Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:54:24.317Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Behavior Genetic Analysis of Trait Emotional Intelligence and Alexithymia: A Replication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2013

Holly M. Baughman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
Julie Aitken Schermer
Affiliation:
Management and Organizational Studies, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
Livia Veselka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
Juliette Harris
Affiliation:
Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King's College London, London, UK
Philip A. Vernon*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
*
address for correspondence: Philip A. Vernon, Department of Psychology, Social Science Centre, University of Western Ontario, Room 7418, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This replication study examines relations between alexithymia and trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) at the phenotypic, genetic, and environmental levels. A sample of 1,444 same-sex twin pairs (850 MZ pairs and 594 DZ pairs) completed the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. A subset of 494 same-sex twin pairs (287 MZ pairs and 207 DZ pairs) had earlier completed the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire. Individual differences in alexithymia were attributable to genetic, non-shared environmental, and shared environmental factors. All but one of the facets of alexithymia were negatively and significantly correlated with the factors of trait EI, and these phenotypic correlations were entirely attributable to correlated genetic and correlated non-shared environmental factors. These bivariate results provide a valuable replication of those of Baughman et al. (Twin Research and Human Genetics, Vol. 14, 2011, pp. 539–543), which was conducted with substantially smaller samples of twins.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013

Alexithymia is characterized by the inability to identify and express one's emotions, an incapacity to distinguish between one's emotions and bodily sensations, and a tendency to engage in externally oriented thinking (EOT) — a cognitive style that shows a preference for the external details of everyday life rather than for content related to a person's inner experience (Bagby et al., Reference Bagby, Parker and Taylor1994a). Alexithymia has been linked to various physical and mental health conditions, such as posttraumatic stress disorder, somatic illness, substance abuse, and cancer (Krystal et al., Reference Krystal, Giller and Ciccheti1986; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Bagby and Parker1997; Todarello et al., Reference Todarello, Pesa, Zaka, Martino and Lattanzio1989). Less research, however, has been conducted in the area of individual differences in alexithymia or regarding alexithymia's relations with other personality traits, such as emotional intelligence.

Emotional Intelligence

Emotional intelligence (EI), also referred to as interpersonal intelligence, concerns the ability to understand and manage oneself and others in social situations (Thorndike, Reference Thorndike1920). EI can be operationalized either as a trait or as an ability. Trait EI is concerned with individuals’ perceived capacity to understand, process, and use information about emotions in everyday life, and is therefore assessed by self-report measures. Ability EI is characterized by people's actual abilities and is measured by performance tasks (Petrides & Furnham, Reference Petrides and Furnham2001). However, researchers have encountered psychometric difficulties when measuring ability EI (Pérez et al., Reference Pérez, Petrides, Furnham, Schulze and Roberts2005), so the present study will focus on emotional intelligence as a personality trait rather than as an ability. Previous studies pertaining to trait EI have reported positive associations between the construct and numerous other variables, including life satisfaction, well-being, and social support (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Saklofske and Egan2004).

Trait Emotional Intelligence and Alexithymia

Given that EI and alexithymia are conceptually similar in their shared focus on emotion-related understanding (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Taylor and Bagby2001), the relation between them has been of some interest. Researchers have commonly assessed EI using the BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) and Schutte et al.'s (Reference Schutte, Malouff, Hall, Haggerty, Cooper, Golden and Domheim1998) self-report measure of EI, both of which define the trait as an ability, and therefore offer limited scope in its assessment (Petrides et al., Reference Petrides, Furnham and Frederickson2004). That being said, this line of research has consistently reported moderate-to-strong negative correlations between EI and alexithymia, as measured by the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (e.g., Austin et al., Reference Austin, Saklofske and Egan2004; Parker et al., Reference Parker, Taylor and Bagby2001; Saklofske et al., Reference Saklofske, Austin and Minski2003), suggesting an inverse relation between the variables of interest.

In order to study EI as a personality trait, the short form of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue-SF) was employed in the present study. Few studies have examined this measure of trait EI in relation to alexithymia. Pham et al. (Reference Pham, Ducro and Luminet2010) reported a strong negative correlation between the TAS total score and TEIQue in a forensic sample of psychopaths. At the subclinical level, Mikolajczak et al. (Reference Mikolajczak, Luminet, Leroy and Roy2007) reported significant negative correlations between all factors of alexithymia and trait EI. Similarly, Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011) found significant negative correlations between all factors of alexithymia and trait EI, with the exceptions of alexithymia's EOT factor and the trait EI dimensions of emotion regulation and stress management.

Behavior Genetic Studies of Alexithymia

Relatively little research has been conducted regarding the etiological underpinnings of alexithymia. Early researchers reported various risk factors for alexithymia, which were primarily environmental or familial in nature (Berenbaum, Reference Berenbaum1996; Fukunishi et al., Reference Fukunishi, Kawamura, Ishikawa, Ago, Sei, Morita and Rahe1997). More recent studies, however, have supported the role of genetic factors. To date, five behavior genetic (BG) studies of alexithymia have been conducted, with inconsistent findings.

A study by Heiberg and Heiberg (Reference Heiberg and Heiberg1977) suggested that alexithymia is influenced solely by genetic factors. Valera and Berenbaum (Reference Valera and Berenbaum2001) demonstrated that the EOT factor of alexithymia is influenced by genetic factors, but also reported that remaining dimensions — difficulty identifying feelings (DIF) and difficulty describing feelings (DDF) — were influenced by both genetic and non-shared environmental factors. These studies suffered a number of limitations, including small sample sizes and measurement problems.

A large-scale adult twin study of 8,785 twin pairs suggested that individual differences in all three factors of alexithymia are attributable to genetic and non-shared environmental factors (Jørgensen et al., Reference Jørgensen, Zachariae, Skytthe and Kyvik2007). Two recent BG studies reported similar results (Baughman et al., Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011; Picardi et al., Reference Picardi, Fagnani, Gigantesco, Toccaceli, Lega and Stazi2011). Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011) further reported that the phenotypic correlations that they observed between alexithymia and trait EI were attributable to correlated genetic and correlated non-shared environmental factors. The sample sizes in the study by Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011), however, were very small and the results of what they referred to as a pilot study need to be replicated with a larger sample.

The Present Study

The present study aimed to replicate the findings of Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011) using a larger sample of twins. Based on Baughman et al.'s results, it is predicted that individual differences in alexithymia will be attributable to genetic and non-shared environmental factors. It is also predicted that all facets of alexithymia will be negatively correlated with all factors of the TEIQue-SF, and that these correlations will themselves be due to correlated genetic and correlated non-shared environmental factors.

Method

Participants

Participants were 850 monozygotic (MZ) pairs (771 female and 79 male pairs) and 594 dizygotic (DZ) same-sex twin pairs (556 female and 38 male pairs) who completed the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20). A subsample of 287 MZ same-sex twin pairs (257 female and 30 male pairs) and 207 DZ (191 female and 16 male pairs) had previously completed the TEIQue-SF. The average age of the twins was 60.5 years (SD = 13.37), and they ranged in age from 18 to 89 years. Participants were recruited from the Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology at Kings College London in the United Kingdom. The twins’ zygosity was previously established either by genome scans (100% accurate), DNA tests (99.5% accurate), or via their responses to the ‘Peas in the Pod’ zygosity questionnaire (95% accurate).

Self-report questionnaires were sent to these adult twins, who are all volunteers from the Twins UK Adult Twin Registry (Spector & Williams, Reference Spector and Williams2006). All were ascertained from the general population and shown to be comparable to age-matched population singletons. These unselected MZ and DZ twins have been recruited since 1992 using twin registers and national media campaigns, and have been used in a wide variety of studies (www.twinsuk.ac.uk). For historical reasons, the cohort is predominantly female. The twins in the registry are not selected for any particular trait and they volunteer to take part in studies that cover a wide range of traits and common medical conditions (Andrew et al., Reference Andrew, Hart, Snieder, De Lange, Spector and MacGregor2001). The study was approved by the St Thomas Hospital research ethics committee, and all twins in the study provided their informed consent.

Measures

Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20)

The TAS-20 (Bagby et al., Reference Bagby, Parker and Taylor1994a) is a 20-item self-report questionnaire that requires participants to rate items on a five-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). The TAS-20 yields a global alexithymia score, as well as scores on three dimensions of alexithymia: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF, seven items), difficulty describing feelings (DDF, five items), and externally oriented thinking (EOT, eight items). Example items for each of the facets are as follows: ‘I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling’ (DIF), ‘I am able to describe my feelings easily’ (DDF- reverse coded), and ‘I prefer to analyze problems rather than just describe them’ (EOT). The TAS-20 has also been found to have excellent psychometric properties (Bagby et al., Reference Bagby, Parker and Taylor1994b; Parker et al., Reference Parker, Taylor and Bagby2003).

Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire–Short Form (TEIQue-SF)

The TEIQue-SF (Petrides, Reference Petrides2009) is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 30 items to which participants respond on a five-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree; 5 = completely agree). The measure yields a global trait EI score, as well as scores on four factors: well-being, self-control, emotionality, and sociability. Internal consistency reliability coefficients for the global and factor scores have been reported to range from .66 to .89 in the UK twin sample (Veselka et al., Reference Veselka, Petrides, Schermer, Cherkas, Spector and Vernon2010).

Procedure

In 2011, all volunteers in the Twins UK Adult Twin Registry were asked to complete a battery of self-report questionnaires. A total of 6,552 individuals agreed to participate and, of these, 1,444 adult same-sex twin pairs (850 MZ; 594 DZ) completed the TAS-20 and a number of other measures not pertinent to the present report. A subsample of 494 same-sex twin pairs (287 MZ; 206 DZ) had previously completed a battery of questionnaires in 2009, which included the TEIQue-SF. All participants provided their informed consent and were mailed a debriefing form upon completion of the study.

Analyses

We conducted univariate and bivariate behavioral genetic analyses using the Mx software package (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes1999) to determine the extent to which individual differences in alexithymia and the phenotypic correlations between alexithymia and trait EI can be accounted for by common genetic and/or common environmental factors. For the bivariate analyses, we used the method of Cholesky or triangular factor analysis (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992) to assess the cross-correlations within twin pairs (i.e., the correlation between one twin's score on one variable with their co-twin's score on another variable). We began by fitting a full ACE model to our data to investigate potential correlated genetic (A), common environmental (C), and/or unique environmental (E) effects. Subsequently, reduced AE and CE models were tested. The models with the lowest χ2 change value (relative to the full model) and the lowest AIC value were selected as the best-fitting models. For each of the correlations presented in Table 2, a reduced AE model was found to have the best fit. Consequently, genetic (rg) and non-shared environmental (re) correlations were estimated.

Due to the very large number of female twins in our samples, we also re-ran the univariate analyses separately among just females. We also tested for sex differences in alexithymia and trait EI and examined correlations between these variables and age.

Results

Shown in Table 1 are the MZ and DZ twin correlations and the results of univariate BG analyses on the TAS-20, which were carried out with the software package Mx (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes1999). These results are based on the full sample of 1,444 twin pairs. As can be seen, MZ correlations are larger than DZ correlations for all four dimensions of alexithymia, and, in each case, the best-fitting model showed that individual differences were attributable to additive genetic (a), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) factors. Heritabilities range between .25 and .32 and all parameter estimates are significant at the .05 level. All reduced models (i.e., AE, CE, and E only) yielded significantly poorer fits. When these analyses were re-run among just the female twins, the results were virtually identical. Moreover, although there were significant mean differences between males and females on several facets of alexithymia and trait EI (females scoring significantly higher than males on emotionality; males scoring significantly higher than females on self-control, sociability, DDF, EOT, and total TAS), the actual effect sizes for these differences were very small: the largest effect size (for emotionality) was .36. Finally, we did not correct the data for age because all correlations between facets of alexithymia and age were less than .10 (averaging just .05 in absolute value).

TABLE 1 Twin Correlations and Univariate Model-Fitting Results for the TAS-20

DIF = difficulty identifying feelings; DDF = difficulty describing feelings; EOT = externally oriented thinking; a 2 = additive genetic effects; e 2 = non-shared environmental effects.

Shown in Table 2 are the phenotypic correlations (rp) between the four alexithymia dimensions and the five factors of the TEIQue-SF. As expected, all correlations are negative, and all but one of these 20 correlations are significant at the .001 level. Specifically, the correlation between alexithymia's EOT scale and trait EI self-control did not reach significance. Also shown in Table 2 are the results of bivariate BG analyses, which were also run with Mx. These results reveal that the significant phenotypic correlations that we observed are entirely attributable to correlated genetic and correlated non-shared environmental factors. Genetic correlations range between −.06 and −.69 and all but two are significant at the .05 level. Non-significant genetic correlations were observed between the EOT subscale of the TAS-20 and trait EI emotionality and self-control. Non-shared environmental correlations range between −.02 and −.38. All but one of these correlations — the association between the self-control and EOT scales — are also significant at the .05 level.

TABLE 2 Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations Between the TAS-20 and the TEIQue-SF

rp = phenotypic correlation; rg = genetic correlation; rc = shared environmental correlation; re = non-shared environmental correlation; DIF = difficulty identifying feelings; DDF = difficulty describing feelings; EOT = externally oriented thinking. For all of the paired variables, the best-fitting model was an AE-only model.

*p < .001, two tailed.

Discussion

In this study, our goal was to replicate the findings of Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011). Specifically, we used a large twin sample to investigate the contributions of genetic and environmental factors to individual differences in alexithymia. We further assessed the phenotypic, genetic, and environmental correlations between alexithymia and trait EI. Univariate BG analyses of alexithymia showed that individual differences in the trait were influenced by genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors. These findings are in contrast to those of Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011), where only significant genetic and non-shared environmental factors were noted. The genetic component observed supports previous suggestions that biological underpinnings may contribute to the development of alexithymia (e.g., Ham et al., Reference Ham, Lee, Lee, Kim, Choi, Oh and Choi2005; Larsen et al., Reference Larsen, Brand, Bermond and Hijman2003). The contribution of shared environmental factors in the present study is an unusual finding, given that most BG studies of personality show no shared environmental effects (see Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Vernon, Feiler, Boyle, Matthews and Saklofske2008, for a review of over 50 years of research). In a study of MZ and DZ twins reared together and reared apart, however, Tellegen et al. (Reference Tellegen, Lykken, Bouchard, Wilcox, Segal and Rich1988) noted that there may be situations in which reared-together co-twins are themselves an important shared environmental source of within-pair similarity. This could only occur, however, among DZ twins and not among MZ twins, because only DZ twins have genetically determined within-pair differences in the first place. It is possible that such an effect is operating with respect to alexithymia, and was not noted by Baughman et al. (Reference Baughman, Schwartz, Schermer, Veselka, Petrides and Vernon2011), given the small sample size they used. Additional research, however, should be conducted to investigate whether this is a real effect or merely an anomaly.

Looking at relations between alexithymia and trait EI, with the exception of alexithymia's EOT factor and the self-control dimension of trait EI, all phenotypic correlations were statistically significant. The negative correlations we found between the two sets of variables are consistent with previous research (e.g., Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Luminet, Leroy and Roy2007; Pham et al., Reference Pham, Ducro and Luminet2010). At a conceptual level, such associations make sense, as high scores on trait EI reflect proficiency in understanding and communicating emotional information, whereas high scores on alexithymia imply a state of deficiency in processing or describing emotions. The non-significant correlation between EOT subscale and the self-control factor of the TEIQue-SF does imply a lack of association between a cognitive style that is more attuned to practical life details than to the inner experience and one's ability to self-regulate in an emotional context. That being said, the EOT factor did correlate significantly and moderately with the remaining trait EI factors, and therefore it does show the expected inverse relation with trait EI overall. It should be noted that participants in the present report completed the TEIQue and the TAS-20 approximately 2 years apart, so all of the correlations that we observed are likely lower-bound estimates of the true population correlations between these measures.

Our bivariate BG analyses revealed that the phenotypic correlations between trait EI and alexithymia are entirely attributable to genetic and non-shared environmental factors. These results provide a valuable replication of Baughman et al.'s (2011) small-sample pilot study, in which similar results were reported. The findings allow us to conclude that, although alexithymia and trait EI are distinct, they do also share common attributes. Furthermore, many of the genetic correlations we reported between the trait sets were large, suggesting that neurobiological studies of alexithymia and trait EI may identify underlying biological factors common to both.

The present study is, of course, not without its limitations. The manner by which responses were obtained was self-report in nature, which could have resulted in response biases despite participants being encouraged to answer all items honestly and that they were not aware of the traits being studied. This potential is especially possible given the socially undesirable nature of alexithymic traits, and the socially desirable nature of high trait EI. To check for these response effects, it is suggested that future studies explore alexithymia and trait EI using peer report or multitrait–multimethod formats. Additionally, the nature of the sample may have further impacted our results. Specifically, the over-representation of female participants, which is due to historical reasons, may have resulted in more extreme scores given that females tend to score higher on a number of trait EI factors (Petrides & Furnham, Reference Petrides and Furnham2000) and lower on alexithymia (Levant et al., Reference Levant, Hall, Williams and Hasan2009) in comparison to males. These scores could, in turn, have led to more pronounced negative correlations between the variables in our sample. That being said, the findings were largely in line with previous investigations of alexithymia and trait EI (e.g., Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Luminet, Leroy and Roy2007; Pham et al., Reference Pham, Ducro and Luminet2010) and therefore are likely to be accurate representations of the true nature of the associations.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express gratitude toward the twins who voluntarily participated in this research project. We are also grateful for the help and support from the staff of the Department of Twin Research (DTR). The DTR is largely supported by the Wellcome Trust. Furthermore, this research project received support from the Department of Health and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) comprehensive Biomedical Research Centre award to Guy's and St Thomas’ NHS Foundation Trust in partnership with King's College, London.

References

Andrew, T., Hart, D. J., Snieder, H., De Lange, M., Spector, T. D., & MacGregor, A. J. (2001). Are twins and singletons comparable? A study of disease-related and lifestyle characteristics in adult women. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 4, 464477.Google Scholar
Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., & Egan, V. (2004). Personality, well-being and health correlates of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 547558.Google Scholar
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994a). The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale-I. Item selection and cross validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994b). The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale-II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 3340.Google Scholar
Baughman, H. M., Schwartz, S., Schermer, J. A., Veselka, L., Petrides, K. V., & Vernon, P. A. (2011). A behavioural-genetic study of alexithymia and its relationships with trait emotional intelligence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14, 539543.Google Scholar
Berenbaum, H. (1996). Childhood abuse, alexithymia and personality disorder. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41, 585595.Google Scholar
Fukunishi, I., Kawamura, N., Ishikawa, T., Ago, Y., Sei, H., Morita, Y., & Rahe, R. H. (1997). Mothers’ low care in the development of alexithymia: A preliminary study in Japanese college students. Psychological Report, 80, 143146.Google Scholar
Ham, B. J., Lee, M. S., Lee, Y. M., Kim, M. K., Choi, M. J., Oh, K. S., . . . Choi, I. G. (2005). Association between the catechol O-methyltransferase Val 108/158Met polymorphism and alexithymia. Neuropsychobiology, 52, 151154.Google Scholar
Heiberg, A., & Heiberg, A. (1977). Alexithymia: An inherited trait? A study of twins. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 28, 221225.Google Scholar
Johnson, A. M., Vernon, P. A., & Feiler, A. R. (2008). Behavioral genetic studies of personality: An introduction and review of the results of 50+ years of research. In Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment: Vol. 1. Personality theories and models (pp. 145173). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, M., Zachariae, R., Skytthe, A., & Kyvik, K. (2007). Genetic and environmental factors in alexithymia: A population-based study of 8,785 Danish twin pairs. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 369375.Google Scholar
Krystal, J. H., Giller, E. L., & Ciccheti, D. V. (1986). Assessment of alexithymia on post-traumatic stress disorder and somatic illness: Introduction of a reliable measure. Psychosomatic Medicine, 48, 8494.Google Scholar
Larsen, J. K., Brand, N., Bermond, B., & Hijman, R. (2003). Cognitive and emotional characteristics of alexithymia: A review of neurobiological studies. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 533541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levant, R. F., Hall, R. J., Williams, C. M., & Hasan, N. T. (2009). Gender differences in alexithymia. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 10, 190203.Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., Leroy, C., & Roy, E. (2007). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue). Journal of Personality Assessment, 88, 338353.Google Scholar
Neale, M., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (1999). Mx: Statistical modelling (5th ed.). Richmond, VA: Richmond Medical College of Virginia.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C. & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families, Norwell, MA: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, G., & Bagby, R. M. (2001). The relationship between emotional intelligence and alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 107115.Google Scholar
Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, G., & Bagby, R. M. (2003). The 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale reliability and factorial validity in a community population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55, 269275.Google Scholar
Pérez, J. C., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2005). Measuring trait emotional intelligence. In Schulze, R. & Roberts, R. D. (Eds.), International handbook of emotional intelligence (pp. 181201). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V. (2009). Technical manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue). London: London Psychometric Laboratory.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2000). Gender differences in measured and self-estimated trait emotional intelligence. Sex Roles, 42, 449461.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigated with reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15, 425448.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V., Furnham, A., & Frederickson, N. (2004). Emotional intelligence. The Psychologist, 17, 574577.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V., Vernon, P. A., Schermer, J. A., Ligthart, L., Boomsma, D. I., & Veselka, L. (2010). Relationships between trait emotional intelligence and the Big Five in the Netherlands. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 906910.Google Scholar
Pham, T. H., Ducro, C., & Luminet, O. (2010). Psychopathy, alexithymia and emotional intelligence in a forensic hospital. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 9, 2432.Google Scholar
Picardi, A., Fagnani, C., Gigantesco, A., Toccaceli, V., Lega, I., & Stazi, M. A. (2011). Genetic influences of alexithymia and their relationship with depressive symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 71 (4), 256263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., & Minski, P. S. (2003). Factor structure and validity of a trait emotional intelligence measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 707721.Google Scholar
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., & Domheim, L. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spector, T. D., & Williams, F. M. (2006). The UK Adult Twin Registry (TwinsUK). Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 899906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, G. M., Bagby, M., & Parker, J. D. A. (1997). Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10311039.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). Intelligence and its use. Harper's Magazine, 140, 227235.Google Scholar
Todarello, O., La Pesa, M. W., Zaka, S., Martino, V., & Lattanzio, E. (1989). Alexithymia and breast cancer: Survey of 200 women undergoing mammography. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 51, 5155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valera, E. M. & Berenbaum, H. (2001). A twin study of alexithymia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70 (5), 239246.Google Scholar
Veselka, L., Petrides, K. V., Schermer, J. A., Cherkas, L. F., Spector, T. D., & Vernon, P. A. (2010). Phenotypic and genetic relations between the HEXACO dimensions and trait emotional intelligence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 13, 6671.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Twin Correlations and Univariate Model-Fitting Results for the TAS-20

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations Between the TAS-20 and the TEIQue-SF