Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T10:09:56.119Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional eating and obesity in adults: the role of depression, sleep and genes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 March 2020

Hanna Konttinen*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 18, 00014, Helsinki, Finland Department of Food and Nutrition, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 66, 00014, Helsinki, Finland
*
Corresponding author: Hanna Konttinen, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Stress and other negative emotions, such as depression and anxiety, can lead to both decreased and increased food intake. The term ‘emotional eating’ has been widely used to refer to the latter response: a tendency to eat in response to negative emotions with the chosen foods being primarily energy-dense and palatable ones. Emotional eating can be caused by various mechanisms, such as using eating to cope with negative emotions or confusing internal states of hunger and satiety with physiological changes related to emotions. An increasing number of prospective studies have shown that emotional eating predicts subsequent weight gain in adults. This review discusses particularly three lines of research on emotional eating and obesity in adults. First, studies implying that emotional eating may be one behavioural mechanism linking depression and development of obesity. Secondly, studies highlighting the relevance of night sleep duration by showing that adults with a combination of shorter sleep and higher emotional eating may be especially vulnerable to weight gain. Thirdly, an emerging literature suggesting that genes may influence body weight partly through emotional eating and other eating behaviour dimensions. The review concludes by discussing what kind of implications these three avenues of research offer for obesity prevention and treatment interventions.

Type
Conference on ‘Malnutrition in an obese world: European perspectives’
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Emotions and eating are both inherent and recurring part of our daily lives. Research has also demonstrated that they interact with each other in multiple ways: emotional states influence the quantity and quality of foods eaten, and food intake has affective consequences that may influence subsequent food choices(Reference Gibson, Shepherd and Raats1). Various physiological, psychological and social processes are likely to be involved in this interaction. Importantly, food intake is considered to be controlled by both homeostatic and hedonic processes with the former concerning the regulation of energy-balance and the latter the influence of pleasure, reward and palatability in food intake(Reference Blundell, Dalton, Gibbons and Hankey2). It might be argued that the interaction between emotions and eating has strengthened in the contemporary food-rich environments where an increasing proportion of eating is driven by hedonic processes(Reference Lowe and Butryn3).

This review focuses mainly on the influence of negative emotions on eating behaviour and weight changes as well as on the underlying psychological processes. Especially, three lines of literature related to emotional eating (i.e. eating triggered by negative emotions) and obesity in adults are discussed: First, studies focusing on the symptoms of depression, emotional eating and development of obesity are summarised; secondly, research on the interaction between sleep duration and emotional eating in affecting weight gain; finally, studies examining emotional eating and other eating behaviour dimensions in the context of the genetic architecture of obesity are reviewed. Before discussing these three avenues of research, some key definitions and findings of the negative emotion–eating behaviour literature are briefly overviewed.

The effect of negative emotions on eating behaviour

What are emotions and why do we experience them? Emotions arise when we evaluate a situation as being relevant to a currently active goal(Reference Gross and Gross4). For example, if an individual learns that s/he has been selected to a highly motivating job, s/he is likely to experience positive emotions, such as excitement and joy. The opposite situation of not being selected is likely to lead to experiencing various negative emotions, such as disappointment and sadness. Another important characteristic is that emotions are multifaceted phenomena that involve changes in three domains: subjective experience, behaviour and physiology (e.g. changes in heart rate, blood pressure, respiration, sweating and gastrointestinal motility). It is thus plausible that emotions serve several important functions in our daily life: they direct our attention, influence our decision making and motivate our behaviour among other things. Compared to emotions, moods often last longer and may appear in the absence of obvious stimuli(Reference Gross and Gross4). The term ‘stress’ is also frequently used in the negative emotion–eating behaviour literature. While both stress and emotion involve whole-body responses to significant events, stress usually refers to negative affective responses and emotion to both negative and positive affective states(Reference Lazarus5).

During the past decades of research, it has become clear that stress and negative emotions may lead to both decreased and increased food intake and heightened preference for palatable energy-dense foods (for reviews, see(Reference Macht6,Reference Devonport, Nicholls and Fullerton7) ). Fig. 1 summarises how the influence of negative emotions on eating depends on at least three factors: the characteristics of emotions, individuals and situations. High-intensity or high-arousal emotions (e.g. fear, tension) tend to suppress eating as they are related to physiological and behavioural responses that reduce appetite and interfere with eating(Reference Macht6). In contrast, negative emotions with more moderate levels of arousal or intensity may decrease or increase food intake depending on the characteristics of an individual and a situation (e.g. available foods). The term ‘emotional eating’ has been widely used to refer to the latter response: a tendency to eat in response to negative emotions with the chosen foods being primarily energy-dense and highly palatable ones. While the reasons for these individual differences are not fully understood, various mechanisms are likely to be involved, such as those linked to adverse childhood experiences, learning history, chronic stress and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis functioning and cortisol secretion(Reference van Strien8).

Fig. 1. Overview on how the influence of negative emotions on eating depends on the characteristics of emotions, individuals and situations (e.g. available foods).

Various self-report questionnaires have been developed to assess the tendency to emotional eating (for a review, see(Reference Frayn and Knäuper9)). One of the most widely used is the thirty-three-item Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire(Reference Van Strien, Frijters and Bergers10) containing scales for emotional eating (thirteen items, e.g. Do you have a desire to eat when you are irritated?), external eating (ten items, e.g. If food smells and looks good, do you eat more than usual?) and restrained eating (ten items, e.g. Do you try to eat less at mealtimes than you would like to eat?). The emotional eating scale of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire measures respondents’ desire to eat under different negative emotions, including stress, depression and anxiety. The eighteen-item Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ)(Reference Karlsson, Persson and Sjostrom11) with scales for emotional eating, uncontrolled eating and cognitive restraint has gained popularity in epidemiological research where shorter questionnaires are often preferred. The eighteen-item TFEQ is a revised and shortened version of the original fifty-one-item TFEQ(Reference Stunkard and Messick12). The emotional eating scale of the eighteen-item TFEQ contains only three items asking respondents to evaluate how negative emotions affect their eating (When I feel anxious, I find myself eating; When I feel blue I often overeat; When I feel lonely, I console myself by eating). Although this review focuses on emotional eating, it is noteworthy that emotional eating often co-occurs with other dimensions of eating behaviour, particularly external eating characterised by overeating in response to food-related cues, such as sight and smell of attractive foods(Reference van Strien8).

Many experimental studies (for a systematic review, see(Reference Devonport, Nicholls and Fullerton7)) have shown that individuals, who score high on these emotional eating scales, tend to eat more energy-dense foods after inducement of negative emotions in the laboratory (e.g. via a sad movie) compared to emotionally neutral control situations. However, not all experiments have found this effect and several factors are likely to explain the inconsistency. Some null findings might be explained by the misclassification of participants to ‘emotional eaters’ and ‘no emotional eaters’ due to the use of the median splits and/or insufficient amount of participants with extreme scores(Reference van Strien, Herman and Anschutz13). It is also possible that emotional eating follows only specific discrete negative emotions and these triggering emotions vary between individuals(Reference Altheimer and Urry14). Moreover, emotional eating may depend on social context with the laboratory representing a non-natural situation where it is less likely to manifest(Reference Altheimer and Urry14).

Several psychological mechanisms likely underlie emotional eating and a more detailed review on this topic and related theories can be found elsewhere(Reference Macht6,Reference van Strien8) . In short, individuals may engage in emotional eating as an attempt to cope with stress and other negative emotions. However, in the long-term, emotional eating is often a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy. Even though experimentally induced negative mood state has found to improve immediately after eating palatable food, the effect tends to be short-term and can be followed by other negative emotions, such as feelings of guilt(Reference Macht and Mueller15,Reference Haedt-Matt, Keel and Racine16) . Emotional eating may also be a result of a poor interoceptive awareness characterised by deficits in sensitivity to bodily sensations. It is possible that individuals with poor interoceptive awareness confuse bodily states related to emotions with physiological internal states guiding satiety and hunger. Finally, in dieters, emotional stimuli may impair their ability to restrict food intake because processing emotions requires cognitive resources.

Emotional eating, food intake and obesity in observational studies

Epidemiological studies utilising different types of FFQ to assess food intake have demonstrated that higher self-reported emotional eating is related to greater intakes of sweet and non-sweet energy-dense foods and snacks, such as sweets and chocolate, cakes and biscuits, sweet and savoury pastries, pizza, hamburgers, French fries and sausages(Reference de Lauzon, Romon and Deschamps17Reference Paans, Gibson-Smith and Bot20). A relatively large (with more than 400 participants) 4-week diary study provided interesting insights by investigating day-to-day within-person effects of daily hassles on food intake(Reference O'Connor, Jones and Conner21). Participants were asked to list each food eaten between meals and to report each stressor or hassle experienced and its intensity at the end of each day. The study found that daily hassles were related to increased consumption of high-fat or sugar snacks, particularly in emotional eaters. As emotional eating is related to personality, correlating particularly with neuroticism characterised by the propensity to experience negative emotions(Reference Elfhag and Morey22), these findings might also partly reflect the effects of underlying personality traits on linking stress and eating behaviour.

Moreover, observations from the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey suggest that emotional eating relates to a distinct pattern of eating architecture(Reference Magklis, Howe and Johnson23). In other words, individuals with higher emotional eating scores on the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire tended to have smaller meals and more frequent snacking over a longer period every day (with an earlier start and a later finish time) than those scoring lower on emotional eating. These observations are potentially important by suggesting that interventions promoting healthy food intake in emotional eaters might benefit from targeting the number, timing and energetic content of snacks. However, further research is needed to identify what are the most effective behaviour change strategies to encourage such changes in emotional eaters.

In line with the afore-mentioned results, self-reported emotional eating has been associated with higher BMI, waist circumference and body fat percentage in cross-sectional studies(Reference van Strien, Herman and Verheijden24Reference Peneau, Menard and Mejean26). Although emotional eating was unrelated to 2-year change in BMI in a French community-based cohort(Reference de Lauzon-Guillain, Basdevant and Romon27), other prospective cohort studies conducted in the Netherlands, Finland, France, USA and Korea have shown that adults (both genders or only women) with higher emotional eating at baseline tend to gain more weight over time(Reference Koenders and van Strien28Reference Konttinen, van Strien and Mannisto33). Taken together, epidemiological evidence rather consistently supports the idea that self-reported emotional eaters consume energy-dense foods more frequently and are at increased risk for developing obesity.

Symptoms of depression and emotional eating

Depression represents one type of negative emotion or mood that may trigger food intake in emotional eaters, and research has shown that individuals with elevated depressive symptoms report higher levels of emotional eating(Reference Konttinen, Silventoinen and Sarlio-Lahteenkorva25,Reference Ouwens, van Strien and van Leeuwe34,Reference Paans, Bot and van Strien35) . However, depression is a heterogeneous syndrome with atypical and melancholic depression increasingly being differentiated in depression studies(Reference Penninx36). While melancholic depression is typically characterised by loss of appetite and subsequent weight loss, atypical depression is characterised by vegetative symptoms of increased appetite and weight gain. The prevalence of the latter depression subtype is not as low as the name suggests. Epidemiological studies using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders criteria indicated that 15–29 % of depressed patients have atypical depression, and the corresponding prevalence rate from the clinical studies was 18–36 %(Reference Lojko and Rybakowski37). Emotional eating has been proposed to represent a marker of atypical depression, because it shares with this depression subtype the atypical feature of increased appetite in response to distress. In line with this, Paans et al. found in the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety that vegetative symptoms characteristic of atypical depression (increase in appetite, weight gain) had relatively stronger positive associations with emotional eating than other individual depressive symptoms(Reference Paans, Bot and van Strien35).

Accumulating evidence from cross-sectional and prospective studies further suggests that emotional eating is one pathway linking depression with weight gain and obesity(Reference Konttinen, Silventoinen and Sarlio-Lahteenkorva25,Reference van Strien, Konttinen and Homberg30,Reference Vittengl32,Reference Konttinen, van Strien and Mannisto33,Reference Clum, Rice and Broussard38Reference van Strien, Winkens and Toft40) . The results from the few prospective studies analysing weight change over time as an outcome are particularly interesting. van Strien et al. observed that emotional eating acted as a mediator between depression and 5-year weight gain in Dutch mothers (although not in Dutch fathers)(Reference van Strien, Winkens and Toft40). More specifically, depressive symptoms were related to higher emotional eating and emotional eating predicted a greater increase in BMI independently of depressive symptoms. Depression was also indirectly related to BMI change via emotional eating. Konttinen et al. extended these results by showing in a large population-based cohort of Finnish 25–74-year-olds that the effects of depression on a greater increase in BMI and waist circumference over 7 years were mediated by emotional eating(Reference Konttinen, van Strien and Mannisto33).

In addition, it has become clear that the relationship between depression and obesity is bidirectional: besides that depression can lead to subsequent weight gain and development of obesity, it is also possible that obesity leads to later development of depression(Reference Rooke and Thorsteinsson41,Reference Luppino, de Wit and Bouvy42) . The findings from a national sample of mid-life US adults with 18-year follow-up suggest that emotional eating plays a role in this bidirectional depression–obesity association(Reference Vittengl32). Path analyses conducted in women revealed that physical impairment, social dysfunction and emotional eating mediated the development of obesity from depression, and that physical impairment and emotional eating mediated the development of depression from obesity.

Sleep duration and emotional eating

Insufficient sleep has been postulated to be one of the key non-traditional (i.e. not directly related to energy-balance) determinants of excess body weight with different lines of evidence linking short sleep duration and positive energy-balance(Reference Tremblay43). But what is insufficient sleep or short sleep duration? Grandner et al. have outlined working definitions for these and related terms(Reference Grandner, Patel and Gehrman44). They define sleep insufficiency as a reduction in sleep time of a magnitude associated with negative health outcomes, and point out that many epidemiological studies use the term short sleep to refer to a habitual sleep time of 6 h or less. The hectic pace of contemporary lifestyle in prosperous societies is prone to interfere with sleeping, and the occurrence of insomnia-related symptoms has risen in several countries during the past decades. For example, epidemiological data from Finland demonstrated that the prevalence of working-age adults reporting occasional insomnia-related symptoms increased from 30 to 45 % between 1972 and 2012(Reference Kronholm, Partonen and Harma45). It is however noteworthy that findings on secular trends in adult sleep duration have been more inconsistent(Reference Geiker, Astrup and Hjorth46).

In general, the present literature points to the importance of studying whether short sleep duration and poor sleep quality interact with emotional eating in affecting weight gain. First, it has become clear that insufficient sleep and stress are closely intertwined: the former can be both a marker of perceived stress and a source of stress itself(Reference Geiker, Astrup and Hjorth46). Secondly, evidence is emerging that insufficient sleep interferes with emotion regulation via a number of neurobiological, behavioural and cognitive processes(Reference Palmer and Alfano47). For example, insufficient sleep produces more negative and less positive emotions, and can affect the ability to choose an appropriate emotion regulation strategy and implement that strategy successfully. Thirdly, epidemiological studies in adults have consistently associated short sleep duration with higher total energy intake and higher total fat intake(Reference Dashti, Scheer and Jacques48). Short sleep may also associate with irregular eating behaviour; short sleepers tend to eat fewer main meals and more frequent, smaller, energy-dense and highly palatable snacks throughout the day (and particularly at night) than longer sleepers.

To date, at least three studies have provided evidence that short sleep duration might strengthen the association between emotional eating and subsequent weight gain. In a sample of Dutch employees, women with a combination of short sleep duration and high emotional eating experienced the greatest increase in BMI over 2 years(Reference van Strien and Koenders49). Chaput et al. assessed a broader construct of disinhibition (i.e. a tendency to overeat in response to food cues or emotional cues) in a sample of French Canadian adults with 6-year follow-up(Reference Chaput, Despres and Bouchard50). They found that individuals having both short sleep duration and high disinhibition were more likely to gain weight and increase their waist circumference over time, while short-duration sleepers having low disinhibition were not more likely to increase their adiposity indicators than were average-duration sleepers. Moreover, a similar pattern was observed in relation to energy intake assessed using a 3-day food record. In the large Finnish adult cohort discussed in the previous section, emotional eating predicted higher increase in BMI and waist circumference over 7 years in shorter sleepers (7 h or less), but not in longer sleepers (9 h or more)(Reference Konttinen, van Strien and Mannisto33).

It is important to note that short sleep duration represents only a proxy measure for insufficient sleep; future research should therefore explore these associations using a diverse set of sleep measures and in specific population groups, including shift workers. Short sleep is likely to be intertwined with perceived stress in individuals who reduce their sleep time to meet other demands of daily life and in those experiencing sleep problems, but not in those for whom short sleep schedule represents their natural way of functioning(Reference Grandner, Patel and Gehrman44). The role of chronotype (i.e. individual's inherent preference for mornings or evenings) in this context also deserves attention because eveningness has shown to correlate positively with both depression and emotional eating(Reference Konttinen, Kronholm and Partonen51).

Genetic susceptibility to obesity and emotional eating

An increasing number of studies have investigated emotional eating and other eating behaviour dimensions in the context of the genetic architecture of obesity. Genetic factors have a substantial influence on within-population variation in BMI: in a large pooled study of 1 40 000 twin pairs aged 20–80 years, the heritability estimates for BMI (i.e. the proportion of total BMI variation explained by genetic variation) varied between 60 and 80 % depending on age(Reference Silventoinen, Jelenkovic and Sund52). Albeit learning and environmental factors are important determinants of eating behaviours, observations from twin studies imply that also genetic factors influence these behaviours (assessed using the eighteen-item TFEQ or the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire). In Swedish, Finnish and British adult twins, the size of the heritability estimates for restrained eating (26–63 %), uncontrolled eating (45–69 %) and emotional eating (9–60 %) varied between small and moderate(Reference Tholin, Rasmussen and Tynelius53,Reference Keskitalo, Tuorila and Spector54) . Similarly, a study in Korean adults (twins and their family members) reported heritability estimates of 31 % for restrained eating, 25 % for emotional eating and 25 % for external eating(Reference Sung, Lee and Song55).

The emergence of genome-wide association studies has made it possible to identify common genetic variants that have only a small effect on the trait, such as BMI. Meta-analyses combining the results of genome-wide association studies of European-descent individuals have identified hundreds of SNP associated with higher BMI(Reference Yengo, Sidorenko and Kemper56,Reference Locke, Kahali and Berndt57) . Although the identified SNP explain only a small amount of total genetic variation in BMI, these findings have opened the opportunity to extend understanding of the mechanisms that mediate the effects of common genetic variants on obesity. Appetite-related eating behaviours (including emotional eating) have postulated to represent one such pathway. This idea is supported by the data that many of the BMI-related SNP are expressed particularly in the hypothalamus and pituitary gland, which have a key role in regulating food intake and energy expenditure(Reference Locke, Kahali and Berndt57).

In two Finnish adult cohorts(Reference Konttinen, Llewellyn and Wardle58) and in French and UK adult cohorts(Reference de Lauzon-Guillain, Clifton and Day59), participants with a higher polygenetic risk score of obesity had also a slightly higher tendency to emotional and uncontrolled eating. Moreover, the positive effects of the polygenetic risk score on BMI and waist circumference were partly mediated by these eating behaviours. Comparable results were obtained in a Canadian study in which perceived hunger, habitual disinhibition and situational disinhibition (although not emotional disinhibition) acted as mediators(Reference Jacob, Drapeau and Tremblay60). Mediation was not formally tested in two US cohorts of older adults, but the effect of the polygenetic risk score on BMI weakened slightly after controlling for restrained, uncontrolled and emotional eating(Reference Cornelis, Rimm and Curhan61). In line with these molecular genetic findings, the twin study mentioned earlier found that phenotypic correlations between BMI and emotional eating or uncontrolled eating were largely due to shared genetic factors (which may also include gene–environment interactions)(Reference Keskitalo, Tuorila and Spector54).

Although this cross-sectional research offers tentative evidence that genes influence obesity via appetite-related eating behaviours, important questions remain(Reference Llewellyn62). Other processes are likely to be involved; for instance, studies have consistently found that genetic susceptibility to obesity is more pronounced in physically inactive individuals than in active individuals(Reference Kilpelainen, Qi and Brage63,Reference Ahmad, Rukh and Varga64) . It is also possible that the causal pathways are more complex than those implied by the tested mediation models. While the examined eating behaviours were postulated to affect the development of obesity in these models, reverse causation might operate via different mechanisms, such as body fat gain impairing the control of food intake, and increased body size leading to greater experiences of weight stigma and stress(Reference Blundell, Dalton, Gibbons and Hankey2,Reference Tomiyama65) .

Conclusions

What kind of practical implications do these three lines of research provide for obesity prevention and treatment programmes? Essentially, they suggest that individuals with a high susceptibility to emotional eating might benefit from interventions that teach emotion regulation skills and distress tolerance strategies and aim to improve emotional well-being. A recent review concluded that several such approaches have shown promising results in reducing emotional eating and facilitating weight loss, including mindfulness, acceptance and commitment therapy, cognitive behaviour therapy and dialectical behaviour therapy(Reference Frayn and Knäuper9). However, further high-quality studies on this topic are still needed.

The literature also suggests that interventions that improve other lifestyle behaviours, such as sleep and physical activity, might offer a novel avenue to promote healthy food intake patterns in emotional eaters. A pilot study performed in habitually short sleepers indicated that sleep extension was feasible and led to decreased intake of free sugars(Reference Al Khatib, Hall and Creedon66). While the study did not contain information on emotional eating or other eating behaviour dimensions, it encourages continuing this type of intervention research. Moreover, physical exercise is used as a treatment for depression disorders(Reference Cooney, Dwan and Mead67), and some data exist that exercise can alleviate unhealthy food intake in response to stress(Reference Leow, Jackson and Alderson68).

Perhaps paradoxically, research into the genetic architecture of obesity has reinforced the view that there is an urgent need to modify the present food-rich environment(Reference Llewellyn62). Evidence that genetic factors play a role in individual differences in eating behaviours emphasises that people are not equal in terms of their inherent vulnerability to the environment in which palatable energy-dense foods are constantly available, cheap and intensively promoted. In fact, this is a part of a wider and highly topical discussion that a radical transformation of the global food system is urgently needed in order to improve human health and well-being and environmental sustainability on Earth(Reference Willett, Rockstrom and Loken69).

Financial Support

This work was supported by the Academy of Finland (grant numbers 314135, 309157).

Conflict of Interest

None.

Authorship

The author had sole responsibility for all aspects of preparation of this paper.

References

Gibson, EL (2006) Mood, emotions and food choice. In The Psychology of Food Choice, pp. 113140 [Shepherd, R and Raats, M, editors]. Wallingford, UK: CABI.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blundell, JE, Dalton, M & Gibbons, C (2018) Food intake and appetite in the aetiology of obesity. In Advanced Nutrition and Dietetics in Obesity, pp. 97105 [Hankey, C, editor]. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Lowe, MR & Butryn, ML (2007) Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol Behav 91, 432439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, JJ (2014) Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation, pp. 320 [Gross, JJ, editor]. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, RS (1993) From psychological stress to the emotions: a history of changing outlooks. Annu Rev Psychol 44, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macht, M (2008) How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite 50, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devonport, TJ, Nicholls, W & Fullerton, C (2019) A systematic review of the association between emotions and eating behaviour in normal and overweight adult populations. J Health Psychol 24, 324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T (2018) Causes of emotional eating and matched treatment of obesity. Curr Diab Rep 18, 35. [Epublication].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frayn, M & Knäuper, B (2018) Emotional eating and weight in adults: a review. Curr Psychol 37, 924933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Strien, T, Frijters, JE, Bergers, GP et al. (1986) The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int J Eat Disord 5, 295315.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson, J, Persson, LO, Sjostrom, L et al. (2000) Psychometric properties and factor structure of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men and women. Results from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24, 17151725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stunkard, AJ & Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 29, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T, Herman, CP, Anschutz, DJ et al. (2012) Moderation of distress-induced eating by emotional eating scores. Appetite 58, 277284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altheimer, G & Urry, HL (2019) Do emotions cause eating? The role of previous experiences and social context in emotional eating. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 28, 234240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macht, M & Mueller, J (2007) Immediate effects of chocolate on experimentally induced mood states. Appetite 49, 667674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haedt-Matt, AA, Keel, PK, Racine, SE et al. (2014) Do emotional eating urges regulate affect? Concurrent and prospective associations and implications for risk models of binge eating. Int J Eat Disord 47, 874877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lauzon, B, Romon, M, Deschamps, V et al. (2004) The three-factor eating questionnaire-R18 is able to distinguish among different eating patterns in a general population. J Nutr 134, 23722380.Google Scholar
Elfhag, K, Tholin, S & Rasmussen, F (2008) Consumption of fruit, vegetables, sweets and soft drinks are associated with psychological dimensions of eating behaviour in parents and their 12-year-old children. Public Health Nutr 11, 914923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konttinen, H, Mannisto, S, Sarlio-Lahteenkorva, S et al. (2010) Emotional eating, depressive symptoms and self-reported food consumption. A population-based study. Appetite 54, 473479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paans, NPG, Gibson-Smith, D, Bot, M et al. (2019) Depression and eating styles are independently associated with dietary intake. Appetite 134, 103110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, DB, Jones, F, Conner, M et al. (2008) Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. Health Psychol 27, S20S31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfhag, K & Morey, LC (2008) Personality traits and eating behavior in the obese: poor self-control in emotional and external eating but personality assets in restrained eating. Eat Behav 9, 285293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magklis, E, Howe, LD & Johnson, L (2019) Eating style and the frequency, size and timing of eating occasions: a cross-sectional analysis using 7–day weighed dietary records. Sci Rep 9, 15133. [Epublication]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T, Herman, CP & Verheijden, MW (2009) Eating style, overeating, and overweight in a representative Dutch sample. Does external eating play a role? Appetite 52, 380387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konttinen, H, Silventoinen, K, Sarlio-Lahteenkorva, S et al. (2010) Emotional eating and physical activity self-efficacy as pathways in the association between depressive symptoms and adiposity indicators. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 10311039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peneau, S, Menard, E, Mejean, C et al. (2013) Sex and dieting modify the association between emotional eating and weight status. Am J Clin Nutr 97, 13071313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lauzon-Guillain, B, Basdevant, A, Romon, M et al. (2006) Is restrained eating a risk factor for weight gain in a general population? Am J Clin Nutr 83, 132138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenders, PG & van Strien, T (2011) Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. J Occup Environ Med 53, 12871293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, YM, Lee, K, Sung, J et al. (2013) Changes in eating behaviors and body weight in Koreans: the healthy twin study. Nutrition 29, 6670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T, Konttinen, H, Homberg, JR et al. (2016) Emotional eating as a mediator between depression and weight gain. Appetite 100, 216224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benard, M, Bellisle, F, Etile, F et al. (2018) Impulsivity and consideration of future consequences as moderators of the association between emotional eating and body weight status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 15, 84. [Epublication]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vittengl, JR (2018) Mediation of the bidirectional relations between obesity and depression among women. Psychiatry Res 264, 254259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konttinen, H, van Strien, T, Mannisto, S et al. (2019) Depression, emotional eating and long-term weight changes: a population-based prospective study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 16, 28. [Epublication]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouwens, MA, van Strien, T & van Leeuwe, JF (2009) Possible pathways between depression, emotional and external eating. A structural equation model. Appetite 53, 245248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paans, NPG, Bot, M, van Strien, T et al. (2018) Eating styles in major depressive disorder: results from a large-scale study. J Psychiatr Res 97, 3846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penninx, BW (2017) Depression and cardiovascular disease: epidemiological evidence on their linking mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 74, 277286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lojko, D & Rybakowski, JK (2017) Atypical depression: current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 13, 24472456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clum, GA, Rice, JC, Broussard, M et al. (2014) Associations between depressive symptoms, self-efficacy, eating styles, exercise and body mass index in women. J Behav Med 37, 577586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldschmidt, AB, Crosby, RD, Engel, SG et al. (2014) Affect and eating behavior in obese adults with and without elevated depression symptoms. Int J Eat Disord 47, 281286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T, Winkens, L, Toft, MB et al. (2016) The mediation effect of emotional eating between depression and body mass index in the two European countries Denmark and Spain. Appetite 105, 500508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rooke, SE & Thorsteinsson, EB (2008) Examining the temporal relationship between depression and obesity: meta-analyses of prospective research. Health Psychol Rev 2, 94109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luppino, FS, de Wit, LM, Bouvy, PF et al. (2010) Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67, 220229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, A (2018) Obesity management: what should we do if fat gain is necessary to maintain body homeostasis in a modern world? Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 9, 285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandner, MA, Patel, NP, Gehrman, PR et al. (2010) Problems associated with short sleep: bridging the gap between laboratory and epidemiological studies. Sleep Med Rev 14, 239247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronholm, E, Partonen, T, Harma, M et al. (2016) Prevalence of insomnia-related symptoms continues to increase in the Finnish working-age population. J Sleep Res 25, 454457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiker, NRW, Astrup, A, Hjorth, MF et al. (2018) Does stress influence sleep patterns, food intake, weight gain, abdominal obesity and weight loss interventions and vice versa? Obes Rev 19, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, CA & Alfano, CA (2017) Sleep and emotion regulation: an organizing, integrative review. Sleep Med Rev 31, 616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dashti, HS, Scheer, FA, Jacques, PF et al. (2015) Short sleep duration and dietary intake: epidemiologic evidence, mechanisms, and health implications. Adv Nutr 6, 648659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Strien, T & Koenders, PG (2014) Effects of emotional eating and short sleep duration on weight gain in female employees. J Occup Environ Med 56, 659666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaput, JP, Despres, JP, Bouchard, C et al. (2011) The association between short sleep duration and weight gain is dependent on disinhibited eating behavior in adults. Sleep 34, 12911297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konttinen, H, Kronholm, E, Partonen, T et al. (2014) Morningness-eveningness, depressive symptoms, and emotional eating: a population-based study. Chronobiol Int 31, 554563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silventoinen, K, Jelenkovic, A, Sund, R et al. (2017) Differences in genetic and environmental variation in adult BMI by sex, age, time period, and region: an individual-based pooled analysis of 40 twin cohorts. Am J Clin Nutr 106, 457466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tholin, S, Rasmussen, F, Tynelius, P et al. (2005) Genetic and environmental influences on eating behavior: the Swedish young male twins study. Am J Clin Nutr 81, 564569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keskitalo, K, Tuorila, H, Spector, TD et al. (2008) The three-factor eating questionnaire, body mass index, and responses to sweet and salty fatty foods: a twin study of genetic and environmental associations. Am J Clin Nutr 88, 263271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sung, J, Lee, K, Song, YM et al. (2010) Heritability of eating behavior assessed using the DEBQ (Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire) and weight-related traits: the healthy twin study. Obesity (Silver Spring) 18, 10001005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yengo, L, Sidorenko, J, Kemper, KE et al. (2018) Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for height and body mass index in approximately 700000 individuals of European ancestry. Hum Mol Genet 27, 36413649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locke, AE, Kahali, B, Berndt, SI et al. (2015) Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature 518, 197206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konttinen, H, Llewellyn, C, Wardle, J et al. (2015) Appetitive traits as behavioural pathways in genetic susceptibility to obesity: a population-based cross-sectional study. Sci Rep 5, 14726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lauzon-Guillain, B, Clifton, EA, Day, FR et al. (2017) Mediation and modification of genetic susceptibility to obesity by eating behaviors. Am J Clin Nutr 106, 9961004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, R, Drapeau, V, Tremblay, A et al. (2018) The role of eating behavior traits in mediating genetic susceptibility to obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 108, 445452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelis, MC, Rimm, EB, Curhan, GC et al. (2014) Obesity susceptibility loci and uncontrolled eating, emotional eating and cognitive restraint behaviors in men and women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, E135E141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llewellyn, CH (2018) Genetic susceptibility to the “obesogenic” environment: the role of eating behavior in obesity and an appetite for change. Am J Clin Nutr 108, 429430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kilpelainen, TO, Qi, L, Brage, S et al. (2011) Physical activity attenuates the influence of FTO variants on obesity risk: a meta-analysis of 218,166 adults and 19,268 children. PLoS Med 8, e1001116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahmad, S, Rukh, G, Varga, TV et al. (2013) Gene × physical activity interactions in obesity: combined analysis of 111,421 individuals of European ancestry. PLoS Genet 9, e1003607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomiyama, AJ (2014) Weight stigma is stressful. A review of evidence for the cyclic obesity/weight-based stigma model. Appetite 82, 815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Al Khatib, HK, Hall, WL, Creedon, A et al. (2018) Sleep extension is a feasible lifestyle intervention in free-living adults who are habitually short sleepers: a potential strategy for decreasing intake of free sugars? A randomized controlled pilot study. Am J Clin Nutr 107, 4353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooney, G, Dwan, K & Mead, G (2014) Exercise for depression. JAMA 311, 24322433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leow, S, Jackson, B, Alderson, JA et al. (2018) A role for exercise in attenuating unhealthy food consumption in response to stress. Nutrients 10, 176. [Epublication]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willett, W, Rockstrom, J, Loken, B et al. (2019) Food in the anthropocene: the EAT-lancet commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet 393, 447492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Overview on how the influence of negative emotions on eating depends on the characteristics of emotions, individuals and situations (e.g. available foods).