Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:19:25.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is ‘another’ psychiatry possible?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

Diana Rose
Affiliation:
Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
Nikolas Rose*
Affiliation:
Australian National University, Canberra, Australia Institute of Advanced Studies, University College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Nikolas Rose, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this paper, we examine a number of approaches that propose new models for psychiatric theory and practices: in the way that they incorporate ‘social’ dimensions, in the way they involve ‘communities’ in treatment, in the ways that they engage mental health service users, and in the ways that they try to shift the power relations within the psychiatric encounter. We examine the extent to which ‘alternatives’ – including ‘Postpsychiatry’, ‘Open Dialogue’, the ‘Power, Threat and Meaning Framework’ and Service User Involvement in Research – really do depart from mainstream models in terms of theory, practice and empirical research and identify some shortcomings in each. We propose an approach which seeks more firmly to ground mental distress within the lifeworld of those who experience it, with a particular focus on the biopsychosocial niches within which we make our lives, and the impact of systematic disadvantage, structural violence and other toxic exposures within the spaces and places that constitute and constrain many everyday lives. Further, we argue that a truly alternative psychiatry requires psychiatric professionals to go beyond simply listening to the voices of service users: to overcome epistemic injustice requires professionals to recognise that those who have experience of mental health services have their own expertise in accounting for their distress and in evaluating alternative forms of treatment. Finally we suggest that, if ‘another psychiatry’ is possible, this requires a radical reimagination of the role and responsibilities of the medically trained psychiatrist within and outside the clinical encounter.

Type
Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

To ask this question is to presume that something – maybe a lot – is amiss with current psychiatry or rather with the variety of approaches available within psychiatry today. There have, of course, been dozens of critiques of the ‘medical model’ and the ‘psychiatrisation’ of sadness, anxiety and other relatively normal variations of mood and affects, but we will not rehearse them here (Conrad, Reference Conrad1992; Horwitz & Wakefield, Reference Horwitz and Wakefield2007; Lane, Reference Lane2007; Pilgrim, Reference Pilgrim2019; Rose, Perry, Rae, & Good, Reference Rose, Perry, Rae and Good2017; Scull, Reference Scull2015). Nor will we examine recent attempts to reshape psychiatric by adopting a ‘biopsychosocial approach’ (e.g. Savalescu, Davies, Roache, Davies, and Loebel, Reference Savalescu, Davies, Roache, Davies and Loebel2020). Rather, we shall look at those developments that call themselves ‘alternatives’ to psychiatry and compare them with aspects of mainstream psychiatry with two purposes. First, to highlight some of the shortcomings of current psychiatry and second to see how far these ‘alternatives’ really do depart from mainstream models in terms of theory, practice and empirical research. What is shared and what differs between these different approaches and ‘the mainstream’? If ‘another psychiatry’ is possible, what would be the role and responsibilities of the medically trained psychiatrist within and outside the clinical encounter.

The attempts to produce ‘another’ psychiatry range from social psychiatry to postpsychiatry. Social psychiatry is associated with figures such as Aubrey Lewis and Michael Shepherd and tried to situate the person in their social milieu (Bynum, Porter, & Shepherd, Reference Bynum, Porter and Shepherd2004; Lewis, Reference Lewis1953). But no developed theory of the ‘social’ was ever produced. The ‘social’ in social psychiatry was transformed into ‘the environment’, but this was weakly specified and often amounted only to a set of provoking or protective factors working on an underlying organic or biological constitution. As genetics and genomics once more came to the fore in psychiatric thinking, this relation was often specified in terms of simplistic formula – P = G × E – where the phenotype (P) of an individual – his or her psychiatric diagnosis – was a product of the interaction between genes (G) and environment (E), but this formula gave a scientific gloss to what was rarely if ever a clearly specified relationship (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2006; Modinos et al., Reference Modinos, Iyegbe, Prata, Rivera, Kempton, Valmaggia and McGuire2013)

Postpsychiatry

Postpsychiatry is associated with the work of two psychiatrists, Phil Thomas and Pat Bracken and, as the name suggests, proposes a link between post-modern thinking and a new psychiatry (Bracken et al., Reference Bracken, Fernando, Alsaraf, Creed, Double, Gilberthorpe and Timimi2021; Bracken & Thomas, Reference Bracken and Thomas2001). Bracken takes Foucault as his example of post-modernism which is unfortunate as Foucault was at pains to reject this description of his approach (Bracken, Reference Bracken1995). Postpsychiatry argued that psychiatry as we know it today is culturally and historically specific, rooted in Western Enlightenment ideas of the distinction between the normal and the pathological, and of the nature of the normal itself. Leaving aside the adequacy of this characterisation, there were two main implications of this argument for psychiatry, both of them focussed on practice. The first, based on their own work with the Bradford Home Treatment Service which was established in 1996 was to suggest that Home Treatment Teams were an alternative way of thinking and practicing, stressing the importance of context and culture, and the need to interpret symptoms as meaningful, opening a space for dialogue where the professional works with the patient to uncover the specific roots of her distress in her own life situation. In fact, Home Treatment Teams were not pioneered by post-psychiatry, but by Sashi Sashidharan and community activist Errol Francis in an attempt to address racism in services (Sashidharan & Francis, Reference Sashidharan and Francis1999). However as they have been implemented in practice, at least in the UK, they have changed the setting but not the interventions: they have become a way of coping with the lack of in-patient facilities, combining risk assessment in the patient's home – was the patient an immediate risk to themselves or others – and checking on medication adherence: the involvement of non-psychiatric professionals in the team is mainly a way of coping with the limited availability of medically trained psychiatrists themselves (Wheeler et al., Reference Wheeler, Lloyd-Evans, Churchard, Fitzgerald, Fullarton, Mosse and Johnson2015).

Second, Postpsychiatry calls for the mental health survivor movement to be taken seriously, and for treatment to focus on the meanings and values of the recipient of mental health care. But while the emphasis was on the way in which such meanings and values should figure in the treatment encounter, the suggestion that this might enable power-sharing, allowing the patient to produce his or her own interpretation of their symptoms, ignored some fundamental asymmetries in the powers of patient and professional (Perestelo-Perez, Gonzalez-Lorenzo, Perez-Ramos, Rivero-Santana, & Serrano-Aguilar, Reference Perestelo-Perez, Gonzalez-Lorenzo, Perez-Ramos, Rivero-Santana and Serrano-Aguilar2011). We can see this in the UK's National Service Framework for Mental Health (Department of Health, 1999) which Bracken and Thomas cites as a further example of the aspect of ‘post-psychiatry.’ This stressed the crucial importance of the involvement of service users. Users expectations were to be core to its principles: the Service was to ‘(a) meaningfully involve users and their carers; (b) deliver high quality treatment and care which is effective and acceptable; (c) be non-discriminatory; (d) be accessible: help when and where it is needed; (e) promote user safety and that of their carers, staff and the wider public; (f) offer choices which promote independence….’ (Thornicroft, Reference Thornicroft2000). While these values seem virtuous, the involvement of users themselves in the process of development of this Framework did not bode well for its implementation. One of us (DR) interviewed all 8 users who were involved in developing the NSF and all said they would never do such work again – indeed five did not last the course. The service users felt that they were not allocated work commensurate with their knowledge and skills. The Black users had a particularly torrid time and were distressed upon recalling it. They experienced racism from white users and were completely ignored by the Black professionals (Wallcraft, Read, & Sweeney, Reference Wallcraft, Read and Sweeney2003). Of course, one cannot extrapolate from the problems in the development of the Framework to its implementation in practice, but they do demonstrate some of the realities of ‘user involvement’ in policy development. Genuine change must come from understanding what is implied by a genuine sharing of powers between professionals and users, recognising the importance of distinct forms of expertise – including expertise from experience of services themselves. Experience shows that, whatever the aims, such collaborations are often underpinned by a tacit assumption that, when it comes to making decisions, ‘professional knows best’ which almost always prioritises short term symptom management, because the time and resources to effect the radical change to personal, social and economic circumstances prioritised by users are usually absent.

A lesson here is that new ideas that seemed to have potential for a radical transformation of psychiatry, that is to say a shift in the power relations between professionals and users, and a recognition that symptoms are, often if not always, an intelligible response to context bound problems in the user's lifeworld and experience, seem doomed to be recuperated back into individualised treatment, even if accompanied by a greater openness to listen compassionately to the voices of those experiencing distress. Thus the ‘Recovery’ approach, started by service users like Patricia Deegan and Mary O'Hagan (Deegan, Reference Deegan1988) as a collective endeavour has been turned into an individual ‘treatment’ by workers such as Mike Slade and Larry Davidson (Borg & Davidson, Reference Borg and Davidson2008; Slade, Reference Slade2009). And it is a normalising journey, that is to say, one that has as its aim the restoration of the client to a particular version of normal life set not by the ‘client’, but by the professionals involved (Landry, Reference Landry2017; D. Rose, Reference Rose2022). We can also see this pattern of initial radicalism followed by recuperation to normal practice in in the strategy of Patient and Public Involvement (PPI) in research and policy that was pioneered by the UK's National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The PPI programme appeared to offer a radical promise, to involve service users in developing research priorities and to explore novel research methods. But in practice it did not challenge the traditional forms of ‘evidence based medicine’ with its narrow, symptom based criteria for success. Indeed, NIHR has now closed the PPI programme, and it has been argued that the strategy of involvement of the public and service users in health care was actually a technology of ‘legitimation’ for conventional psychiatric research (Harrison & Mort, Reference Harrison and Mort1998).

Psychological alternatives

Two other ‘alternatives’ have recently come to the fore: Open Dialogue and the Power-Threat-Meaning framework. Interestingly, these are not led by psychiatrists but psychotherapists and psychologists respectively. Nonetheless, they position themselves against conventional approaches in psychiatry so a comparison is in order.

Open dialogue: engaging the community

Open Dialogue may be seen as a form of ‘family therapy’ or ‘network therapy’ which can also involve the community (Olson, Seikkula, & Ziedonis, Reference Olson, Seikkula and Ziedonis2014). The approach originated in the work of a team in a psychiatric hospital in Northern Finland: the ‘client’-however distressed–their family, members of their social network and all professionals involved are invited to the first and subsequent meetings, which often take place in the client's home. A team is formed that seeks to understand what it is in the life of the client and their relations with others that has led to this crisis, and which develops usually non-medical-ways to resolve it. While this approach has shown some success in the specific biopsychosocial niche in which it developed, in small local communities in a sparsely populated region, it is not clear if it would work elsewhere. There is an ongoing Randomised Control Trial of Open Dialogue in England, but early results suggest that it meets various obstacles in inner-city settings (Pilling et al., Reference Pilling, Clarke, Parker, James, Landau, Weaver and Craig2022). Open Dialogue actually shares key features with conventional psychiatry – it involves the presence of therapist as expert and a focus on the individual patient albeit as seen as part of wider interpersonal system. The possibility of bringing community members into the therapeutic space is novel both conceptually and practically, although the idea of inviting peers to Open Dialogue sessions has received very ambivalent responses (Razzaque & Stockmann, Reference Razzaque and Stockmann2016). More generally, the difficulty of transplanting this practice from one specific niche to other locations confirms the evidence from ethnographic work (Ecks, Reference Ecks2013; Han, Reference Han2012) that the fate of all ‘interventions’ is always ‘context specific’. The crucial importance of context is as true for standard randomised control trials (RCT) of psychiatric interventions: these almost always rely on decontextualised categorical ICD or DSM diagnoses, which assume patients in very different lifeworlds are suffering from a discrete condition characterised by a given set of symptoms, and that psychiatric drugs will work in similar ways in radically different contexts. While much earlier transcultural psychiatry argued that in most cases, cultural variability lay in in the expressions of mental disorders, rather than in underlying disorders themselves, there is much ethnographic evidence to show that this is not the case, and that forms of mental distress are specific to particular biopsychosocial niches. We would point, for example, to the work of Lawrence Kirmayer and Allan Young (Kirmayer & Young, Reference Kirmayer and Young1998), or that of Stefan Ecks in India, (Ecks, Reference Ecks2005, Reference Ecks2013), or to Joseph Gone's research on the very specific form of intergenerational trauma amongst First Nation Canadians (Gone, Reference Gone2013).

Gone issues a general ‘provocation’ to community psychology to move away from ‘ego centric’ notions of the person, and to recognise that other knowledge traditions have alternative healing practices based on quite different and often non-individualised understandings of mental distress (Gone, Reference Gone2016). This recognition, that both mental distress and responses to it are specific to what we are terming ‘biopsychosocial niches’ poses fundamental challenges to the idea of ‘scaling up’ which is so prevalent in much Euro-American psychiatric research, including the Movement for Global Mental Health, that approaches which may seem effective in one particular location can be ‘scaled up’. Gone's specific research may be very local, but his ‘provocation’ requires us to consider how we can truly incorporate an understanding of ‘social context’ on practices to alleviate mental distress.

Further, the move from the individual to engage ‘the community’ in practices such as Open Dialogue, does not address the longstanding, complex, social shaping of adversity – precarious lives in poverty, poor housing, polluted environments, social exclusion, isolation, stigma, racism and more – which many users of mental health services have experienced, often over years and indeed generations. While most psychiatrists are aware that there are ‘social determinants’ of mental distress, they are invoked, if at all, as background to their clinical priority of an intervention on the troubled individual. If psychiatry remains, fundamentally, an individual clinical practice, however much it makes reference to ‘context’ it will never be able to address, let alone redress, those social determinants that bring individuals to the clinic in the first place. It may be argued that it is not the role of psychiatrists to address such determinants. We will return to this point in our conclusions. It is true that there has long been a regrettable divide in medical practice and thought – and in medical prestige – between clinical practice and public health or prevention. Despite the impassioned cases made by physicians such as Thomas McKeown (McKeown & Lowe, Reference McKeown and Lowe1966), Paul Farmer (Farmer, Reference Farmer1996; Farmer, Nizeye, Stulac, & Keshavjee, Reference Farmer, Nizeye, Stulac and Keshavjee2006) and Michael Marmot (Marmot, Reference Marmot2015; Marmot & Bell, Reference Marmot and Bell2012), the lessons of social medicine dating back to Virchow in the mid nineteenth century have not proved palatable to many of those who teach and practice medicine. Although there are signs of change (Adams, Behague, Caduff, Löwy, & Ortega, Reference Adams, Behague, Caduff, Löwy and Ortega2019), most public health messages still focus on individual behaviour change – stop smoking, avoid ‘fast foods’ – rather than attacking the conditions – such as price differentials between ‘fast’ and ‘healthy’ foods' that are well-known to promote such behaviours and to advantage those who profit from them. Indeed, when it comes to most mental health service users, healthy lifestyles, even if they were affordable, may be powerless in the face of the ‘side-effects’ of psychiatric drug treatment itself.

Power, threat and meaning?

Perhaps one can find some bases of an alternative approach in the relatively recent history of mainstream psychiatry. Adolf Meyer, at one time the most prominent psychiatrist in the US, was one of the earliest proponents of an approach to understanding the ailment of patient through the practice of ‘Formulation’ in which he sought to bring together all the biographical, social and psychological influences that had shaped the life of the patient, and which he believed were necessary to understand before attempting a diagnosis and developing a treatment plan (Lamb, Reference Lamb2014). From his ‘psychobiological’ perspective, social and biological factors that affect someone throughout their entire life should be considered when understanding and treating a patient (Lief, Reference Lief1948). His approach also stressed the need to engage with the external environment of the patient, for example by advocating community support and occupational therapy. Despite the fact that ‘formulation’ is still a part of the training of many psychiatrists, it tends to be the medic's account of potential diagnoses for the presenting disorder. Thus trainees are advised that a formulation should include an account of the patient's background and living situation, a summary of relevant features of the patient's history and current situation, a proposed plan of treatment and prognosis. Since the 1980s, the Meyerian approach, which situated the very roots and character of the expression of distress, as well as the potential opportunities for treatment within the lifeworld of the patient, has been largely relegated to history. This is when we see the general acceptance of the belief, most famously inscribed in American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Statistical Manuals, but also present in the International Classification of Disease's approach to mental disorders, that mental distress could be compartmentalised into a number of specific disease entities (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg2002; Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg2006). The corollary was that that identification of the relevant disease entity was the key task of diagnosis, and one from which treatment – these days either targeted pharmaceutic or psychological intervention on the individual – would follow.

However, the formulation approach, or at least the centrality of what is called formulation, has recently been revived in the Power, Threat Meaning Framework (PTM) developed by clinical psychologists in the UK, and explicitly pitted against an particular image of contemporary, exemplified by the APA's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (Johnstone & Boyle, Reference Johnstone and Boyle2018). Given the belief embodied in versions of the DSM from the 1980s that universal neurobiological abnormalities underpinned each diagnostic category, one might expect, then that those who developed this alternative would be critical of attempts to find universal biological bases for expressions of mental distress. But in fact biology, albeit in a different form, is central even though it is situated as a ‘mediator’ rather than as a primary cause of mental distress. PTM argues that nearly all expressions of mental distress are underpinned by evolved, pre-conscious, universal ‘threat’ responses such as the ‘fight or flight’ response’ The proposition that distress is, in many if not all cases, a response to ‘threat’ is highly controversial (Duntley & Buss, Reference Duntley and Buss2008). Contemporary approaches to human evolution emphasise the ways in which evolved human behavioural and mental capacities are not fixed, but highly plastic, and shaped by, and enacted within, their specific biopsychosocial milieu (Clark, Reference Clark1997; Ginsburg & Jablonka, Reference Ginsburg and Jablonka2019; Laland, Reference Laland2017; Sterelny, Reference Sterelny2003). As neurobiologists working on stress and its consequences have pointed out, whether or not a particular experience triggers to cascade of hormones commonly understood as the ‘threat response’ depends almost entirely on whether a particular situation is perceived as dangerous, threatening or stressful by the individual concerned at the time, and this is highly dependent on the external environment in which the individual finds themselves (McEwen, Reference McEwen2012). But the practical application of the PTM framework seems to entail reinterpreting events in a person's present and past in terms of threats even if they may not have perceived them as such themselves. These recurrent patterns identified in this Framework may not be classical ‘disease entities’ but the proposition seems to be that mental distress can be accounted for in terms of a small number of fixed patterns that can be identified by experts in the distressed person's life history. While the claim is that the mentally distressed person is included in the process of formulation, it seems that the answer – that their distress is to be accounted for as an the activation of an evolved response to a threat that they have experienced from some powerful person or institution – is known to the therapist in advance.

Both Open Dialogue and the formulation approach of PTM share an interest in psychological treatment; indeed, it seems that the psychologist or psychotherapist is to displace the psychiatrist as the leading expert in mental distress, its causes and treatments. It is usually thought such treatment is ‘softer’ because it does not entail the ‘side effects’ that come with medication. However, psychological therapies can involve their own forms of power. For example, some approaches place the onus for ‘recovery’ on the individual themselves and those who have experienced them often find that they instil guilt if you ‘fail to get better’ (Jackson & Rizq, Reference Jackson and Rizq2019). Indeed, some approaches explicitly aim for ‘responsibilisation’ and thus, even if implicitly, accord blame to those who cannot ‘succeed’ in taking responsibility for the management of their own lives (Howell & Voronka, Reference Howell and Voronka2012). Such treatments can also be administered coercively or at least with the threat of removal of welfare benefits if you refuse, as for example in the role that CBT practitioners have acquired in the benefit system in the UK. The placement of psychologists in Job Centres with non-attendance for therapy resulting in loss of income has been called ‘psychocompulsion’ in that it is a way of obliging people to attend for therapy, with the threat of loss of benefits if they refuse (Friedli & Stearn, Reference Friedli and Stearn2015).

Alternatives to coercion

None of the alternatives we have discussed so far address the issue of compulsory detention in psychiatry. Yet many service users and some professionals argue that what marks psychiatry out from the rest of medicine is its power to detain and treat involuntarily, and that this should cease. Others argue that, however residually, psychiatrists must retain this power to prevent psychiatric patients harming themselves or others when under the sway of their illness. In this debate, which often concerns rare cases of violence towards others, one rarely hears the voice of the patient. One study (D. Rose, Evans, Laker, & Wykes, Reference Rose, Evans, Laker and Wykes2015) analysed focus groups of patients who had recently been on inpatient wards as well as focus groups of nurses. The two dominant themes were (lack of) communication and coercion. When it came to instances of coercion, the two groups, saw the ‘same event’ differently. If a situation looked to be getting out of control, for the patients this was because they ‘were caged like animals’; ‘locked in a tiny space for weeks’ – there was a reason why they were ‘kicking off’. The nurses however saw this as an expression of the illness, a symptom – to be medicated away by force if necessary. Can this use of force be justified in the name of the ‘best interests of the patient’, or, if force was not an option, could other non-coercive means be used to calm the distressed person? Many people who use services live in violent surroundings, and this often leads to violence becoming normalised. But the fact that violence can be normalised does not make it right nor excuse it. Indeed, we know that those with a psychiatric label are more likely to be victims rather than perpetrators of violence (Carr et al., Reference Carr, Holley, Hafford-Letchfield, Faulkner, Gould, Khisa and Megele2017; Keating, Robertson, McCulloch, & Francis, Reference Keating, Robertson, McCulloch and Francis2002). Surely a hospital should be one place where those who are mentally distressed can recover free from the threat of violence and coercion. But the current state of inpatient wards in the UK at least does nothing to break this cycle of violence or, as Keating et al., put it, when talking of the experience of Black service users, Circles of Fear (Keating et al., Reference Keating, Robertson, McCulloch and Francis2002). For all the calls to treat people ‘with respect’ this is incompatible with holding them down and injecting them with strong sedative drugs.

In fact, incidents of violence perpetrated by those with a diagnosis of mental disorder are rare, and, in the absence of drug or alcohol use, their incidence is no greater than within the population as a whole (Ullrich, Keers, & Coid, Reference Ullrich, Keers and Coid2014). But what is to be done when these rare events do happen? Some argue that, whatever their psychiatric diagnosis, those who perpetrate such acts should be treated as any other criminal and jailed. Others call for the increase of forensic provision. These are terrible alternatives. At least most prison sentences are time limited, where diversion to a psychiatric institution seldom involves a time limit. In our view, this problem should be seen as part of the structure of incarceration as such. We need a break with the whole system of acute wards for those experiencing a crisis of mental distress, and a move towards the use of crisis houses in these situations. It will be objected that crisis houses cannot take patients detained under mental health legislation. This is an enormous obstacle. Detention is vastly over-used but the possibility of a short stay under conditions of control, in a radically different environment, just might persuade the person who has been driven to violence that they should give the place a try, and even accept medication to alleviate the crisis in the short term.. Many psychiatrists, no doubt, will see this as wishful thinking or ‘impractical’ (like alleviation of poverty). There has been research on the use of crisis houses (Gilburt et al., Reference Gilburt, Slade, Rose, Lloyd-Evans, Johnson and Osborn2010; Gilburt, Rose, & Slade, Reference Gilburt, Rose and Slade2008; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Bingham, Billings, Pilling, Morant, Bebbington and Dalton2004; Venner, Reference Venner2009) but the results are ambiguous, and this approach has not been widely implemented, perhaps because the issue is surrounded by fear on all sides – including fear of litigation. As we have said, the issue is made even more complex by the role of substance use in violence by those with a psychiatric diagnosis. But there are alternatives that could be tried, and have not been, through lack of imagination and because of fear. Any true alternative to current practices in psychiatry must include a radical re-think of the whole practice of incarceration, which places violence to and by patients within this context.

Public health psychiatry

In these examples of ‘alternatives’ there seems to be an unresolved problem of the ‘social’ – lurking in the shadows but never explicated in a satisfactory way. What about what could be called ‘Public Health Psychiatry’? (Mezzich, Reference Mezzich2007). This has many meanings and many potential components: social determinants of distress, community interventions, population prevention. There are signs of an emerging recognition of the need for ‘prevention’ in psychiatry, especially focussed on children and young people and this is welcome (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Correll, Arango, Berk, Patel and Ioannidis2021); we return to this in our conclusions to this paper. But even those who accept the need to address population level issues through strategies of prevention tend to regard most ‘social determinants’ – such as poverty, racisms and social exclusion – as outside the purview of medically trained psychiatrists. Instead, some seek to target specific, individualised, sites of risk, for example in the strategies that focus on early parenting behaviours or ‘adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) (Barker, Reference Barker2007; Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, Reference Shonkoff, Boyce and McEwen2009). Others seek to emulate the anti-smoking strategies in cancer prevention by targeting the use of highly potent cannabis that is a risk factor for psychosis (Murray & Cannon, Reference Murray and Cannon2021). In such approaches, it remains the behaviour of the individual – the inadequate mother, the cannabis smoker – who is the target of treatment and of prevention. Public health psychiatry still tends to frame the problem it addresses in terms of specific diagnostic categories – even when it is aware of the issue of ‘comorbidities’ – and aims to change the attitudes, behaviour and choices of the individual even where the intervention includes, for example, placing treatment resources in the community and building community based support mechanisms. So the ‘public’ in Public Health Psychiatry is an aggregate of individuals, not something sui generis.

Public health psychiatry does not seek to replace clinical psychiatry. It is argued, and we agree, that some will suffer so extremely that they need individual attention, although we would argue that this should always be combined with a strategy to address at least the proximal social and environmental pressures that have triggered extreme distress, such as appalling housing, domestic abuse, poverty or the experience of racism before making a psychiatric diagnosis and prescribing medication. Instead of reading complaints about such matters as symptoms, or as simply contexts beyond their remit, psychiatric professionals could use their authority to intervene in such matters, both individually and at community level. No doubt some committed professionals do so, contacting the relevant social authorities on behalf of their patient. But the absence of almost any recognition of the constitutive role of ‘social conditions' in the standard psychiatric interview, or the reduction of these to matters of external context in the reduced form that formulation now takes, means this would require a fundamental shift in perspective, though not an impossible one.

We are then, again, left with the issue of how social determinants are to be conceived. The approach tends to regard social determinants as variables which can be assessed with regard to their contribution to the whole (regression analysis or other models which disaggregate the social and look for ‘key ingredients (Brooke, Reference Brooke1959). While some anthropologists of mental health have focussed on the key role of ‘social suffering’ (Kleinman, Das, & Lock, Reference Kleinman, Das and Lock1997), and clinicians like the late Paul Farmer (Farmer, Reference Farmer1996; Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Nizeye, Stulac and Keshavjee2006) have pointed to the causal role of structural violence, such language is usually regarded as unacceptable to psychiatry because it is ‘value-laden’, and even ‘political’ (Rylko-Bauer & Farmer, Reference Rylko-Bauer, Farmer, Brady and Burton2016). But mental distress needs to be understood as arising from the actual, social and material experience of individuals as they make their lives enmeshed with others in the spaces and places they live – what one might call their ‘biopsychosocial niches’ (Rose, Birk, & Manning, Reference Rose, Birk and Manning2021). It arises out of their everyday experiences, as they negotiate their life, manage their living conditions, find foods for themselves and their families, cope with multiple material, familial, bureaucratic and social stressors, often in deeply impoverished environments, saturated by memories and meanings, suffused by fears of violence, by pollution, and multiple exposures to small and large traumatic events. While these are often effaced in the correlational styles of thought of psychiatric epidemiology, people make their lives in these small scale worlds and we are beginning to have a clearer understanding of how such multiple pathogenic exposures get under the skin and afflict body and soul across time. To address these requires more than community support and easy access to mental health services. But it does not require us to imagine utopian cities but to listen to the voices of those who experience distress about what they find pathological, and what might be potentially salutogenic, in the reality of their everyday lives – maybe fewer pawn shops and bookmakers, better food stores, small parks, safe spaces for children to play, cheap and readily available early childcare and sure-start facilities, and, of course, some financial security perhaps through such methods as unconditional cash transfers or universal basic income (Roe & McCay, Reference Roe and McCay2021; N. Rose et al., Reference Rose, Birk and Manning2021; N. Rose & Fitzgerald, Reference Rose and Fitzgerald2021). Perhaps many years of medical training does not equip psychiatrists to understand such matters let alone engage in them. But it should.

The voice of the patient

If a developed concept of the social is missing from all these approaches, then so is something else – the voice of the patient. The obverse of the missing social is the spotlight on the decontextualised individual. Even the radical movement Psychiatrica Democratica which sought to close the Italian psychiatry hospitals, did not allow for autonomous user groups (Scheper-Hughes & Lovell, Reference Scheper-Hughes and Lovell1987). Anti- psychiatrists found meaning in their patients' delusions – but it was their meaning, not the patients. Current uses of formulation reframe the patient's experience in the categories and explanatory frameworks of the professionals. Some psychological approaches take this to greater extremes than psychiatric ones, as in current developments in the ‘Recovery Approach’ (D. Rose, Reference Rose2022).

There are many reasons for this absence: above all the lingering doubt that patients have anything sensible to say, a moral as well as cognitive position. Their symptoms are who they are and their symptoms are meaningless or, much less often, accorded meaning – interpreted – by experts. We argue that to fully situate this patient, to take account of their lives, requires more both in research and in the clinic. This is because the ‘voice’ of the patient exists in a different epistemic space to that underpinned by classification, diagnosis and the idea of intervention. None of the ‘alternatives’ we have looked attempt to grasp that epistemic space. This space can be called ‘experience’, but while it is now common to talk of ‘lived experience’ as raw, immediate and authentic awareness, from our perspective, experience needs to be understood as a concept, and its exclusion needs to be placed in the context of relations of power.

To understand what we mean by this, we need to recognise that, in addition to the power imbalance in the psychiatric encounter, the voice of the patient is excluded because of what Miranda Fricker calls ‘epistemic injustice’ – an ethical as well as a social subjugation (Fricker, Reference Fricker2007). First, patients are not seen as credible at the level of knowledge – the (perceived) incoherence or meaningless ramblings of the mental patient lead to a general doubt about the veridicality of anything they say. This is a formidable form of power, exercised over what can count as valid knowledge. Second there is what she calls ‘hermeneutic injustice’ which means there is no publicly available discourse in which to articulate the situation of the mental patient and all that follows from this. There is a ‘hermeneutic lacuna’. In the case of post-psychiatry, which we discussed earlier, Pat Bracken allies his hopes for dialogue between patients and professionals with what Jurgen Habermas terms an ‘ideal speech situation’ (Habermas, Honneth, & Joas, Reference Habermas, Honneth and Joas1991). But Habermas does not take account of the fact that language is not a neutral or universal means of communication; language comes in different registers and may even contain important gaps that prevent communication or even representation. Any dialogue must draw on systems of meaning that people may or may not share. If they are not shared then the question of which will prevail and permeate social understanding and decisions about action is one of power and the relation of professionals to their clients, however committed to equality, always entails power because one party can make decisions to which the other is subject.

Research

Psychiatry, after all, aims to practice evidence-based medicine. It claims an epistemology of empiricism but that apparent neutral search for facts is underpinned by strong orienting frameworks such as diagnosis, mental pathology, neurochemical anomalies and treatment (the intervention). Evidence from RCTs is prized above all else, despite the fact that many RCTs have historically been funded by the pharmaceutical industry which has an influence on the publication of their results (Turner, Cipriani, Furukawa, Salanti, & de Vries, Reference Turner, Cipriani, Furukawa, Salanti and de Vries2022; Turner, Matthews, Linardatos, Tell, & Rosenthal, Reference Turner, Matthews, Linardatos, Tell and Rosenthal2008). Despite the many criticisms of the applicability of evidence from RCTs to the experience of the products tested when they are used in everyday life, they continue to have a key place in psychiatric research, in large part because of the hierarchy of knowledge within the academy (Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Cromley, Perez, Dai, Mara and Balsai2020; Slade & Priebe, Reference Slade and Priebe2001). Further, the conclusions of RCTs are driven by a very particular use of statistics, in which one outcome measure is selected, and behavioural independent variables are chosen to enter into the calculations, because of the need to ‘power’ the sample to give statistically meaningful results. Public health psychiatry may have some different orienting categories but social determinants are still largely seen as variables, amenable to disaggregation (class, ethnicity, age, gender…) and framed as if they themselves were value-free. And these variables are only as good as is their representation of the world. For instance, socio-economic status is still widely used in studies of the effects of social class on mental disorder but the ‘world’ has outstripped that system with the changing nature of employment, the rise of a knowledge and service economy and increasing precarity in the form of zero hours contracts, multiple jobs, in-work welfare benefits and so on (Neilson & Rossiter, Reference Neilson and Rossiter2008). Again, while research studies using methods such as RCTs think in terms of ‘pure’ samples (for example, subjects with an homogeneity of diagnosis), as evidence on ‘multimorbidity’ shows, this is not the way people and populations, healthy or sick, are made up. The heterogeneity that is our common reality is excluded.

Once again, in this kind of research, ‘subjects’ usually have no voice either in the conduct of the trial itself or in decisions about research methodology, study design and outcome measures. But if service users, even when ‘involved’ as in PPI, they have no influence over what are considered to be appropriate methods and measures, and their involvement amounts to mere tinkering at the edges (D. Rose, Reference Rose2015). Indeed, research findings from participatory methods have the lowest ranking in the much-vaunted hierarchy of evidence (Evans, Reference Evans2003). The survivor voice is thus erased in the methods of research as such, even while it is claimed at the same time that involvement of mental health service users, in consultations and the like means that the silence has been broken.

Ecological and social niches

As we have said, we propose a way of thinking about mental distress as embodied and emplaced within ‘niches’, the actual conditions of existence that characterise the lives of different groups, infused by a range of often toxic exposures, suffused by meanings and memories, hopes and fears. A ‘niche’ cannot be disaggregated into component parts or factors which can be given weights in an equation – they are lived as one specific mode of existence. Even attempts to adopt such a broad vision of all those potential damaging ‘exposures’ that impinge on the lives of individuals – such as the exposome – tend to assume that these can be isolated and measured, perhaps even added into a score, somehow eliding their subjectively experienced and lived character (see N. Rose & Fitzgerald, Reference Rose and Fitzgerald2021, Chapter 5). How, then, beyond epidemiological correlations, can it be researched? We would propose research that uses ethnographies, mental maps, video diaries, even apps that ‘sample’ experience in real time – the many ways now available of seeing that capture how adversity gets under the skin in multiple ways. We need to consult with communities to find out their priorities for research and the way to do it. We need to see how structural factors such as racism and poverty operate in those forms of life. Racism and poverty both operate in ways ranging from the brutal to microagressions and so we require concepts that can encompass this variety and see these not as causal structures with measurable effects but a series of loops over time. Time is important in mental health because ‘chronic’ conditions are not a biological inevitability but a product of repetitions over time, in the spaces and places that constitute and constrain the everyday lives of those with these diagnoses just as surely as did confinement in a psychiatric institution (Bister, Reference Bister2018; Bister, Klausner, & Niewöhner, Reference Bister, Klausner, Niewöhner, Blok and Farias2016). The temporal and cumulating consequences of the psychiatric medication, stress, poor diet, inadequate housing, social stigma and poverty experienced by those with such diagnoses challenges the idea that these conditions are inherently degenerative (Murray, Bora, Modinos, & Vernon, Reference Murray, Bora, Modinos and Vernon2022). To understand the temporal accumulation of these material and symbolic harms in this way would offer up possibilities of intervening to change trajectories.

Sometimes it seems as if such changes can only happen at a large-scale level, with the enactment of political and economic policies that could, over time, create more equality and social justice across society as a whole. We would argue that those who take ‘social determinants’ of mental health seriously should certainly campaign at that level, despite the unpardonable lack of action on these issues by the major psychiatric organisations, who inexplicably carry on blithely as if the diagnostic abilities, explanatory models and treatment capacities of their profession were not in crisis (N. Rose, Reference Rose2016, Reference Rose2018; Scull, Reference Scull2021) But just as niches are local, so small changes can alter people's lives especially if they are the focus of collective action, which requires transcending psychiatry's individualisation to recognise that it is actually therapeutic for individuals to come to understand their own distress as not a matter of individual pathology, let alone personal failing, but arising out of shared experiences. But these small changes are not easy because the everyday practices that are its targets are interlinked and consolidated in everything from architecture to language-in-action. Change can function as an exemplar – if we can show that something can change it is more likely to change again. If we know what makes for change and what makes for stasis we can multiply the first and abandon the second, at least for that context. But conventional approaches, such as ‘Theories of Change’ or ‘logic models’ do not help us here, because they are linear, play down context and, as with RCTs, when they are researched they usually have a single primary outcome (Horwitz & Scheid, Reference Horwitz and Scheid1999). Intervening in multiple dimensions of ‘the context’ is more likely to make things better. While some forms of expert support may indeed be empowering, some do the opposite, consolidating what, in the old language of the sociology of deviance, used to be understood as a career as a mental patient, where that diagnostic label loops back and reshapes the identity of the individual themselves. In this way, psychiatry – and many alternatives offered by the allied psy professions – may actually entrench the problems which they try to ‘treat’.

So is another psychiatry possible?

A genuine alternative would attend as much to what is missing in contemporary psychiatric thought and practise as much as what is present. But if structural violence, intergenerational trauma, social suffering, exclusion and the voice of the patient collectively conceived were recognised as foundational to the experience of both common and severe mental distress, would the medical discipline of psychiatry still be allotted the key role in understanding and treating mental disorders.

At the least psychiatry would no longer be able to claim, or pretend, that it has a monopoly of scientific knowledge of these ailments or of methods of mitigating them. We are not here referring to the ‘turf wars’ currently ongoing between some psychiatries and some psychologies. And, of course, we recognise that psychiatrists themselves are working in ‘cramped spaces’, their decisions are constrained by the policies of insurance companies, hospital trusts, and the shadow of the law, and hospital managers, and often negotiated with psychologists, nurses and even, sometimes with patients. But what we are suggesting would require them to work with, and often be subordinate to, the many other professional and non-professional forms of expertise that are required to support people in the real world in which they live which for some is a maddening world. Experts in housing, finance, hostility of different kinds (racism, patriarchy, homophobia) and the manifestations of the political and economic configurations which characterise different parts of the world variously are needed. Neither psychiatry as currently constituted, nor any of the proposed ‘alternatives’ incorporate this range of expertise. Jonathan Metzl has proposed that the psychiatric curriculum should be built around ideas of ‘structural competency’ (as opposed to cultural competency’) – that is to say it should focus not on the medics understanding the cultures of their patients but on them understanding the social conditions that have shaped their lives (Metzl & Hansen, Reference Metzl and Hansen2014, Reference Metzl and Hansen2018). To achieve this would not simply mean refiguring the ‘multi-disciplinary team’ because these disciplines are themselves limited: each operates in its own silo, no matter how genuine is the attempt to build conversations between them. Nor would it simply be that engaging the expertise of patients would bring a fundamentally different discourse into the whole, although it would radically alter the whole because it would inflect and change each component. More fundamentally it would invert the gaze thorough which distress was to be understood; it would require that all those professionals tried, in whatever way they could, to take the patient's point of view, to try to imagine the world as it is experienced by the patient. A move to epistemic justice demands that the voice of the patient and the experienced reality of the patient, is central to any system of supports and the holistic knowledge of actual lives that this would bring would render the idea of distinct ‘components’ redundant. Another psychiatry would be one that turns ‘patient involvement in research’ to ‘researcher involvement in patient-led systems.

Many psychiatrists would agree with much of what we have argued, but feel constrained to work within the boundaries of the clinical encounter, however aware they are of the kinds of systematic and structural forces we have described. Some psychiatrists are making important steps to challenge and change psychiatric orthodoxy in the direction we have indicated. But we wonder how many psychiatrists would accept the consequential reduction in their claims that they are the exponents of highly effective, neurobiological based, targeted treatment of brain disorders, like their peers in other biomedical specialities. The leaders of the psychiatric establishment are likely to resist such a reconfiguration of their profession. But then paradigm shifts are aways resisted precisely because they signal a fundamental problem with ‘normal’ science. As Kuhn recognised, the time before a paradigm shift is replete with dangers (Kuhn, Reference Kuhn2012). There is a long march ahead, but we believe that another psychiatry, along the lines we have tried to sketch here, is possible.

Conflict of interest

Neither of the authors have any conflict of interest

References

Adams, V., Behague, D., Caduff, C., Löwy, I., & Ortega, F. (2019). Re-imagining global health through social medicine. Global Public Health, 14, 13831400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, D. J. (2007). The origins of the developmental origins theory. Journal of Internal Medicine, 261, 412417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bister, M. D. (2018). The concept of chronicity in action: Everyday classification practices and the shaping of mental health care. Sociology of Health & Illness, 40, 3852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bister, M. D., Klausner, M., & Niewöhner, J. (2016). The cosmopolitics of ‘niching’. Rendering the city habitable along infrastructures of mental health care. In Blok, A. & Farias, I. U. (Eds.), Urban cosmopolitics. Agencements, assemblies, atmospheres (pp. 187206). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Borg, M., & Davidson, L. (2008). The nature of recovery as lived in everyday experience. Journal of Mental Health, 17, 129140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracken, P. (1995). Beyond liberation: Michel Foucault and the notion of a critical psychiatry. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 2, 113.Google Scholar
Bracken, P., Fernando, S., Alsaraf, S., Creed, M., Double, D., Gilberthorpe, T., … Timimi, S. (2021). Decolonising the medical curriculum: Psychiatry faces particular challenges. Anthropology & Medicine, 28(4), 420428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bracken, P., & Thomas, P. (2001). Postpsychiatry: A new direction for mental health. British Medical Journal, 322, 724727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooke, E. M. (1959). National statistics in the epidemiology of mental illness. Journal of Mental Science, 105, 893908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bynum, W. F., Porter, R., & Shepherd, M. (2004). The anatomy of madness: Essays in the history of psychiatry. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Carr, S., Holley, J., Hafford-Letchfield, T., Faulkner, A., Gould, D., Khisa, C., & Megele, C. (2017). Mental health service user experiences of targeted violence and hostility and help-seeking in the UK: A scoping review. Global Mental Health, 4, e25. doi: 10.1017/gmh.2017.22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Gene–environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 583590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Conrad, P. (1992). Medicalization and social control. Annual Review of Sociology, 18, 209232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deegan, P. E. (1988). Recovery: The lived experience of rehabilitation. Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal, 11, 1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department of Health. (1999). The national service framework for mental health: Modern standards and service models. London: Department of Health.Google Scholar
Duntley, J. D., & Buss, D. M. (2008). Evolutionary psychology is a metatheory for psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 19, 3034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ecks, S. (2005). Pharmaceutical citizenship: Antidepressant marketing and the promise of demarginalization in India. Anthropology & Medicine, 12, 239254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ecks, S. (2013). Eating drugs: Psychopharmaceutical pluralism in India. New York: NYU Press.Google Scholar
Evans, D. (2003). Hierarchy of evidence: A framework for ranking evidence evaluating healthcare interventions. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 12, 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, P. (1996). On suffering and structural violence: A view from below. Daedalus, 125, 261283.Google Scholar
Farmer, P. E., Nizeye, B., Stulac, S., & Keshavjee, S. (2006). Structural violence and clinical medicine. PLoS Medicine, 3, e449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fricker, M. (2007). Epistemic injustice: Power and the ethics of knowing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedli, L., & Stearn, R. (2015). Positive affect as coercive strategy: Conditionality, activation and the role of psychology in UK government workfare programmes. Medical Humanities, 41, 4047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Correll, C. U., Arango, C., Berk, M., Patel, V., & Ioannidis, J. P. (2021). Preventive psychiatry: A blueprint for improving the mental health of young people. World Psychiatry, 20, 200221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilburt, H., Rose, D., & Slade, M. (2008). The importance of relationships in mental health care: A qualitative study of service users’ experiences of psychiatric hospital admission in the UK. BMC Health Services Research, 8, 92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilburt, H., Slade, M., Rose, D., Lloyd-Evans, B., Johnson, S., & Osborn, D. P. J. (2010). Service users’ experiences of residential alternatives to standard acute wards: Qualitative study of similarities and differences. British Journal of Psychiatry, 197, S26S31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsburg, S., & Jablonka, E. (2019). The evolution of the sensitive soul: Learning and the origins of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gone, J. P. (2013). Redressing first nations historical trauma: Theorizing mechanisms for indigenous culture as mental health treatment. Transcultural Psychiatry, 50, 683706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gone, J. P. (2016). Alternative knowledges and the future of community psychology: Provocations from an American Indian healing tradition. American Journal of Community Psychology, 58, 314321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habermas, J., Honneth, A., & Joas, H. (1991). Communicative action. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Han, C. (2012). Life in debt: Times of care and violence in neoliberal Chile. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S., & Mort, M. (1998). Which champions, which people? Public and user involvement in health care as a technology of legitimation. Social Policy & Administration, 32, 6070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horwitz, A. V., & Scheid, T. L. (1999). A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Horwitz, A. V., & Wakefield, J. C. (2007). The loss of sadness: How psychiatry transformed normal sorrow into depressive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Howell, A., & Voronka, J. (2012). Introduction: The politics of resilience and recovery in mental health care. Studies in Social Justice, 6, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C., & Rizq, R. (Eds.) (2019). The industrialisation of care: Counselling, psychotherapy and the impact of IAPT. PCCS Books.Google Scholar
Johnson, S., Bingham, C., Billings, J., Pilling, S., Morant, N., Bebbington, P., … Dalton, J. (2004). Women's experiences of admission to a crisis house and to acute hospital wards: A qualitative study. Journal of Mental Health, 13, 247262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, L., & Boyle, M. (2018). The power threat meaning framework: An alternative nondiagnostic conceptual system. Journal of Humanistic Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022167818793289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, A., Cromley, J., Perez, T., Dai, T., Mara, K., & Balsai, M. (2020). The role of context in educational RCT findings: A call to redefine “evidence-based practice”. Educational Researcher, 49, 285288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, F., Robertson, D., McCulloch, A., & Francis, E. (2002). Breaking the circles of fear: A review of the relationship between mental health services and African and Caribbean communities. London: The Sainsbury Centre for Mental Health.Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., & Young, A. (1998). Culture and somatization: Clinical, epidemiological, and ethnographic perspectives. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 420430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinman, A., Das, V., & Lock, M. M. (1997). Social suffering. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (2012). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. (2017). Darwin's unfinished symphony. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, S. D. (2014). Pathologist of the mind: Adolf Meyer and the origins of American psychiatry. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, D. (2017). Survivor research in Canada: ‘Talking’ recovery, resisting psychiatry, and reclaiming madness. Disability & Society, 32, 14371457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, C. (2007). Shyness: How normal behavior became a sickness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, A. (1953). Health as a social concept. The British Journal of Sociology, 4, 109124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lief, A. (1948). The commonsense psychiatry of Dr. Adolf Meyer. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Marmot, M. (2015). The health gap: The challenge of an unequal world. London: Bloomsbury.Google ScholarPubMed
Marmot, M., & Bell, R. (2012). Fair society, healthy lives. Public health, 126, S4S10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (2012). Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1718017185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKeown, T., & Lowe, C. R. (1966). An Introduction to social medicine. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Metzl, J. M., & Hansen, H. (2014). Structural competency: Theorizing a new medical engagement with stigma and inequality. Social Science & Medicine, 103, 126133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzl, J. M., & Hansen, H. (2018). Structural competency and psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 115116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mezzich, J. E. (2007). Psychiatry for the person: Articulating medicine's science and humanism. World Psychiatry, 6, 65.Google ScholarPubMed
Modinos, G., Iyegbe, C., Prata, D., Rivera, M., Kempton, M. J., Valmaggia, L. R., … McGuire, P. (2013). Molecular genetic gene–environment studies using candidate genes in schizophrenia: A systematic review. Schizophrenia Research, 150, 356365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R., Bora, E., Modinos, G., & Vernon, A. (2022). Schizophrenia: A developmental disorder with a risk of non-specific but avoidable decline. Schizophrenia Research, 243, 181186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., & Cannon, M. (2021). Public health psychiatry: An idea whose time has come. World Psychiatry, 20, 222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neilson, B., & Rossiter, N. (2008). Precarity as a political concept, or, fordism as exception. Theory, Culture & Society, 25, 5172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, M., Seikkula, J., & Ziedonis, D. (2014). The key elements of dialogic practice in open dialogue: Fidelity criteria. The University of Massachusetts Medical School, 8, 2017.Google Scholar
Perestelo-Perez, L., Gonzalez-Lorenzo, M., Perez-Ramos, J., Rivero-Santana, A., & Serrano-Aguilar, P. (2011). Patient involvement and shared decision-making in mental health care. Current Clinical Pharmacology, 6, 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilgrim, D. (2019). Key concepts in mental health. New York: SAGE.Google Scholar
Pilling, S., Clarke, K., Parker, G., James, K., Landau, S., Weaver, T., … Craig, T. (2022). Open dialogue compared to treatment as usual for adults experiencing a mental health crisis: Protocol for the ODDESSI multi-site cluster randomised controlled trial. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 113, 106664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Razzaque, R., & Stockmann, T. (2016). An introduction to peer-supported open dialogue in mental healthcare. BJPsych Advances, 22, 348356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roe, J., & McCay, L. (2021). Restorative cities: Urban design for mental health and wellbeing. London: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, D. (2015). The contemporary state of service-user-led research. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 2, 959960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, D. (2022). The fate of the idea of recovery today: A user-centred analysis. Community Psychology in Global Perspective, 8, 103119.Google Scholar
Rose, D., Evans, J., Laker, C., & Wykes, T. (2015). Life in acute mental health settings: Experiences and perceptions of service users and nurses. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24, 9096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, D., Perry, E., Rae, S., & Good, N. (2017). Service user perspectives on coercion and restraint in mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry International, 14, 5961.Google ScholarPubMed
Rose, N. (2016). Neuroscience and the future for mental health? Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25, 95100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, N. (2018). Our psychiatric future: The politics of mental health. Polity.Google Scholar
Rose, N., Birk, R., & Manning, N. (2021). Towards neuroecosociality: Mental health in adversity. Theory, Culture & Society, 39, 121144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, N., & Fitzgerald, D. (2021). The urban brain: Mental health in the vital city. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, C. E. (2002). The tyranny of diagnosis: Specific entities and individual experience. The Milbank Quarterly, 80, 237260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, C. E. (2006). Contested boundaries – psychiatry, disease, and diagnosis. Perspectives In Biology and Medicine, 49, 407424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rylko-Bauer, B., & Farmer, P. (2016). Structural violence, poverty, and social suffering. In Brady, D. & Burton, L. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the social science of poverty (pp. 4774). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sashidharan, S., & Francis, E. (1999). Racism in psychiatry necessitates reappraisal of general procedures and Eurocentric theories. British Medical Journal, 319, 254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savalescu, J., Davies, L. W., Roache, R., Davies, W., & Loebel, J. P. (2020). Psychiatry reborn: Biopsychosocial psychiatry in modern medicine. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheper-Hughes, N., & Lovell, A. (1987). Psychiatry inside out: Selected writings of Franco Basaglia. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Scull, A. (2015). Madness in civilization: A cultural history of insanity, from the Bible to Freud, from the madhouse to modern medicine. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scull, A. (2021). American psychiatry in the new millennium: A critical appraisal. Psychological Medicine, 51, 27622770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., & McEwen, B. S. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301, 22522259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, M. (2009). Personal recovery and mental illness: A guide for mental health professionals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slade, M., & Priebe, S. (2001). Are randomised controlled trials the only gold that glitters? British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 286287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Thornicroft, G. (2000). National service framework for mental health. Psychiatric Bulletin, 24, 203206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, E. H., Cipriani, A., Furukawa, T. A., Salanti, G., & de Vries, Y. A. (2022). Selective publication of antidepressant trials and its influence on apparent efficacy: Updated comparisons and meta-analyses of newer versus older trials. PLoS Medicine, 19, e1003886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, E. H., Matthews, A. M., Linardatos, E., Tell, R. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2008). Selective publication of antidepressant trials and its influence on apparent efficacy. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 252260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullrich, S., Keers, R., & Coid, J. W. (2014). Delusions, anger, and serious violence: New findings from the MacArthur violence risk assessment study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40, 11741181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venner, F. (2009). Risk management in a survivor-led crisis service. Mental Health Practice, 13, 1823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallcraft, J., Read, J., & Sweeney, A. (2003). On our own terms. London: Sainsbury Centre for Mental Health.Google Scholar
Wheeler, C., Lloyd-Evans, B., Churchard, A., Fitzgerald, C., Fullarton, K., Mosse, L., … Johnson, S. (2015). Implementation of the crisis resolution team model in adult mental health settings: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 15, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed