Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T14:16:15.124Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An fMRI investigation of the relations between Extraversion, internalizing psychopathology, and neural activation following reward receipt in the Human Connectome Project sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2020

Courtland S. Hyatt*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Emily S. Hallowell
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Max M. Owens
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, USA
Brandon M. Weiss
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Lawrence H. Sweet
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Joshua D. Miller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Courtland S. Hyatt, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Quantitative models of psychopathology (i.e., HiTOP) propose that personality and psychopathology are intertwined, such that the various processes that characterize personality traits may be useful in describing and predicting manifestations of psychopathology. In the current study, we used data from the Human Connectome Project (N = 1050) to investigate neural activation following receipt of a reward during an fMRI task as one shared mechanism that may be related to the personality trait Extraversion (specifically its sub-component Agentic Extraversion) and internalizing psychopathology. We also conducted exploratory analyses on the links between neural activation following reward receipt and the other Five-Factor Model personality traits, as well as separate analyses by gender. No significant relations (p < .005) were observed between any personality trait or index of psychopathology and neural activation following reward receipt, and most effect sizes were null to very small in nature (i.e., r < |.05|). We conclude by discussing the appropriate interpretation of these null findings, and provide suggestions for future research that spans psychological and neurobiological levels of analysis.

Type
Empirical Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Extraversion is a broad, higher order personality trait domain that is associated with lower level facets such as gregariousness, dominance, and positive emotionality. Historically, Extraversion or constructs akin to it have been instantiated in many major models of personality (e.g., Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1975; Tellegen, Reference Tellegen, Tuma and Maser1985; Wiggins, Trapnell, & Phillips, Reference Wiggins, Trapnell and Phillips1988), and there is strong, cross-cultural support for the positive relations between Extraversion and happiness, general positive affect, and subjective well-being (DeNeve & Cooper Reference DeNeve and Cooper1998; Diener, Sandvik, Pavot, & Fujita, Reference Diener, Sandvik, Pavot and Fujita1992; Lucas, Diener, Grob, Suh, & Shao, Reference Lucas, Diener, Grob, Suh and Shao2000). One potential mechanism linking Extraversion to these adaptive outcomes is processing of reward (Smillie, Reference Smillie2013), which is consistent with physiological models of the agentic components of Extraversion that emphasize motivation for and sensitivity to environmental reward (Gray, Reference Gray1970; Wilson, Barrett, & Gray, Reference Wilson, Barrett and Gray1989). Multiple lines of empirical evidence support this Extraversion–reward link. For example, at the self-report level, Extraversion is related to overt reward-based constructs, such as sensitivity to reward (Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Kimbrel, Hundt, Cobb, Nelson-Gray and Lootens2007) and reward responsiveness (Segarra, Poy, López, & Moltó, Reference Segarra, Poy, López and Moltó2014). Extraversion is also related to a wide range of constructs adjacent to reward, including predicted levels of positive affect during a range of activities, as well as retrospective reports of enjoyment (Hoerger, Chapman, & Duberstein, Reference Hoerger, Chapman and Duberstein2016; Zelenski et al., Reference Zelenski, Whelan, Nealis, Besner, Santoro and Wynn2013).

Beyond these domain-level links, there is evidence that sub-components of Extraversion are differentially related to these adaptive psychological outcomes that are thought to be largely rooted in experiences of reward (e.g., well-being). Specifically, enthusiasm (from the Big Five Aspects Scale; DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson, Reference DeYoung, Quilty and Peterson2007), which captures the communal components of Extraversion like positive emotionality and sociability, is more strongly related to most traditional indices of well-being such as life satisfaction, positive relationships, and feelings of meaning and mastery. Alternately, the facet assertiveness, which captures the agentic components of Extraversion like dominance, is more closely associated with feelings of autonomy (Sun, Kaufman, & Smillie, Reference Sun, Kaufman and Smillie2018). Overall, these lines of evidence suggest lower order aspects of Extraversion bear differential relations to these reward-adjacent constructs depending on how they are operationalized. In other words, these differential links may help clarify the nomological networks that support each level of the Extraversion trait hierarchy. Although the lower level trait components tend to “hang together” (i.e., be positively related to one another, bear similar relations to external criteria, etc.), it is important to examine them separately to determine the extent to which they converge and diverge in relations to important outcomes.

Another exemplary case of this complexity is the set of relations between Extraversion, its facets, and psychopathology, where the available literature indicates important relations at both the domain and facet level. At the domain level, Extraversion is concurrently, negatively related to a host of disorders of internalizing psychopathology, including dysthymic disorder, social phobia, panic disorder, major depressive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, and agoraphobia (Kotov, Gamez, Watson, & Schmidt, Reference Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt and Watson2010).Footnote 1 Moving away from traditional categorical diagnoses, quantitative models of psychopathology (i.e., HiTOP; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Eaton2017) identify a construct akin to the low pole of Extraversion (called Detachment) as a major spectrum of psychopathology, and a construct deemed Detachment is also instantiated in the DSM-5 Alternative Model of Personality Disorder diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Under this novel framework, trait elevations on Detachment can be used in support of a diagnosis of various personality disorders (e.g., avoidant personality disorder). Consistent with the links between Extraversion and well-being, the various processes (i.e., psychological, biological, social) that characterize Extraversion also appear to be protective factors against internalizing psychopathology. Psychotherapeutic intervention is associated with increases in Extraversion (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Luo, Briley, Chow, Su and Hill2017), and in a recent randomized-controlled trial, participants assigned to “act-extraverted” condition (vs. control condition) experienced greater increases in positive affect (Jacques-Hamilton, Sun, & Smillie, Reference Jacques-Hamilton, Sun and Smillie2019). Similarly, one of the most consistently supported psychotherapeutic interventions for depressive symptoms is behavioral activation, and a major element of behavioral activation treatments involves helping an individual increase the frequency with which they engage in pleasurable and rewarding events (Cuijpers, Van Straten, & Warmerdam, Reference Cuijpers, Van Straten and Warmerdam2007; Lejuez, Hopko, Acierno, Daughters, & Pagoto, Reference Lejuez, Hopko, Acierno, Daughters and Pagoto2011), consistent with the notion that depression is due, in part, to a reduction of positive reinforcement (e.g., Lewinsohn, Reference Lewinsohn, Friedman and Katz1974; Lewinsohn & Graf, Reference Lewinsohn and Graf1973).

More granular examinations have clarified that lower, more specific levels of the Extraversion hierarchy are better suited to capturing variance in aspects of internalizing psychopathology than the broader domain. In a recent review, Watson and colleagues (Reference Watson, Stanton, Khoo, Ellickson-Larew and Stasik-O’Brien2019b) describe the differences that emerge in Extraversion – psychopathology relations as one moves “down” from the domain level to the facet level. A full review of these relations is beyond the scope of this manuscript, but several important trends emerged. First, both the agentic (e.g., dominance, assertiveness) and communal aspects of Extraversion (e.g., positive emotionality, enthusiasm) bore negative relations to self-report and interview ratings of depression, social dysfunction, as well as the pathological traits withdrawal, anhedonia, intimacy avoidance, and anxiousness measured by the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (DeYoung, Carey, Krueger, & Ross, Reference DeYoung, Carey, Krueger and Ross2016; Krueger, Derringer, Markon, Watson, & Skodol, Reference Krueger, Derringer, Markon, Watson and Skodol2012). Notably, the communal components of Extraversion generally bore statistically significantly stronger correlations to these indices of internalizing psychopathology than the agentic components. Second, the agentic components of Extraversion bore medium to large, positive associations with certain pathological traits typically associated with externalizing psychopathology, including risk-taking, exhibitionism, manipulativeness, grandiosity, and attention-seeking. The communal components of Extraversion bore either small, positive, or null relations to these externalizing variables. In sum, multiple lines of evidence converge on the conclusion that Extraversion bears meaningful relations to various psychiatric disorders across methodologies (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Stasik, Ellickson-Larew and Stanton2015), but these findings become more nuanced as one considers a more multi-faceted approach to measuring Extraversion and internalizing psychopathology.

1. The incentive facilitation model and extraversion-related reward

Given the well-established links between the facets of Extraversion and various indices of reward at the psychological level, a major initiative for personality researchers has been to investigate the degree to which these individual differences are linked to physiological responses to reward. Perhaps the most well-known neurobiological account of Extraversion (Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999) is specific to its agentic components (i.e., assertiveness, dominance); this model posits that the variation in the agentic components of Extraversion is rooted in variations in the incentive/reward motivational systems. Individuals high in agentic components of Extraversion are thought to be characterized by the tendency to encode the reward value of certain classes of stimuli more intensely, or to associate these stimuli with a larger incentive motivation. In other words, the agentic components of Extraversion are theorized to be related to encoding particular stimuli (e.g., interpersonal interactions) as relatively more rewarding, which then facilitates approach-related cognitions and behavior in an effort to attain this reward. Under this model, Depue and Collins (Reference Depue and Collins1999) argue that the agentic components of Extraversion – but not the more affiliative components like gregariousness and warmth – are underpinned by increased dopaminergic transmission in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway, which projects from the ventral tegmental area to regions such as the nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal cortex. Put differently, this model postulates that agentic-extraverted individuals tend to exhibit higher levels of reward-related goal pursuit due, in part, to greater dopaminergic transmission in response to reward receipt. Thus, when agentic-extraverted individuals are placed in a context where they receive a reward, they may experience increased dopaminergic activation in the mesolimbic pathway because they have received a learning signal about the presence of reward in the environment, and/or they experience hedonic enjoyment (although there is evidence that opioid systems are more closely linked to hedonic enjoyment; Berridge, Reference Berridge2007).

Although this model was developed as a biological account of the agentic components of Extraversion, it has implications for other individual difference variables where reward is central to their conceptualization. Indices of internalizing psychopathology, especially depression, are thought to be related to diminished experience of reward (Whitton, Treadway, & Pizzagalli, Reference Whitton, Treadway and Pizzagalli2015). For example, individuals with depressive symptoms appear to be less willing to exert effort to gain reward, and show reduced ability to integrate information about the probability of reward into their decision-making (Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton, & Zald, Reference Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton and Zald2012). Perhaps most pertinently, anhedonia, or a pervasive, reduced ability to forecast or derive enjoyment from one’s experiences, is a central symptom in the DSM-5 criteria for a diagnosis of Major Depressive Disorder (APA, 2013), and it is also a primary variable that distinguishes depression from other forms of internalizing psychopathology like anxiety (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991). On the other hand, although anhedonia may be the depressive symptom that is most conceptually directly related to a lack of reward, recent evidence in youths suggests that anhedonia can be considered a transdiagnostic construct relevant to the expression of multiple depression and anxiety-related disorders (Conway, Li, & Starr, Reference Conway, Li and Starr2019). Although anhedonia is most traditionally considered a part of a latent depression variable, it appears pertinent to conceptualizing a range of indices of internalizing psychopathology.

1.1 Limitations of existing literature

Despite the comprehensiveness of this neurobiological model of the agentic components of Extraversion, to date, there have been few adequately powered examinations of the relations between Extraversion and reward-processing using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) analysis (Allen & DeYoung, Reference Allen, DeYoung and Widger2017; Yarkoni, Reference Yarkoni, Mikulincer, Shaver, Cooper and Larsen2015). Furthermore, though several existing investigations using electroencephalography speak to this relation, the findings are somewhat mixed. For example, in a longitudinal sample of children, self-report positive affectivity was related to feedback negativity (i.e., activation in the ventral striatum and medial prefrontal cortex in monetary loss trials minus activation in gain trials) in a hierarchical regression model controlling for demographic covariates and negative affectivity, but not at the bivariate level (r = −.09; N = 381; Kujawa et al., Reference Kujawa, Proudfit, Kessel, Dyson, Olino and Klein2015). In adult samples, there is some recent evidence that reward-processing (i.e., activation in medial frontal regions during unpredicted reward trials minus unpredicted non-reward trials) is uniquely positively related to Extraversion (r = .26; N = 100; Smillie et al., Reference Smillie, Jach, Hughes, Wacker, Cooper and Pickering2019), but other studies have not found support for this relation (r = .06; N = 371; Suzuki, Hill, Ait Oumeziane, Foti, & Samuel, Reference Suzuki, Hill, Ait Oumeziane, Foti and Samuel2019).

In terms of functional neuroimaging methodology, there are numerous studies linking Extraversion to dopaminergic reward regions like the ventral striatum (Canli, Sivers, Whitfield, Gotlib, & Gabrieli, Reference Canli, Sivers, Whitfield, Gotlib and Gabrieli2002; Canli et al., Reference Canli, Zhao, Desmond, Kang, Gross and Gabrieli2001; Cohen, Young, Baek, Kessler, & Ranganath, Reference Cohen, Young, Baek, Kessler and Ranganath2005; Mobbs, Hagan, Azim, Menon, & Reiss, Reference Mobbs, Hagan, Azim, Menon and Reiss2005; Schaefer, Knuth, & Rumpel, Reference Schaefer, Knuth and Rumpel2011; Wu, Samanez-Larkin, Katovich, & Knutson, Reference Wu, Samanez-Larkin, Katovich and Knutson2014), but virtually all of the previous work on Extraversion and fMRI reward-processing was statistically underpowered, with most studies using samples under 20 participants. This raises concerns regarding the stability (Schönbrodt & Perugini, Reference Schönbrodt and Perugini2013), replicability, and generalizability of these effects. A notable exception is a study by Civai and colleagues (2016), which examined the neural activation during selection between two payment options to assess the neural correlates of delayed reward decision-making (N = 250); Extraversion was not related to activation in any region assessed, which included reward-relevant regions such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. However, it is possible that the decision-making aspects of making delayed reward discounting decisions may have modulated the activation of these regions in a different way than tasks involving simple reward receipt.

Investigations of the links between internalizing psychopathology (especially depression) and neural reward-processing have generally provided evidence that depression is related to reduced reward system activity during anticipation and receipt of reward (e.g., Hall, Milne, & MacQueen, Reference Hall, Milne and MacQueen2014; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Christopher May, Siegle, Ladouceur, Ryan, Carter and Dahl2006; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Ryan, Phillips, Manuck, Worthman, Moyles and Dahl2010; Pizzagalli et al., Reference Pizzagalli, Holmes, Dillon, Goetz, Birk, Bogdan and Fava2009; Simon et al., Reference Simon, Biller, Walther, Roesch-Ely, Stippich, Weisbrod and Kaiser2010; Smoski et al., Reference Smoski, Felder, Bizzell, Green, Ernst, Lynch and Dichter2009). Unfortunately, much like the fMRI literature on Extraversion, these studies bear methodological limitations involving relatively small samples (i.e., all but one study under N = 100) and the use of extreme group designs (i.e., comparing individuals high and low in depressive symptoms). While this approach is intuitive, there are statistical ramifications to these designs that preclude estimation of the full population, including the portion of the population “in between” these groups (see Preacher, Rucker, MacCallum, & Nicewander, Reference Preacher, Rucker, MacCallum and Nicewander2005). This is unfortunate because data suggest that depression is a dimensional rather than categorical construct (Hankin, Fraley, Lahey, & Waldman, Reference Hankin, Fraley, Lahey and Waldman2005), and this measurement approach also limits the application of certain analyses (e.g., tests of non-linearity). In sum, while there is some precedent for relating internalizing psychopathology to alterations in reward-processing, well-powered analyses and dimensional measurement are needed.

1.2 Current study

Given these strong and consistent relations between Extraversion, its facets, and components of internalizing psychopathology, it has been posited that these constructs may be understood in terms of common neurobiological processes (DeYoung & Krueger, Reference DeYoung and Krueger2018), that is, complex phenotypes like Extraversion or internalizing psychopathology represent constellations of various psychological (i.e., affective, cognitive, motivational) processes that aggregate to represent a larger latent construct (e.g., DeYoung, Reference DeYoung2015). Since each of these micro-features is, to some degree, tied to and substantiated by one’s physiology, a viable avenue for gaining a more robust understanding of the convergence/divergence of constructs is to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms that characterize them. Reward-processing is precisely this type of shared mechanism.

The goal of the current study was to examine the relations between Extraversion, two of its constituent facets (referred to as Agentic Extraversion and Communal Extraversion), a continuous measure of internalizing psychopathology, and neural activation following reward receipt using a functional neuroimaging methodology. We used existing data (N = 1050) from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) to investigate these relations (Van Essen et al., Reference Van Essen, Smith, Barch, Behrens, Yacoub and Ugurbil2013a). To measure neural activation following reward receipt, we used a gambling task developed by Delgado and colleagues (2000, 2003). During this task, participants complete a card-guessing game wherein they receive feedback regarding the outcome of their guesses. In “reward” trials, feedback about correct guesses is accompanied by the receipt of $1.

To operationalize neural activation following reward receipt, we contrasted the activation in the brain’s reward system during reward trials to that during loss trials in which participants lose $.50, consistent with prior literature (Delgado et al., Reference Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll and Fiez2000; May et al., Reference May, Delgado, Dahl, Stenger, Ryan, Fiez and Carter2004; Tricomi, Delgado, & Fiez, Reference Tricomi, Delgado and Fiez2004). Of note, we operationalized reward system regions in two ways. In the first approach, we used empirically defined regions of interest (ROIs) made from the activation of participants in the current study to reward outcomes. In the second approach, we used a priori defined ROIs identified using Neurosynth’s (https://neurosynth.org/) automated meta-analysis of studies operationalizing reward using task fMRI. The rationale for this dual approach was to examine our hypotheses in ROIs with high internal validity in the current sample (i.e., empirical ROIs), as well as ROIs consistent with prior literature in the interest of generalizability (i.e., a priori ROIs). By doing so, we aimed to minimize the chances that one ROI scheme was insufficient to detect any important effects (i.e., Type II error).

The primary hypothesis in the current study was that the domain Extraversion (as measured by the NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992) would have a small (i.e., r = .10), positive relation to neural activation following reward receipt. We hypothesized that Agentic Extraversion would have a larger relation to neural activation following reward receipt than Communal Extraversion. Additionally, we hypothesized that internalizing psychopathology (as measured by the Achenbach Adult Self-Report scale of adaptive functioning; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003) would have a small, negative relation to neural activation following reward receipt (as measured by the brain response to reward during the gambling task in reward-related ROIs; Delgado et al., Reference Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll and Fiez2000, Reference Delgado, Locke, Stenger and Fiez2003). A secondary hypothesis advanced was that the personality trait Neuroticism would also exhibit a small, negative relation to neural activation following reward receipt. This hypothesis was based on the preponderance of self-report-based evidence that Neuroticism is the personality trait most strongly linked to internalizing psychopathology (e.g., Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt and Watson2010). In contrast to Extraversion and internalizing psychopathology, there have been remarkably few studies on Neuroticism and neural reward-processing (see reviews by Allen & DeYoung, Reference Allen, DeYoung and Widger2017; Servaas et al., Reference Servaas, Van Der Velde, Costafreda, Horton, Ormel, Riese and Aleman2013), and, therefore, this secondary hypothesis was derived from these self-report findings rather than the fMRI literature. To test discriminant validity around these relations, we also conducted exploratory analyses on the relations between the other Five-Factor Model personality traits (i.e., Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness) and an index of externalizing psychopathology and neural activation following reward receipt. Like Neuroticism, there are very few studies on the relations between neural reward-processing and these other traits and externalizing psychopathology. Since we did not have a priori reasons to suspect relations based on prior literature, we expected null relations between neural reward-processing and Openness, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and externalizing psychopathology.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

A sample of 1206 young adults was recruited as part of HCP (Van Essen et al., Reference Van Essen, Smith, Barch, Behrens, Yacoub and Ugurbil2013a, Reference Van Essen, Ugurbil, Auerbach, Barch, Behrens, Bucholz and Della Penna2013b). Informed consent was obtained for all participants. Participants were community-dwelling healthy adults between 22 and 35 years old with no significant history of psychiatric disorder, substance abuse, neurological disorder or damage, cardiovascular disease, or Mendelian genetic disease. Participants were included in the current analyses if they had valid data for all three of the major variable categories in the current investigation (i.e., personality, psychopathology, functional activation following reward receipt). Full fMRI reward data were initially available for 1081 subjects. However, we were notified on March 9, 2020, that an error has occurred with the processing of some of the fMRI data, which reduced this number to 1057 (https://wiki.humanconnectome.org/display/PublicData/HCP+Data+Release+Updates%3A+Known+Issues+and+Planned+fixes). An additional seven participants were missing personality and psychopathology data. This yielded a final sample of 1050 participants. Demographic information for this sample can be found in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic information on the current sample (N = 1050)

2.2 Materials and procedures

2.2.1 Personality

Traits from the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality were measured using the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992), which is a 60-item measure that indexes characteristic patterns of thoughts, emotions, and behaviors. A reliability estimate was computed for each trait: Neuroticism (α = .84, ω = .84), Extraversion (α = .77, ω = .78), Openness (α = .75, ω = .76), Agreeableness (α = .76, ω = .77), and Conscientiousness (α = .81, ω = .83).

To test our hypotheses regarding Agentic and Communal Extraversion, we created composites using relevant items from the NEO-FFI. We compared the items from the NEO-FFI to the longer NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992) to identify which items on the NEO-FFI captured each of the Extraversion facets that are instantiated in longer measures (i.e., Warmth, Gregariousness, Assertiveness, Activity, Excitement-Seeking, Positive Emotions) assessed by this longer measure. To create the Agentic Extraversion variable, we included items that captured the Assertiveness (1 item) and Activity (3 items) facets of Extraversion, based on previous work (Gaughan, Miller, Pryor, & Lynam, Reference Gaughan, Miller, Pryor and Lynam2009) describing the relations between the NEO facets and the agentic and communal aspects of Positive Emotionality as measured by the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (Tellegen & Waller, Reference Tellegen, Waller, Boyle, Matthews and Saklofske2008). Notably, these results also suggested that the Achievement-Striving (3 items) and Self-Discipline (3 items) facets of Conscientiousness are also central to Agentic Extraversion, and indeed this is consistent with the original description of Agentic Extraversion provided by Depue and Collins (Reference Depue and Collins1999): “social dominance and the enjoyment of leadership roles, assertiveness, exhibitionism, and a subjective sense of potency in accomplishing goals” (p. 492). Thus, Agentic Extraversion was represented by 10 NEO-FFI itemsFootnote 2 (α = .74, ω = .77). To create the eight-item Communal Extraversion variable (α = .73, ω = .75), we used NEO-FFI items measuring the Extraversion facets Warmth (1 item), Gregariousness (2 items), Excitement-Seeking (1 item), and Positive Emotions (4 items).

2.2.2 Psychopathology

Psychopathology was measured using the Achenbach Adult Self-Report (ASR) scale of adaptive functioning, a 123-item measure of adaptive functioning for adults that includes items on emotional, behavioral, and social problems (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003). Responses produce both internalizing and externalizing psychopathology composites which were used in the present analyses. The internalizing psychopathology composite comprises subscales for Anxiety/Depression, Withdrawnness, and Somatic Complaints, while the externalizing psychopathology composite includes Aggression, Rule-Breaking, and Intrusive Behavior subscales. Reliability estimates were computed for each index: Internalizing (α = .91, ω = .91) and Externalizing (α = .86, ω = .87).

2.2.3 Reward-processing

The present study used an fMRI compatible adaptation of a gambling task developed by Delgado and colleagues to elicit neural activation following reward receipt (2000). In this task, participants are asked to guess whether the unknown number on a card is higher or lower than 5, and told that the accuracy of their guess that will result in a win or loss of money. Card numbers range from 1 to 9, and participants indicate their guess by pressing one of two buttons on the response box. Feedback about the number on the card is given as either 1) a green up arrow with “$1” for reward trials, 2) a red down arrow next to −$0.50 for loss trials; or 3) gray double-headed arrow for neutral trials (i.e., the number “5”). A fixed algorithm is used to ensure all participants experience the same number and pattern of wins and losses regardless of their guess (i.e., the task is “fixed”). In each of the two runs, there are two blocks of mostly reward (six reward trials pseudo-randomly interleaved with either one neutral and one loss trial, two neutral trials, or two loss trials) and two blocks of mostly loss (six loss trials interleaved with either one neutral and one reward trial, two neutral trials, or two reward trials), interleaved with four rest blocks in which participants passively view a fixation cross (15 s each). This results in a total of 32 reward events, 32 loss events, and four neutral events. All participants are paid the amount they won in the task (which is the same for all participants).

Participants have up to 1.5 s to respond, followed by feedback for 1 s. There is a 1-s interstimulus interval (ISI) in which a fixation cross is presented. Thus, one modeled event is 3.5 s total (response, feedback, ISI) with the only difference between reward trials and loss trials being the direction of the outcome. This modeling approach maximizes statistical power (i.e., samples multiple whole-brain images per trial), accounts for minor differences in hemodynamic response across participants by allowing for the capture of responses occurring slightly before or after the true “outcome” period which is only 1 s in duration, and is consistent with existing literature (Delgado et al., Reference Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll and Fiez2000; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Christopher May, Siegle, Ladouceur, Ryan, Carter and Dahl2006; May et al., Reference May, Delgado, Dahl, Stenger, Ryan, Fiez and Carter2004; Tricomi et al., Reference Tricomi, Delgado and Fiez2004) and defaults published by the HCP. This task has been shown to reliably elicit activations in reward-related regions (Delgado et al., Reference Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll and Fiez2000; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Christopher May, Siegle, Ladouceur, Ryan, Carter and Dahl2006; May et al., Reference May, Delgado, Dahl, Stenger, Ryan, Fiez and Carter2004; Tricomi et al., Reference Tricomi, Delgado and Fiez2004). Also consistent with prior work using this reward-processing task (Delgado et al., Reference Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll and Fiez2000; May et al., Reference May, Delgado, Dahl, Stenger, Ryan, Fiez and Carter2004; Tricomi et al., Reference Tricomi, Delgado and Fiez2004), fMRI response during “win” trials (i.e., when $1.00 reward was received) was compared to response during “loss” trials (i.e., when participants lost $.50). As “neutral” trials (i.e., participants neither won nor lost) represented a very small proportion of trials (two neural trials per run, four total), they were not used as a baseline.

2.2.4 Functional magnetic resonance imaging protocol

MRI was conducted using a 32-channel head coil on a 3 T Siemens Skyra (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany). T2* echoplanar fMRI data were collected during the gambling task (Delgado et al., Reference Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll and Fiez2000). A multi-band acceleration factor of 8 was used. Two task fMRI runs lasting 3:12 minutes each were completed with a TR = 720 ms, TE = 33.1 ms, flip angle = 52 degrees, FOV = 208 × 180 mm, and 72 2-mm-thick sagittal slices, resulting in 2.0-mm isotropic voxels (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Burgess, Harms, Petersen, Schlaggar, Corbetta and Nolan2013). High-resolution T1-weighted structural images were acquired with a resolution of 0.7 mm3 isotropic (FOV = 224 × 240, matrix = 320 × 320, 256 sagittal slices; TR = 2400 ms and TE = 2.14 ms). The quality checking procedure completed to ensure all T1 scans were of high quality is documented in Marcus et al. (Reference Marcus, Harms, Snyder, Jenkinson, Wilson, Glasser and Hodge2013).

2.2.5 Quantification of reward task fMRI response

Data were downloaded from the HCP database having been preprocessed using the minimal preprocessing pipeline (Glasser et al., Reference Glasser, Sotiropoulos, Wilson, Coalson, Fischl, Andersson and Van Essen2013). This pipeline includes gradient unwarping, field-map-based EPI distortion correction, motion correction, registration of EPI to the structural scan and into MNI152 space, and grand-mean intensity normalization.

All subsequent fMRI data processing and analysis were then conducted using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages software (AFNI; Cox, 1996). Spatial smoothing was done to minimally preprocessed data using a 6-mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian filter. General linear modeling was conducted with regressors for the time course of events in each condition (win trials, loss trials, neutral trials), convolved with a hemodynamic response function, six dimensions of motion (x, y, z, roll, pitch, yaw), and linear, quadratic, and cubic trends. To capture reward-based hemodynamic response at the same point for all participants, the task was modeled as an event-related design with regressors individualized to each participant.

Empirically defined ROIs were created from group-level activation maps aggregating the contrast of win vs. loss trials across all participants (see Figure 1 and Table 2). Group summary activation maps for these ROIs were created using one-sample voxelwise t tests and thresholded to p < 1−45 with a cluster threshold of 20 voxels. This unusually stringent threshold was used to resolve separate core clusters of activation, as activation was robust throughout most of the brain. Average activation from all voxels of each ROI was extracted into SPSS (Version 24.0) for hypothesis testing. A priori defined ROIs were made using the term “reward” in Neurosynth’s automated lexical meta-analysis software found at https://neurosynth.org/ (see Figure 2 and Table 3). Average activation from all voxels of each ROI was also extracted for hypothesis testing into SPSS.

Figure 1. Empirically defined regions of interest from group-level activation of wins vs. losses.

Note: Regions of interest exhibited significant activity during the win conditions on the gambling task compared to losses; Talairach Z-plane coordinates = +60 to −5-mm slices; thresholded to uncorrected p = 1−45

Table 2. Empirically defined regions of interest

Note. L, left; R, right; center of mass coordinates are in Talairach space (RAI).

Figure 2. A priori defined regions of interest associated with reward-processing.

Note: Brain regions associated with “reward” in Neurosynth Meta-analysis; Talairach Z-plane coordinates = +25 to −30 in 5-mm slices; thresholded to false discovery rate q = .05.

Table 3. A priori defined regions of interest

Note. L, left; R, right; center of mass coordinates are in Talairach space (RAI).

2.3 Analyses

2.3.1 Hypothesis testing

After establishing task-related ROIs, we computed Pearson’s correlations to examine the relations between Five-Factor Model personality traits, indices of psychopathology, and neural activation following reward receipt in this sample. As rates of psychopathology tend to differ for males and females, with females reporting higher rates of internalizing symptoms and males higher externalizing symptoms (Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema2012), we examined gender’s influence on relations of interest. Rather than using gender as a moderator in our analyses, we elected to repeat the above analyses with male and female subsamples.

2.3.2 Power analysis

A power analysis was conducted using the pwr package in R (Champely, Reference Champely2018). With N = 1050, a significant value = .005 for a two-tailed test, power estimates ranged from 67.0% (r = .10) to >99.9% (r = .20), suggesting the primary analyses were relatively well powered to detect small-to-medium effect sizes. The separate gender analyses were relatively underpowered to locate small effect sizes (r = .10) at this significance threshold (α = .005): 26.9% for the male subsample (N = 480) and 33.8% for the female subsample (N = 570). However, the separate gender analyses were relatively well powered for medium effects (r = .20) at this significance threshold (α = .005): 94.8% in the male subsample and 97.8% in the female subsample.

2.3.3 Pre-registration note

Analyses were pre-registered prior to being conducted at https://osf.io/ysqmz. The data used in the current analyses had been processed as a part of the HCP minimal pre-processing pipeline prior to pre-registration and were downloaded and used by our group for studies unrelated to the topic of the current manuscript (i.e., reward-processing associations with ADHD symptoms, Owens et al., Reference Owens, Mackillop, McNally, Balodis and Sweet2019b; structural correlates of personality and internalizing symptoms, Hyatt et al., Reference Hyatt, Owens, Gray, Carter, MacKillop, Sweet and Miller2019; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Hyatt, Gray, Carter, MacKillop, Miller and Sweet2019a). Correlations between the personality and psychopathology domains have been presented in manuscripts that predated the current project (e.g., Hyatt et al., Reference Hyatt, Owens, Gray, Carter, MacKillop, Sweet and Miller2019; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Hyatt, Gray, Carter, MacKillop, Miller and Sweet2019a). Group activation maps for the reward task used in the current manuscript are presented by Owens and colleagues (Reference Owens, Mackillop, McNally, Balodis and Sweet2019b). Consistent with recent recommendations (Benjamin et al., Reference Benjamin, Berger, Johannesson, Nosek, Wagenmakers, Berk and Johnson2018), we pre-registered a threshold of p < .005 to determine the statistical significance of the relations between neural reward-processing and self-report variables (i.e., personality traits, psychopathology indices).

Three deviations from pre-registration were made in the present study. First, we pre-registered that we would conduct our analyses on N = 1075 since we had valid reward-processing data for N = 1081 but were missing valid personality and psychopathology data for N = 6. However, we discovered after pre-registration that we were missing valid personality and psychopathology data for N = 7, and that functional data for 25 participants were improperly processed; therefore, our final sample was slightly smaller than was pre-registered (N = 1050). Second, we did not pre-register that we would conduct analyses on Agentic or Communal Extraversion, but rather these were added based on a recommendation provided during peer-review.

The third deviation from pre-registration was that we pre-registered that we would conduct a series of multiple regression analyses, in which either the five FFM traits or the two psychopathology indices were entered simultaneously as predictors of the empirically or a priori defined ROIs. We planned to conduct these analyses to examine the relative predictive utility of the hypothesized predictors (i.e., Extraversion, Neuroticism, internalizing psychopathology) compared to the non-hypothesized predictors. However, given the consistently null findings observed in the current analyses, we chose to exclude these analyses, given that the predictive utility of all psychological-level variables individually was minimal. Another consideration that leads to this exclusion was Type I error: the large number of models that would be estimated for these multiple regression analyses (i.e., two sets of predictors [personality traits, psychopathology indices] X 21 ROIs [12 empirically defined ROIs, nine a priori defined ROIs] X three samples [full, men-only, women-only] = 126 additional models) would also considerably increase Type I error risk in the form of spurious suppressor effects while providing little potential benefit given the lack of effects found in individual predictor analyses.

3. Results

3.1 Relations between personality and psychopathology

Pearson’s correlation values for the relations between personality traits and psychopathology indices can be found in Table 4, and effect sizes are discussed such that small: r = |.10|, medium: r = |.20|, large: r = |.30|, and very large: r ≥ |.40| (Funder & Ozer, Reference Funder and Ozer2019). As expected, Neuroticism and Extraversion bore very large relations to internalizing psychopathology that were positive and negative, respectively, and Agreeableness and Conscientiousness bore very large and large (respectively), negative relations to externalizing psychopathology. The FFM personality traits displayed interrelations that were consistent with previous meta-analytic estimates (Van der Linden, te Nijenhuis, & Bakker, Reference Van der Linden, te Nijenhuis and Bakker2010). The internalizing and externalizing psychopathology composites displayed a very large, positive relation as expected (e.g., Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Eaton2017).

Table 4. Relations between personality traits and psychopathology indices

Note: Internalizing and externalizing represent psychopathology composites of relevant subscales of the ASR; *p <.001.

3.2 ROI analyses relating reward-processing to personality and psychopathology

3.2.1 Empirically defined ROIs

Pearson’s correlation values for the relations between reward-processing in empirically defined ROIs and personality traits and indices of psychopathology can be found in Table 5. In the full sample, no significant correlations were found between neural activity when receiving a reward and personality traits at the threshold p < .005, and the vast majority of effect sizes were null to very small in magnitude. Similarly, neither internalizing nor externalizing psychopathology was significantly correlated with neural activity when receiving a reward in the full sample.

Table 5. Pearson’s Correlations between fMRI reward task processing in empirically defined ROIs, personality, and psychopathology

Note: L, left; R, right; values on the same row as the name of the personality trait or psychopathology index represent correlations presented for the entire sample (N = 1050); Male, correlations in the male subsample only (N = 480); Female, correlations in the female subsample only (N = 570); N, Neuroticism; E,Extraversion; O, Openness; A, Agreeableness; C, Conscientiousness; Int., internalizing psychopathology composite; Ext., = externalizing psychopathology composite.

3.2.2 A priori defined ROIs

Pearson’s correlation values for the relations between reward-processing in a priori defined ROIs and personality traits and indices of psychopathology can be found in Table 6. Again, no significant correlations between reward-processing and personality traits or psychopathology indices were found at p < .005, and all effect sizes were null to very small.

Table 6. Pearson’s correlations between fMRI reward task processing in a priori defined ROIs, personality, and psychopathology

Note: Values on the same row as the name of the personality trait or psychopathology index represent correlations presented for the entire sample (N = 1050); Male, correlations in the male subsample only (N = 480); Female, correlations in the female subsample only (N = 570); N, Neuroticism; E, Extraversion; O, Openness; A, Agreeableness; C, Conscientiousness; Int., internalizing psychopathology composite; Ext., externalizing psychopathology composite.

4. Discussion

Despite clear links between Extraversion and self-reports of constructs that are thought to be related to reward (e.g., happiness, well-being, positive emotions, excitement-seeking; Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1980; Watson & Clark, Reference Watson and Clark1997), empirical work on Extraversion and reward-processing from a neural level has been limited in previous investigations by small sample sizes and associated lack of statistical power (Button et al., Reference Button, Ioannidis, Mokrysz, Nosek, Flint, Robinson and Munafò2013). In the current study, our primary aim was to investigate the hypothesized positive relations between Extraversion (specifically Agentic Extraversion) and neural activation following receipt of a reward, as well as the hypothesized negative relations between internalizing psychopathology and neural activation following reward receipt in large community sample (N = 1050). To address this question, we used two approaches to operationalizing the reward network ROIs (i.e., empirically defined and a priori defined) and conducted exploratory analyses in subsamples divided by gender (NMen = 480, Nwomen = 570). In addition, the secondary aims of the current study were to test the hypothesized negative relations between neural reward-processing and Neuroticism, as well as to conduct exploratory analyses on the other FFM traits and externalizing psychopathology. We chose to use a threshold of p < .005 in this study as a way to balance Type I and Type II error risks (i.e., Benjamin et al., Reference Benjamin, Berger, Johannesson, Nosek, Wagenmakers, Berk and Johnson2018), recognizing that this would enhance the risk of Type I error relative to more stringent methods of multiple-comparison correction (e.g., Bonferroni correction), which we deemed acceptable for testing associations in a network of regions with previously demonstrated association with Extraversion.

Contrary to hypotheses, we found no evidence for statistically significant (p < .005) relations between Extraversion and reward-processing in any of the empirically or a priori defined ROIs in the full sample, nor in the subsamples separated by gender. This same pattern of null findings was found for both Agentic Extraversion and Communal Extraversion variables. In terms of effect size, all of the relations observed between Extraversion, its facets, and reward-processing in an empirically or a priori defined ROI were null to very small in nature: across 189 examined effect sizes (three traits [Extraversion, Agentic Extraversion, Communal Extraversion] x 21 ROIs x three samples [full, men-only, women-only]), 183 (96.8%) were equal to or less than r = .05. Similar patterns were also observed for the relations between neural reward-processing and internalizing psychopathology, as well as neural reward-processing and Neuroticism: there were no statistically significant relations, and virtually all effect sizes were minuscule. Additionally, we conducted exploratory analyses on the relations between the other FFM traits and neural reward-processing, as well as on the relation between externalizing psychopathology and neural reward-processing. These non-hypothesized relations were also null.

This pattern of results is inconsistent with the predominant biological account of Agentic Extraversion (Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999) that hypothesizes that this trait is related to greater dopaminergic transmission following reward receipt. This presents a challenge to this model, and suggests that basic animal models of Agentic Extraversion and dopaminergic functioning may not be adequate for describing this phenomenon in humans. However, these results should not be overinterpreted to suggest that Extraversion and its facets are not related to experiences of reward – only that they appear unrelated to reward as operationalized in this narrow, fMRI task-based context. As previously detailed, there is a multitude of findings at the psychological level of analysis that suggests that self-reported experience of reward (and related constructs) are positively related to Extraversion and negatively related to internalizing psychopathology. Thus, we believe that psychological reward-processing is an empirically supported mechanism that links these constructs. However, the current results suggest that Extraversion, its facets, and internalizing psychopathology are not related to reward (relative to a baseline of a smaller loss) at the neural level of analysis as measured by the fMRI task used herein. This does not suggest that other elements of reward-processing in the brain (e.g., other neural networks) could not prove to be such a unifying mechanism.

4.1 Limitations

There are several key limitations that must be noted about the current investigation. First, the reward-processing task used provides a very specific operationalization of reward-processing: relative neural activation immediately following receipt of a reward (i.e., $1) in a given ROI compared to activation immediately following a smaller loss ($.50). This analytic strategy does not permit the identification of pure reward-related activation per se, but rather reward-processing compared to a baseline of a minor loss. The drawback to the current approach is that the fMRI signal used may be a consequence of “deactivations” due to losing as well as activations due to reward, which creates an ambiguity surrounding the interpretation of significant findings that cannot be disentangled from the data available. Although using reward trials compared to the neutral trials would represent a more focal way to isolate the intended effect, the limited number of neutral trials (i.e., 4 trials, <15 seconds) is not sufficient to reliably form a neural signal that can be used to compare to the reward trials. Additionally, it is possible that the relatively minor nature of the loss ($.50) did not render an acute loss response such that it significantly impacted the reward activation signal.

A second limitation involves psychometric concerns with the current reward-processing task. Recent work suggests that this task has relatively low test–retest reliability when examining ventral striatum activity in N = 45 participants from the HCP sample who were re-tested between approximately 2 and 7 months after the initial scan (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Knodt, Ireland, Morris, Poulton, Ramrakha and Hariri2019). Although we examined different ROIs in a much larger sample, this type of critique is consistent with a broader current of field-wide criticism regarding the inadequate psychometric properties of many fMRI tasks (e.g., Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Knodt, Ireland, Morris, Poulton, Ramrakha and Hariri2019; Herting, Gautam, Chen, Mezher, & Vetter, Reference Herting, Gautam, Chen, Mezher and Vetter2018; Turner, Paul, Miller, & Barbey, Reference Turner, Paul, Miller and Barbey2018) as well as behavioral tasks and psychological measures more broadly (e.g., Enkavi et al., Reference Enkavi, Eisenberg, Bissett, Mazza, MacKinnon, Marsch and Poldrack2019; Hussey & Hughes, Reference Hussey and Hughes2020). Thus, the current results must be understood with this important consideration in mind. Of note, this limitation is also present in essentially all prior research that did find associations between reward-related fMRI response and Extraversion or internalizing psychopathology, and thus represents a limitation of the field of fMRI as a whole rather than one specific to the current study.

Another important psychometric limitation pertains to the operationalization of Agentic Extraversion and Communal Extraversion. Although we believe we took a reasonable, empirically grounded approach to operationalizing these variables using the available NEO-FFI data, this measure was not developed to produce these subscales that we derived, and thus we acknowledge that more comprehensive assessments of Extraversion and its facets (e.g., Big Five Aspects Scale; NEO PI-R) would allow a more rigorous test of the hypothesized relations. An analogous limitation should be noted for the measure of internalizing psychopathology. We believe a more informative, rigorous test of the relations between internalizing psychopathology and neural activation following reward receipt would involve a more granular measure of psychopathology that permits examination of a broad internalizing domain alongside more specific symptoms (e.g., anhedonia, low mood). We considered creating these kinds of composites based on the item-level ASR but determined that there were not a sufficient number of relevant items to create sufficiently reliable and valid symptom composites, and thus our results cannot speak to these symptom-level relations.

A fourth limitation is that we are unable to parse the distinction between anticipatory and consummatory reward-processing, which is unfortunate since there are some theoretical reasons to suspect that Extraversion may be uniquely linked to anticipatory reward-processing (Smillie, Reference Smillie2013). Thus, despite the current null results, Extraversion may still be related to aspects of reward-processing in the brain, but limitations of the current reward paradigm do not allow the data to speak to this possibility. However, one strength of the study is that the ROIs used were based on two different operational definitions of the reward network, suggesting that idiosyncrasies of a single definition were not driving the lack of significant results. In a prior study (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Hyatt, Gray, Carter, MacKillop, Miller and Sweet2019a), the current task was shown to activate regions of the brain traditionally activated by reward outcomes in other task paradigms (Oldham et al., Reference Oldham, Murawski, Fornito, Youssef, Yücel and Lorenzetti2018). Although some of the empirically defined regions were not among those most commonly reported in the reward literature (e.g., bilateral cerebellum), key regions from the reward outcome literature (Oldham et al., Reference Oldham, Murawski, Fornito, Youssef, Yücel and Lorenzetti2018) were all represented, including the ventral striatum, ventromedial PFC/rostral ACC, and the posterior cingulate. Furthermore, the maps made from Neurosynth were based on a meta-analysis of 922 studies, suggesting they represent reward network regions broadly supported in the literature. Thus, although the task used in the present study cannot clearly differentiate between important elements of reward-processing, it does provide a relatively generalizable test of the relationship of Extraversion and internalizing psychopathology to the neural activation following reward receipt.

Another limitation is that although a priori power analyses suggested that statistical power to locate the hypothesized, small-to-medium effect sizes (r = |.10| to |.20|) was very high in the full sample (N = 1050), the power to locate small effects (r = |.10|) was quite low (i.e., below chance) for the subsample analyses separated by gender. At the time of pre-registration, we believed that a hypothesized effect size of r = |.20| for the current relations of interest was reasonable. In hindsight, this was a significant overestimation of likely effect sizes, which led us to conduct the gender subsample analyses when we were relatively underpowered to find true, small effects. We reported these gender subsample analyses to be consistent with our pre-registration and in the interest of transparency, but acknowledge that these analyses are not adequately powered to reliably locate the effects found here, and thus must be interpreted with caution. Finally, although our sample size was relatively large for this type of effort, participants were relatively physically and psychologically healthy (see exclusion criteria in Van Essen et al., Reference Van Essen, Ugurbil, Auerbach, Barch, Behrens, Bucholz and Della Penna2013b). We believe it is essential to generalizability to examine these relations in samples characterized by higher rates of psychopathology, especially internalizing symptoms that are of most interest to this investigation.

4.2 Recommendations and future directions

As the corpus of evidence linking personality and psychopathology continues to grow, we believe that a thorough account of these constructs and the mechanisms that link them must involve multiple levels of analysis (e.g., biological, affective, cognitive, motivational, behavioral). However, the current research highlights some of the difficulties associated with this type of research: datasets large enough to conduct well-powered investigations across levels of analysis are scarce, the conclusions that one can draw from the fMRI tasks are often limited because they are designed to capture a very specific neural response, and the effect sizes of the relations between levels of analysis are likely to be very small (e.g., Poldrack et al., Reference Poldrack, Monahan, Imrey, Reyna, Raichle, Faigman and Buckholtz2018). Given these obstacles, we are encouraged by the trend toward larger, open datasets (e.g., HCP, U.K. Biobank, Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study), which can be harnessed to address important questions about the neural underpinnings of complex phenotypes. Moreover, another potential pitfall is the lack of standardization of procedural components of task fMRI paradigms, such as stimulus design, timing, and instructions given to participants. We believe that efforts to better standardize fMRI tasks by making consensus best practice recommendations (e.g., Verbruggen et al., Reference Verbruggen, Aron, Band, Beste, Bissett, Brockett and Colzato2019 for the stop-signal task) is also a welcome development in addressing these issues; we are hopeful that similar initiatives will manifest for reward-processing tasks.

In terms of future directions, we believe that several of the limitations of the current work are illustrative of the types of issues that researchers are likely to encounter when pursuing this type of integrative project, and we hope these acknowledgements will contribute to the avoidance of similar issues in future data collection efforts. First, we emphasize the pressing need for a more thoroughly validated laboratory tasks that allow researchers to draw conclusions about important behaviors. This involves, at a bare minimum, establishing indices of construct validity in well-powered samples (e.g., Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson2019), and providing a detailed quantification protocol that permits an operational definition that is congruent with the conceptual definition of the construct that one is attempting to capture. Moreover, we reiterate the need for more precise measurement of psychological constructs as well. The personality and psychopathology literature provides strong support for hierarchical conceptualizations of these constructs, and, therefore, measures designed to capture them should reflect this complex structure. Specifically, personality measures that permit assessment of domains and facets, as well as psychopathology measures that capture both broader latent constructs and more granular symptoms, are strongly preferred.

A final, sobering perspective to consider is that the current results are consistent with the null or very small effect sizes reported in multiple separate efforts to link personality traits to structural and functional neural variables in large samples (e.g., Avinun, Israel, Knodt, & Hariri, Reference Avinun, Israel, Knodt and Hariri2019; Baranger et al., Reference Baranger, Few, Sheinbein, Agrawal, Oltmanns, Knodt and Bogdan2020; Gray, Owens, Hyatt, & Miller, Reference Gray, Owens, Hyatt and Miller2018; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Jahn, Hyatt, Owens, Carter, Sweet, Miller and Haas2020). If one advocates a monist view of the brain and psychological functioning, these results can be difficult to square since this view implies that individual differences in emotions, motivations, behaviors, etc. necessarily emerge from a variety of physiological processes interacting with material elements of the environment. To compound this issue, this pattern of null findings does not appear to extend to all psychological individual differences, as there is recent evidence from a very large sample (N = 29,004) that intelligence bears a large relation (r = .28) to total brain volume, as well as other more specific regional correlates (e.g., insula volume; Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob, & Deary, Reference Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob and Deary2019). So, what might account for the observed lack of relations for personality traits (beyond the limitations previously listed)? There are several additional considerations that may help reconcile these null findings with this existential position.

First, it is possible that personality traits (at the level currently assessed) are too broad, or encompass too diverse of an array of psychological processes to neatly and uniquely map onto a single neurobiological index. Given that personality traits capture wide swaths of thoughts, emotions, and behaviors over the lifespan, we believe this is a reasonable conclusion (also see Yarkoni, Reference Yarkoni, Mikulincer, Shaver, Cooper and Larsen2015), and we reiterate our previous point about the importance of measuring specific individual difference variables. Second, it is possible that the current neuroimaging tools are not sufficient to capture the critical inter-individual neural variation that is relevant to accounting for personality differences. Given the major advancements in neuroimaging technology in recent decades, equipment shortcomings seem to be a questionable and unsatisfying explanation. On the contrary, it may well be that researchers are just beginning to completely harness all of the sophisticated data gathered by modern neuroimaging devices. We believe that a potentially viable avenue for future research is to investigate the degree to which personality and psychopathology are related to neural variables of various operationalizations (e.g., single neuron; neural networks), which are becoming more accessible and analyzable.

Third, it is possible that age is a moderator of personality–neural activation relations, such that more substantial links emerge at earlier stages of development. Given that estimates of environmental influences on personality traits tend to increase across childhood and adolescence (e.g., Briley & Tucker-Drob, Reference Briley and Tucker-Drob2014), it is possible the neural responses to various environmental stimuli might demonstrate theoretically predicted relations in younger individuals who are still undergoing critical phases of social development and learning behavioral contingencies that exist in their interpersonal worlds. Fourth, we analyzed neural activation in response to reward receipt as a between-person variable based on a relatively simple series of stimuli (i.e., feedback about trial outcome). It is possible that a task paradigm with more intensive within-person assessment (e.g., assessing neural response to a range of stimuli of varying intensities) may yield a more robust and reliable operationalization of neural response to reward receipt. Of note, this appears less relevant for structural indices, which demonstrate excellent reliability (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Knodt, Ireland, Morris, Poulton, Ramrakha and Hariri2019). Finally, a fifth possibility is that the effects of neural activity on personality are more readily identifiable “downstream” in neural circuitry. In other words, although the relation between a trait and relative degree of neural activation may be null when examined at the level of the brain, that neural activation may manifest in between-person variation in observable macro-level physiological (e.g., startle response, facial gestures) and cognitive (e.g., decision-making, selective attention) processes that are more directly and meaningfully tied to personality traits and/or indices of psychopathology.

4.3 Conclusions

In the largest sample to date, the personality domain Extraversion, its sub-components Agentic Extraversion and Communal Extraversion, as well as index of internalizing psychopathology were not significantly associated with neural activation following receipt of a reward. These results were inconsistent with theoretical explanations of the role of reward-processing in personality and psychopathology, but are consistent with other recent evidence that the relations between personality and the brain are, as currently measured, null or very small. We believe this work highlights the importance of clear operationalizations of complex constructs when investigating biological underpinnings, and we are hopeful that careful psychometric considerations will improve our search to better understand the biological substrates of personality and psychopathology.

Acknowledgments and Conflicts of Interest Declaration

The data used in this project are from the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University in St. Louis. The authors are deeply appreciative of the Human Connectome Project for open access to its data.

At the time of submission, the authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Footnotes

In R. D. Latzman, G. Michelini, C. G. DeYoung, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Novel investigations of the connection between quantitative personality psychopathology models and neuroscience.

1 To add to this complexity: while not reviewed extensively here, high levels of Extraversion have also been specifically linked to aspects of mania such as elation and excess energy (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Ellickson-Larew, Stanton, Levin-Aspenson, Khoo, Stasik-O’Brien and Clark2019a, Reference Watson, Stanton, Khoo, Ellickson-Larew and Stasik-O’Brien2019b). Moreover, Extraversion has been linked to personality disorder constructs typically associated with externalizing psychopathology, namely narcissism and psychopathy. Extraversion is related to the agentic, grandiose aspects of narcissism (Paulhus, 2002), which is seen as central to this construct by academicians, clinicians, and lay persons (Miller, Lynam, Hyatt, & Campbell, 2017). Additionally, historical writings (Cleckley, Reference Cleckley1941) and contemporary models of psychopathy (e.g., Triarchic Model of Psychopathy; Patrick, Fowles, & Krueger, 2009) identify a construct called “Boldness” or “Fearless Dominance,” which can be viewed as an admixture of high Extraversion and low Neuroticism (e.g., Hyatt, Crowe, Lynam, & Miller, 2020).

2 The NEO-FFI items included in the Agentic Extraversion variable are items 10, 25, 30, 32, 35, 47, 50, 52, 57, and 60. The NEO-FFI items included in the Communal Extraversion variable are items 2, 7, 12, 17, 22, 27, 37, and 42.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, University of Vermont.Google Scholar
Allen, T. A., & DeYoung, C. G. (2017). Personality neuroscience and the five factor model. In Widger, T. A. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of the five factor model (pp. 319352). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Avinun, R., Israel, S., Knodt, A. R., & Hariri, A. R. (2019). Little evidence for associations between the big five personality traits and variability in brain gray or white matter. bioRxiv, 658567. https://doi.org/10.1101/658567 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baranger, D. A., Few, L. R., Sheinbein, D. H., Agrawal, A., Oltmanns, T. F., Knodt, A. R., … & Bogdan, R. (2020). Borderline personality traits are not correlated with brain structure in two large samples. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5, 669677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.02.006 Google Scholar
Barch, D. M., Burgess, G. C., Harms, M. P., Petersen, S. E., Schlaggar, B. L., Corbetta, M., … Nolan, D. (2013). Function in the human connectome: Task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage, 80, 169189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E. J., Berk, R., … Johnson, V. E. (2018). Redefine statistical significance. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 610. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0189-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K. C. (2007). The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology, 191, 391431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briley, D. A., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2014). Genetic and environmental continuity in personality development: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 13031331. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037091 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 365376.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T., Sivers, H., Whitfield, S. L., Gotlib, I. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science, 296, 2191. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1068749 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 3342. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.33 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champely, S. (2018). pwr: Basic functions for power analysis. R package version 1.2-2. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pwr Google Scholar
Civai, C., Hawes, D. R., DeYoung, C. G., & Rustichini, A. (2016). Intelligence and extraversion in the neural evaluation of delayed rewards. Journal of Research in Personality, 61, 99108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.02.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316336. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.316 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (2019). Constructing validity: New developments in creating objective measuring instruments. Psychological Assessment, 31, 14121427. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000626 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.Google Scholar
Cohen, M. X., Young, J., Baek, J.-M., Kessler, C., & Ranganath, C. (2005). Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 851861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. C., Li, Y. I., & Starr, L. R. (2019). Trait anhedonia is a transdiagnostic correlate of internalizing problems during adolescence. Journal of Research in Personality, 81, 5663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.05.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668678. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.4.668 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Cox, S. R., Ritchie, S. J., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Deary, I. J. (2019). Structural brain imaging correlates of general intelligence in UK Biobank. Intelligence, 76, 101376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101376 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P., Van Straten, A., & Warmerdam, L. (2007). Behavioral activation treatments of depression: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 318326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.11.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, M. R., Locke, H. M., Stenger, V. A., & Fiez, J. A. (2003). Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: Effects of valence and magnitude manipulations. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 2738. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.3.1.27 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, M. R., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, C., Noll, D. C., & Fiez, J. A. (2000). Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 30723077. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2000.84.6.3072 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491517. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Cybernetic big five theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 3358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Carey, B. E., Krueger, R. F., & Ross, S. R. (2016). Ten aspects of the Big Five in the Personality Inventory for DSM-5. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7, 113123. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., & Krueger, R. F. (2018). A cybernetic theory of psychopathology. Psychological Inquiry, 29, 117138. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2018.1513680 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880896. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E., Sandvik, E. D., Pavot, W., & Fujita, F. (1992). Extraversion and subjective well-being in a US national probability sample. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 205215. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(92)90039-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, M. L., Knodt, A. R., Ireland, D., Morris, M. L., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., … Hariri, A. R. (2019). Poor test-retest reliability of task-fMRI: New empirical evidence and a meta- analysis. bioRxiv, 681700. Retrieved from: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/681700v1.article-info Google Scholar
Enkavi, A. Z., Eisenberg, I. W., Bissett, P. G., Mazza, G. L., MacKinnon, D. P., Marsch, L. A., & Poldrack, R. A. (2019). Large-scale analysis of test–retest reliabilities of self-regulation measures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 54725477. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818430116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (junior and adult). San Diego, CA: EdITS.Google Scholar
Forbes, E. E., Christopher May, J., Siegle, G. J., Ladouceur, C. D., Ryan, N. D., Carter, C. S., … Dahl, R. E. (2006). Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: An fMRI study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 10311040. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01673.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, E. E., Ryan, N. D., Phillips, M. L., Manuck, S. B., Worthman, C. M., Moyles, D. L., … Dahl, R. E. (2010). Healthy adolescents’ neural response to reward: Associations with puberty, positive affect, and depressive symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 162172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.006 Google ScholarPubMed
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 156168. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919847202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaughan, E. T., Miller, J. D., Pryor, L. R., & Lynam, D. R. (2009). Comparing two alternative measures of general personality in the assessment of psychopathy: A test of the NEO PI-R and the MPQ. Journal of Personality, 77, 965996. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00571.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., … Van Essen, D. C. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage, 80, 105124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 249266. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(70)90069-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. C., Owens, M. M., Hyatt, C. S., & Miller, J. D. (2018). No evidence for morphometric associations of the amygdala and hippocampus with the five-factor model personality traits in relatively healthy young adults. PLoS ONE, 13, e0204011. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, G. B., Milne, A. M., & MacQueen, G. M. (2014). An fMRI study of reward circuitry in patients with minimal or extensive history of major depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264, 187198. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0437-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, B. L., Fraley, R. C., Lahey, B. B., & Waldman, I. D. (2005). Is depression best viewed as a continuum or discrete category? A taxometric analysis of childhood and adolescent depression in a population-based sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 96110. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.96 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herting, M. M., Gautam, P., Chen, Z., Mezher, A., & Vetter, N. C. (2018). Test-retest reliability of longitudinal task-based fMRI: Implications for developmental studies. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 33, 1726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoerger, M., Chapman, B., & Duberstein, P. (2016). Realistic affective forecasting: The role of personality. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 13041316. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1061481 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussey, I., & Hughes, S. (2020). Hidden invalidity among 15 commonly used measures in social and personality psychology. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3, 166184. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919882903 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyatt, C. S., Crowe, M. L., Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (2020). Components of the Triarchic Model of Psychopathy and the Five-Factor Model domains share largely overlapping nomological networks. Assessment, 27, 7288. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119860903 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyatt, C. S., Owens, M. M., Gray, J. C., Carter, N. T., MacKillop, J., Sweet, L. H., & Miller, J. D. (2019). Personality traits share overlapping neuroanatomical correlates with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1 ¸ 111. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000391 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques-Hamilton, R., Sun, J., & Smillie, L. D. (2019). Costs and benefits of acting extraverted: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 15381556. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000516 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 768821. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020327 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Eaton, N. R. (2017). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 454477. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000258 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Derringer, J., Markon, K. E., Watson, D., & Skodol, A. E. (2012). Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychological Medicine, 42, 18791890. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002674 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kujawa, A., Proudfit, G. H., Kessel, E. M., Dyson, M., Olino, T., & Klein, D. N. (2015). Neural reactivity to monetary rewards and losses in childhood: Longitudinal and concurrent associations with observed and self-reported positive emotionality. Biological Psychology, 104, 4147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejuez, C. W., Hopko, D. R., Acierno, R., Daughters, S. B., & Pagoto, S. L. (2011). Ten year revision of the brief behavioral activation treatment for depression: Revised treatment manual. Behavior Modification, 35, 111161. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445510390929 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). A behavioral approach to depression. In Friedman, R. J. & Katz, M. M. (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 157185). Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, P. M., & Graf, M. (1973). Pleasant activities and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41, 261268. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035142 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the fundamental features of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 452468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.452 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, D. S., Harms, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., Jenkinson, M., Wilson, J. A., Glasser, M. F., … Hodge, M. (2013). Human Connectome Project informatics: Quality control, database services, and data visualization. NeuroImage, 80, 202219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, J. C., Delgado, M. R., Dahl, R. E., Stenger, V. A., Ryan, N. D., Fiez, J. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward-related brain circuitry in children and adolescents. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 359366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Hyatt, C. S., & Campbell, W. K. (2017). Controversies in narcissism. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13, 291315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, J. T., Kimbrel, N. A., Hundt, N. E., Cobb, A. R., Nelson-Gray, R. O., & Lootens, C. M. (2007). An analysis of reinforcement sensitivity theory and the five-factor model. European Journal of Personality, 21, 869887. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.644 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mobbs, D., Hagan, C., Azim, E., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2005). Personality predicts activity in reward and emotional regions associated with humor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 1650216506. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0408457102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 161187. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldham, S., Murawski, C., Fornito, A., Youssef, G., Yücel, M., & Lorenzetti, V. (2018). The anticipation and outcome phases of reward and loss processing: A neuroimaging meta- analysis of the monetary incentive delay task. Human Brain Mapping, 39, 33983418. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24184 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M. M., Hyatt, C. S., Gray, J. C., Carter, N. T., MacKillop, J., Miller, J. D., & Sweet, L. H. (2019a). Cortical morphometry of the five-factor model of personality: Findings from the Human Connectome Project full sample. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14, 381395. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsz017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M. M., Mackillop, J., McNally, S., Balodis, I., & Sweet, L. H. (2019b). Neural activation in reward network associated with inattentive symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Preprint available at https://psyarxiv.com/p6mnk/ CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 913938. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000492 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulhus, D. L. (2001). Normal narcissism: Two minimalist accounts. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 228230.Google Scholar
Pizzagalli, D. A., Holmes, A. J., Dillon, D. G., Goetz, E. L., Birk, J. L., Bogdan, R., … Fava, M. (2009). Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 702710. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., MacCallum, R. C., & Nicewander, W. A. (2005). Use of the extreme groups approach: A critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychological Methods, 10, 178192. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.2.178 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poldrack, R. A., Monahan, J., Imrey, P. B., Reyna, V., Raichle, M. E., Faigman, D., & Buckholtz, J. W. (2018). Predicting violent behavior: What can neuroscience add? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 111123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, B. W., Luo, J., Briley, D. A., Chow, P. I., Su, R., & Hill, P. L. (2017). A systematic review of personality trait change through intervention. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 117141. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, M., Knuth, M., & Rumpel, F. (2011). Striatal response to favorite brands as a function of neuroticism and extraversion. Brain Research, 1425, 8389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 609612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segarra, P., Poy, R., López, R., & Moltó, J. (2014). Characterizing Carver and White’s BIS/BAS subscales using the Five Factor Model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 61, 1823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.12.027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Servaas, M. N., Van Der Velde, J., Costafreda, S. G., Horton, P., Ormel, J., Riese, H., & Aleman, A. (2013). Neuroticism and the brain: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies investigating emotion processing. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 15181529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, J. J., Biller, A., Walther, S., Roesch-Ely, D., Stippich, C., Weisbrod, M., & Kaiser, S. (2010). Neural correlates of reward processing in schizophrenia—relationship to apathy and depression. Schizophrenia Research, 118, 154161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2009.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smillie, L. D. (2013). Extraversion and reward processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 167172. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412470133 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smillie, L. D., Jach, H. K., Hughes, D. M., Wacker, J., Cooper, A. J., & Pickering, A. D. (2019). Extraversion and reward-processing: Consolidating evidence from an electroencephalographic index of reward-prediction-error. Biological Psychology, 107735. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107735 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smoski, M. J., Felder, J., Bizzell, J., Green, S. R., Ernst, M., Lynch, T. R., & Dichter, G. S. (2009). fMRI of alterations in reward selection, anticipation, and feedback in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 118, 6978. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.034 Google ScholarPubMed
Sun, J., Kaufman, S. B., & Smillie, L. D. (2018). Unique associations between Big Five personality aspects and multiple dimensions of well-being. Journal of Personality, 86, 158172. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12301 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, T., Hill, K. E., Ait Oumeziane, B., Foti, D., & Samuel, D. B. (2019). Bringing the brain into personality assessment: Is there a place for event-related potentials? Psychological Assessment, 31, 488501. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In Tuma, A. H. & Maser, J. D. (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (p. 681706). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Exploring personality through test construction: Development of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. In Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and testing: Vol. II. Personality measurement and assessment (pp. 261292). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treadway, M. T., Bossaller, N. A., Shelton, R. C., & Zald, D. H. (2012). Effort-based decision-making in major depressive disorder: A translational model of motivational anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 553558. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028813 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tricomi, E. M., Delgado, M. R., & Fiez, J. A. (2004). Modulation of caudate activity by action contingency. Neuron, 41, 281292. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00848-1 Google ScholarPubMed
Turner, B. O., Paul, E. J., Miller, M. B., & Barbey, A. K. (2018). Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of task-based fMRI studies. Communications Biology, 1, 62. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-018-0073-z Google ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 767793). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Ellickson-Larew, S., Stanton, K., Levin-Aspenson, H. F., Khoo, S., Stasik-O’Brien, S. M., & Clark, L. A. (2019a). Aspects of extraversion and their associations with psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128, 777794. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000459 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Stanton, K., Khoo, S., Ellickson-Larew, S., & Stasik-O’Brien, S. M. (2019b). Extraversion and psychopathology: A multilevel hierarchical review. Journal of Research in Personality, 81, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.04.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Stasik, S. M., Ellickson-Larew, S., & Stanton, K. (2015). Extraversion and psychopathology: A facet-level analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 432446. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, B., Jahn, A., Hyatt, C. S., Owens, M. M., Carter, N. T., Sweet, L. H., Miller, J. D., & Haas, B. W. (2020). Investigating the neural substrates of externalizing symptoms and social-cognitive theory of mind: An fMRI examination of functional activity and synchrony. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
Wiggins, J. S., Trapnell, P., & Phillips, N. (1988). Psychometric and geometric characteristics of the Revised Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS-R). Multivariate Behavioral Research, 23, 517530. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2304_8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, G. D., Barrett, P. T., & Gray, J. A. (1989). Human reactions to reward and punishment: A questionnaire examination of Gray’s personality theory. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 509515. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02339.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitton, A. E., Treadway, M. T., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28, 712. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000122 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, C. C., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Katovich, K., & Knutson, B. (2014). Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of incentive anticipation. NeuroImage, 84, 279289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.055 Google ScholarPubMed
Van der Linden, D., te Nijenhuis, J., & Bakker, A. B. (2010). The general factor of personality: A meta-analysis of Big Five intercorrelations and a criterion-related validity study. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 315327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.03.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Essen, D. C., Smith, S. M., Barch, D. M., Behrens, T. E., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K., & Wu-Minn HCP Consortium. (2013a). The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: An overview. NeuroImage, 80, 6279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041 Google ScholarPubMed
Van Essen, D. C., Ugurbil, K., Auerbach, E., Barch, D., Behrens, T. E. J., Bucholz, R., … Della Penna, S. (2013b). The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage, 62, 22222231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verbruggen, F., Aron, A. R., Band, G. P., Beste, C., Bissett, P. G., Brockett, A. T., … Colzato, L. S. (2019). A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. Elife, 8, e46323. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46323.001 Google ScholarPubMed
Yarkoni, T. (2015). Neurobiological substrates of personality: A critical overview. In Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Cooper, M. L., & Larsen, R. J. (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 4. Personality processes and individual differences. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Zelenski, J. M., Whelan, D. C., Nealis, L. J., Besner, C. M., Santoro, M. S., & Wynn, J. E. (2013). Personality and affective forecasting: Trait introverts underpredict the hedonic benefits of acting extraverted. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 10921108. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032281 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information on the current sample (N = 1050)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Empirically defined regions of interest from group-level activation of wins vs. losses.Note: Regions of interest exhibited significant activity during the win conditions on the gambling task compared to losses; Talairach Z-plane coordinates = +60 to −5-mm slices; thresholded to uncorrected p = 1−45

Figure 2

Table 2. Empirically defined regions of interest

Figure 3

Figure 2. A priori defined regions of interest associated with reward-processing.Note: Brain regions associated with “reward” in Neurosynth Meta-analysis; Talairach Z-plane coordinates = +25 to −30 in 5-mm slices; thresholded to false discovery rate q = .05.

Figure 4

Table 3. A priori defined regions of interest

Figure 5

Table 4. Relations between personality traits and psychopathology indices

Figure 6

Table 5. Pearson’s Correlations between fMRI reward task processing in empirically defined ROIs, personality, and psychopathology

Figure 7

Table 6. Pearson’s correlations between fMRI reward task processing in a priori defined ROIs, personality, and psychopathology