Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T07:06:04.722Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Weight change increases the odds of psychological distress in middle age: bidirectional analyses from the Whitehall II Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2018

Anika Knüppel*
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Martin J. Shipley
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Clare H. Llewellyn
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Eric J. Brunner
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Anika Knüppel, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Mood disorders and adiposity are major public health challenges. Few studies have investigated the bidirectional association of weight and waist circumference (WC) change with psychological distress in middle age, while taking into account the potential U-shape of the association. The aim of this study was to examine the bidirectional association between psychological distress and categorical change in objectively measured weight and WC.

Methods

We analysed repeated measures (up to 17 522 person-observations in adjusted analyses) of psychological distress, weight and WC from the Whitehall II cohort. Participants were recruited at age 35–55 and 67% male. Psychological distress was assessed using the General Health Questionnaire. We used random-effects regressions to model the association between weight and WC changes and psychological distress, with and without a 5-year lag period.

Results

Psychological distress was associated with weight and WC gain over the subsequent 5 years but not the second 5-year period. Weight gain and loss were associated with increased odds for incident psychological distress in models with and without time-lag [odds ratio (OR) for incident psychological distress after 5-year time-lag: loss 1.20, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.00–1.43; gain>5% 1.20, 95% CI 1.02–1.40]. WC changes were only associated with psychological distress in models without time-lag (OR for incident psychological distress: loss 1.29, 95% CI 1.02–1.64; gain>5% 1.33, 95% CI 1.11–1.58).

Conclusions

Weight gain and loss increase the odds for psychological distress compared with stable weight over subsequent 10 years. In contrast, the association between psychological distress and subsequent weight and WC changes was limited to the first 5 years of follow-up.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Introduction

Mental health and obesity are two major public health challenges in the European region and Western world (WHO, 2015, 2017). An estimated one in three Europeans suffer from symptoms of depression and anxiety at least once in their lifetime, and in the UK, prevalence peaks at middle age (Spiers et al., Reference Spiers, Bebbington, McManus, Brugha, Jenkins and Meltzer2011; Steel et al., Reference Steel, Marnane, Iranpour, Chey, Jackson, Patel and Silove2014). The prevalence of overweight and obesity also peaks in middle age, at which more than half of adults in developed countries are either overweight or obese (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Fleming, Robinson, Thomson, Graetz, Margono, Mullany, Biryukov, Abbafati, Abera, Abraham, Abu-Rmeileh, Achoki, AlBuhairan, Alemu, Alfonso, Ali, Ali, Guzman, Ammar, Anwari, Banerjee, Barquera, Basu, Bennett, Bhutta, Blore, Cabral, Nonato, Chang, Chowdhury, Courville, Criqui, Cundiff, Dabhadkar, Dandona, Davis, Dayama, Dharmaratne, Ding, Durrani, Esteghamati, Farzadfar, Fay, Feigin, Flaxman, Forouzanfar, Goto, Green, Gupta, Hafezi-Nejad, Hankey, Harewood, Havmoeller, Hay, Hernandez, Husseini, Idrisov, Ikeda, Islami, Jahangir, Jassal, Jee, Jeffreys, Jonas, Kabagambe, Khalifa, Kengne, Khader, Khang, Kim, Kimokoti, Kinge, Kokubo, Kosen, Kwan, Lai, Leinsalu, Li, Liang, Liu, Logroscino, Lotufo, Lu, Ma, Mainoo, Mensah, Merriman, Mokdad, Moschandreas, Naghavi, Naheed, Nand, Narayan, Nelson, Neuhouser, Nisar, Ohkubo, Oti, Pedroza, Prabhakaran, Roy, Sampson, Seo, Sepanlou, Shibuya, Shiri, Shiue, Singh, Singh, Skirbekk, Stapelberg, Sturua, Sykes, Tobias, Tran, Trasande, Toyoshima, van de Vijver, Vasankari, Veerman, Velasquez-Melendez, Vlassov, Vollset, Vos, Wang, Wang, Weiderpass, Werdecker, Wright, Yang, Yatsuya, Yoon, Yoon, Zhao, Zhou, Zhu, Lopez, Murray and Gakidou2014).

The nature of the association between obesity and psychological distress is unclear; it could be driven by adiposity increasing the risk of psychological distress, or by psychological distress increasing the risk of obesity. Both pathways may operate (Luppino et al., Reference Luppino, de Wit, Bouvy, Stijnen, Cuijpers, Penninx and Zitman2010). Studies using an instrumental-variable design have not been able to clarify the dominant direction of the association. As there are no known single-nucleotide polymorphisms for psychological distress, a study used maternal mental health as an instrumental variable for offspring adolescent depression. Findings suggest that adolescent depression was a causal predictor of adult obesity, but not the opposite (Hamer et al., Reference Hamer, Batty and Kivimaki2016). In contrast, two Mendelian randomization studies support adiposity as a cause of depression and psychological distress (Kivimaki et al., Reference Kivimaki, Jokela, Hamer, Geddes, Ebmeier, Kumari, Singh-Manoux, Hingorani and Batty2011; Jokela et al., Reference Jokela, Elovainio, Keltikangas-Järvinen, Batty, Hintsanen, Seppälä, Kähönen, Viikari, Raitakari, Lehtimäki and Kivimäki2012). Nevertheless, the nature of the obesity–psychological distress association could differ by age, and there could be common underlying factors that predispose individuals to both conditions (Luppino et al., Reference Luppino, de Wit, Bouvy, Stijnen, Cuijpers, Penninx and Zitman2010; Kivimaki et al., Reference Kivimaki, Jokela, Hamer, Geddes, Ebmeier, Kumari, Singh-Manoux, Hingorani and Batty2011; Jokela et al., Reference Jokela, Elovainio, Keltikangas-Järvinen, Batty, Hintsanen, Seppälä, Kähönen, Viikari, Raitakari, Lehtimäki and Kivimäki2012).

While many studies including middle-aged and older adults found that depression prospectively increases weight and waist circumference (WC) (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Sutin and Zonderman, Reference Sutin and Zonderman2012; Brumpton et al., Reference Brumpton, Langhammer, Romundstad, Chen and Mai2013; Lasserre et al., Reference Lasserre, Glaus, Vandeleur, Marques-Vidal, Vaucher, Bastardot, Waeber, Vollenweider and Preisig2014; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014; de Wit et al., Reference de Wit, van Straten, Lamers, Cuijpers and Penninx2015; Fezeu et al., Reference Fezeu, Batty, Gale, Kivimaki, Hercberg and Czernichow2015; Gibson-Smith et al., Reference Gibson-Smith, Bot, Milaneschi, Twisk, Visser, Brouwer and Penninx2016), studies looking at categorical change (e.g. a change in weight status) also found associations with weight loss, suggesting a U-shaped association (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; de Wit et al., Reference de Wit, van Straten, Lamers, Cuijpers and Penninx2015; Gibson-Smith et al., Reference Gibson-Smith, Bot, Milaneschi, Twisk, Visser, Brouwer and Penninx2016). Similarly, studies that have looked at the association in the direction from weight changes to depressive symptoms have found both positive and negative associations such that weight gain and weight loss are associated with increased depressive symptoms (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Steptoe, Beeken, Kivimaki and Wardle2014; Khalaila and Litwin, Reference Khalaila and Litwin2014; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014); although in some studies the associations depended on covariates or differed by sex (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Steptoe, Beeken, Kivimaki and Wardle2014; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014).

Based on the current evidence, we hypothesized that the prospective relationship between adiposity and psychological distress is both bidirectional and U-shaped. From a public health point of view, establishing the temporal sequence and nature of this relationship in middle age could inform policy makers about targets and timing of interventions and monitoring to reduce the risk for either weight change or mental health issues and thereby increase chances for successful ageing (Brunner et al., Reference Brunner, Shipley, Britton, Stansfeld, Heuschmann, Rudd, Wolfe, Singh-Manoux and Kivimaki2014; Singh-Manoux et al., Reference Singh-Manoux, Sabia, Bouillon, Brunner, Grodstein, Elbaz and Kivimaki2014).

Time-lagged analyses help to disentangle temporal sequences of events and shed light on the direction of cause and effect. To our knowledge, two studies in middle-aged adults have investigated the association in this way (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014). However, they were both based on self-reported weight, which could be particularly sensitive to misreporting as participants would require to have tracked their weight over time (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Gorber et al., Reference Gorber, Tremblay, Moher and Gorber2007; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014). In the light of inconclusive evidence from instrumental analyses and the methodological limitations in studies using time-lagged models, we firstly aim to investigate the bidirectional association of objectively measured weight and WC changes with psychological distress in a cohort of middle-aged British men and women. Secondly, we aim to establish the temporal sequence using time-lagged models in both directions.

Methods

Study population

We used data from the Whitehall II cohort study that started in 1985–88. With a response rate of 73%, the initial sample included 10 308 individuals between 35 and 55 years old. From 1985 to 2013, participants were followed up via questionnaire and visited a research clinic for screening (Marmot and Brunner, Reference Marmot and Brunner2005). The present study has been approved by Joint UCL/UCLH Committee on the Ethics of Human Research and participants have been asked to provide informed consent at every follow-up.

Measures

Relative changes in weight and WC

Weight was measured without shoes and clothing to the nearest 0.1 kg on Soehnle electronic scales every 5 years from 1985–88 to 2012–13. WC was measured by trained staff every 5 years from 1991–93 to 2012–13. Relative change per 5 years was calculated in log percentages [L% = 100 × loge(x t+1/x t), where x t and x t+1 are measurements at time t and t + 1] (Tornqvist et al., Reference Tornqvist, Vartia and Vartia1985) and transformed back to actual percentages for analysis and presentation. Weight/WC loss, moderate and heavy gain were defined as relative change of >−3, >3–5 and >5%, respectively (Stevens et al., Reference Stevens, Truesdale, McClain and Cai2006). Due to the small number of participants losing between −3% and −5% of initial weight, this and the more extreme loss group were combined.

Groups of changers were compared with the stable group, defined as those staying within 3% of their baseline weight or WC.

Psychological distress

Psychological distress was measured with the 30-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) and defined as scoring ⩾5, based on receiver operating characteristic analysis (Stansfeld and Marmot, Reference Stansfeld and Marmot1992; Head et al., Reference Head, Stansfeld, Ebmeier, Geddes, Allan, Lewis and Kivimäki2013). The GHQ is a screening questionnaire for non-psychotic psychological distress and elicits depressive and anxiety symptoms such as having lost sleep over worry, or feeling unhappy or depressed (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg1972; Stansfeld et al., Reference Stansfeld, Fuhrer, Shipley and Marmot2002). GHQ caseness has in some studies been described as a common mental disorder (Kivimäki et al., Reference Kivimäki, Shipley, Batty, Hamer, Akbaraly, Kumari, Jokela, Virtanen, Lowe, Ebmeier, Brunner and Singh-Manoux2014; Knüppel et al., Reference Knüppel, Shipley, Llewellyn and Brunner2017). The cut-off has been found to have good sensitivity (86.4%) and specificity (87.2%) to measure any mental disorder when compared with a clinical interview in a subsample of the study cohort (Head et al., Reference Head, Stansfeld, Ebmeier, Geddes, Allan, Lewis and Kivimäki2013). In sensitivity analyses, associations using higher cut-off scores of ⩾6, ⩾8, ⩾10 were also investigated.

Other variables of interest

Covariates were chosen based on literature review and restricted to variables available at data collection phases with psychological distress and weight data. The following covariates were assessed: sex, age, ethnicity (White, South Asian, Black), marital status, (married/cohabiting, single/divorced/widowed), last employment grade level within the civil service (high, intermediate, low), smoking (never, former, current), alcohol intake (none: ⩽1 unit/week, moderate, heavy: ⩾14 units/week), self-reported physical activity (vigorous, moderate and non/mild) (Kumari et al., Reference Kumari, Head and Marmot2004), sleep duration (five categories from ⩽5 to ⩾9 h/day), baseline body mass index class (BMI) (normal: <25 kg/m2, overweight, obese: ⩾30 kg/m2), weight (kg) or WC (cm), when modelling WC change, diabetes and cardiovascular disease (coronary heart disease and stroke, CVD) based on self-report, clinical examination, Hospital Episode Statistics data or general practitioners contact; information on cancer was based on cancer registration data (Marmot and Brunner, Reference Marmot and Brunner2005); other longstanding illnesses were self-reported and coded as yes, no or missing; menopausal status was assessed by a question on the age when menstrual bleeding stopped.

Intake of antidepressants, anxiety and antipsychotic medication was self-reported at all phases after phase 4 (1995–99), and from phase 1 (1985–88) to 4 (1995–99) medication intake or current treatment was assumed when doctor diagnosis of depression or anxiety was reported.

Statistical analysis

We used 5-year cycles of weight/WC and GHQ data across the 25 years of observation, reflecting the time interval between research clinic screenings (Knüppel et al., Reference Knüppel, Shipley, Llewellyn and Brunner2017). The observed associations were pooled across these four 5-year cycles (Figure S1). We modelled the association of psychological distress and subsequent relative weight/WC change with a mean number of cycles of 2.1 and 1.6 per individual participant, and relative weight/WC change and subsequent incident psychological distress using multinomial and binomial random-effects logistic regression with a mean number of cycles of 2.5 and 2.0, respectively. This allowed us to model simultaneously, for all cycles combined, the association between the exposure at baseline (t 0) with the outcome at end of the cycle (t 1, 5 years later; t 2, 10 years later), while accounting for within- and between-individual differences (Twisk, Reference Twisk2004). Table S1 depicts the included phases for analyses with and without a time-lag period. As WC was not measured at phase 1 (1985–88), only phases 3 (1991–94) to 11 (2012–13) could be included, resulting in three cycles per analysis direction (Figure S1).

In the first set of analyses, we modelled the association of psychological distress with a relative 5-year weight and WC change (Table 1). In a non-lag model, prevalent psychological distress at each baseline (t 0) was modelled to predict a 5-year change in weight/WC (t 1t 0), and in a lag model, the same psychological distress prevalence (t 0) was modelled to predict a 5-year change after a 5-year time-lag (t 2t 1).

Table 1. Mode of analyses by direction, exposure and outcome

Secondly, we modelled the association of a 5-year change in weight and WC and incident psychological distress (Table 1). Incident psychological distress was defined as those having psychological distress, among those who did not have psychological distress at baseline (t 0). In a non-lag model, 5-year change in weight/WC (t 1t 0) was used to predict incident psychological distress at the cycle when the change was recorded (t 1), and in a lag-model, incident psychological distress after 5 years (t 2) (Table 1, Table S1).

Participants with missing information on ethnicity or ethnicity other than White/South Asian/Black were excluded from analyses. To control for selection bias for the four sets of analysis, the analytical sample was restricted to participants who had three consecutive measures of GHQ and two consecutive measures of weight or WC change. For example, participants with data on weight change from phase 1 (1985–88) to 3 (1991–94) also had to have data on weight changes from phase 3 (1991–94) to 5 (1997–99) and information of psychological distress status at phases 1 (1985–88), 3 (1991–94) and 5 (1997–99). This was done to ensure that participants that contributed to a non-lag model also contributed to the lag model and vice versa, and to remove any effect of attrition between the two sets of analyses. Figure S2 shows how the included sample was reached; 18 442 person-observations had sufficient data to be included in analyses between psychological distress and weight change and 12 473 for analyses between psychological distress and WC change. Of these, 4162 and 2623 person-observations, respectively, had psychological distress at each baseline, resulting in 14 280 and 9850 person-observations to be included in the analysis of incident psychological distress. We presented all models with the sample size after exclusion of missing values in covariates (920 person-observations for associations between baseline psychological distress and weight change, 551 and WC change, and 701 for the association between weight change and incident psychological distress at follow-up and 416 between WC change and incident psychological distress at follow-up).

All models were conducted with Stata 14 using the command xtlogit and tested for interaction by sex, analysis cycle and age using likelihood ratio tests (StataCorp., 2015). The base regression models were adjusted for sex, age and ethnicity; the fully adjusted models additionally for baseline BMI class, weight/WC, socio-demographic factors, health behaviours, sleep duration and baseline health status (CVD, diabetes and cancer). To investigate the role of health status over the course of follow-up, health status at follow-up (t 1) was administered in time-lagged models. In sensitivity analyses, main analyses were repeated (a) excluding participants with unknown or intake of antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication at each baseline to account for confounding by treatment of mood disorders; and (b) additionally adjusted for longstanding illnesses in sensitivity analyses, to investigate whether other diseases could confound associations. A posteriori several sensitivity analyses were added: (c) using different cut-off points for GHQ caseness; (d) adjusting additionally for menopausal status and change in status; (e) an analysis for the association of 5-year change in weight and incident psychological distress which excluded participants who had lost weight and reported intentional weight loss based on the question ‘Are you on a slimming diet now?’ in a Food Frequency Questionnaire administered at phases 5 (1997–99), 7 (2002–04) and 9 (2007–09). Finally, participants who became GHQ cases 5 years later were compared on the basis whether they had lost or gained weight following the approach used by Gibson-Smith et al. (Gibson-Smith et al., Reference Gibson-Smith, Bot, Milaneschi, Twisk, Visser, Brouwer and Penninx2016).

Results

At phases 1 (1985–88), 3 (1991–94), 5 (1997–99), 7 (2002–04), 9 (2007–09) and 11 (2012–13), the prevalence proportion of psychological distress was 26.8, 21.6, 21.5, 19.9, 14.3 and 16.3% of the eligible sample.

Table 2 shows the comparison of psychological distress cases and non-cases at phase 3. Psychological distress prevalence was higher in younger participants, women, unmarried participants, those who smoked, were less physically active, slept for <7 h/day and those with obesity. Associations were similar at the following phases; non-white ethnicity, lower grade level and CVD were significantly associated with higher psychological distress prevalence at phases 5 (1997–99) and 7 (2002–04) (not depicted). From phase 3 (1991–94) to 5 (1997–99), 11.2% lost weight, 14.6% gained a moderate amount (3–5%) of weight and 37.1% gained a large amount (>5%) of weight; 6.1% decreased their WC, 14.2% increased their WC moderately (3–5%) and 53.7% heavily (>5%). At phase 3 (1991–94), psychological distress prevalence was higher in those who increased their weight or WC.

Table 2. Cross-sectional associations between psychological distress and covariates at phase 3 (1991–94)

Baseline psychological distress was associated with increased odds for high weight gain (>5%) compared with odds of keeping a stable weight (±3%) in minimally adjusted models and when additionally adjusted for baseline socio-demographic factors, health behaviours, baseline weight, BMI class and disease status (Table 3). Excluding participants with baseline intake of antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication strengthened the association between psychological distress and weight loss [odds ratio (OR) 0.87, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.77–0.98, p = 0.018, fully adjusted models], and there was no difference after additionally adjusted for any longstanding illnesses. When the analysis was based on a 5-year time-lag, baseline psychological distress was not associated with subsequent weight changes (Table 2).

Table 3. Short-term (0–5 years) and long-term (5–10 years) effect of prevalent psychological distress on subsequent weight change

Bold indicates p < 0.05.

a Odds ratios from base model adjusted for age, sex and ethnicity.

b Odds ratios from fully adjusted model: additionally adjusted for marital status, last grade level in civil service, smoking, alcohol intake, physical activity, BMI, weight, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, cancer at baseline.

c Weight change is lagged 5 years after psychological distress assessment (at 0 years).

d Odds ratios additionally adjusted for diabetes, cardiovascular disease, cancer at 5 years.

Compared with person-observations in the stable weight group, participants with gains above 5% of their baseline body weight from 0 to 5 years and losses above 3% had increased odds for incident psychological distress at 5 years and at 10 years (Table 4). This association was independent of baseline socio-demographic factors, health behaviours, weight, BMI class and disease. The associations of weight gain and loss with incident psychological distress at 10 years were marginally attenuated when adjusted for disease at 5 years (p = 0.054) and stayed statistically significant when additionally adjusted for longstanding illnesses at baseline and 5 years (p = 0.041). Excluding participants taking antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication at baseline marginally attenuated the association of high weight gain and incident psychological distress 5 years later in fully adjusted models (OR 1.15, 95% CI 0.99–1.33, p = 0.061) and the association of weight loss with subsequent incident psychological distress at 10 years (OR 1.19, 95% CI 0.99–1.42, p = 0.068, in fully adjusted models).

Table 4. Short-term (0–5 years) and long-term (5–10 years) effect of weight change on subsequent incident psychological distress

a The incident psychological distress is lagged 5 years after weight change (from 0 to 5 years).

b Odds ratios from base model adjusted for age, sex and ethnicity.

c Odds ratios from fully adjusted model: additionally adjusted for marital status, last grade level in civil service, smoking, alcohol intake, physical activity, BMI, weight, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, cancer at baseline.

d Odds ratios additionally adjusted for diabetes, cardiovascular disease, cancer at 5 years.

In sensitivity analyses using cut-off points of 6, 8 and 10 for psychological distress results were generally replicated showing similar ORs at different cut-offs. However, with smaller numbers of participants being classified as psychologically distressed, the association between weight change and psychological distress after a 5-year time lag did not reach statistical significance (see Table S4–S9 for associations with weight change).

Additional adjustment for menopausal status and change in status did not change the conclusions but associations between weight loss (OR 1.28, 95% CI 1.04–1.57) and gain (OR 1.29, 95% CI 1.06–1.59) and incident psychological distress after a lag period were slightly stronger.

Sensitivity analysis excluding participants who lost weight and reported to be on a weight loss diet in a subgroup with available data did not change the association with incident psychological distress (OR for intentional and unintentional weight loss compared with stable weight 1.34, 95% CI 1.02–1.76; OR for unintentional weight loss only 1.37, 95% CI 1.03–1.81).

Associations with WC change in non-lag models were similar to associations with weight change (see Tables S2 and S3). Baseline psychological distress was associated with increased odds for high WC gain (OR 1.22, 95% CI 1.09–1.36) but not after a 5-year time-lag (OR 1.04, 95% CI 0.93–1.16) compared with odds for keeping WC stable. WC loss was associated with increased odds for incident psychological distress at 5 years (OR 1.29, 95% CI 1.02–1.64), but no significant increase at 10 years (OR 1.08, 95% CI 0.83–1.39) and similarly WC gain was associated with an increased odds for incident psychological distress at 5 years (OR 1.33, 95% CI 1.11–1.58) but not at 10 years (OR 1.08, 95% CI 0.90–1.31). There was no association of WC loss with incident psychological distress when participants taking antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication at baseline were excluded (OR 1.26, 95% CI 0.99–1.60, p = 0.065, in models adjusted for age, sex and ethnicity). Additionally, adjusting for any longstanding illnesses did not change results.

There was no evidence for interaction by sex or cycle in any of the modes of analysis. There was an interaction with age (p = 0.028) for the association between psychological distress and weight loss, suggesting that baseline psychological distress was mainly associated with decreased odds for weight loss in the younger participants

Both weight loss and weight gain were shown to have an adverse effect on the long-term likelihood of new psychological distress. We compared participants who lost weight and became psychological distress cases 5 years later with those who gained weight and became psychological distress cases 5 years later (Table 5). People with new psychological distress at follow-up who lost weight on average lost 6.1% (s.d. 3.4) of their initial body weight and those who gained >5% weight gained an average of 9.4% (s.d. 5.0). Compared with weight gainers with psychological distress 5 years later, weight losers were older (p < 0.001), physically less active (p = 0.058), suffered from more diabetes and CVD at baseline and at the end of the weight change. Participants who lost weight were in higher BMI classes at baseline than those who gained weight. There was no difference in antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication intake or GHQ summary score, but some differences (p < 0.1) in answers to questions in the GHQ. Participants who gained weight were more likely to report having felt constantly under strain, participants who lost weight reported more often having not been able to enjoy your normal day-to-day activities, having been taking things hard and having felt that life is entirely hopeless.

Table 5. Comparison of person-observations of participants who lost >−3% weight and person-observations of participants who gained >5% weight and suffered from psychological distress psychological distress 5 years later (at 10 years)

a Reverse scored.

Discussion

This was the first study to investigate the bidirectional association of psychological distress and objectively measured weight and WC changes in middle-aged adults using time-lagged analyses. Our findings confirmed the hypothesis of bidirectional associations between psychological distress and relative change in weight and WC short term. The association between prevalent psychological distress and relative change in weight and WC showed a linear trend, while both loss and gain >5% of weight and WC increased the odds of incident psychological distress. When the first 5 years of follow-up were excluded from analysis, weight changes continued to predict incident psychological distress, while the effect of psychological distress on weight change was restricted to the first 5 years. The findings suggest an association in the direction of weight changes to psychological distress.

Effect of psychological distress on subsequent weight and WC changes

The association between psychological distress and increases in weight and WC in non-lagged models was consistent with previous literature (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Lasserre et al., Reference Lasserre, Glaus, Vandeleur, Marques-Vidal, Vaucher, Bastardot, Waeber, Vollenweider and Preisig2014; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014; Gibson-Smith et al., Reference Gibson-Smith, Bot, Milaneschi, Twisk, Visser, Brouwer and Penninx2016). We did not find a positive association between psychological distress and weight loss in the main analysis as observed by others (de Wit et al., Reference de Wit, van Straten, Lamers, Cuijpers and Penninx2015; Gibson-Smith et al., Reference Gibson-Smith, Bot, Milaneschi, Twisk, Visser, Brouwer and Penninx2016). In fact, when including an age interaction and excluding participants with antidepressant, antipsychotic or anxiety medication intake, which might have a pharmacological effect on adiposity, psychological distress was associated with reduced odds of weight loss compared with those not reporting distress, which was stronger at younger ages.

The null association between psychological distress and weight changes in time-lagged models contrasts results of previous studies using this method. Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007 and Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014 found prevalent depression to be associated with a 2-year weight loss and gain in women after a 2-year time-lag and a 3-year weight gain after a 3-year time-lag, respectively (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014). This discrepancy could be due to the longer period between weight measures and lag length in our study (5 years compared with 2 and 3 years). It is possible that mood disorders only have a short-term effect on weight changes. This association could be explained by dysregulations of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (Peckett et al., Reference Peckett, Wright and Riddell2011; Stetler and Miller, Reference Stetler and Miller2011) or adverse effects on health behaviours, such as physical activity (Azevedo Da Silva et al., Reference Azevedo Da Silva, Singh-Manoux, Brunner, Kaffashian, Shipley, Kivimäki and Nabi2012).

Effect of weight and WC changes on incident psychological distress

We found a robust association between weight and WC change (from 0 to 5 years) on odds of an incident psychological distress at 5 years. This finding was in line with previous results from prospective studies (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Steptoe, Beeken, Kivimaki and Wardle2014; Khalaila and Litwin, Reference Khalaila and Litwin2014; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014). However, some previous findings depended on adjustment for other factors, and in one study, the association with weight gain was restricted to men (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Khalaila and Litwin, Reference Khalaila and Litwin2014; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014).

Weight gain and weight loss (from 0 to 5 years) also increased the chances for mood disorders after a 5-year time-lag (incident psychological distress at 10 years) in models adjusted for covariates at baseline. Although our results differed from the fully adjusted time-lagged models reported by Singh et al. (Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014) and Forman-Hoffman et al. (Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007), they are in line with their unadjusted results (Forman-Hoffman et al., Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014). In contrast to our analyses, Singh et al. (Reference Singh, Jackson, Dobson and Mishra2014) and Forman-Hoffman et al. (Reference Forman-Hoffman, Yankey, Hillis, Wallace and Wolinsky2007) adjusted for covariates at baseline and time-varying factors in fully adjusted models. This could have led to overadjustment as time-varying factors may have collinear associations or act as mediators. In addition, the difference to our findings might be due to the use of self-reported weight. Under-reporting of weight could have resulted in misclassification of participants to the stable weight group, thereby underestimating the true association (Gorber et al., Reference Gorber, Tremblay, Moher and Gorber2007).

The association between weight gain and increased chance of incident mood disorder could be explained by biological and psychological mechanisms. Inflammatory markers have been shown to increase with weight gain and elevated marker levels have been found to be associated with increased risk for psychological distress (Fransson et al., Reference Fransson, Batty, Tabák, Brunner, Kumari, Shipley, Singh-Manoux and Kivimäki2010; Kivimäki et al., Reference Kivimäki, Shipley, Batty, Hamer, Akbaraly, Kumari, Jokela, Virtanen, Lowe, Ebmeier, Brunner and Singh-Manoux2014). Furthermore, weight gain may prompt experiences and perception of weight discrimination, which have been found to explain about 40% of the association of obesity and psychological well-being (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Beeken and Wardle2015).

We tested several potential alternative explanations for the association of weight loss and subsequent psychological distress. We additionally adjusted for health status at 5 years to investigate whether the association was driven by a change in health status over follow-up; while results lost statistical significance the estimates were only marginally attenuated. Our finding was in line with the findings by Jackson et al. (Reference Jackson, Steptoe, Beeken, Kivimaki and Wardle2014) who found that the association between weight loss and depressed mood could not be explained by changes in health and major life events (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Steptoe, Beeken, Kivimaki and Wardle2014). We excluded participants with intake of antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication and found small differences in associations. In sensitivity analyses, we tested whether the association was driven by unintentional weight loss. We could not show a difference in the association, suggesting that the association might be independent of weight loss intention. Additional adjustments for longstanding illnesses, menopausal status and status change also did not change the conclusions.

Results from weight loss trials show that lifestyle interventions can improve mood. But the null association between weight loss and depression symptoms within groups suggests that the effect is not fully due to weight loss itself (Fabricatore et al., Reference Fabricatore, Wadden, Higginbotham, Faulconbridge, Nguyen, Heymsfield and Faith2011). Outside of a controlled environment, the difficulties surrounding the attainment of weight loss could motivate unhealthy behaviours and negative thoughts (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Steptoe, Beeken, Kivimaki and Wardle2014). Compared with a general population, those who are successfully losing weight have been shown to have more depressive symptoms, concerns about health and body shape, engage in binge eating and unhealthy dieting practices (Feller et al., Reference Feller, Müller, Mayr, Engeli, Hilbert and de Zwaan2015). We compared those with incident psychological distress at 10 years that had gained weight to those who had lost weight to elucidate whether they had specific characteristics. Those who lost weight and got depressed were sicker, older and more overweight than those who gained weight. We found little difference in reported symptoms of psychological distress, although a few answers could point to that those who lost weight could have severe symptoms with a severe effect on daily life. Further research is warranted to elucidate the prospective effect of weight loss on mood outside of controlled trial settings.

Limitations

Limitations need to be considered in the interpretation of our results. Our study was based on a non-representative cohort, which might reduce generalizability of results. For example, the prevalence of obesity in this study (8.0%) was lower than reported in the Health Survey for England in 1993 (13.6%) (Zaninotto et al., Reference Zaninotto, Head, Stamatakis, Wardle and Mindell2009). Nevertheless, hazard ratios produced in the Whitehall II study have been found to be similar when investigating associations between common risk factors for CVD and CVD risk to those from representative cohorts even though disease incidence and prevalence of exposures differed (Batty et al., Reference Batty, Shipley, Tabák, Singh-Manoux, Brunner, Britton and Kivimäki2014). Representativeness is further affected by attrition with healthier participants being more likely to remain and be included in this research (Jokela et al., Reference Jokela, Singh-Manoux, Shipley, Ferrie, Gimeno, Akbaraly, Head, Elovainio, Marmot and Kivimäki2011).

Mental health was assessed using a population screening tool which may have led to some misclassification of participants. However, the GHQ at a cut-off of 5 was found to be a sensitive and specific measure for any mental disorder in this cohort (Head et al., Reference Head, Stansfeld, Ebmeier, Geddes, Allan, Lewis and Kivimäki2013). Further, sensitivity analyses using higher cut points produced similar associations. Psychological distress has been found to be recurrent in 36% of cases in Whitehall II; it cannot be ruled out that associations might differ by number of caseness episodes (Jokela et al., Reference Jokela, Singh-Manoux, Shipley, Ferrie, Gimeno, Akbaraly, Head, Elovainio, Marmot and Kivimäki2011). Moreover, the 5-year period between screening phases could not be investigated.

Despite the wide range of covariates included in the models, residual confounding could still be present. Hypothetically, there may be pleiotropic genetic predisposition for both psychological distress and higher or lower adiposity. Furthermore, use of antidepressants, antipsychotic or anxiety medication could not be directly identified in data collection up to phase 4 and was inferred from self-reported doctor diagnoses of depression and anxiety.

Finally, the relatively long period between screening phases did not fully allow for the role of short-term changes in weight, WC and psychological distress to be investigated.

The present study examined bidirectional associations between psychological distress and adiposity across 10 years. In mid-life, it appears there are long-term adverse effects of weight loss and substantive weight gain, over 5%, on psychological distress. Conversely, low mood was associated with weight and WC gain in the short term (0–5 years), but this effect was not evident in the longer term. Our findings suggest that monitoring of weight changes in both directions could help identify persons at risk of mood disturbances in middle age. Interventions encouraging the maintenance of weight could have a protective effect on mental health.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718003379

Author ORCIDs

Anika Knüppel, Clare H. Llewellyn 0000-0003-1049-4836, 0000-0002-0066-2827

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants in the Whitehall II Study, Whitehall II researchers and support staff who make the study possible. The UK Medical Research Council (MR/K013351/1; G0902037), British Heart Foundation (RG/13/2/30098), and the US National Institutes of Health (R01HL36310, R01AG013196) have supported collection of data in the Whitehall II Study. MJS is partly supported by the British Heart Foundation.

Financial support

This research is part of the Multi-country collaborative project on the role of Diet, Food-related behaviour, and Obesity in the prevention of Depression (MooDFOOD) and was supported by the Seventh Framework Programme of the European Commission (FP7-KKBE-2013-2-1-01).

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Azevedo Da Silva, M, Singh-Manoux, A, Brunner, EJ, Kaffashian, S, Shipley, MJ, Kivimäki, M and Nabi, H (2012) Bidirectional association between physical activity and symptoms of anxiety and depression: the Whitehall II study. European Journal of Epidemiology 27, 537546.Google Scholar
Batty, GD, Shipley, M, Tabák, A, Singh-Manoux, A, Brunner, E, Britton, A and Kivimäki, M (2014) Generalizability of occupational cohort study findings. Epidemiology 25, 932933.Google Scholar
Brumpton, B, Langhammer, A, Romundstad, P, Chen, Y and Mai, XM (2013) The associations of anxiety and depression symptoms with weight change and incident obesity: The HUNT Study. International Journal of Obesity 37, 12681274.Google Scholar
Brunner, EJ, Shipley, MJ, Britton, AR, Stansfeld, SA, Heuschmann, PU, Rudd, AG, Wolfe, CD, Singh-Manoux, A and Kivimaki, M (2014) Depressive disorder, coronary heart disease, and stroke: dose–response and reverse causation effects in the Whitehall II cohort study. European Journal of Preventive Cardiology 21, 340346.Google Scholar
de Wit, LM, van Straten, A, Lamers, F, Cuijpers, P and Penninx, BWJH (2015) Depressive and anxiety disorders: associated with losing or gaining weight over 2 years? Psychiatry Research 227, 230237.Google Scholar
Fabricatore, AN, Wadden, TA, Higginbotham, AJ, Faulconbridge, LF, Nguyen, AM, Heymsfield, SB and Faith, MS (2011) Intentional weight loss and changes in symptoms of depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Obesity 35, 13631376.Google Scholar
Feller, S, Müller, A, Mayr, A, Engeli, S, Hilbert, A and de Zwaan, M (2015) What distinguishes weight loss maintainers of the German Weight Control Registry from the general population? Obesity 23, 11121118.Google Scholar
Fezeu, LK, Batty, DG, Gale, CR, Kivimaki, M, Hercberg, S and Czernichow, S (2015) Is the relationship between common mental disorder and adiposity bidirectional? Prospective analyses of a UK general population-based study. Ed. MM Glymour. PLoS ONE 10, e0119970.Google Scholar
Forman-Hoffman, VL, Yankey, JW, Hillis, SL, Wallace, RB and Wolinsky, FD (2007) Weight and depressive symptoms in older adults: direction of influence? The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 62, S43S51.Google Scholar
Fransson, EI, Batty, GD, Tabák, AG, Brunner, EJ, Kumari, M, Shipley, MJ, Singh-Manoux, A and Kivimäki, M (2010) Association between change in body composition and change in inflammatory markers: an 11-year follow-up in the Whitehall II study. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 95, 53705374.Google Scholar
Gibson-Smith, D, Bot, M, Milaneschi, Y, Twisk, JW, Visser, M, Brouwer, IA and Penninx, BWJH (2016) Major depressive disorder, antidepressant use, and subsequent 2-year weight change patterns in the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 77, e144e151.Google Scholar
Goldberg, DP (1972) The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire. A Technique for the Identification and Assessment of Non-Psychotic Psychiatric Illness. Maudsley monographs. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gorber, SC, Tremblay, M, Moher, D and Gorber, B (2007) A comparison of direct vs. self-report measures for assessing height, weight and body mass index: a systematic review. Obesity Reviews 8, 307326.Google Scholar
Hamer, M, Batty, GD and Kivimaki, M (2016) Depressive symptoms and obesity: instrumental variable analysis using mother-offspring pairs in the 1970 British Cohort Study. International Journal of Obesity 40, 17891793.Google Scholar
Head, J, Stansfeld, SA, Ebmeier, KP, Geddes, JR, Allan, CL, Lewis, G and Kivimäki, M (2013) Use of self-administered instruments to assess psychiatric disorders in older people: validity of the General Health Questionnaire, the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale and the self-completion version of the revised Clinical Interview Schedule. Psychological Medicine 43, 26492656.Google Scholar
Jackson, SE, Steptoe, A, Beeken, RJ, Kivimaki, M and Wardle, J (2014) Psychological changes following weight loss in overweight and obese adults: a prospective cohort study. Ed. IH Franken. PLoS ONE 9, e104552.Google Scholar
Jackson, SE, Beeken, RJ and Wardle, J (2015) Obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and psychological well-being in older adults in England: obesity, discrimination, and well-being. Obesity 23, 11051111.Google Scholar
Jokela, M, Singh-Manoux, A, Shipley, MJ, Ferrie, JE, Gimeno, D, Akbaraly, TN, Head, J, Elovainio, M, Marmot, MG and Kivimäki, M (2011) Natural course of recurrent psychological distress in adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders 130, 454461.Google Scholar
Jokela, M, Elovainio, M, Keltikangas-Järvinen, L, Batty, GD, Hintsanen, M, Seppälä, I, Kähönen, M, Viikari, JS, Raitakari, OT, Lehtimäki, T and Kivimäki, M (2012) Body mass index and depressive symptoms: instrumental-variables regression with genetic risk score. Genes, Brain and Behavior 11, 942948.Google Scholar
Khalaila, R and Litwin, H (2014) Changes in health behaviors and their associations with depressive symptoms among Israelis aged 50+. Journal of Aging and Health 26, 401421.Google Scholar
Kivimaki, M, Jokela, M, Hamer, M, Geddes, J, Ebmeier, K, Kumari, M, Singh-Manoux, A, Hingorani, A and Batty, GD (2011) Examining overweight and obesity as risk factors for common mental disorders using fat mass and obesity-associated (FTO) genotype-instrumented analysis: the Whitehall II Study, 1985–2004. American Journal of Epidemiology 173, 421429.Google Scholar
Kivimäki, M, Shipley, MJ, Batty, GD, Hamer, M, Akbaraly, TN, Kumari, M, Jokela, M, Virtanen, M, Lowe, GD, Ebmeier, KP, Brunner, EJ and Singh-Manoux, A. (2014) Long-term inflammation increases risk of common mental disorder: a cohort study. Molecular Psychiatry 19, 149.Google Scholar
Knüppel, A, Shipley, MJ, Llewellyn, CH and Brunner, EJ (2017) Sugar intake from sweet food and beverages, common mental disorder and depression: prospective findings from the Whitehall II study. Scientific Reports 7, 6287.Google Scholar
Kumari, M, Head, J and Marmot, M (2004) Prospective study of social and other risk factors for incidence of type 2 diabetes in the Whitehall II study. Archives of Internal Medicine 164, 18731880.Google Scholar
Lasserre, AM, Glaus, J, Vandeleur, CL, Marques-Vidal, P, Vaucher, J, Bastardot, F, Waeber, G, Vollenweider, P and Preisig, M (2014) Depression with atypical features and increase in obesity, body mass index, waist circumference, and fat mass: a prospective, population-based study. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 880.Google Scholar
Luppino, FS, de Wit, LM, Bouvy, PF, Stijnen, T, Cuijpers, P, Penninx, BW and Zitman, FG (2010) Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 220229.Google Scholar
Marmot, M and Brunner, E (2005) Cohort profile: the Whitehall II study. International Journal of Epidemiology 34, 251256.Google Scholar
Ng, M, Fleming, T, Robinson, M, Thomson, B, Graetz, N, Margono, C, Mullany, EC, Biryukov, S, Abbafati, C, Abera, SF, Abraham, JP, Abu-Rmeileh, NME, Achoki, T, AlBuhairan, FS, Alemu, ZA, Alfonso, R, Ali, MK, Ali, R, Guzman, NA, Ammar, W, Anwari, P, Banerjee, A, Barquera, S, Basu, S, Bennett, DA, Bhutta, Z, Blore, J, Cabral, N, Nonato, IC, Chang, J-C, Chowdhury, R, Courville, KJ, Criqui, MH, Cundiff, DK, Dabhadkar, KC, Dandona, L, Davis, A, Dayama, A, Dharmaratne, SD, Ding, EL, Durrani, AM, Esteghamati, A, Farzadfar, F, Fay, DFJ, Feigin, VL, Flaxman, A, Forouzanfar, MH, Goto, A, Green, MA, Gupta, R, Hafezi-Nejad, N, Hankey, GJ, Harewood, HC, Havmoeller, R, Hay, S, Hernandez, L, Husseini, A, Idrisov, BT, Ikeda, N, Islami, F, Jahangir, E, Jassal, SK, Jee, SH, Jeffreys, M, Jonas, JB, Kabagambe, EK, Khalifa, SEAH, Kengne, AP, Khader, YS, Khang, Y-H, Kim, D, Kimokoti, RW, Kinge, JM, Kokubo, Y, Kosen, S, Kwan, G, Lai, T, Leinsalu, M, Li, Y, Liang, X, Liu, S, Logroscino, G, Lotufo, PA, Lu, Y, Ma, J, Mainoo, NK, Mensah, GA, Merriman, TR, Mokdad, AH, Moschandreas, J, Naghavi, M, Naheed, A, Nand, D, Narayan, KMV, Nelson, EL, Neuhouser, ML, Nisar, MI, Ohkubo, T Oti, SO, Pedroza, A, Prabhakaran, D, Roy, N, Sampson, U, Seo, H, Sepanlou, SG, Shibuya, K, Shiri, R, Shiue, I, Singh, GM, Singh, JA, Skirbekk, V, Stapelberg, NJ, Sturua, L, Sykes, BL, Tobias, M, Tran, BX, Trasande, L, Toyoshima, H, van de Vijver, S, Vasankari, TJ, Veerman, JL, Velasquez-Melendez, G, Vlassov, VV, Vollset, SE, Vos, T, Wang, C, Wang, X, Weiderpass, E, Werdecker, A, Wright, JL, Yang, YC, Yatsuya, H, Yoon, J, Yoon, SJ, Zhao, Y, Zhou, M, Zhu, S, Lopez, AD, Murray, CJ and Gakidou, E. (2014) Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. The Lancet 384, 766781.Google Scholar
Peckett, AJ, Wright, DC and Riddell, MC (2011) The effects of glucocorticoids on adipose tissue lipid metabolism. Metabolism 60, 15001510.Google Scholar
Singh, G, Jackson, CA, Dobson, A and Mishra, GD (2014) Bidirectional association between weight change and depression in mid-aged women: a population-based longitudinal study. International Journal of Obesity 38, 591596.Google Scholar
Singh-Manoux, A, Sabia, S, Bouillon, K, Brunner, EJ, Grodstein, F, Elbaz, A and Kivimaki, M (2014) Association of body mass index and waist circumference with successful aging: adiposity and successful aging. Obesity 22, 11721178.Google Scholar
Spiers, N, Bebbington, P, McManus, S, Brugha, TS, Jenkins, R and Meltzer, H (2011) Age and birth cohort differences in the prevalence of common mental disorder in England: National Psychiatric Morbidity Surveys 1993–2007. The British Journal of Psychiatry 198, 479484.Google Scholar
Stansfeld, SA and Marmot, MG (1992) Social class and minor psychiatric disorder in British Civil Servants: a validated screening survey using the General Health Questionnaire. Psychological Medicine 22, 739749.Google Scholar
Stansfeld, SA, Fuhrer, R, Shipley, MJ and Marmot, MG (2002) Psychological distress as a risk factor for coronary heart disease in the Whitehall II Study. International Journal of Epidemiology 31, 248255.Google Scholar
StataCorp (2015) Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.Google Scholar
Steel, Z, Marnane, C, Iranpour, C, Chey, T, Jackson, JW, Patel, V and Silove, D (2014) The global prevalence of common mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. International Journal of Epidemiology 43, 476493.Google Scholar
Stetler, C and Miller, GE (2011) Depression and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activation: a quantitative summary of four decades of research. Psychosomatic Medicine 73, 114126.Google Scholar
Stevens, J, Truesdale, KP, McClain, JE and Cai, J (2006) The definition of weight maintenance. International Journal of Obesity 30, 391399.Google Scholar
Sutin, AR and Zonderman, AB (2012) Depressive symptoms are associated with weight gain among women. Psychological Medicine 42, 23512360.Google Scholar
Tornqvist, L, Vartia, P and Vartia, YO (1985) How should relative changes be measured? The American Statistician 39, 4346.Google Scholar
Twisk, JW (2004) Longitudinal data analysis. A comparison between generalized estimating equations and random coefficient analysis. European Journal of Epidemiology 19, 769776.Google Scholar
WHO (2015) The European Mental Health Action Plan 2013–2020 – WHO. Available at http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0020/280604/WHO-Europe-Mental-Health-Acion-Plan-2013-2020.pdf (Accessed 28 September 2017).Google Scholar
WHO (2017) WHO/Europe approaches to obesity – WHO. Available at http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/noncommunicable-diseases/obesity/obesity. (Accessed 22 February 2017).Google Scholar
Zaninotto, P, Head, J, Stamatakis, E, Wardle, H and Mindell, J (2009) Trends in obesity among adults in England from 1993 to 2004 by age and social class and projections of prevalence to 2012. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 63, 140146.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Mode of analyses by direction, exposure and outcome

Figure 1

Table 2. Cross-sectional associations between psychological distress and covariates at phase 3 (1991–94)

Figure 2

Table 3. Short-term (0–5 years) and long-term (5–10 years) effect of prevalent psychological distress on subsequent weight change

Figure 3

Table 4. Short-term (0–5 years) and long-term (5–10 years) effect of weight change on subsequent incident psychological distress

Figure 4

Table 5. Comparison of person-observations of participants who lost >−3% weight and person-observations of participants who gained >5% weight and suffered from psychological distress psychological distress 5 years later (at 10 years)

Supplementary material: File

Knüppel et al. supplementary material

Figures S1-S2 and Tables S1-S9

Download Knüppel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 74.9 KB