. However, if we wish to understand the cyclical dynamics observed in our analysis, we argue that we must go beyond the social movement framework, supplementing it with insights from research on “moralizing” and “moral convergence” (Mooijman et al. Reference Mooijman, Hoover, Lin, Ji and Dehghani2018). Diagnostic collective action frames, we suggest, lay the groundwork for offline mobilization by generating moral justification for action. Diagnostic frames are foundational to an extremist group’s sense of their place in an unjust world. However, in addition to believing that action is morally justified, engaging in violence is more likely when members of extremist organizations also trust that they are acting in common cause, on the basis of shared values. Prompted by offline encounters that enhance this sensibility, solidarity-building motivational frames rise in prominence online, and the prominence of these messages may in turn help to spur offline violence.
In laying out these arguments and findings, we begin with a discussion of the core theoretical insights guiding our analysis. Next, we turn to our research design and methodologies, including an overview of our case study, the Proud Boys’ communication on Telegram. This section also offers a description of our computational approaches and how they relate to previous work and ends with an overview of our statistical analyses. We then lay out our findings and conclude with a discussion of their theoretical and practical implications.
THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
Online Communication and Offline Activities
Relative to previous eras, social media has empowered a more diffuse set of grassroots actors and groups to organize and mobilize its members for action (Shirky Reference Shirky2008). A growing body of research has substantiated the correlation between Internet use and political participation (see Boulianne Reference Boulianne2015), illuminating a number of dynamics and nuances in this relationship. For example, one study found that specific types of social media use (e.g., expressive, informational, and relational) increased citizen engagement, whereas other uses (e.g., identity and entertainment) did not (Skoric et al. Reference Skoric, Zhu, Goh and Pang2016). Other research has shown that the affordances of different platforms affect the likelihood of participation in distinct ways. For instance, while strong ties on Facebook prove more effective for protest-related communication, weak ties appear to be key on Twitter (Valenzuela, Correa, and Gil de Zúñiga Reference Valenzuela, Correa and de Zúñiga2018).
A large body of research has also documented the ways in which right-wing extremist groups specifically make use of the Internet to recruit followers, broadcast their intolerant worldview and rhetoric, inculcate a sense of identity and community among members, and organize offline activities (Greene Reference Greene2019; Munn Reference Munn2019; Scrivens, Davies, and Frank Reference Scrivens, Davies and Frank2020). However, the body of systematic empirical inquiries into the correlation between online speech and offline activities by right-wing extremist groups is still nascent (Müller and Schwarz Reference Müller and Schwarz2021; Reference Müller and Schwarz2023; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Burnap, Javed, Liu and Ozalp2020). One analysis of word frequencies in Proud Boys’ Gab communications before and after the far-right “Free Speech” rally in Berkeley and the Unite the Right riot in Charlottesville found that post-event Gab posts included more incident-driven rhetoric than the pre-rally speech, which the researchers suggest may serve a myth-making function for the group in the wake of controversial activities (Reid, Valasik, and Bagavathi Reference Reid, Valasik and Bagavathi2020). Another study found that increased online engagement on Facebook between oppositional protest groups predicted increased violence between these groups when they met offline (Gallacher, Heerdink, and Hewstone Reference Gallacher, Heerdink and Hewstone2021).
Online Communication and Social Identity
Another emergent body of literature applies concepts related to group identity to analyses of social media data. Much of this work draws from social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel and Turner2004), in which individuals’ sense of self-worth, belongingness, and identity largely stem from their membership in groups. Moreover, the in-groups to which an individual belongs are often defined and evaluated relative to other groups (i.e., out-groups). The resulting tension shapes many dimensions of intergroup interactions and conflict. An analysis of the recent resurgence of populism, for example, demonstrates how in-group/out-group frames that pit the common man against corrupt elites for governmental failings increase individuals’ support for populist candidates and ideas (Busby, Gubler, and Hawkins Reference Busby, Gubler and Hawkins2019).
Social media platforms offer a low-cost, accessible, relatively efficient platform for group members to find one another to coalesce around and negotiate their collective identity and perceived position in society relative to other groups, as well as organize for collective action on behalf of the group’s interests (Khazraee and Novak Reference Khazraee and Novak2018; Makki et al. Reference Makki, DeCook, Kadylak and JuYoung Lee2018; Velasquez and Montgomery Reference Velasquez and Montgomery2020; Wang, Liu, and Gao Reference Wang, Liu and Gao2016). “Social groupings and divisions are constituted in and through communication…the identities that are consequential for politics are not structural givens, they are created over time by political and social actors and made salient at particular moments as an organizing basis of political life” (Kreiss, Lawrence, and McGregor Reference Kreiss, Lawrence and McGregor2020, 4). For example, a recent analysis of Facebook and Twitter has found that posts mentioning a political out-group were shared approximately twice as often as those about the in-group (Rathje, Van Bavel, and Van Reference Rathje, Van Bavel and Van Der Linden2021). Within the context of right-wing extremism, there is also evidence of white supremacist groups using memes for socialization and identity-building (DeCook Reference DeCook2018), and another study shows how white nationalists use the online message board Stormfront to attempt to appeal to mainstream whites, by emphasizing pride and communal well-being that creates a rhetorical distance between white nationalism and white supremacy (Hartzell Reference Hartzell2020).
Collective Action Framing
Pulling these strands of literature together, our analysis considers whether and how online communication that features specific types of collective action frames might increase the likelihood that members of a right-wing extremist group will participate in offline events. We focus on collective action frames because these often serve to construct and maintain group identity, create a shared worldview and understanding of where that group fits into society, and mobilize members to act on behalf of the group. Similar to media frames discussed in the political communication literature, collective action frames highlight specific dimensions of an issue or event and call to mind certain values, allowing the frames’ purveyors—whether media outlets or social movement organizations—the ability to influence how their audience perceives and interprets the information relayed in a message and, in some instances, shaping their reactions to it (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007). Whereas broadcast media traditionally had substantial latitude to select and deploy such frames, social media has empowered average citizens and grassroots groups to, on occasion, challenge and recast even dominant frames (Guggenheim et al. Reference Guggenheim, Mo Jang, Young Bae and Russell Neuman2015; Jackson and Foucault Reference Jackson and Foucault Welles2015).
In this study, we investigate the volume and proportion of Telegram messages posted to Proud Boys-affiliated channels that feature one or more of three high-level categories of collective action frames—diagnostic, prognostic, and motivational—as well as two subcategories of diagnostic frames—injustice and othering. (Please see Supplementary Table A2 for more information regarding the annotation scheme and examples of each of the frames.)
Diagnostic Collective Action Frames
We begin with diagnostic frames, which identify some problem and/or attribute blame for a problem (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000) that is perceived to negatively affect the group and its members. For example, right-wing extremist groups commonly decry censorship, perceived threats to their racial or religious identity, the decline of certain social values or norms, and societal changes that they believe have emasculated or diminished men’s traditional role in society. Within this diagnostic frame category, we examine two prevalent subcategories—namely, “injustice” and “othering” frames. These subframes are identified in relevant bodies of literature as both prevalent components of right-wing extremist discourse and effective collective action frames for mobilizing members of groups more broadly.
Injustice frames define an action, policy, event, or phenomenon as fundamentally unfair to those within an in-group. Previous studies substantiate the prevalence of injustice frames in the rhetoric used by white extremist groups (Adams and Roscigno Reference Adams and Roscigno2005; Berbrier Reference Berbrier1998; Reference Berbrier2000; Bubolz and Simi Reference Bubolz and Simi2019), in which a Manichean worldview portrays whites as the aggrieved in-group. When a group’s identity centers on a shared sense of being aggrieved, this increases the likelihood of mobilization because members are more likely to engage in collective action when its goal is to regain their in-group’s “appropriate” or “deserved” status by confronting those they believe are responsible for this state of affairs (Kawakami and Dion Reference Kawakami and Dion1995; Van Zomeren, Postmes, and Spears Reference Van Zomeren, Postmes and Spears2008). Moreover, when victimhood and injustice are associated with group identity, members are more likely to feel anger about that perceived injustice, which increases the likelihood that members will take action on behalf of the group (Mackie, Devos, and Smith Reference Mackie, Devos and Smith2000).
Whereas injustice frames focus on the perceived unjust treatment of the in-group, othering frames turn the focus to out-groups. Increasing the perceived threat an out-group poses, dehumanizing its members, and/or amplifying the perceived incompatibility between the groups and their ways of life can be an effective catalyst for mobilizing in-group members to act against an out-group (Postmes et al. Reference Postmes, Branscombe, Spears and Young1999; Simon and Klandermans Reference Simon and Klandermans2001; Van Zomeren, Postmes, and Spears Reference Van Zomeren, Postmes and Spears2008). “Research in the field of identity framing has indicated that in-group mobilization results from priming a severe threat to the well-being of the group… motivating the in-group to take action” (Bos et al. Reference Bos, Schemer, Corbu, Hameleers, Andreadis, Schulz and Schmuck2020, 6). This amplified sense of threat also helps provide moral cover or justification for the actions taken to defend the in-group from this threatening other. “In concrete terms, this could be manifest as the argument that white supremacists are simply concerned with the survival of their people, and that if some on the fringe feel that urgent action is required as a result of dangers posed by sinister outside forces, that is understandable” (Berbrier Reference Berbrier2000, 187).
Taken together, diagnostic frames generate a sense of threat against and, crucially, a moral justification for acting to protect (Mooijman et al. Reference Mooijman, Hoover, Lin, Ji and Dehghani2018; Skitka, Bauman, and Sargis Reference Skitka, Bauman and Sargis2005; Skitka and Morgan Reference Skitka and Morgan2014) group members and their interests. This leads to the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1: An increase in the frequency and/or proportion of diagnostic frames in online communications among members of the Proud Boys predicts an increased likelihood of its members participating in an offline event.
Hypothesis 1A: An increase in the frequency and/or proportion of injustice frames in online communications among members of the Proud Boys predicts an increased likelihood of its members participating in an offline event.
Hypothesis 1B: An increase in the frequency and/or proportion of othering frames in online communications among members of the Proud Boys predicts an increased likelihood of its members participating in an offline event.
Prognostic Collective Action Frames
If diagnostic frames point to problems and attribute blame for those problems, prognostic frames, in turn, “suggest solutions” and “identify strategies, tactics, and targets” for addressing a problem (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000, 201). The logic connecting prognostic frames with offline participation is perhaps the most self-evident, since by definition these messages entail some sort of call-to-action to protest, address, or take action to ameliorate some perceived problem afflicting the in-group. However, in some cases, the solution presented is more abstract and broadly conceived, including calls to come together to “do something” to reclaim the group’s perceived way of life.
In considering prognostic frames, we hypothesize as follows:
Hypothesis 2: An increase in the frequency and/or proportion of prognostic frames in online communications among members of the Proud Boys predicts an increased likelihood of its members participating in an offline event.
Motivational Collective Action Frames
Lastly, motivational frames provide “a rationale for action” or a “vocabulary of motives” for mobilizing (Snow and Benford Reference Snow and Benford1988, 202) by positively priming some aspect of the in-group identity. Motivational frames serve to boost morale, pride, and/or a sense of belonging or duty to that group, each of which increases the likelihood of mobilization (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000; Berbrier Reference Berbrier1998; Reference Berbrier2000; Van Zomeren, Postmes, and Spears Reference Van Zomeren, Postmes and Spears2008). To this end, these messages often reference shared values, principles, priorities, norms, and/or characteristics of that group’s identity. Additionally, motivational messages can serve to increase the group’s sense of its own strength, numbers, and/or support from powerful allies, tilting the cost–benefit calculus in favor of engaging in an offline activity. “Appraisals of in-group strength produce anger toward an opponent out-group, and anger is a potent predictor of offensive action tendencies” (Mackie, Devos, and Smith Reference Mackie, Devos and Smith2000, 613; see also Mooijman et al. Reference Mooijman, Hoover, Lin, Ji and Dehghani2018; Skitka, Bauman, and Sargis Reference Skitka, Bauman and Sargis2005), which is exemplified by the sharp increase in hate crimes and extremist activities in the years following the election of Donald Trump to the presidency (Williamson and Gelfand Reference Williamson and Gelfand2019).
Finally, motivational frames can “supply adherents with compelling reasons or rationales for taking action and provide participants with justifications for actions undertaken on behalf of the movement’s goals, particularly when their behavior is called into question by friends, family or coworkers” (Benford and Hunt Reference Benford and Hunt1992, 41). In this sense, these frames can also function to destigmatize, mainstream, and legitimize the group’s image and intolerant worldview. For right-wing extremists, “The mandate to love and have pride in one’s heritage are presented as universally valid… People are exhorted to ‘love your heritage and love your culture’ and ‘everyone should be allowed to do so’” (Berbrier Reference Berbrier1998, 442). Such messages can, thus, also function to provide moral cover for actions taken by group members.
This generates the following prediction:
Hypothesis 3: An increase in the frequency and/or proportion of motivational frames in online communications among members of the Proud Boys predicts an increased likelihood of its members participating in an offline event.
RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY
Case Study—The Proud Boys and Telegram
Founded by far-right political commentator Gavin McInnes in 2016, the Proud Boys are an all-male, far-right neo-fascist group that describes themselves as “proud Western chauvinists who refuse to apologize for creating the modern world” (McBain Reference McBain2020). Classified as a far-right extremist organization by the FBI (Wilson Reference Wilson2018), as of 2021, they had 116 chapters across 46 states. Proud Boys members often mobilize alongside other white supremacist groups, including at high-profile events such as the Charlottesville Unite the Right rally and the UC Berkeley protests of 2017. The Proud Boys also regularly participate in violent street brawls and frequently use social media to encourage violence toward perceived enemies (Anti-Defamation League 2018). Their visible and documented participation in the January 6 insurrection has also made them a focal point for prosecution and investigation efforts by multiple government agencies (Walker Reference Walker2022).
The Proud Boys have long relied on a variety of social media platforms for both internal and external communication—recruiting new members, planning activities, growing solidarity among their adherents, and attempting to share and mainstream their beliefs with the wider public. However, in recent years, major social media platforms have taken relatively strict enforcement action against the Proud Boys, including the founder McInnes. In 2018, both Twitter and Facebook announced that they would deplatform the group and quickly removed the largest accounts linked to the organization and its leaders (Linton Reference Linton2018). Like other right-wing extremist groups, the Proud Boys responded to these bans by migrating to smaller, alternative social media platforms, such as Telegram. As of 2021, there was a multitude of Proud Boys Telegram channels, the largest of which had more than 28,000 members.
Telegram is a free cloud-based instant messaging platform created in 2013 by brothers Pavel and Nikolai Durov, amid their own troubles with the authoritarian Russian state’s censorship of online speech (Urman and Katz Reference Urman and Katz2022). Telegram offers several options for engagement, including private one-on-one conversations, group chats, and both private and public channels that are controlled by admins. According to the Telegram website,Footnote 1 the company does not partake in extensive content-moderation policies, only removing pornographic material and blocking ISIS-related terrorist activity on its public channels (Walther and McCoy Reference Walther and McCoy2021). As a result, Telegram has attracted users from across a range of hate-based organizations.
Though Telegram has been a favored platform for extremist groups for a number of years—with its popularity increasing dramatically following the January 6th Capitol insurrection (Dickson Reference Dickson2021)—to date, relatively few studies have been published that investigate how these groups make use of Telegram. One recent analysis of the content of a dozen far-right Telegram channels identified white grievances, and blaming minorities for those grievances, as dominant themes in their discussions (Al-Rawi Reference Al-Rawi2021)—paralleling the diagnostic subframes of injustice and othering that we investigate here. Another study of three far-right groups in Germany (QAnon, Identitarian Movement, and Querdenken) identified the presence of several radicalizing narratives, including anti-elitism, support for violence, activism, and conspiracy theories (Schulze et al. Reference Schulze, Hohner, Greipl, Girgnhuber, Desta and Rieger2022)—types of speech that have implications for mobilization.
Data Collection
We collected our data from publicly viewable Proud Boys-affiliated Telegram channels. Typically, each Telegram channel is set up by its owner to allow broadcast or one-way communication from a small set of senders to a broader set of general channel users. However, some channels are interactive, allowing channel followers to share and respond to posts. Content in a channel primarily consists of messages comprised of text, images, audio files, and/or video files. To collect content and metadata from Proud Boys channels, we used Telethon, a Python-based interface for the Telegram API. Data provided by the Telegram API include channel and post-level metadata, as well as the content of the posts themselves.
Channel metadata provided by the Telegram API include the unique identification number, title, creation date, current count of users, and various channel settings (e.g., usage configurations, administrator restrictions, and whether the channel is a bot), as well as the actual messages sent in the channel. Messages can be either original content posted to a channel or forwarded content from another channel. We did not collect user handles pertaining to the individuals who posted on these channels.
We started with a seed list of 10 Proud Boys-affiliated broadcast channels on Telegram identified by leading organizations working in this domain.Footnote 2 To grow the list of channels, we relied on mentions of other channels and the “forwarding” feature within Telegram, whereby content can be shared between channels. Each time we encountered a new mention of a channel or content forwarded from a channel that was not already on our list, we added the channel to our list, collected its data, and followed all of its channels. This snowball “crawling” approach resulted in a list of more than 2,900 unique channels that are closely affiliated with the Proud Boys.
This network, depicted in Figure 1, was created by using all forwardings from the 571 channels with full histories (self-citations were included). Forwardings include direct re-posts from other channels/groups (sometimes with their added comments) in message texts. It has 2,934 nodes. The node size represents eigenvector centrality, which measures a node’s importance while giving consideration to the importance of its neighbors. The larger the node size is, the greater influence the node has.
The dataset for this analysis comprises the text of 514,368 messages posted between January 1, 2020 and July 26, 2022 in 92 channels that explicitly identify as Proud Boys-affiliated in their username, title, or bio.Footnote 3 Although this is a relatively small portion of the full list of Proud Boys-aligned channels, these 92 channels form the core of the larger communication network on Telegram. To illustrate this, Figure 1 shows the message-forwarding network among all 2,934 channels. The nodes are colored by type: Black nodes represent explicitly Proud Boys-affiliated channels, and gray indicates other far-right related channels that promote Proud Boys-aligned content. The size of the node is based on how often messages were forwarded from that channel to another within the network. Almost all of the explicitly Proud Boys-affiliated channels are near the center of the network graph and are also comparatively large, meaning these channels serve as important hubs for sharing Proud Boys-related content with the larger far-right Telegram ecosystem.
Computational Approach to Collective Action Frame Classification
Most work focused on developing computational models for classifying extremist communication online has been shaped by a content moderation lens. That is, this work has been motivated largely by an interest in identifying and in turn responding to—for example, by removing, hiding, or down-ranking—harmful content and those who produce it, especially using automated means (Ahmed, Vidgen, and Hale Reference Ahmed, Vidgen and Hale2022; Kiritchenko, Nejadgholi, and Fraser Reference Kiritchenko, Nejadgholi and Fraser2021). This has led to particular focus on concepts such as abusive language, toxicity, and hate speech (Bianchi et al. Reference Bianchi, Hills, Rossini, Hovy, Tromble and Tintarev2022; Davidson et al. Reference Davidson, Warmsley, Macy and Weber2017; Mercan et al. Reference Mercan, Jamil, Hameed, Magsi, Bazai and Shah2021; Schmidt and Wiegand Reference Schmidt and Wiegand2019; Waseem and Hovy Reference Waseem and Hovy2016). Social media platforms themselves tend to shape their policies around manifestly harmful communication (Meta 2022). In most instances, extremist communication is treated as a discrete speech act, with methods and interventions attending to individual posts.
These automated approaches have grown in sophistication over time, especially as interdisciplinary work has helped generate models rooted in richer and more nuanced conceptual frameworks (Bianchi et al. Reference Bianchi, Hills, Rossini, Hovy, Tromble and Tintarev2022; Vidgen et al. Reference Vidgen, Nguyen, Margetts, Rossini and Tromble2021). This body of work has also helped to identify and address some of the most unambiguously worrisome content online. However, less attention has been paid to types of communication that may lead to harmful outcomes but are not themselves, prima facie, hateful or abusive.
As laid out in the aforementioned theoretical framework, it is likely that extremist groups’ adherents are mobilized to action by a variety of frames that are part of relatively mundane, everyday discourse. And as such, if we are to better understand whether and how right-wing extremist groups’ online communication is related to offline mobilization, we need computational models that allow us to capture and analyze these more subtle and complex communicative dynamics. With this in mind, we have fine-tuned a state-of-the-art natural language processing (NLP) model, relying on supervised machine learning techniques, to identify collective action frames at scale.
Frame Annotation
In order to identify collective action frames within the messages in our dataset, we fine-tuned a pretrained language model using a gold label dataset of 12,189 messages labeled by a team of five trained undergraduate and graduate students at George Washington University. Annotators applied labels on the basis of a detailed codebook developed by two of the study’s authors. Pairs of students independently annotated posts in batches. The unit of analysis was the Telegram post, meaning that a single post could contain more than one frame, and each post was annotated with a binary “yes/no” for the presence or absence of a specific frame. Any disagreements between the paired annotators were identified, and the students met to resolve their disagreements. In order to prevent discrepancies from developing across annotators, the student pairs rotated with each batch, with one student designated in each rotation to attend the resolution meetings, observing and sharing any apparent discrepancies with the full team. The team then collectively agreed on a standard and clarified the codebook where needed.
The period in which these standards were being set and updated in the guidelines was treated as a training phase. Data from the training phase were re-annotated once the guidelines were settled. Once inter-annotator agreement was consistently high (above 0.7 for Gwet’s A.C. and 80% for agreement, with averages of 0.77 and 86% across all five labels), the team began the full annotation phase. The procedures remained the same during this phase, with one student continuing to observe resolution meetings. We measured inter-annotator agreement for every batch of messages, with averages of 0.82 Gwet’s A.C. score and 88% agreement across the full gold label dataset.
Computational Modeling and Analysis
Pretrained language models (Devlin et al. Reference Devlin, Chang, Lee and Toutanova2019; Radford et al. Reference Radford, Wu, Child, Luan, Amodei and Sutskever2019) are now among the most prominent techniques in NLP. These models are built with deep neural networks that are pretrained on corpora made of billions of words in a language modeling task: they learn to predict the next words (or missing words) from pieces of text. After pretraining, these models can be adapted to solve many tasks, such as classification, in many different languages with great performance (Nozza, Bianchi, and Hovy Reference Nozza, Bianchi and Hovy2020). Thus, we followed a common paradigm in current NLP research by making use of one such pretrained model—DeBERTa (He, Gao, and Chen Reference He, Gao and Chen2021), which has shown excellent performance in text classification tasks—and fine-tuned the model for our specific classification task—that is, detecting and labeling collective action frames.
Preprocessing
We applied only minor cleaning tasks to the data. We did not remove links from the text, but we split them to separate the words in the subdirectory part of the URL. For example, the link “www.shop.com/buy-our-stuff” was transformed to “www shop com buy our stuff.” Minor cleaning is applied to both the data for tuning the model and the dataset for the final analysis.
Model
DeBERTa is trained on a multi-label classification task—meaning that, given a message as an input, the model is trained to predict one or more labels that occur within the message, at the same time.
We trained the model on the collective action frames. In keeping with the standard approach in machine learning, we randomly divided the data into three sets: training (80% of the data), validation (10% of the data), and test (10% of the data). The training set was used to start training the model, while the validation set was used to select at which point, during the training, the model reached its best performance. Validation was run every 100 steps, with a total batch size of eight with a gradient accumulation of eight using a learning rate of 5e-5. The learning rate was identified using a grid search over the following set of values [1e-5, 5e-6, 8e-6, 9e-6, 5e-5].Footnote 4
Finally, we checked the results using the test set to ensure the macro-F1 scores of the labels met the accepted thresholds.Footnote 5 As an additional check of these results, we randomly sampled 376 previously unseen posts that were then labeled by one of the same student annotators who labeled the training set, who was blind to the results of the computational analysis. A comparison of the labels predicted by the computational model to those assigned by the student annotator yielded percent agreement rates ranging from 85% to 97% for the five categories, with an average Gwet’s AC score of 0.91.
Statistical Analysis
To investigate how the Proud Boys deploy various collective action frames ahead of and in response to offline activities, we merged the manually and computationally labeled datasets of Telegram messages with a subset of ACLED’s US Crisis Monitor data (ACLED 2019; Clionadh et al. Reference Clionadh, Linke, Hegre and Karlsen2010), consisting of 376 events in which members of the Proud Boys were identified as participating in between January 1, 2020, and July 26, 2022. In some cases, the Proud Boys organization is the primary or only organized group identified as participating in an event, in other cases they are one of the multiple groups.
The specific subcategories of Proud Boys-affiliated events identified in the ACLED data during this time period include arrests (7), attacks (2), changes to group/activity (1), peaceful protests (268), protests with (nonviolent) intervention (19), looting/property destruction (1), violent demonstrations (56), and events classified as “other” (22).Footnote 6 For the purpose of our analysis, we focus on two overarching categories, nonviolent protests and violent events. Violent events include those categorized as attacks, violent demonstrations, or looting/property destruction, whereas nonviolent protests include peaceful protests and protests that may have entailed some type of interaction with another group and/or authorities, but for which no known acts of violence or injuries occurred.Footnote 7
We collapsed both this subset of ACLED data and our Telegram dataset into the same seven-day periods, and then merged these together into a single time-series dataset consisting of weekly measures of the number of nonviolent protests Proud Boys members participated in, the number of violent events that Proud Boys members participated in, the percentage of Telegram posts that included each type of collective action frame, and the number of posts (logged) containing each type of frame during a given week.Footnote 8 This data structure permits time series analyses of weekly data to investigate the temporal correlation between and uni- or bidirectional predictive power of the prevalence and proportion of specific types of collective action frames with Proud Boys members’ participation in offline events.
Using Granger causality tests (Granger Reference Granger1969), we examine whether specific collective action frames “Granger cause” offline events and vice versa. Granger causality tests are a staple in political communication investigations of intermedia agenda-setting effects. For example, studies have employed these models to analyze whether political blogs set the agenda for traditional news media outlets or vice versa (Meraz Reference Meraz2011), as well as the bidirectional agenda-setting relationships between traditional media outlets and both social media platforms (Groshek and Groshek Reference Groshek and Groshek2013; Su, Hu, and LaiLee Reference Su, Hu and LaiLee2020) and aggregate trends in online searches (Ragas, Tran, and Martin Reference Ragas, Tran and Martin2014).
Despite the model’s name, Granger causality tests do not test for causality in the theoretical sense—that is, they do not empirically substantiate that a specific factor caused another—nor do we seek to demonstrate causality in this research. Rather, Granger causality tests the predictive power or incremental forecasting value of one variable (and its lags) on another variable in the model. In other words, it assesses whether including a specific variable improves the model’s ability to accurately predict subsequent levels of another variable.
We complement these findings with impulse response functions (IRFs), which trace the impact of a one-standard-deviation “shock” (i.e., impulse) in one variable on the subsequent behavior of other variables in the model and itself (Hamilton Reference Hamilton1994; Lütkepohl Reference Lütkepohl2005). Thus, IRFs also do not test for a causal relationship between the variables but, rather, calculate and map the interrelationships within and between variables over time.Footnote 9
To generate the Granger causality and IRF results, we begin with vector autoregressive (VAR) models, which model a vector of variables dependent on their own lags as well as on the lags of the other variables included in the vector (Lütkepohl Reference Lütkepohl2005). Before conducting the VAR tests, we examined the data to ensure they adhered to the assumptions integral to this type of analysis. First, we employed Akaike information criterion (AIC) and final prediction error (FPE) tests to identify the optimal lag lengths (Akaike Reference Akaike, Parzen, Tanabe and Kitagawa1988; Takeshi Reference Takeshi1985), which indicated up to two weeks as the optimal lag length across the models.Footnote 10 After doing so, we conducted augmented Dickey–Fuller (ADF) tests, in which all of the variables exhibited stationarity at the optimal number of two lags (Dickey and Fuller Reference Dickey and Fuller1979; Hamilton Reference Hamilton1994). We next tested the results of the VAR models for stability (Hamilton Reference Hamilton1994; Lütkepohl Reference Lütkepohl1993), as well as the absence of autocorrelation of the residuals using Lagrange multiplier (LM) tests (Davidson and MacKinnon Reference Davidson and MacKinnon1993; Johansen Reference Johansen1995). These tests confirm that the data satisfactorily meet the conditions for a VAR analysis at level for two lags.Footnote 11
FINDINGS
Our analysis reveals a clear correlation between the percentage and frequency of posts to Proud Boys Telegram channels that include one or more of the collective action frames and the participation of members in offline events. Before discussing our results in more detail in the following subsections, however, it is useful to reiterate that these models do not test for a causal relationship in the theoretical sense. Rather, our objective is to test whether these variables are temporally correlated with one another, such that changes to one or more of the variables forecast or predict subsequent behavior or trends of the other variables. Thus, in the context of this longitudinal analysis, the term “predict” does not entail any claim of an empirical test of causality. This approach instead investigates how Proud Boys’ online discussions relate to their offline activities by illuminating the types of discursive appeals they use while mobilizing ahead of and in the aftermath of offline events.
Overview
We consider both the sheer number (i.e., frequency) of messages and the percentage (i.e., proportion) of messages featuring a given frame per week, each of which captures a meaningful dimension of the Telegram information ecosystem and conversations happening between members of the Proud Boys. Whereas frequency captures the extent and magnitude of a particular type of conversation, the relative percentage of posts featuring a given frame captures the tenor and focus of the Proud Boys discourse at a given point in time.Footnote 12
Beginning with an overview of the presence of these frames in Proud Boys conversations on Telegram, each of the collective action frames was a regular component of messages posted to these Telegram channels. Diagnostic frames are the most prevalent, appearing in an average of 34% of the weekly posts to the 92 channels included in our analysis, with averages of 13% of the weekly messages including an injustice frame and 11% including an othering frame. The next most common type of frame is prognostic, which appeared in an average of 12% of the weekly posts, followed by motivational frames at 9%. Moving to frequency, for the 92 channels comprising our core network of Proud Boys-affiliated channels, we find an average of 3,839 messages posted per week—ranging from a minimum of 356 messages to a maximum of 49,228 total messages in a specific week—with weekly averages of 1,185 messages featuring diagnostic frames, 423 featuring prognostic frames, and 293 featuring motivational frames.
Nonviolent Protests
The results of our analysis of nonviolent protests fail to substantiate the mobilizing capacity of any of the three collective action frames hypothesized by the social movement literature—neither the proportion nor frequency of any of these frames predicts participation in an impending nonviolent protest. However, nonviolent protests strongly predict an increase in the percentage of messages that feature a motivational frame over the following weeks. (Please see Table 1 and Figure 2 for these results.) This raises the possibility that offline nonviolent protests may function to provide fodder for or a focal point that shapes subsequent online conversations between members of the Proud Boys, the potential implications of which are discussed in the following sections.
Note: Significance levels indicated as *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.
Violent Events
In contrast to nonviolent protests, violent events are strongly predicted by an increase in the percentage of posts that include either a diagnostic or a motivational frame, as well as an increase in the number of messages that include a motivational frame. (Please see Table 2 and Figure 3.) These results substantiate Hypotheses 1 and 3—messages that lament or assign blame for some problem perceived to afflict the Proud Boys, or that positively prime in-group identity by emphasizing pride, solidarity, or morale, each increase the likelihood that Proud Boys members will participate in a violent event in the coming weeks. Turning to the diagnostic subframes, an increase in the percentage of messages that contain either an injustice or othering frame predicts an increased likelihood of a violent event in the coming weeks (p ≤ 0.07 and p ≤ 0.09, respectively)—in line with Hypotheses 1A and 1B.Footnote 13 (Please see Figure 4.)
Note: Significance levels indicated as *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.
Regional Analysis: A Robustness Check
To further investigate the predictive power of diagnostic and motivational frames for violent offline events, we shift the unit of analysis to a time series cross-sectional test of this correlation at the state level. Ideally, a geographically focused approach would entail a spatial analysis of geolocated Telegram data. However, correlating social media data with geographic outcomes remain prohibitively problematic, primarily due to the lack of data containing geolocation information and because self-reported user locations are often misleading (Hecht et al. Reference Hecht, Hong, Suh and Chi2011). Public channel data exposed by the Telegram API include metadata such as the unique identification number, title, and creation date, but not geographic location. Telegram users can opt in to provide their geolocation; however, very few do so. Moreover, because users self-select to provide their geolocation, this subset of data is biased in a manner that would be problematic for an analysis on this topic. Taken together, the sparse and unreliable nature of geolocated Telegram data renders it difficult to map online communication to offline events in a valid manner.
Additionally, our network analysis of the Proud Boys-affiliated channels revealed that this network is highly connected, with a mean undirected average clustering coefficient of 0.692 (Hansen et al. Reference Hansen, Shneiderman, Smith and Himelboim2020).Footnote 14 On average, about 34% of these Proud Boys-affiliated channels’ content was forwarded from other channels, including 11.4% directly from other explicitly Proud Boys-affiliated channels, highlighting this group’s highly networked nature on Telegram. Thus, online text messages of Proud Boys channels were not independent but rather strongly influenced by other channels—further demonstrating the limitations associated with mapping interdependent online communication with localized offline events.
On the theoretical side, rather than explicit calls-to-action (i.e., prognostic frames) being predictive of offline events in this context, it is messages that lament or assign blame for some problem perceived to afflict the Proud Boys (i.e., diagnostic frames) or that positively prime in-group identity (i.e., motivational frames) that predict Proud Boys members’ offline participation in a violent event. Thus, it is likely that the conversations and messages that motivate individuals to participate in offline activities are not geographically constrained—messages lauding the participation of Proud Boys in a protest in Seattle, or which bemoan some offense or injustice suffered by Proud Boys members in Idaho, could feasibly inspire offline activity by Proud Boys members in Minnesota.
Being mindful of these methodological and theoretical limitations, an analysis of this relationship at the regional level would nevertheless provide a useful robustness check of our main findings. To this end, we conducted a time series cross-sectional analysis of the 53 channels that explicitly self-identify with a particular region (e.g., “Houston Proud Boys” or “Indiana Proud Boys”), employing a first-difference model to test the correlation between the specific types of messages on these regional channels and participation in violent activities by Proud Boys in those respective states.Footnote 15 The results of this additional analysis further substantiate the correlation between motivational frames and violent offline events. In this case, the number of messages featuring a motivational frame shared on a regional channel is correlated with Proud Boys members’ participation in violent offline events in that state (p < 0.05). However, the correlation between diagnostic frames and violent offline events is nonsignificant. (Please see Table 3 for results.)
Note: Total observations = 3,099; number of groups = 53; range of observations/group = 11–133; r2 = 0.003. Significance levels indicated as *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.
An Online Messaging–Offline Action Cycle
Considering the analyses of nonviolent protests and violent events in tandem with one another illuminates a potential reciprocal relationship shared by online communication and offline events. Whereas Proud Boys members’ participation in nonviolent protests increases the percentage of motivational frames in their Telegram posts over the following weeks, this boost in the percentage and number of motivational frames, in turn, increases the likelihood that Proud Boys members will engage in an upcoming violent event. This raises the question of whether nonviolent protests may help lay the groundwork for later violent activities, in part, by shaping the tenor and focus of online conversations between group members. Nonviolent protests, for example, could provide fodder for online rallying cries and appeals to members’ sense of group pride, duty, or morale in the days and weeks following a protest. Additionally, these protests may function as a tangible show of force, increasing members’ sense of their strength in numbers and solidarity. As posited by collective action theory, these types of discursive appeals can recalibrate members’ cost–benefit calculus, such that participation in risky violent activities becomes more likely to be deemed as worthwhile, which we discuss further in the following section.Footnote 16
In a preliminary investigation of this potential reciprocal dynamic, superimposing the IRFs for nonviolent protests with percent motivational frames and violent events suggests a four-week timeline for the temporal relationships shared by these factors. (Please see Figure 6.) Whereas the effect of a one-deviation shock to nonviolent protests on percent motivational frames peaks at two weeks after the shock, the predicted effect of a one-deviation shock to motivational frames on violent events peaks two weeks after that. This is complemented by an IRF test of the direct temporal relationship shared by nonviolent protests and violent events, which reveals that nonviolent events predict the occurrence of a violent event four weeks later. (Please see Figure 5.)
Utilizing a structural equation model, we tested whether the increase in the percentage of motivational frames following nonviolent protests mediates their predictive power for subsequent violent events. (Please see Table 4.) A modified Baron and Kenney test (Iacobucci, Saldanha, and Deng Reference Iacobucci, Saldanha and Deng2007) finds that the effect of nonviolent protests on violent events is partially mediated by the increase in the percentage of messages that contain a motivational frame at the 0.10 significance level (X - > M: B = 0.002 and p = 0.10; M - > Y: B = 5.471 and p = 0.06; Sobel’s z-test: B = 0.01, p = 0.21), with an estimated 8% of the direct effect of nonviolent events on violent events mediated by the increase in the percentage of motivational messages. Although it should be stipulated that mediation analyses are limited by a number of caveats (Agler and De Boeck Reference Agler and De Boeck2017)—and, thus, these results should be considered with a grain of salt—this provides a starting point to investigate whether nonviolent protests are leveraged in online communication to make discursive appeals that increase group members’ willingness to participate in subsequent violent events.
Note: N = 130; coefficient of determination = 0.09; significance levels indicated as *p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.
Theoretical Implications
We did not undertake the aforementioned analysis with theoretically derived expectations in mind regarding the ways in which offline activities might impact online communication. Though scholars in the collective action framing tradition view framing as an act of meaning-making, this body of work has given more attention to the ways in which frames spur action. Yet our longitudinal analysis suggests a cyclical pattern between offline activities and certain online speech, which we believe merits further exploration in future research.
These findings also have potential theoretical implications for the social movement literature, as well as our understanding of the link between online communication and offline participation. In this regard, we suggest supplementing collective action framing theory with insights drawn from social and political psychology on “moralizing” and “moral convergence.” This body of work finds that attaching moral reasoning to—that is, moralizing—an issue or cause is associated with the following:
(a) greater preferred social and physical distance from attitudinally dissimilar others, (b) intolerance of attitudinally dissimilar others in both intimate (e.g., friend) and distant relationships…, (c) lower levels of good will and cooperativeness in attitudinally heterogeneous groups, and (d) a greater inability to generate procedural solutions to resolve disagreements (Skitka, Bauman, and Sargis Reference Skitka, Bauman and Sargis2005, 895).
Such social distance and unwillingness to cooperate, in turn, make risky behavior more palatable (Mooijman et al. Reference Mooijman, Hoover, Lin, Ji and Dehghani2018). For extremist groups, who, by definition, operate outside of the mainstream, offline mobilization is always at least somewhat risky, and thus, attaching moral reasoning to their activities is essential. Indeed, as our data suggest, these moral messages—in the form of diagnostic frames—are nearly omnipresent in online discussions between group members. We argue that these types of messages are key components of how a group conceptualizes its position and plight in an unjust world—moralizing prerequisites for mobilization. However, for mobilization to tip into violence, simple moralizing may not be enough. Support for violence is more likely when “moral convergence” has also taken place (Mooijman et al. Reference Mooijman, Hoover, Lin, Ji and Dehghani2018)—that is, when members believe that others within the group share these moral values. We argue that motivational frames, which positively prime in-group identity by emphasizing a group’s shared values, pride, strength and/or solidarity, constitute one such avenue for moral convergence.
One of the best opportunities for members of extremist groups to develop a sense of moral convergence, we suggest, is via offline interaction—such as at protest events, where members see firsthand that others think and feel as they do. They bond over shared ritual and risk. However, these same events are also subsequently recounted and celebrated online, which increases the groups’ sense of solidarity and camaraderie more broadly. Following an offline event, then, feelings of solidarity precipitated by offline activity translate to increased expressions of that solidarity online in the form of motivational frames, which enhances the sense of moral convergence for members more broadly. And with these perceptions heightened for more adherents, the willingness to engage in violence may itself grow stronger—resulting in the online messaging–offline action cycle represented in our empirical data.
CONCLUSION
The results of our empirical analysis support key insights drawn from social movement and collective action framing theory—both diagnostic and motivational messages prove predictive of Proud Boys’ participation in violent offline events, substantiating the capacity for these collective action frames to help mobilize group members. However, our results also point to the need for further theoretical development and testing. Not only are prognostic frames uncorrelated with the Proud Boys’ offline activities, but the relationships between diagnostic and motivational frames, on the one hand, and violent and nonviolent events, on the other, prove more complex than hypothesized. Combining collective action framing theory with insights from research on moralizing and moral convergence, we have offered an enhanced theoretical framework that attends to the cyclical nature of the observed relationship between online messaging and offline action. However, this inductively derived framework will require much more testing in future research into different extremist groups, platforms, and time frames.
Our findings also have important implications from a practical, content moderation perspective. While previous approaches to large-scale content detection and moderation have primarily focused on uncivil, hateful, and other toxic content tied to right-wing extremism (Ahmed, Vidgen, and Hale Reference Ahmed, Vidgen and Hale2022; Bianchi et al. Reference Bianchi, Hills, Rossini, Hovy, Tromble and Tintarev2022), by shifting attention to the Proud Boys’ use of collective action frames over time, we have shown how social media can be harnessed by groups to cultivate a worldview and sense of in-group identity and solidarity that lays the groundwork for mobilizing its members to participate in offline activities. While these sorts of messages are often angry and contentious, they are not always explicitly hateful.
That prognostic frames, which most often take the form of explicit calls-to-action, are not predictive of offline events in our analysis also has practical implications, particularly because these types of messages receive considerable attention from social media platforms. From a technical perspective, such calls-to-action are among the easiest to automatically detect, and platforms are particularly likely to moderate posts that call for, especially violent, offline action. However, it is also the case that this type of communication is more likely to occur via private or even encrypted channels, which are typically outside the domain of content moderation efforts. To be clear, we do not contend that platforms should allow such posts to remain unmoderated. However, our findings reveal the limitations of approaches that focus relatively narrowly on specific posts and their explicit content.
To the degree that other right-wing extremist groups share similar worldviews and grievances, it is likely that the dynamics identified in this study could generalize to similar groups on Telegram, both within and outside of the United States. However, the cost–benefit calculus of engaging in violence is likely to be contingent on additional group resources as well as the national context—suggesting that, even if these underlying dynamics function similarly, the threshold point at which the potential benefits of violent activity exceed its risks will vary.
Regarding the degree to which our findings may generalize to other platforms, it is useful to consider several factors, such as platform features, moderation policies, user base, and the overall focus of the platform. For example, relative to platforms that require verified identities, a platform that allows anonymity may lead to more aggressive or controversial behavior (Suler Reference Suler2004). Additionally, platforms with strict moderation policies may lead to more covert or coded language usage, while those with minimal moderation may encourage more overt and explicit extremist content (Bhat and Klein Reference Bhat and Klein2020). Finally, the composition of the user base and homophilic patterns of a platform’s network structures may also moderate the content of online discussions, particularly in terms of the formation of echo chambers and the nature of the information shared within (Cinelli et al. Reference Cinelli, De Francisci Morales, Galeazzi, Quattrociocchi and Starnini2021). Thus, although right-wing extremist activity may exhibit some similarities across different platforms, unique platform characteristics will influence the way extremist group members communicate, organize, and engage with one another.
While right-wing extremist groups do use social media platforms to plan logistics for various actions and events, they also mobilize through other, more subtle forms of communication. If we are to meaningfully enhance our understanding of the relationship between online communication by extremist groups and their offline behavior, we must take a more nuanced view of these communicative dynamics and how they develop over time. Using state-of-the-art computational techniques to capture the theoretically rich and nuanced concept of collective action framing—ultimately uncovering the complex, cyclical nature of these dynamics—we have shown how such work might progress.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055423001478.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Documentation, datasets, and all analysis scripts used in the statistical analyses are openly available at the American Political Science Review Dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OAOJQZ. The raw dataset containing the text of the Telegram posts, which was used in the hand-labeling and computational modeling procedures, will not be made publicly available. Although this dataset includes only text messages from public channels and no user’s personally identifiable information was collected by the authors, it still may be possible to reidentify users. To protect privacy and comply with the EU’s General Data Protection Regulation standards, these raw data will only be shared for limited research purposes and require a signed data-sharing agreement. Readers interested in requesting access to the raw data should email: [email protected].
Acknowledgements
We first thank our incredible team of research assistants: Autumn Archuleta, Carolyne Im, Elizabeth Irwin, Olivia Legault, Max Zweig, and others. We also thank the Institute for Data, Democracy & Politics and the John S. and James L. Knight Foundation for their support of our work. Thanks also to the editors and reviewers for their valuable feedback, as well as to those individuals at the Bridging Divides Initiative, the Holocaust Museum, Moonshot, and ADL who shared their time to chat with us during the early brainstorming phase of this project. All mistakes are our own.
FUNDING STATEMENT
This research is funded by the John S. and James L. Knight Foundation through a grant to the Institute for Data, Democracy & Politics at The George Washington University.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no ethical issues or conflicts of interest in this research.
ETHICAL STANDARDS
The authors affirm this research did not involve human subjects.
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.