Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T21:03:27.843Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive Skills and Academic Achievement in the Dystrophinopathies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2018

Robert J. Fee
Affiliation:
The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Queens College, Queens, New York Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Columbia University, New York, New York
Jacqueline Montes
Affiliation:
Departments of Neurology and Rehabilitation and Regenerative Medicine, Columbia University, New York, New York
Jennifer L. Stewart
Affiliation:
The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Queens College, Queens, New York
Veronica J. Hinton*
Affiliation:
The Graduate Center, City University of New York, Queens College, Queens, New York Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center and Department of Neurology, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Veronica J. Hinton, Sergievsky Center, Columbia University, 630 West 168th Street, P&S Box 16, New York , NY 10032. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives: To examine academic performance in dystrophinopathy as a function of dystrophin gene mutation position as well as intellectual function, executive skills, socioeconomic status (SES), behavior, and physical ability. Methods: In a cross-sectional study, boys with dystrophinopathy (ages 5–17; n=50) completed tests of academics (Woodcock-Johnson-III: spelling, reading, calculation and total scores), executive functioning (selective attention/inhibitory control, set shifting, working memory, and processing speed), single word comprehension and nonverbal reasoning. Motor skills were assessed and parents provided demographic information and child behavioral assessments. Dystrophin gene mutation positions were dichotomized into groups (upstream versus downstream of exon 43, location of isoforms previously linked to intellectual impairment). Genetic mutation groups were compared on measures of academic achievement, and multiple regression analyses examined unique and joint contributions of executive skills, intelligence quotient (IQ), SES, motor abilities, behavior, and mutation positions to academic outcomes. Results: Academic performance was slightly, yet significantly, lower than IQ and varied as a function of dystrophin gene position, wherein boys possessing the downstream mutation exhibited greater impairment than boys with the upstream mutation. Digit span forward (indexing verbal span), but no other measure of executive function, contributed significant variance to total academic achievement, spelling and calculation. Conclusions: Weak academic performance is associated with dystrophinopathy and is more common in downstream mutations. A specific deficit in verbal span may underlie inefficiencies observed in children with dystrophinopathy and may drive deficits impacting academic abilities. (JINS, 2018, 24, 928–938)

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2018 

INTRODUCTION

The dystrophinopathies are X linked muscle diseases primarily affecting males and range in severity due to the abnormal expression of the protein dystrophin. Various dystrophin isoforms are expressed in different cell types, including nerve and glial cells in the brain. In normal brains according to both autopsy and animal studies, dystrophin has been localized to areas of the cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum (Anderson, Head, Rae, & Morley, Reference Anderson, Head, Rae and Morley2002). The role of dystrophin in the central nervous system (CNS) is still unclear, but abnormal dystrophin likely influences brain circuitry affecting the development of brain structures (Gorecki, Lukasiuk, Szklarczyk, & Kaczmarek, Reference Gorecki, Lukasiuk, Szklarczyk and Kaczmarek1998; Kim, Wu, & Black, Reference Kim, Wu and Black1995; Lidov, Reference Lidov1996; Sogos, Curto, Reali, & Gremo, Reference Sogos, Curto, Reali and Gremo2002) and function (Anderson, Head, & Morley, Reference Anderson, Head and Morley2003, Reference Anderson, Head and Morley2004). Altered or absent dystrophin may cause a cascade of molecular abnormalities affecting cell stabilization, protection from damage, and regulation in signaling pathways (Allen, Whitehead, & Froehner, Reference Allen, Whitehead and Froehner2016).

Neurodevelopmental difficulties are common in the disorder. The absence of, or incomplete isoforms of, dystrophin have been hypothesized to impact brain functioning (Anderson, Head, & Morley, Reference Anderson, Head and Morley2012; Mehler, Reference Mehler2000). Neurocognitive evidence suggests areas of specific impairment rather than generalized deficits. Although there is an increased risk of overall intellectual impairment, most children with dystrophinopathy have average intellectual functioning (Cotton, Voudouris, & Greenwood, Reference Cotton, Voudouris and Greenwood2001).

There is evidence that cognitive deficits in dystrophinopathy are related to the molecular abnormalities in the disorder. A variety of mutations in the dystrophin gene alters expression of CNS dystrophin isoforms. Full-length dystrophin (Dp427) as well as other dystrophin isoforms including Dp71 and Dp140 are normally found in the brain and their absence is negatively associated with cognitive abilities (Bardoni et al., Reference Bardoni, Felisari, Sironi, Comi, Lai, Robotti and Bresolin2000; Daoud, Angeard, et al., Reference Daoud, Angeard, Demerre, Martie, Benyaou, Leturcq and Tuffery2009; Waite, Brown, & Blake, Reference Waite, Brown and Blake2012. Mutations affecting Dp71), typically distal of exon 63, and Dp140, localized to exon 44–45, are associated with a greater frequency of intellectual impairment (Daoud et al., Reference Daoud, Angeard, Demerre, Martie, Benyaou, Leturcq and Tuffery2009; Lenk, Hanke, Thiele, & Speer, Reference Daoud, Angeard, Demerre, Martie, Benyaou, Leturcq and Tuffery1993; Moizard et al., Reference Moizard, Toutain, Fournier, Berret, Raynaud, Billard and Moraine2000; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Betts, Maroulis, Gilissen, Pedersen, Mowat and Buckley2010; Tuffery et al., Reference Tuffery, Lenk, Roberts, Coubes, Demaille and Claustres1995).

Ricotti et al. (Reference Ricotti, Mandy, Scoto, Pane, Deconinck, Messina and Muntoni2016) found an increased prevalence of cognitive and behavioral deficits within individuals with specific downstream gene mutations (Ricotti et al., Reference Ricotti, Mandy, Scoto, Pane, Deconinck, Messina and Muntoni2016). Our research group discovered that downstream mutations are linked to lower intelligence quotients (IQ) as well as reduced digit forward span (Fee et al., 2017, in submission), a proposed measure of verbal span capacity (Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein, & Stern, Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004). The frequency and severity of cognitive dysfunction thus appears to be related to mutations affecting more distal areas of the gene, located downstream from exon 43.

Performance on academic measures in the dystrophinopathies is generally lower than expected, with most children falling below expected age and grade levels (Billard et al., Reference Billard, Gillet, Signoret, Uicaut, Bertrand, Fardeau and Santini1992; Hendriksen & Vles, Reference Hendriksen and Vles2006; Hinton, De Vivo, Nereo, Goldstein, & Stern, Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Nereo, Goldstein and Stern2001; Hinton, DeVivo, Fee, Goldstein, & Stern, Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004; Leibowitz & Dubowitz, Reference Leibowitz and Dubowitz1981; Worden & Vignos, Reference Worden and Vignos1962). Moreover, academic scores of children with dystrophinopathy are depressed relative to nonverbal IQ, and when compared to unaffected siblings across reading, spelling and math (Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004).

There is substantial evidence for an increased rate of reading disabilities and delays in developing reading skills and poor spelling abilities (Astrea et al., Reference Astrea, Pecini, Gasperini, Brisca, Scutifero, Bruno and Battini2015; Billard, Gillet, Barthez, Hommet, & Bertrand, Reference Billard, Gillet, Barthez, Hommet and Bertrand1998; Dorman, Hurley, & D’Avignon, Reference Dorman, Hurley and D’Avignon1988; Fabbro et al., Reference Fabbro, Marini, Felisari, Comi, D’Angelo, Turconi and Bresolin2007; Hendriksen & Vles, Reference Hendriksen and Vles2006; Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004). There is less research examining computational math skills, but our studies found performance to be weak when compared to nonverbal IQ and unaffected siblings (Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Nereo, Goldstein and Stern2001, Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004). Overall, across studies, individuals with dystrophinopathies have generalized weakness on tests of academics.

Executive functioning deficits may underlie impaired academic achievement evident in children with dystrophinopathies. There is a wide range of cognitive functions ascribed to executive tasks; the ability to focus and shift attention, organize and plan assignments, and problem solve are essential for attaining academic goals. Executive skills have been strongly associated with academic performance in healthy children (Best, Miller, & Naglieri, Reference Best, Miller and Naglieri2011), wherein specific executive abilities such as inhibition, shifting, and working memory have been linked to performance in specific academic subject areas (St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole, Reference St Clair-Thompson and Gathercole2006). Working memory and planning (Cutting, Materek, Cole, Levine, & Mahone, Reference Cutting, Materek, Cole, Levine and Mahone2009; Sesma, Mahone, Levine, Eason, & Cutting, Reference Sesma, Mahone, Levine, Eason and Cutting2009), shifting (van der Sluis, de Jong, & van der Leij, Reference van der Sluis, de Jong and van der Leij2007), and cognitive flexibility (Welsh, Nix, Blair, Bierman, & Nelson, Reference Welsh, Nix, Blair, Bierman and Nelson2010) have been shown to predict reading skills.

Executive skills are also associated with the development of math skills (Clark, Pritchard, & Woodward, Reference Clark, Pritchard and Woodward2010; Mazzocco & Kover, Reference Mazzocco and Kover2007), and working memory more specifically has been predictive of math achievement (DeStefano & LeFevre, Reference DeStefano and LeFevre2004; Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, Nugent, & Numtee, Reference Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, Nugent and Numtee2007; Swanson & Sachse-Lee, Reference Swanson and Sachse-Lee2001). Even over time, working memory capacity determined by performance on span tasks has been linked to both mathematics and science skill development (Gathercole, Pickering, Knight, & Stegmann, Reference Gathercole, Pickering, Knight and Stegmann2004).

With respect to children with dystrophinopathy, although our prior work has shown that IQ and digit span performance contributed to academic achievement (Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004), the role of other executive skills has not yet been thoroughly explored. In a large cohort of children with dystrophinopathy, contributions of both forward and backward digit span were shown to be associated with reading performance, with forward span having the greatest contribution (Leaffer, Fee, & Hinton, Reference Leaffer, Fee and Hinton2016).

Although we have argued that performance on digit span may reflect an underlying “core deficit” in children with dystrophinopathy (Cyrulnik et al., Reference Cyrulnik, Fee, Batchelder, Kiefel, Goldstein and Hinton2008; Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004), others have suggested more generalized executive skill deficits (Donders & Taneja, Reference Donders and Taneja2009; Mento, Tarantino, & Bisiacchi, Reference Mento, Tarantino and Bisiacchi2011; Wicksell, Kihlgren, Melin, & Eeg-Olofsson, Reference Wicksell, Kihlgren, Melin and Eeg-Olofsson2004). We recently documented generalized deficits across multiple areas of executive functioning in children with dystrophinopathy (Fee et al., 2017, in submission). Surprisingly, however, only performance on forward digit span, and no other measure of executive functioning, was associated with mutation position.

In addition to executive functioning abilities, several other factors may contribute to academic performance. First, IQ is strongly associated with academic achievement (Mayes, Calhoun, Bixler, & Zimmerman, Reference Mayes, Calhoun, Bixler and Zimmerman2009; McGrew, Flanagan, Keith, & Vanderwood, Reference McGrew, Flanagan, Keith and Vanderwood1997). Second, demographic variables such as socioeconomic status (SES) and parental education are associated with academic performance (Bradley & Corwyn, Reference Bradley and Corwyn2002; Duncan & Murnane, Reference Duncan and Murnane2011). Third, behavioral indicators (prosocial behavior, motivation, and the extent of individual academic engagement) (Komarraju & Nadler, Reference Komarraju and Nadler2013; Wentzel, Reference Wentzel1993), have been shown to be associated with both academic performance, and the classroom environment for learning (Wang & Eccles, Reference Wang and Eccles2013). Fourth, chronic illness is also associated with academic performance (Taras & Potts‐Datema, Reference Taras and Potts‐Datema2005). Fifth, physical limitations may limit school interactions as well as attendance, thus influencing individual academic functioning (Pinquart & Teubert, Reference Pinquart and Teubert2011). Among children with dystrophinopathies, any of these factors might play a significant role in academic achievement.

Most studies examining academics in the dystrophinopathies have used homogenous samples that lacked diversity, failing to consider the multiple factors that contribute to academic performance including cognitive ability, educational opportunity, SES, motor deficits, and the psychosocial stressors of having a physical disability. Additionally, the relationship between mutation position and academic skills has not been explored. To clarify specific predictors of individual performance, the contributions of generalized executive deficits as well as other individual factors including IQ, physical limitations, mutation position, behavior and SES were examined with respect to real world functioning, as measured by academic skill level in a diverse sample of 50 boys with dystrophinopathy.

Given prior evidence, we hypothesized that academic skills in the present sample would: (1) be lower than normative data from similar-aged children; (2) be more impaired in boys with downstream genetic mutations than those with upstream mutations; (3) share significant variance and overlap with executive deficits (selective attention/inhibitory control, set-shifting, working memory and processing speed) including digit span. Exploratory analyses examined whether illness severity, SES, IQ, behavioral variables, and demographic variables also impacted academic performance.

METHODS

Sample

Boys (n=50) were included given the nature of the disorder and the prevalence of dystrophinopathy among females is quite rare. Participants were enrolled from the Pediatric Neuromuscular Center and Muscular Dystrophy Association (MDA) clinic located at New York Presbyterian Hospital, the academic hospital associated with Columbia University. The clinic population came from the greater New York City (NYC) metropolitan area and a wide range of SES levels. Inclusion criteria included a genetically confirmed diagnosis of dystrophinopathy (both Duchenne muscular dystrophy [45 participants] and Becker muscular dystrophy [5 participants] were included) and between 5 and 17 years of age. Those potential participants not proficient in English were excluded from the study because of the use of measures normed on English speaking samples. All other participants who were capable of giving assent, spoke English as a primary language (not of parents), an interest in research participation, and in relatively good health with no comorbid medical diagnosis apart from the diagnosis of dystrophinopathy were included.

Sixty-four percent of the sample was receiving glucocorticoid therapy at time of participation for treatment, but no other reported medication. On a self-report parent measure, 14% (seven participants) were described as having attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, 4% (two participants) a Spectrum Disorder, and 32% (16 participants) were generally described as having developmental delay (including language). Thirty-six percent (18 participants) were bilingual, but considered English as their primary language.

Procedures

The study was approved by the institutional review board (IRB) at Columbia University Medical Center (IRB #AAAA5627) and The Graduate Center of City University of New York and was supported by a MDA grant. After an introduction by the treating physician, interested participants were described protocol details by the study coordinator. All parents or guardians gave informed consent and all participants gave assent before enrollment.

Evaluations were conducted in a quiet room free of potential distractions, and breaks were provided; assessment duration was approximately 2 hr. Neuropsychological measures were administered to all children in a standardized order, chosen to assess a broad range of intellectual function that emphasized attention/executive skills and minimized potential confounding effects of impaired motor abilities. Data were scored and converted to standardized scores using normative data. Additionally, participant medical records were reviewed for the results of genome sequencing and dystrophin gene mutation analysis, and mutation position was recorded for each participant. Data were coded without links to identifying information, and entered into a secure database; files were then stored in a locked file cabinet. Patient confidentiality was protected and ensured by the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act, Institutional Review Board regulations, and the study team.

Genetic Mutation Groups

Mutation positions were categorized into two groups: upstream of exon 43 and downstream of exon 43 (including Dp 140 and Dp 71).

Assessment Measures

Standardized neuropsychological measures with strong normative data were selected for the assessment. Measures were carefully chosen to minimize the amount of motor skills needed and most were able to be answered with a verbal response or by pressing a computer button.

IQ composite

Participants completed two measures that were used as proxies for general IQ: The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-IV (PPVT-IV) (Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn2007), a measure of single word comprehension, and the Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-II (CTONI-II), a nonverbal measure of analogical reasoning, categorical classification, and sequential reasoning. Both measures are highly correlated with the Full Scale IQ score of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children with ranges from .69 to .88 (Rossen, Shearer, Penfield, & Kranzler, Reference Rossen, Shearer, Penfield and Kranzler2005). Raw scores were age-standardized and converted to z scores. Scores on the PPVT-IV and CTONI-II were combined and the mean was used to create a composite IQ score for analysis.

Academic function

The Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, Reference Woodcock, McGrew and Mather2001) is a comprehensive and extensively used test battery for assessing academic achievement skills in individuals aged 2 to 90 years and normed on diverse communities in the U.S. population. The measure has strong reliability and validity, possessing correlations ranging from .60 to .70 with other academic and IQ measures. The subtests of Single Word Reading, Spelling and Calculation were calculated for each child and the Total Academic score is composed of the combined performance on the subtests. Age-standardized scores were used in analysis.

Executive skills

Selected subtests from The National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox were chosen given the assessment’s wide use among diverse populations and representative normative data for individuals between ages of 3 to 85 in the U.S. population. Subtests from the Cognitive Toolbox (Hodes, Insel, Landis, & NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research, Reference Hodes, Insel and Landis2013; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Bauer, Zelazo, Wallner-Allen, Dikmen, Heaton and Gershon2013) were administered, including attention (Flanker Inhibitory Control Test), set-shifting (Dimensional Change Card Sort Test), working memory (List Sorting Working Memory Test), and processing speed (Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test). These measures possessed short administration times and normative data matched the study sample.

First, the Flanker task required the participant to focus on a stimulus in the midst of congruent and incongruent stimuli, recording both reaction time (RT) and accuracy. Second, the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test measured set-shifting by assessing two dimensions, both the color and shape of an object; moreover, cognitive flexibility was evaluated by having the individual shift sets by dimensions, recording RT and accuracy. Third, the List Sorting Working Memory Test measured span and sequencing of visually and orally presented stimuli (animals and foods). The participant organized the items in size order from smallest to largest, first within a single dimension and then on two dimensions. The score was equal to the number of items recalled and sequenced correctly. Fourth, the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test measured processing speed. Participants had 85 s to rapidly judge as many item sets as possible (whether two pictures presented were the same or different).

Additionally, the Digit Span subtest from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (Wechsler, 2004), was administered as a measure of verbal span and working memory. Participants were asked to repeat numbers in the same order, or in the reverse order, of presentation. Total score reflects performance on the entire measure, and individual maximum span length of both forward and backward administration was also calculated.

Motor function

The Brooke and Vignos Scale was used to assess motor functioning (Brooke et al., Reference Brooke, Griggs, Mendell, Fenichel, Shumate and Pellegrino1981; Vignos, Spencer, & Archibald, Reference Vignos, Spencer and Archibald1963). The scales include measurements of upper and lower extremity functioning. Scores were categorized into minimal impairment (1–5); able to walk, climb stairs, and full use of arms, moderate (6–12); able to walk, but unable to climb stairs and not able to raise hands over head; and severe impairment (13–16); wheelchair bound, limited use of upper extremities. Fine motor abilities were also assessed using a finger tapping task to ensure participants were able to respond accurately using the computer keyboard for the NIH Toobox measures. Each motor measure was administered by, or supervised by, a licensed physical therapist in the MDA clinic. For regression analysis, upper and lower extremity motor scores were combined for analysis. Individuals with a gross motor score of 13–16 and finger tapping less than −1.5 standard deviation from the mean were categorized as severely impaired.

SES

Family income was reported by parents in a family history questionnaire by checking off a range of incomes provided for the household. Income was based on reported household income and number of individuals in the home and then compared to the NYC census report for annual median household income; income was categorized into low, middle, high-income levels. Income was recoded for analysis comparing low to middle/high income.

Behavior

Parental report behavior measures were examined for potential contributions to cognitive performance. The Behavior Assessment System for Children, second edition (BASC-2) (Reynolds & Kamphaus, Reference Reynolds and Kamphaus2004), is a multidimensional assessment that evaluates various aspects of behavior and personality from the perspective of the parent. The BASC-2 was used to determine overall behavioral issues both externalizing problems (attention difficulties, conduct problems, hyperactivity) and internalizing problems (anxiety, depression, and somatization) by using the Behavioral Symptoms Index (BSI) score. The BASC-2 yielded a T-score for the BSI score. Adverse outcome was coded as a score of T > 67 on the scale and dichotomized (elevated versus within normal limits).

Additionally, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) was used to assess executive function and self-regulation in everyday life and was examined as a potential marker for deficits in attention/executive skills. The scale is designed for children 5 to 18 years of age and has been well standardized. The measure provided information on different aspects of executive functioning through an overall Global Executive Composite score (GEC). T-Scores were generated for the composite scale and a T score >65 was categorized as elevated. The BRIEF GEC composite score was dichotomized into elevated or within normal limits and used for analysis.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive demographic analyses were based on standardized scores. Shapiro-Wilk analyses were run to ensure the data were normally distributed for parametric analyses and Levene’s test assessed for homogeneity of variances. For regression analysis, variables were entered into a correlation matrix to determine their association with each other. Multivariate outliers were investigated by examination of Mahalanobis Distance and predicted residual values. Multicollinearity was examined by inspecting the correlation coefficients and tolerance/VIF values. Independence of observations was determined by the Durbin-Watson statistic. Independence of errors from regression predictors was confirmed by histogram evaluation and P–P plot of standardized residuals.

Within-subject analysis

Paired t tests were conducted across participants to compare performance on each academic measure (reading, spelling, calculation, total) with individual IQ composite estimates. Alpha was set at .05.

Group analysis

Welch’s test (independent sample t test for unequal variances and sample sizes) was conducted between mutation groups (downstream of exon 43: n=32; upstream of exon 43; n=18) for the following dependent measures of academic performance: reading, spelling, calculation, and total. Alpha was set at .05.

Regression analysis

Four linear regressions were computed for the following outcome variables indexing academic performance: (1) reading, (2) spelling, (3) calculation, and (4) total. Frequencies and distributions of each variable were determined and each was entered into a correlation matrix to determine whether it was associated with the outcome variable of interest. Significant independent variables were entered as predictors in two steps. Executive function measures including digit span forward and backward were included in the first block of the regression model (model I). The motor, demographic, SES, IQ, mutation position, and behavior were then added to the overall regression model in step two (model II). Standardized beta values (β) were used to indicate the relative influence of each predictor. Alpha was set at .01 to reduce the probability of Type I error.

RESULTS

Table 1 illustrates demographic information for the sample, demonstrating that families who agreed to participate came from diverse backgrounds with a broad range of SES and education.

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics of the total sample

* p<.01.

Within-Subject Analysis

Across participants, reading, calculation, and total academic scores, although well within normal limits, were lower than expected when compared to IQ (Table 2).

Table 2 Neuropsychological mean performance standardized data

*p<.05.

PPVT-IV=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-IV; CTONI-II=Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-II.

Group Analysis

Demographic information between groups was not significantly different except for SES; more boys with downstream mutations came from low-income families (Table 1). As demonstrated in other studies, participants with mutations downstream of exon 43 had greater intellectual impairments (F(1,48)=7.05; p=.01). Boys with a downstream mutation also exhibited significantly poorer performance on reading, spelling, calculation, and total academic measures than boys with an upstream mutation (Figure 1).

Fig. 1 Between-mutation position group comparisons on academic performance. *= p <.05; effect sizes, partial n 2: reading =.19, spelling = .15, calculation =.11, total = .16.

Regression Analysis

Of note is that only 44 of the 50 participants originally enrolled had completed parent questionnaire measures. Of those, 35 completed them in English and 9 completed them in Spanish, based on the parent or guardian’s primary language. To ensure that those participants whose parents did not compete the questionnaires were comparable to those whose parents did complete the scales, multiple exploratory independent sample t tests were run. Group age, IQ, motor abilities, income, behavioral measures, mutation position, and performance on all cognitive measures did not differ between the six families whose scales were not completed and those with completed scales, suggesting the missing data were reflective of the overall sample (Table 3).

Table 3 Characteristics of the sample for regression analysis (n=44)

PPVT-IV=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-IV; CTONI-II=Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-2; WNL=Within Normal Limits; BASC=Behavior Assessment System for Children-2; BSI=Behavior Symptoms Index; BRIEF=Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning; GEC=Global Executive Composite

Regression assumptions were met for subsequent analyses, wherein no standardized residual outliers >2.5 standard deviations were present, there was no evidence of multicollinearity, errors were independent and normally distributed, and relationships between predictor and outcome variables were linear and homoscedastic.

The regression model for the cognitive executive measures (model I) was significant (F(6,38)=5.71; p=.001) with digit span forward as the only significant predictor of total academic achievement in the model (β=.35; t(44)=2.55; p=.009). For total academic achievement, the overall model (model II) was significant, F(12,32)=4.02; p=.003, with predictors accounting for 53% of the variance; however, digit span forward was again the only significant predictor in the model, with better performance predicting higher total scores (β=.45; t(44)=2.45; p=.001).

For individual academic areas, the overall models were also significant. Model I was significant for all subject areas, reading (F(6,38)=4.60; p=.001); R 2 Adjusted =.41), spelling (F(6,38)=3.12; p=.005); R 2 Adjusted =.41) and calculation (F(6,38)=6.13; p=.000); R 2 Adjusted =.54); greater digit span forward significantly predicted better spelling (β=.35; t(44)=2.48; p=.008) and better calculation (β=.38; t(44)=2.65; p=.009) performance. Both digit forward (β=.37; t(44)=3.00; p=.003 ) and digit backward (β=.35; t(44)=2.52; p=.009) significantly predicted reading. For model II, all predictors significantly predicted reading (F(12,32)=4.32; p=.001); R 2 Adjusted =.52), spelling (F(12,32)=4.06; p=.009); R 2 Adjusted =.45), and calculation (F(12,32)=4.47; p=.002); R 2 Adjusted =.57).

Higher digit span forward scores significantly predicted better spelling (β=.37; t(44)=2.55; p=.004) and calculation (β=.42; t(44)=3.04; p=.005) performance. Of note is that for reading, no individual variable contribution was significant, yet the contributions of both digit span forward and digit span backward approached significance (β=.35; t(44)=2.42; p=.02, and β=.34; t(44)=2.21; p=.03, respectively). Thus, overall, higher digit span forward predicted better academic performance even when considering the contribution of all executive and demographic variables (Figure 2)

Fig. 2 Partial regression plot for digit span forward. Higher digit span forward, better academic performance across subject areas: (A) total academic achievement, (B) reading, (C), spelling (D) calculation.

DISCUSSION

The goal of this study was to help clarify understanding about academic performance in children with dystrophinopathy. This was done by examining academic functioning and the association with dystrophin gene mutation position and by determining if known executive weaknesses (selective attention/inhibitory control, set-shifting, working memory, processing speed) contribute significantly to academic performance, when studied in combination with motor limitations, intellectual function, behavior, SES, and mutation position.

As expected, and consistent with our previous finding, academic skills are reduced in children with dystrophinopathy. Even though the mean difference from IQ across subject areas was less than a standard deviation and, therefore, not great enough to meet criteria for a specific learning disorder, the finding of reduced academic performance nonetheless may represent significant difficulties for these children in real world functioning. These findings extend and replicate our prior research (Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004) indicating that academics are overall weak across subject areas for children with dystrophinopathy.

Our findings also revealed, as predicted, that individuals with more downstream dystrophin gene mutations performed more poorly across academic measures. Overall mean performance across subject areas was significantly lower for those with mutations downstream of exon 43 as compared to average performance for those upstream. These results are consistent with those showing IQ is lower in individuals with mutations downstream of exon 43 (Daoud et al., Reference Daoud, Angeard, Demerre, Martie, Benyaou, Leturcq and Tuffery2009; Rasic et al., Reference Rasic, Vojinovic, Pesovic, Mijalkovic, Lukic, Mladenovic and Romac2014; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Betts, Maroulis, Gilissen, Pedersen, Mowat and Buckley2010; Wingeier et al., Reference Wingeier, Giger, Strozzi, Kreis, Joncourt, Conrad and Steinlin2011), as is digit span performance (Ricotti et al., Reference Ricotti, Mandy, Scoto, Pane, Deconinck, Messina and Muntoni2016; Fee et al., 2017, in submission). Thus, mutations that disrupt the dystrophin isoforms Dp140 and Dp 71 affect academic performance.

Our work examined multiple things that may contribute to real world academic function. Interestingly, our regression model revealed that among boys with dystrophinopathy, academic achievement was best predicted by performance on digits forward even when potential contributing factors including IQ, SES, illness severity, mutation position, and behavior were entered in the model. Digits forward was not only predicative of total academic achievement, but also predicted performance in spelling and calculation. Moreover, there was a trend toward digits forward also contributing to reading. Performance on other tests of executive functioning did not contribute significantly, contrary to our initial hypothesis.

Our current results replicate our previous studies that found that digit span contributed to academic performance in children with dystrophinopathy (Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Fee, Goldstein and Stern2004) and digit span (both forward and backward span) contributed to the variance in reading performance in a large cohort of children with dystrophinopathy (Leaffer et al., Reference Leaffer, Fee and Hinton2016). Unlike those studies, the current study extended the investigation by examining the contribution of a variety of executive skills, as measured by individual performance and parent ratings, on academic performance. Although children with dystrophinopathy have generalized weakness across executive functions (Fee et al., 2017, in submission), surprisingly, only digit span forward significantly predicted the variance observed on academic tests.

Performance on digits forward likely reflects individual capacity embedded in several more complex abilities including executive and academic tasks. The finding that forward digit span, rather than IQ or the other variables, accounted for the largest amount of statistical variance is valuable knowledge for real world functioning. In academics, research has shown that increased load imposes large demands on working memory and thus limited capacity in working memory results in poor academic performance (Gathercole et al., Reference Gathercole, Pickering, Knight and Stegmann2004). Working memory has been demonstrated to be a strong predictor of academic achievement (Alloway & Alloway, Reference Alloway and Alloway2010). True deficits in the dystrophinopathies are likely an interaction of weakness in multiple areas including specific aspects of executive functioning. Learning is incremental, and difficulty in the acquisition of basic concepts that depend on linguistic capacity may result in academic failure as material becomes more complex.

We have proposed in a series of publications that the core cognitive deficit in the dystrophinopathy group is a limited verbal span. This hypothesis is based on a repeated pattern of poor performance on digit span, weak story memory, and impaired sentence repetition found in our studies suggesting a reduced capacity in linguistic load (Hinton et al., Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Nereo, Goldstein and Stern2001; Hinton, Fee, Goldstein, & De Vivo, Reference Hinton, Fee, Goldstein and De Vivo2007). It is also based on the finding that even across intellectual level, performance on tasks of digit span and story memory remains selectively low for children with dystrophinopathy (Hinton, De Vivo, Nereo, Goldstein, & Stern, Reference Hinton, De Vivo, Nereo, Goldstein and Stern2000). We expanded our investigation of verbal span and found that both digits forward and backward was poor demonstrating that decreased span as well as working memory was deficient. The combined deficits in verbal span and executive control was found to contribute to poor reading performance (Leaffer et al., Reference Leaffer, Fee and Hinton2016).

More recently, we challenged our assumption of a core deficit in verbal span, and found that across executive skills, children with dystrophinopathy have a generalized weakness in both cognitive and behavioral executive functioning (Fee et al., 2017, in submission). Yet, despite finding generalized executive deficits, only limited forward span was associated with mutation position (Fee et al., 2017, in submission). The current study again challenged the verbal span hypothesis by predicting that generalized executive deficits would contribute significant variance to academic performance, yet the results found that only forward span predicted academic performance. Taken together, the combined findings of digit span forward being the only executive skill to be associated with mutation position and the only executive skill to contribute significantly to academic performance support the hypothesis of a distinct weakness in the working memory system of individuals with dystrophinopathy. Both executive deficits and limitations in linguistic load are likely part of that system. Together, they reflect an underlying core deficit in verbal span in this group.

The strengths of the current study lie in the design to examine academic outcomes that represent real world functioning. Academics not only measure knowledge based learning but also reflect individual potential for success in future goals. Individual abilities were examined across academic areas to better isolate the source of cognitive weakness. The current study examined a wide range of executive skills in the context of several individual variables including physical functioning and demographics to decipher underlying contribution to performance. We chose a variety of psychometrically strong measures controlling for several potential confounds to delineate potential deficits in specific aspects of functioning. The finding of one specific deficit, performance on digits forward, in the midst of so many potential contributing factors, including IQ, physical abilities, behavior, and demographic factors, predicting academic performance emphasizes the strength of the finding.

Additionally, this study is the first to examine molecular associations with real world academic performance. This not only aids in the understanding of the relationship between a disorder with a known molecular cause and selective cognitive impairments, but also a better understanding of the various contributions to academic achievement. Finally, the sample included individuals from diverse backgrounds, which may be more representative of the dystrophinopathy population than found in many other cognitive studies. The found correlation between frequency of subjects with a downstream mutation and family income in our sample may further support other data demonstrating the significant impact of SES on cognitive development (Bradley & Corwyn, Reference Bradley and Corwyn2002) and may be indicative of a gene–environment interaction that should be further explored within this population.

The study also has several limitations. The study used a relatively small sample by including only those with fully completed measures. Although the sample size was deemed adequate and produced valid findings, a larger sample for the regression model might provide more robust results and greater confidence in conclusions. Another potential weakness was the lack of a control group. The sample’s performance was compared to normative data; however, each participant was examined as his own control strengthening the internal validity of the study. Finally, our academics measures assessed basic skills of language and arithmetic; more complex academic measures would help to further describe the extent and specifics of academic impairment within the population. Future studies could expand on our results and examine other measures that examine these cognitive constructs further.

This study contributes to further understanding the role of disrupted dystrophin in the brain and its impact on functioning. In addition, the study provides information that could aid in improving real world functioning for children with dystrophinopathy. Weaknesses in working memory disrupts everyday activities, particularly in the academic environment that requires organization and goal-directed behavior. The identification of these specific areas of weakness provides targets for remediation that can help in the attainment of appropriate developmental gains promoting a better quality of life.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are very grateful to the families who took the time and effort to participate in our project. Special thanks to Sally Dunaway, PT, DPT, Ashwini K. Rao, OTR, EdD, Mercedes Vega Villar, Fiona McMahon, Ruta Patel, and Justine Payne for their contribution to the physical therapy assessments. This work was supported by a grant from the Muscular Dystrophy Association to V.J.H. Disclosures: Fee, R.J., Montes, J, Stewart, J.L., and Hinton, V.J. declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

REFERENCES

Allen, D.G., Whitehead, N.P., & Froehner, S.C. (2016). Absence of dystrophin disrupts skeletal muscle signaling: Roles of Ca2+, Reactive oxygen species, and nitric oxide in the development of muscular dystrophy. Physiological Reviews, 96(1), 253305.Google Scholar
Alloway, T.P., & Alloway, R.G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 2029.Google Scholar
Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., & Morley, J.W. (2003). Altered inhibitory input to Purkinje cells of dystrophin-deficient mice. Brain Research, 982(2), 280283.Google Scholar
Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., & Morley, J.W. (2004). Long-term depression is reduced in cerebellar Purkinje cells of dystrophin-deficient mdx mice. Brain Research, 1019(1-2), 289292.Google Scholar
Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., & Morley, J.W. (2012). Duchenne muscular dystrophy and brain function. In D.M. Hegde (Ed.), Muscular dystrophy. London: InTech.Google Scholar
Anderson, J.L., Head, S.I., Rae, C., & Morley, J.W. (2002). Brain function in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Brain, 125(Pt 1), 413.Google Scholar
Astrea, G., Pecini, C., Gasperini, F., Brisca, G., Scutifero, M., Bruno, C., & Battini, R. (2015). Reading impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A pilot study to investigate similarities and differences with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 45-46, 168177.Google Scholar
Bardoni, A., Felisari, G., Sironi, M., Comi, G., Lai, M., Robotti, M., & Bresolin, N. (2000). Loss of Dp140 regulatory sequences is associated with cognitive impairment in dystrophinopathies. Neuromuscular Disorders, 10(3), 194199.Google Scholar
Best, J.R., Miller, P.H., & Naglieri, J.A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327336.Google Scholar
Billard, C., Gillet, P., Barthez, M., Hommet, C., & Bertrand, P. (1998). Reading ability and processing in Duchenne muscular dystrophy and spinal muscular atrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40(1), 1220.Google Scholar
Billard, C., Gillet, P., Signoret, J.L., Uicaut, E., Bertrand, P., Fardeau, M., & Santini, J.J. (1992). Cognitive functions in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A reappraisal and comparison with spinal muscular atrophy. Neuromuscular Disorders, 2(5-6), 371378.Google Scholar
Bradley, R.H., & Corwyn, R.F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371399.Google Scholar
Brooke, M.H., Griggs, R.C., Mendell, J.R., Fenichel, G.M., Shumate, J.B., & Pellegrino, R.J. (1981). Clinical trial in Duchenne dystrophy. I. The design of the protocol. Muscle Nerve, 4(3), 186197.Google Scholar
Clark, C.A., Pritchard, V.E., & Woodward, L.J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176.Google Scholar
Cotton, S., Voudouris, N.J., & Greenwood, K.M. (2001). Intelligence and Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Full-scale, verbal, and performance intelligence quotients. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43(7), 497501.Google Scholar
Cutting, L.E., Materek, A., Cole, C.A., Levine, T.M., & Mahone, E.M. (2009). Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 3454.Google Scholar
Cyrulnik, S.E., Fee, R.J., Batchelder, A., Kiefel, J., Goldstein, E., & Hinton, V.J. (2008). Cognitive and adaptive deficits in young children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD). Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(5), 853861.Google Scholar
Daoud, F., Angeard, N., Demerre, B., Martie, I., Benyaou, R., Leturcq, F., & Tuffery, S. (2009). Analysis of Dp71 contribution in the severity of mental retardation through comparison of Duchenne and Becker patients differing by mutation consequences on Dp71 expression. Human Molecular Genetics, 18(20), 37793794.Google Scholar
DeStefano, D., & LeFevre, J.A. (2004). The role of working memory in mental arithmetic. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(3), 353386.Google Scholar
Donders, J., & Taneja, C. (2009). Neurobehavioral characteristics of children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 295304.Google Scholar
Dorman, C., Hurley, A.D., & D’Avignon, J. (1988). Language and learning disorders of older boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 30(3), 316327.Google Scholar
Duncan, G.J., & Murnane, R.J. (2011). Whither opportunity?: Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-IV) (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Fabbro, F., Marini, A., Felisari, G., Comi, G.P., D’Angelo, M.G., Turconi, A.C., & Bresolin, N. (2007). Language disturbances in a group of participants suffering from Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A pilot study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104(2), 663676.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 116.Google Scholar
Geary, D.C., Hoard, M.K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development, 78(4), 13431359.Google Scholar
Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Gorecki, D.C., Lukasiuk, K., Szklarczyk, A., & Kaczmarek, L. (1998). Kainate-evoked changes in dystrophin messenger RNA levels in the rat hippocampus. Neuroscience, 84(2), 467477.Google Scholar
Hendriksen, J.G., & Vles, J.S. (2006). Are males with Duchenne muscular dystrophy at risk for reading disabilities? Pediatric Neurology, 34(4), 296300.Google Scholar
Hinton, V.J., De Vivo, D.C., Fee, R.J., Goldstein, E., & Stern, Y. (2004). Investigation of poor academic achievement in children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19(3), 146154.Google Scholar
Hinton, V.J., De Vivo, D.C., Nereo, N.E., Goldstein, E., & Stern, Y. (2000). Poor verbal working memory across intellectual level in boys with Duchenne dystrophy. Neurology, 54(11), 21272132.Google Scholar
Hinton, V.J., De Vivo, D.C., Nereo, N.E., Goldstein, E., & Stern, Y. (2001). Selective deficits in verbal working memory associated with a known genetic etiology: The neuropsychological profile of duchenne muscular dystrophy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(1), 4554.Google Scholar
Hinton, V.J., Fee, R., Goldstein, E.M., & De Vivo, D.C. (2007). Verbal and memory skills in males with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(2), 123128.Google Scholar
Hodes, R.J., Insel, T.R., Landis, S.C. & NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research. (2013). The NIH toolbox: Setting a standard for biomedical research. Neurology, 80(11 Suppl 3), S1.Google Scholar
Kim, T.W., Wu, K., & Black, I.B. (1995). Deficiency of brain synaptic dystrophin in human Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Annals of Neurology, 38(3), 446449.Google Scholar
Komarraju, M., & Nadler, D. (2013). Self-efficacy and academic achievement: Why do implicit beliefs, goals, and effort regulation matter? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 6772.Google Scholar
Leaffer, E.B., Fee, R.J., & Hinton, V.J. (2016). Digit span performance in children with dystrophinopathy: A verbal span or working memory contribution? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(7), 777784.Google Scholar
Leibowitz, D., & Dubowitz, V. (1981). Intellect and behaviour in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 23(5), 577590.Google Scholar
Lenk, U., Hanke, R., Thiele, H., & Speer, A. (1993). Point mutations at the carboxy terminus of the human dystrophin gene: implications for an association with mental retardation in DMD patients. Human Molecular Genetics, 2(11), 18771881.Google Scholar
Lidov, H.G. (1996). Dystrophin in the nervous system. Brain Pathology, 6(1), 6377.Google Scholar
Mayes, S.D., Calhoun, S.L., Bixler, E.O., & Zimmerman, D.N. (2009). IQ and neuropsychological predictors of academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 238241.Google Scholar
Mazzocco, M.M., & Kover, S.T. (2007). A longitudinal assessment of executive function skills and their association with math performance. Child Neuropsychology, 13(1), 1845.Google Scholar
McGrew, K.S., Flanagan, D.P., Keith, T.Z., & Vanderwood, M. (1997). Beyond g: The impact of Gf-Gc specific cognitive abilities research on the future use and interpretation of intelligence tests in the schools. School Psychology Review, 26, 189210.Google Scholar
Mehler, M.F. (2000). Brain dystrophin, neurogenetics and mental retardation. Brain Research Reviews, 32(1), 277307.Google Scholar
Mento, G., Tarantino, V., & Bisiacchi, P.S. (2011). The neuropsychological profile of infantile Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(8), 13591377.Google Scholar
Moizard, M.P., Toutain, A., Fournier, D., Berret, F., Raynaud, M., Billard, C., & Moraine, C. (2000). Severe cognitive impairment in DMD: Obvious clinical indication for Dp71 isoform point mutation screening. European Journal of Human Genetics, 8(7), 552556.Google Scholar
Pinquart, M., & Teubert, D. (2011). Academic, physical, and social functioning of children and adolescents with chronic physical illness: A meta-analysis. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 37(4), 376389.Google Scholar
Rasic, M.V., Vojinovic, D., Pesovic, J., Mijalkovic, G., Lukic, V., Mladenovic, J., & Romac, S. (2014). Intellectual ability in the Duchenne muscular dystrophy and dystrophin gene mutation location. Balkan Journal of Medical Genetics, 17(2), 2535.Google Scholar
Reynolds, C.R., & Kamphaus, R.W. (2004). BASC-2: Behavior assessment system for children. Toronto: Pearson Canada Assessment.Google Scholar
Ricotti, V., Mandy, W.P., Scoto, M., Pane, M., Deconinck, N., Messina, S., & Muntoni, F. (2016). Neurodevelopmental, emotional, and behavioural problems in Duchenne muscular dystrophy in relation to underlying dystrophin gene mutations. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 58(1), 7784.Google Scholar
Rossen, E.A., Shearer, D.K., Penfield, R.D., & Kranzler, J.H. (2005). Validity of the comprehensive test of nonverbal intelligence (CTONI). Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23(2), 161172.Google Scholar
Sesma, H.W., Mahone, E.M., Levine, T., Eason, S.H., & Cutting, L.E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15(3), 232246.Google Scholar
Sogos, V., Curto, M., Reali, C., & Gremo, F. (2002). Developmentally regulated expression and localization of dystrophin and utrophin in the human fetal brain. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development, 123(5), 455462.Google Scholar
St Clair-Thompson, H.L., & Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745759.Google Scholar
Swanson, H.L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(3), 294321.Google Scholar
Taras, H., & Potts‐Datema, W. (2005). Chronic health conditions and student performance at school. Journal of School Health 75(7), 255266.Google Scholar
Taylor, P.J., Betts, G.A., Maroulis, S., Gilissen, C., Pedersen, R.L., Mowat, D.R., & Buckley, M.F. (2010). Dystrophin gene mutation location and the risk of cognitive impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Public Library of Science One, 5(1), e8803.Google Scholar
Tuffery, S., Lenk, U., Roberts, R.G., Coubes, C., Demaille, J., & Claustres, M. (1995). Protein truncation test: Analysis of two novel point mutations at the carboxy terminus of the human dystrophin gene associated with mental retardation. Human Mutation, 6(2), 126135.Google Scholar
van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P.F., & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35(5), 427449.Google Scholar
Vignos, P.J., Spencer, G.E., & Archibald, K.C. (1963). Management of progressive muscular dystrophy of childhood. Journal of the American Medical Association, 184(2), 8996.Google Scholar
Waite, A., Brown, S.C., & Blake, D.J. (2012). The dystrophin–glycoprotein complex in brain development and disease. Trends in Neurosciences, 35(8), 487496.Google Scholar
Wang, M.-T., & Eccles, J.S. (2013). School context, achievement motivation, and academic engagement: A longitudinal study of school engagement using a multidimensional perspective. Learning and Instruction, 28, 1223.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV(WISC-IV) Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weintraub, S., Bauer, P.J., Zelazo, P.D., Wallner-Allen, K., Dikmen, S.S., Heaton, R.K., & Gershon, R.C. (2013). NIH toolbox cognition battery: Introduction and pediatric data. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 115.Google Scholar
Welsh, J.A., Nix, R.L., Blair, C., Bierman, K.L., & Nelson, K.E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43.Google Scholar
Wentzel, K.R. (1993). Does being good make the grade? Social behavior and academic competence in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 357.Google Scholar
Wicksell, R.K., Kihlgren, M., Melin, L., & Eeg-Olofsson, O. (2004). Specific cognitive deficits are common in children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46(3), 154159.Google Scholar
Wingeier, K., Giger, E., Strozzi, S., Kreis, R., Joncourt, F., Conrad, B., & Steinlin, M. (2011). Neuropsychological impairments and the impact of dystrophin mutations on general cognitive functioning of patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 18(1), 9095.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III: Tests of Academic and Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Worden, D.K., & Vignos, P.J. Jr., (1962). Intellectual function in childhood progressive muscular dystrophy. Pediatrics, 29, 968977.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics of the total sample

Figure 1

Table 2 Neuropsychological mean performance standardized data

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Between-mutation position group comparisons on academic performance. *=p<.05; effect sizes, partial n2: reading =.19, spelling = .15, calculation =.11, total = .16.

Figure 3

Table 3 Characteristics of the sample for regression analysis (n=44)

Figure 4

Fig. 2 Partial regression plot for digit span forward. Higher digit span forward, better academic performance across subject areas: (A) total academic achievement, (B) reading, (C), spelling (D) calculation.