Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:28:38.751Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relations among Socioeconomic Status, Perceived Stress, and the Home Language Environment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2023

Emma R. HART
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University, USA
Sonya V. TROLLER-RENFREE
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University, USA
Jessica F. SPERBER
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University, USA
Kimberly G. NOBLE*
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Kimberly G. Noble, 525 W 120th St. Box 54N New York, NY, 10027. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While socioeconomic disparities in the home language environment have been well established, the mechanisms explaining these disparities are poorly understood. One plausible mechanism is heightened stress. The current study investigated whether maternal perceived stress was 1) associated with measures of the home language environment, and 2) mediated the relation between socioeconomic disparities and the home language environment. Data from three independent studies were analyzed, which together comprised 322 mother-child dyads. Two studies included mothers and their six- to twelve-month-old infants (N = 227). The third included mothers and their five- to nine-year-old children (N = 95). Mothers reported their educational attainment, income, and stress. Language Environment Analysis (LENA) measured the home language environment. As has been previously reported, socioeconomic disparities were observed in adult words and conversational turns. Stress did not mediate these associations, nor was it associated with adult words or conversational turns. Alternate mechanisms for future exploration are discussed.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Socioeconomic disparities in language development have been long documented: children from disadvantaged backgrounds tend to score lower on traditional measures of English language skill than do their more advantaged peers (Justice, Jiang, Purtell, Schmeer, Boone, Bates & Salsberry, Reference Justice, Jiang, Purtell, Schmeer, Boone, Bates and Salsberry2019; Noble, McCandliss & Farah, Reference Noble, McCandliss and Farah2007; Pace, Luo, Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, Reference Pace, Luo, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2017). The quantity and quality of language that children are exposed to plays an important role in explaining these differences (Brito, Reference Brito2017; Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean & Huston, Reference Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean and Huston2009; Pace et al., Reference Pace, Luo, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2017; Perkins, Finegood & Swain, Reference Perkins, Finegood and Swain2013). Indeed, exposure to more words and engagement in reciprocal parent-child conversational interactions are considered key processes for scaffolding language development both in early and middle childhood (Ford, Elmquist, Merbler, Kriese, Will & McConnell, Reference Ford, Elmquist, Merbler, Kriese, Will and McConnell2020; Merz, Maskus, Melvin, He & Noble, Reference Merz, Maskus, Melvin, He and Noble2020; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013).

In a widely cited study, Hart and Risley (Reference Hart and Risley1995) estimated that children raised in disadvantaged socioeconomic circumstances heard up to 30 million fewer words by their third birthday than children growing up in more privileged circumstances. While the validity, magnitude, and interpretation of these findings has been disputed (Dailey & Bergelson, Reference Dailey and Bergelson2022; Golinkoff, Hoff, Rowe, Tamis-LeMonda & Hirsh-Pasek, Reference Golinkoff, Hoff, Rowe, Tamis-LeMonda and Hirsh-Pasek2019; Kuchirko, Reference Kuchirko2019; Sperry, Sperry & Miller, Reference Sperry, Sperry and Miller2019), the essence of this often-cited work has been replicated with added nuance: children from socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds do generally tend to hear fewer words at home, but with notable heterogeneity across families (Gilkerson, Richards, Warren, Montgomery, Greenwood, Kimbrough Oller, Hansen & Paul, Reference Gilkerson, Richards, Warren, Montgomery, Greenwood, Kimbrough Oller, Hansen and Paul2017; Rowe, Pan & Ayoub, Reference Rowe, Pan and Ayoub2005; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013). Differences in the home language environment have been associated with differences in brain development and, ultimately, language skills (Pierce, Reilly & Nelson, Reference Pierce, Reilly and Nelson2020; Romeo, Leonard, Robinson, West, Mackey, Rowe & Gabrieli, Reference Romeo, Leonard, Robinson, West, Mackey, Rowe and Gabrieli2018; Romeo et al., Reference Romeo, Leonard, Robinson, West, Mackey, Rowe and Gabrieli2018). Associations between the home environment and language development have been observed as early as in the first two years of life (Halle, Forry, Hair, Perper, Wandner, Wessel & Vick, Reference Halle, Forry, Hair, Perper, Wandner, Wessel and Vick2009; Melvin, Brito, Mack, Engelhardt, Fifer, Elliott & Noble, Reference Melvin, Brito, Mack, Engelhardt, Fifer, Elliott and Noble2017; Noble, Houston, Brito, Bartsch, Kan, Kuperman, Akshoomoff, Amaral, Bloss, Libiger, Schork, Murray, Casey, Chang, Ernst, Frazier, Gruen, Kennedy, Van Zijl, Mostofsky, Kaufmann, Kenet, Anders, Jernigan & Sowell, Reference Noble, Houston, Brito, Bartsch, Kan, Kuperman, Akshoomoff, Amaral, Bloss, Libiger, Schork, Murray, Casey, Chang, Ernst, Frazier, Gruen, Kennedy, Van Zijl, Mostofsky, Kaufmann, Kenet, Anders, Jernigan and Sowell2015; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013).

Despite much research on the associations among socioeconomic factors, the home language environment, and child language development, there has been relatively little investigation of the mechanisms that link distal socioeconomic factors, such as parents’ educational attainment or family income, to the verbal interactions they have with and around their children. Some research has suggested that caregiver knowledge is at play (e.g., Rowe, Reference Rowe2008), and indeed, several interventions have focused on improving language-development-related knowledge and skills among caregivers experiencing socioeconomic disadvantage (see Leung, Hernandez & Suskind, Reference Leung, Hernandez and Suskind2020; Mendelsohn, Huberman, Berkule, Brockmeyer, Morrow & Dreyer, Reference Mendelsohn, Huberman, Berkule, Brockmeyer, Morrow and Dreyer2011).

Notably, some scholars have critiqued the assumption that socioeconomic differences in parent-child verbal interactions are primarily driven by deficits in caregiver knowledge and skill (Adair, Colegrove & McManus, Reference Adair, Colegrove and McManus2017; Dudley-Marling & Lucas, Reference Dudley-Marling and Lucas2009; Ellwood-Lowe, Foushee & Srinivasan, Reference Ellwood-Lowe, Foushee and Srinivasan2021; Kuchirko, Reference Kuchirko2019), and have argued, instead, that the experience of socioeconomic disadvantage may exert structural pressures on caregivers that influence the home language environment in ways unrelated to caregivers’ knowledge or skills (Adair et al., Reference Adair, Colegrove and McManus2017; Dudley-Marling & Lucas, Reference Dudley-Marling and Lucas2009; Ellwood-Lowe et al., Reference Ellwood-Lowe, Foushee and Srinivasan2021; Kuchirko, Reference Kuchirko2019). Some have also argued that the field should embrace new conceptualizations of optimal language development and home language environments, as traditional assessments of both constructs do not incorporate certain skills (e.g., oral narrative skills) and experiences that are common among children from historically marginalized communities (see Dudley-Marling & Lucas, Reference Dudley-Marling and Lucas2009; Gardner-Neblett, Pungello & Iruka, Reference Gardner-Neblett, Pungello and Iruka2012; Kuchirko, Reference Kuchirko2019; also see Noble et al., Reference Noble, Hart and Sperber2021).

It is likely that programs directed towards improving caregiver skills alone may have limited effectiveness if they do not target the underlying structural forces that continuously exert pressures on families. Such structural pressures may lead to increased stress (Algren, Ekholm, Nielsen, Ersbøll, Bak & Andersen, Reference Algren, Ekholm, Nielsen, Ersbøll, Bak and Andersen2018; Glasscock, Andersen, Labriola, Rasmussen & Hansen, Reference Glasscock, Andersen, Labriola, Rasmussen and Hansen2013; Hackman, Farah & Meaney, Reference Hackman, Farah and Meaney2010; McLoyd, Reference McLoyd1998; Senn et al., Reference Senn, Walsh and Carey2014), which in turn may impact caregivers’ language use with their children (Perkins et al., Reference Perkins, Finegood and Swain2013). Indeed, maternal stress has often been theorized as a critical mechanism driving socioeconomic disparities in child and family outcomes, including caregiver behaviors (see Conger, Wallace, Sun, Simons, McLoyd & Brody, Reference Conger, Wallace, Sun, Simons, McLoyd and Brody2002; Evans, Boxhill & Pinkava, Reference Evans, Boxhill and Pinkava2008; Farah, Reference Farah2017; Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017; McLoyd, Reference McLoyd1998). For example, Family Stress Theory has purported that economic hardship can impose stress and strain on caregivers, affecting how they interact with their children.

Empirical investigation of this theory has suggested that caregivers experiencing greater stress tend to engage in fewer developmentally supportive caregiving behaviors (see Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017 for a review). Less explored, however, is the extent to which stress influences the frequency with which caregivers speak in the home and engage in conversational turns with their children. Some studies have found that higher caregiver stress is associated with lower performance on traditional measures of child language development (D’Souza, Crawford, Buckley, Underwood, Peterson, Bird, Morton & Waldie, Reference D’Souza, Crawford, Buckley, Underwood, Peterson, Bird, Morton and Waldie2019; Troller-Renfree, Hart, Sperber, Fox & Noble, Reference Troller-Renfree, Hart, Sperber, Fox and Noble2022), though evidence is mixed (Lehr, Wecksell, Nahum, Neuhaus, Teel, Linares & Diaz, Reference Lehr, Wecksell, Nahum, Neuhaus, Teel, Linares and Diaz2016; Lin, Xu, Huang, Jia, Zhang, Yan & Zhang, Reference Lin, Xu, Huang, Jia, Zhang, Yan and Zhang2017). It is thus plausible that caregiver stress might influence the home language environment in ways that are important for child language development.

To our knowledge, just one recent study of a small low- to middle-income sample has investigated this, reporting that higher maternal perceived stress and a greater number of stressful life events were associated with fewer words spoken in the home, and fewer parent-child conversational turns (Pierce et al., Reference Pierce, Reilly and Nelson2020). The current study aimed to replicate and extend those findings in a larger, socioeconomically diverse group of mothers and their infants and children. This study made use of data from three independent studies, two composed of mothers and their six- to twelve-month-old infants, and the third composed of mothers and their five- to nine-year-old children. Building from previous reports that family socioeconomic status (SES) was associated with measures of the home language environment in two of the samples used in the present study (Brito, Troller-Renfree, Leon-Santos, Isler, Fifer & Noble, Reference Brito, Troller-Renfree, Leon-Santos, Isler, Fifer and Noble2020; Merz et al., Reference Merz, Maskus, Melvin, He and Noble2020), we hypothesized that higher maternal perceived stress would also be associated with the quality of the home language environment. Specifically, we hypothesized that greater maternal perceived stress would be related to fewer adult words, as well as fewer parent-child conversational turns. Further, we hypothesized that maternal perceived stress would significantly mediate the relation between socioeconomic factors and these measures of the home language environment.

Methods

Participants

Data from three separate studies were analyzed, which altogether sampled 322 mother-child dyads. Studies 1 and 2 included mothers and their six- to twelve-month old infants (combined N = 227 dyads, together termed the Combined Infant Sample). Study 3 included mothers and their five-to-nine-year-old children (N = 95 dyads, termed the Child Sample). All three studies recruited mothers from the New York City metropolitan area to study the associations among early experience and child development. Participants were recruited to be intentionally socioeconomically diverse based on maternal educational attainment. Detailed participant demographics for all three samples are presented in Table 1; all three studies included racially and ethnically diverse samples (see Table 1 for details).

Table 1. Descriptives for Studies 1, 2, and 3

* p < 0.05

** p < .01

*** p < .001

+ p < .10

Note. Study 1 included 94 participants. Study 2 included 133 participants. Study 3 included 95 participants. Maternal age is reported in years. Child age is reported in months. Education reflects average years of parental educational attainment. “A.I.” indicates “American Indian,” “N.A.” indicates “Native Alaskan,” “N.H.” indicates “Native Hawaiian,” “P.I.” indicates “Pacific Islander.” LENA recording duration is reported in hours. Adult word, conversational turn, and infant vocalization counts reflect average hourly counts. Percentages for racial group are rounded. Significance stars reported in the “Study 1 vs. Study 2” column reflect statistically significant differences in the characteristics of Study 1 and Study 2 based on Welch’s t-tests. Significance stars reported in the “Child vs. Combined Infant” column reflect statistically significant differences between characteristics of the Combined Infant Sample and Child Sample based on Welch’s t-tests.

Participants from Study 1 included 94 mother-infant dyads recruited from 2016 to 2018. To be included, infants had to be between 5 and 13 months of age, born at or after 36 weeks of gestation, and have no known neurological or developmental complications. All data were collected concurrently, at a single study visit. Participants included 32 six-month-olds, 31 nine-month-olds, and 31 twelve-month-olds (M = 9.44 months, SD = 2.65). 35% of the children were female.

Participants from Study 2 included 133 mothers recruited from 2019 to 2022 in their third trimester of pregnancy to participate in a longitudinal study of child development from birth to 3 years of age. To be included, pregnant mothers had to be 18 years of age or older, carrying a singleton, at least 35 weeks pregnant, and speak English or Spanish. In addition, the fetus had to be free of known neurological or developmental concerns. The current analyses made use of socioeconomic data collected at the prenatal visit, and perceived stress and home language environment data collected at the 6-month visit (M = 7.61 months, SD = 1.32). 56% of the children were female.

Participants from Study 3 included 95 mother-child dyads recruited from 2013 to 2018. To be included, children had to be from households that spoke primarily English, born from a singleton pregnancy, born at full-term, and not have any medical or psychiatric problems. Data analyzed in the present paper were collected concurrently at one study visit. Child participants were 5 to 9 years of age (M = 7.11 years, SD = 1.25). 61% of the children were female.

Procedure

All protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Teachers College and, for Study 3, Columbia University Medical Center. The data reported here from Studies 1 and 3 were collected from mothers and their children at a single laboratory visit. During this visit, mothers completed surveys to capture information on SES, demographics and perceived stress. They were also invited to take a Language Environment Analysis (LENA) device home, and were provided materials to mail back their LENA device after completion of the recording.

In Study 2, pregnant mothers either visited the research laboratory or research assistants visited their homes during their third trimester, at which time socioeconomic and other demographic information was collected. Mothers and infants subsequently visited the laboratory together when the infant was 6 months of age. During this 6-month visit, mothers were invited to complete a survey on perceived stress, and to take LENA devices home to record the home language environment. Mothers were provided materials to mail their LENA device back to the laboratory after completion of the home recording.

Measures

Socioeconomic Status

Parental education

Average parental education was calculated by averaging the number of years of education attained by the mother and the second parent. In cases where the reporting mother was the sole parental caregiver, only maternal educational attainment was used.

Income-to-Needs

Income-to-needs (ITN) ratios were calculated by dividing participants’ total household income by the poverty threshold for the respective family size for the year of data collection. As expected, ITN values were positively skewed and subsequently log-transformed for use in all analyses. Some participants reported that their income was zero dollars. To enable log transformation, one dollar was added to all income values prior to calculating ITN.

Perceived Stress

Perceived maternal stress was assessed using the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10; Cohen & Williamson, Reference Cohen, Williamson, Spacapan and Oskamp1988). The 10-item scale assesses the degree to which the respondent has perceived situations as stressful within the last month. Participants responded to each item using a 5-point Likert scale (0 = never, 4 = very often). Four items were reverse coded before summing across the items. Higher scores indicated greater perceived stress. Mothers needed to complete at least eight of the ten items for their score to be considered valid. The items showed acceptable reliability in the Combined Infant Sample (α = 0.86) and Child Sample (α = 0.81).

Home Language Environment

Language Environment Analysis (LENA) was used to measure the home language environment. The LENA system (LENA Research Foundation, Boulder, CO) is an automated vocalization analysis device that can audio-record the child’s language environment for up to 16 hours. Participants were provided with specially designed child T-shirts to hold the LENA digital language processor (DLP) throughout the recording duration. Strong reliability and validity of the LENA speech identification algorithms has been reported, with over 75% accuracy for both adult and child speech (Gilkerson et al., Reference Gilkerson, Richards, Warren, Montgomery, Greenwood, Kimbrough Oller, Hansen and Paul2017). LENA has also been validated for Spanish-speakers (Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013).

Mothers were provided LENA materials during their visit and instructed to have their child wear the DLP for a full day in the near future when typical caregivers would be present. Once the DLP was returned, the recording was uploaded to a computer and analyzed by the LENA software. The software produces three primary counts: adult words (number of words spoken near the child), conversational turns (number of reciprocal vocalizations by an adult and the target child within 5 seconds), and child vocalizations (defined as a speech segment of any length surrounded by 300 milliseconds or more of non-speech or silence). Adult word counts and adult-child conversational turns were used in the current study.

Additional processing was executed to remove silent periods of 10 minutes or more. This was done to account for the fact that daytime sleep patterns change with developmental age, which could skew the proportion of speech per recording duration. By removing periods of silence of greater than 10 minutes, we ensure that LENA analyses are limited to wakeful time. This process was completed using custom python scripts (see github.com/trollerrenfr/LENA_Scripts). Specifically, segments were removed from the data if there were two consecutive five-minute periods (10 minutes total) in which: 1) there was noise or silence for more than 180 seconds, 2) there were no adult words detected, 3) there were no conversational turns detected, and 4) there were fewer than 10 child vocalizations. Following the removal of data segments that met these criteria, LENA counts were then divided by the duration of LENA recording, to create average hourly counts of adult words and conversational turns. Recordings less than two hours in duration were set as missing in analyses.

Although LENA software has been formally validated for reliable use with children up to four years of age, it has also been used with older children (Romeo et al., Reference Romeo, Leonard, Robinson, West, Mackey, Rowe and Gabrieli2018; Vohr, Topol, Watson, St Pierre & Tucker, Reference Vohr, Topol, Watson, St Pierre and Tucker2014; Wang, Pan, Miller & Cortina, Reference Wang, Pan, Miller and Cortina2014). Previous work tested the reliability of the LENA software at older ages within the Child Sample, reporting a high correlation (r = .74, p < .001) between estimates of child vocalizations using LENA software vs. hand-coded transcriptions (see Merz et al., Reference Merz, Maskus, Melvin, He and Noble2020 for details). This correlation is in line with estimations of the LENA software accuracy among younger children (76%; Gilkerson et al., Reference Gilkerson, Richards, Warren, Montgomery, Greenwood, Kimbrough Oller, Hansen and Paul2017).

Analytic Samples

Given similarity in recruitment methods and maternal demographics for Studies 1 and 2, the samples were combined to provide greater analytic power. Together, Studies 1 and 2 formed the Combined Infant Sample. Study 3 comprised the Child Sample. See Table 1 for details on the similarities and differences between Studies 1 and 2, and between the Combined Infant Sample and Child Sample. A dummy variable for Study was included in all Combined Infant Sample analyses. Several sensitivity checks were also performed to ensure that the combination of these samples did not bias the results (see online supplemental file).

As presented in Table 1, Welch’s t-tests showed relatively few statistically detectable differences between the samples.Footnote 1 Perceived stress scores were, on average, lower in Study 2 (M = 11.44, SD = 6.92) than Study 1 (M = 14.13, SD = 6.45; t = 2.84, p = .005). Notably, average stress levels in Study 1 just barely fell within the ‘moderate stress’ range (i.e., between 14-26), while average stress in Study 2 fell within the ‘low stress’ range (Nedea, Reference Nedea2020). Average stress in the Child Sample, however, was squarely within the ‘moderate stress’ range (M = 17.60, SD = 6.19), and higher than the Combined Infant Sample (M = 12.57, SD = 6.84, t = -6.24, p < .001). Families in the Combined Infant Sample (M = 5.10, SD = 6.70) had higher ITN ratios than families in the Child Sample (M = 2.59, SD = 2.45; t = 4.83, p < .001). As might be expected developmentally, children in the Child Sample heard fewer adult words per hour (t = 2.63, p = .009), but engaged in more conversational turns per hour (t = -4.12, p < .001) than those in the Combined Infant Sample.

Outliers

Outliers in LENA data were investigated, with particular attention toward low-duration recordings. First, to ensure that low-duration recordings did not skew the data, all LENA recordings less than 2 hours in duration were set as missing. There were 5 participants with short LENA recording durations in the Combined Infant Sample, and 3 participants in the Child Sample. Next, all LENA data was set as missing for participants with adult word counts or conversational turn counts greater than three standard deviations from the respective sample average. There was one participant with outlier LENA data in the Combined Infant Sample, and none in the Child Sample. The participant with outlier data in the Combined Infant Sample had LENA recordings that were less than five hours in duration. Finally, in the Child Sample, one additional participant’s LENA values were set to missing, due to parent report of incorrect LENA device usage (parent carried the device in their pocket).

Additional outliers were addressed for continuous non-standardized measures (i.e., average educational attainment, ITN ratios). In the Combined Infant Sample there were four participants with ITN ratios greater than three standard deviations from the mean (ITN > 31.18), which were winsorized to be exactly 3 standard deviations from the mean (31.18). In the Child Sample there were two families with income-to-needs ratios greater than three standard deviations from the average (ITN > 11) which were winsorized to be 11. There were no outliers in average educational attainment in either sample.

Missing Data

Prior to formal missing data imputation procedures, we estimated missing income values using available data to address cases of income missingness. Indeed, in many cases families did not provide precise income figures due to lack of comfort in sharing this information. In Study 1, there were 3 participants who did not report their income, but did indicate which “bin” their income fell within. For these participants, income was assigned to be the median value of the bin they reported (e.g., if bin was “$10,000 to $15,000”, then their income level was assigned as $12,500). In Study 2, 17 participants did not provide income data at the prenatal visit. However, 11 of them subsequently provided income information at the 12-month visit. In these cases, the 12-month income information was used for analyses. In Study 3, there were 4 participants who did not report their income, but who did indicate their income bin. As in Study 1, the average of their indicated bin was used as their income for analyses.

After estimating income for these cases, there was full data coverage for 54.2% of the Combined Infant Sample (134 completed LENA, 203 completed PSS, 216 reported ITN, 227 reported parental educational attainment), and 76.8% of the Child Sample (77 participants completed LENA, 91 completed PSS, 95 completed ITN, and 95 reported parental educational attainment). Full Information Likelihood Estimation (FIML) was utilized to account for missingness (see supplemental file for more details on missingness and for analyses performed without the use of FIML; we note that primary findings were consistent across models that did and did not use FIML.).

Analytic Plan

All analyses were executed using Stata 16.0 (StataCorp, 2019). Primary analyses were performed separately for the Combined Infant Sample and the Child Sample. For both samples, linear regression analyses were performed to identify whether perceived stress was associated with the home language environment (adult words and conversational turns) when controlling for educational attainment and ITN. In each model, perceived stress, average educational attainment and ITN were entered as independent variables. Home language environment measures (average hourly adult word count and average hourly conversational turn count) were entered as dependent variables. Models also included controls for child age at the time of LENA recording, and LENA recording duration. In addition, study affiliation (Study 1 versus Study 2) was entered as a covariate for the Combined Infant Sample models. Next, mediation analyses were performed to determine whether maternal perceived stress mediated any of the statistically significant relations between the socioeconomic factors and home language environment measures. In these analyses, perceived stress served as the mediator, and either adult word count or conversational turn count served as the dependent variable. The same covariates were included in these models as were included in the regressions. All variables were standardized prior to analyses, and standardized Betas were reported (unless otherwise noted).

Several additional sensitivity checks were performed to further test the robustness of findings. These tests are detailed and reported in the online supplementary file. Across models, we tested whether the substantive findings from our primary models held. For both the Combined Infant Sample and Child Sample, these included the introduction of additional covariates beyond those included in the primary models for child sex, maternal age, race (i.e., reference group was whether the parent was White, with dummy variables for whether the parent was Black or whether the parent reported a different racial identity), whether the parent was Hispanic, and duration of time between maternal reports of perceived stress and recording of the home language environment (given that there was often a lag between when mothers completed the PSS and their home LENA recordings). These controls were chosen for both theoretical reasons (i.e., differences in the lived realities of families from different races due to racism and other structural inequities) and empirical reasons (i.e., associations with both independent and dependent variables).

As a final series of robustness checks, we tested the relation among the home language environment and alternate measures of stress for which we had poorer data coverage. Exploratory regression models were executed to test the relations among the home language environment measures and two additional measures of stress: 1) maternal hair cortisol concentration and 2) a stress composite composed of stress-related measures. These results, and measurement details, are presented in the online supplementary file.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

The bottom, left-hand side of Table 2 details the correlations between the key variables of interest within the Combined Infant Sample. Perceived stress was not associated with adult word count or conversational turn count. Perceived stress was also not associated with ITN or educational attainment. Both greater ITN and greater education were moderately associated with greater adult word count and greater conversational turn count.

Table 2. Correlation Matrix- All Studies

Note.

+ p < .10

* p < 0.05

** p < .01

*** p < .001

Note. The bottom, left-hand side of the matrix presents correlations for the Combined Infant Sample (Study 1 and Study 2). The top, right-hand side of the matrix presents correlations for the Child Sample (Study 3). Home language environment measures reflect hourly rates with silent periods removed. Income-to-needs-ratios were log-transformed.

The top, right-hand side of Table 2 presents the correlations between the key variables of interest within the Child Sample. As was the case for the Combined Infant Sample, perceived stress was not associated with either measure of the home environment or either measure of SES. In this sample, ITN was not statistically significantly associated with adult word count of conversational turn count, but educational attainment showed moderate to large correlations with both of these outcomes.

Perceived Stress, SES, and Adult Words

First, regression analyses were performed to determine the unique associations between perceived stress and adult word count. Table 3 presents these results for both samples. In both the Combined Infant Sample (β = -.01, SE = .08, p = .89) and Child Sample (β = .02, SE = .10, p = .84), there was no association between perceived stress and adult word count. When controlling for ITN, parental educational attainment demonstrated a moderate association with adult words in the Combined Infant Sample (β = .32, SE = .08, p < .001) and in the Child Sample (β = .49, SE = .14, p = .001). The coefficients for ITN were smaller and statistically non-significant in the Combined Infant Sample (β = .13, SE = .10, p = .20) and the Child Sample (β = -.16, SE = .15, p = .29).

Table 3. Associations Between Stress and Home Language Environment

Note.

+ p < .10

* p < 0.05

** p < .01

*** p < .001

Note. The Combined Infant Sample was composed of infants from Studies 1 and 2. The Child Sample was composed of participants from Study 3. Home language environment measures reflect hourly rates with silent periods removed. Covariate Set #1 included: LENA recording duration and child age at time of recording. Study affiliation was also included as a covariate in the Combined Infant Sample analyses. All analyses were performed using FIML. Income-to-needs-ratios were log-transformed.

Perceived Stress, SES, and Conversational Turns

Subsequent regression analyses were executed to test whether perceived stress was related to conversational turn counts (see Table 3). As was the case for adult word count, perceived stress was neither associated with conversational turn count in the Combined Infant Sample (β = .02, SE = .09, p = .84), nor in the Child Sample (β = .05, SE = .11, p = .63). As was also the case for adult word count, parental educational attainment showed moderate associations with conversational turns in both the Combined Infant Sample (β = .27, SE = .09, p = .002) and the Child Sample (β = .31, SE = .14, p = .03). ITN did not statistically significantly relate to conversational turns in either the Combined Infant Sample (β = .13, SE = .11, p = .23) or Child Sample (β = .02, SE = .15, p = .88).

Mediation Models

A mediational analysis was conducted to explore whether there was an indirect effect of parental educational attainment on home language inputs through perceived stress, since education was related to both adult word count and conversational turn count in both samples. There was no indirect effect of educational attainment on adult word counts (Combined Infant Sample: β = .00, SE = .00, p = .81; Child Sample: β = .00, SE = .01, p = .79) or conversational turns (Combined Infant Sample: β = .00, SE = .00, p = .95; Child Sample: β = -.01, SE = .01, p = .67) through stress.

Robustness Checks

Several robustness checks were performed to test the sensitivity of these results. Analytic details are provided in the online supplementary file (see Tables S1-S7). These analyses included models that: considered Study 1 and 2 independently; did not use FIML; contained additional control variables; and included a restricted sample of only five- to seven-month-old infants from Studies 1 and 2. Across all robustness checks, perceived stress was not associated with measures of the home language environment. The relations between the socioeconomic variables and home language outcomes were more variable depending on model specification. Taken together, parental educational attainment appeared to be more consistently statistically significantly related to home language measures, though the magnitude of the coefficients was fairly variable from one model to the next.

Additional exploratory analyses were performed with limited sample sizes to investigate whether physiological stress, measured via maternal hair cortisol concentration, and Stress Composites, composed of several stress-related measures, were associated with differences in the home language environment (See Table S8 and S9, respectively). We found that neither hair cortisol concentration nor the Stress Composites were associated with hourly adult word count or conversational turn count.

Discussion

While previous work has investigated socioeconomic disparities in the home language environment, there has been less attention directed towards understanding the mechanisms that explain these disparities. The present study tested whether maternal perceived stress directly related to, and accounted for socioeconomic differences in, the home language environments of six- to twelve-month-old infants and five- to nine-year-old children. While our results replicated past work showing some socioeconomic differences in home language inputs, we found no evidence that perceived stress explained these differences. Supplemental analyses suggested the same pattern of findings for maternal physiological stress and a composite of several stress-related measures. Together, these findings suggest the possibility that perceived stress does not influence the home language environment. These findings highlight the importance of future work investigating which factors, if not perceived stress, drive socioeconomic differences in the home language environment.

These results contradict findings reported by Pierce et al. (Reference Pierce, Reilly and Nelson2020), which was the first study, to our knowledge, that investigated these relations. Pierce et al. (Reference Pierce, Reilly and Nelson2020) found that greater perceived stress was associated with lower home language environment counts in families with infants. Indeed, our Combined Infant Sample focused on infants of the same age, used similar methodologies, and reported similar perceived stress levels as the Pierce et al. study. Of note, the two did differ on some sample characteristics which could potentially explain differences in study findings. Pierce and colleagues’ findings were observed among a small sample (n = 22) of primarily low- and middle-income families, whereas our Combined Infant Sample spanned a larger socioeconomic range (including high-income families). Additionally, LENA counts were generally higher for participants in our sample than Pierce’s sample. Our sample was also composed of mothers who were, on average, older than mothers in Pierce and colleagues’ sample. Additional replications are needed to further probe the links between perceived stress and the home language environment, and to better understand which characteristics moderate these findings, especially given the relatively large standard errors associated with our estimates.

Beyond testing these direct associations between stress and the home language environment, our study also aimed to test whether stress mediated the relation between parental educational attainment and the home language environment. We found no consistent evidence that socioeconomic factors were statistically significantly correlated with perceived stress or, in our supplemental analyses, with physiological stress. Importantly, we observed this pattern in two samples with different average perceived stress levels (mothers in the Child Sample reported ‘moderate stress’, while mothers in the Combined Infant Sample reported ‘low stress’; Nedea, Reference Nedea2020). While some previous research has also reported no relation between these constructs (e.g., Ursache, Noble & Blair, Reference Ursache, Noble and Blair2015), other studies have found that lower SES is generally associated with higher perceived stress (Algren et al., Reference Algren, Ekholm, Nielsen, Ersbøll, Bak and Andersen2018; Glasscock et al., Reference Glasscock, Andersen, Labriola, Rasmussen and Hansen2013; Senn, Walsh & Carey, Reference Senn, Walsh and Carey2014). Importantly, such heterogeneity in findings could be due to the fact that the circumstances and experiences associated with low income and low educational attainment vary widely across families (DeJoseph, Sifre, Raver, Blair & Berry, Reference DeJoseph, Sifre, Raver, Blair and Berry2021). As such, socioeconomic disadvantage may be related to perceived stress in some cases, but not others. Interestingly, ITN and educational attainment showed small to moderate associations (generally statistically significant) with a composite of various stress-related measures in our supplemental models.

It is important to consider that perceptions of stress are distinct from the experience of stressors themselves (see Troller-Renfree et al., Reference Troller-Renfree, Hart, Sperber, Fox and Noble2022 for review). While socioeconomic disadvantage may be associated with exposure to more stressors (Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, Farah and Meaney2010; McLoyd, Reference McLoyd1998), perceptions of stress may be largely independent from the number of stressors a family experiences, and may, instead, be driven by individual, trait-level variability in processing and mental health. Individuals experiencing socioeconomic disadvantage and associated stressors may also adapt so that they are better equipped to handle this stress and, as such, perceive their day-to-day stress levels as relatively low (Ellis, Abrams, Masten, Sternberg, Tottenham & Frankenhuis, Reference Ellis, Abrams, Masten, Sternberg, Tottenham and Frankenhuis2020). Importantly, measures of perceived stress, such as the PSS, may be limited in their ability to capture the nuances of stress appraisal across contexts where stress is more or less chronic and/or severe. It will be worthwhile for future studies to test whether other forms of stress, or stressors themselves, relate to the home language environment. Although we found no associations between the home language environment and alternate measures of stress – including maternal hair cortisol concentration and a composite composed of multiple stress-related measures – these analyses were limited by small sample size. Future work should investigate these relations in larger samples.

In line with past work documenting socioeconomic differences in home language inputs (Gilkerson et al., Reference Gilkerson, Richards, Warren, Montgomery, Greenwood, Kimbrough Oller, Hansen and Paul2017; Rowe et al., Reference Rowe, Pan and Ayoub2005; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013), we generally found that socioeconomic factors were associated with higher hourly counts of adult words and conversational turns (see Brito et al., Reference Brito, Troller-Renfree, Leon-Santos, Isler, Fifer and Noble2020; Merz et al., Reference Merz, Maskus, Melvin, He and Noble2020 for original reporting of these results for Studies 1 and 3). However, in the models reported here, there appeared to be more consistent evidence for links between the home language environment and parental educational attainment rather than family income-to-needs ratios. Notably, our supplemental models showed variability in these associations across different model specifications in terms of both coefficient magnitude and statistical significance. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find evidence that perceived stress mediated these links. These results raise questions about which factors do explain socioeconomic differences in the home language environment.

New insight may be gleaned from future work that moves beyond investigations of individual-level factors (i.e., perceived stress) to focus, instead, on larger environmental factors that may drive these relations. For example, the experience of various structural inequities associated with socioeconomic disadvantage – such as income inequality, housing and food insecurity, unemployment, lack of access to child care, and racism and discrimination – could explain socioeconomic differences in the home language environment. Such structural inequalities may influence caregivers’ home language behaviors through mechanisms that are unrelated to either caregiver competencies or caregivers’ reported perceptions of stress. Indeed, experimentally induced financial scarcity has been shown to decrease child-directed speech even among middle- to high-income individuals (Ellwood-Lowe et al., Reference Ellwood-Lowe, Foushee and Srinivasan2021). Consideration of the influence of such larger forces may prove valuable in understanding why SES is associated with the home language environment, and in imagining potential interventions to address these factors. If structural factors play an important role in explaining socioeconomic differences in early language development, then interventions directed towards improving caregiver knowledge and stress alone may fail without changes to these larger environmental conditions.

Limitations

The current study was limited by its use of mostly concurrent data. Future longitudinal investigation may yield new insight about the relations of perceived stress, socioeconomic circumstances, and the home language environment as children develop. Indeed, it is possible that more cumulative measures of stress collected longitudinally will relate differently to measures of the home language environment than measures that capture relatively recent stress, as used in the current study. The data were also limited by the use of home language measures that captured general speech in the home, and not child-directed speech specifically (see Dailey & Bergelson, Reference Dailey and Bergelson2022). It is possible that measures of maternal child-directed speech are affected by stress in ways that the larger home language environment is not. Future work should explore this possibility. The current study was both strengthened and limited by its use of data from three separate samples. While we consistently found no evidence of a relation between maternal perceptions of stress and the home language environment across these samples, we encourage future investigation of these associations in other, even larger, samples. Indeed, it is possible that unobservable differences between these samples contributed bias to the results, although the inclusion of a control for study affiliation, and execution of a variety of sensitivity checks, reduces this possibility. While the current study used a larger sample than past work, the relatively large standard errors on our estimates suggest that future analyses would benefit from investigation of these relations in still larger samples which are powered to estimate associations between stress and the home language environment with increased precision. Finally, as previously noted, future work will benefit from consideration of other theoretically-motivated factors that may explain socioeconomic differences in the home language environment.

Acknowledgements

This publication is supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01HD093707-01 to KGN and K99HD104923 to SVTR), as well as grants to KGN from the Russell Sage Foundation, the Presidential Scholars in Society and Neuroscience at Columbia University, and Teachers College, Columbia University. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. We thank all families for their participation; Stephen Yazicioglu for his help with the custom python scripts; and Melina Amarante, Olivia Colon, Pooja Desai, Isabel Kovacs, Elaine Maskus, Dayanara Sanchez, and Ana Leon Santos for their help with data collection.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000923000156.

Footnotes

1 We tested for statistical differences in the key variables after the removal of outliers (see following section for details on outlier removal).

References

Adair, J. K., Colegrove, S. S., & McManus, M. E. (2017). How the word gap argument negatively impacts young children of Latinx immigrants’ conceptceptualizations of learning. Harvard Educational Review, 87(3), 309334. https://doi.org/10.17763/1943-5045-87.3.309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Algren, M. H., Ekholm, O., Nielsen, L., Ersbøll, A. K., Bak, C. K., & Andersen, P. T. (2018). Associations between perceived stress, socioeconomic status, and health-risk behaviour in deprived neighbourhoods in Denmark: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 18(1), 250. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5170-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, N. H. (2017). Influence of the Home Linguistic Environment on Early Language Development. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(2), 155162. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732217720699CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brito, N. H., Troller-Renfree, S. V., Leon-Santos, A., Isler, J. R., Fifer, W. P., & Noble, K. G. (2020). Associations among the home language environment and neural activity during infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 43, 100780. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100780CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. (1988). Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In Spacapan, S. & Oskamp, S. (Eds.), The social psychology of health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publishers.Google Scholar
Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 179193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dailey, S., & Bergelson, E. (2022). Language input to infants of different socioeconomic statuses: A quantitative meta-analysis. Developmental Science, 25(3), e13192. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13192CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeJoseph, M. L., Sifre, R. D., Raver, C. C., Blair, C. B., & Berry, D. (2021). Capturing environmental dimensions of adversity and resources in the context of poverty across infancy through early adolescence: A moderated nonlinear factor model. Child Development, 92(4), e457e475. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13504CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Souza, S., Crawford, C. N., Buckley, J., Underwood, L., Peterson, E. R., Bird, A., Morton, S. M. B., & Waldie, K. E. (2019). Antenatal determinants of early childhood talking delay and behavioural difficulties. Infant Behavior and Development, 57, 101388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudley-Marling, C., & Lucas, K. (2009). Pathologizing the language and culture of poor children. Language Arts, 86(5), 362370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Abrams, L. S., Masten, A. S., Sternberg, R. J., Tottenham, N., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2020). Hidden talents in harsh environments. Development and Psychopathology, 34, 95113. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000887CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellwood-Lowe, M. E., Foushee, R., & Srinivasan, M. (2021). What causes the word gap? Financial concerns may systematically suppress child-directed speech. Developmental Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W., Boxhill, L., & Pinkava, M. (2008). Poverty and maternal responsiveness: The role of maternal stress and social resources. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(3), 232237. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408089272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. J. (2017). The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: Correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron, 96(1), 5671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, A. L. B., Elmquist, M., Merbler, A. M., Kriese, A., Will, K. K., & McConnell, S. R. (2020). Toward an ecobehavioral model of early language development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 246258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner-Neblett, N., Pungello, E. P., & Iruka, I. U. (2012). Oral narrative skills: Implications for the reading development of African American childrenChild Development Perspectives6(3), 218224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00225.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Warren, S. F., Montgomery, J. K., Greenwood, C. R., Kimbrough Oller, D., Hansen, J. H. L., & Paul, T. D. (2017). Mapping the early language environment using all-day recordings and automated analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26(2), 248265. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_AJSLP-15-0169CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glasscock, D. J., Andersen, J. H., Labriola, M., Rasmussen, K., & Hansen, C. D. (2013). Can negative life events and coping style help explain socioeconomic differences in perceived stress among adolescents? A cross-sectional study based on the West Jutland cohort study. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 532. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M., Hoff, E., Rowe, M. L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Language matters: Denying the existence of the 30-million-word gap has serious consequences. Child Development, 90(3), 985992. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(9), 651659. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2897CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halle, T., Forry, N., Hair, E., Perper, K., Wandner, L., Wessel, J., & Vick, J. (2009). Disparities in Early Learning and Development: Lessons from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study – Birth Cohort (ECLS-B). Washington, DC: Child Trends.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Justice, L. M., Jiang, H., Purtell, K. M., Schmeer, K., Boone, K., Bates, R., & Salsberry, P. J. (2019). Conditions of poverty, parent–child interactions, and toddlers’ early language skills in low-income families. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 23(7), 971978. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-018-02726-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuchirko, Y. (2019). On differences and deficits: A critique of the theoretical and methodological underpinnings of the word gap. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 19(4), 533562. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798417747029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehr, M., Wecksell, B., Nahum, L., Neuhaus, D., Teel, K. S., Linares, L. O., & Diaz, A. (2016). Parenting stress, child characteristics, and developmental delay from birth to age five in teen mother–child dyads. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(3), 10351043. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0282-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, C. Y. Y., Hernandez, M. W., & Suskind, D. L. (2020). Enriching home language environment among families from low-SES backgrounds: A randomized controlled trial of a home visiting curriculum. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 2435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, Y., Xu, J., Huang, J., Jia, Y., Zhang, J., Yan, C., & Zhang, J. (2017). Effects of prenatal and postnatal maternal emotional stress on toddlers’ cognitive and temperamental development. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnuson, K. A., Sexton, H. R., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Huston, A. C. (2009). Increases in maternal education and young children’s language skills. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 55(3), 319350. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masarik, A. S., & Conger, R. D. (2017). Stress and child development: A review of the Family Stress Model. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 8590. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 21.Google Scholar
Melvin, S. A., Brito, N. H., Mack, L. J., Engelhardt, L. E., Fifer, W. P., Elliott, A. J., & Noble, K. G. (2017). Home environment, but not socioeconomic status, is linked to differences in early phonetic perception abilityInfancy, 22(1), 4255. http://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendelsohn, A. L., Huberman, H. S., Berkule, S. B., Brockmeyer, C. A., Morrow, L. M., & Dreyer, B. P. (2011). Primary care strategies for promoting parent-child interactions and school readiness in at-risk families: The Bellevue Project for Early Language, Literacy, and Education Success. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 165(1). https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.254CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merz, E. C., Maskus, E. A., Melvin, S. A., He, X., & Noble, K. G. (2020). Socioeconomic disparities in language input are associated with children’s language-related brain structure and reading skills. Child Development, 91(3), 846860. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nedea, D. (2020, April). Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) Calculator. MDApp. https://www.mdapp.co/perceived-stress-scale-pss-calculator-389/Google Scholar
Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., Akshoomoff, N., Amaral, D. G., Bloss, C. S., Libiger, O., Schork, N. J., Murray, S. S., Casey, B. J., Chang, L., Ernst, T. M., Frazier, J. A., Gruen, J. R., Kennedy, D. N., Van Zijl, P., Mostofsky, S., Kaufmann, W. E., Kenet, T. D., Anders, M., Jernigan, T. L., & Sowell, E. R. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience, 18(5), 773778. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3983CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science, 10(4), 464480. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, K. G., Hart, E. R., & Sperber, J. F. (2021). Socioeconomic disparities and neuroplasticity: Moving toward adaptation, intersectionality, and inclusion. American Psychologist, 76(9), 1486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3(1), 285308. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, S. C., Finegood, E. D., & Swain, J. E. (2013). Poverty and Language Development : Roles of Parenting and Stress. 10.Google ScholarPubMed
Pierce, L. J., Reilly, E., & Nelson, C. A. (2020). Associations between maternal stress, early language behaviors, and infant electroencephalography during the first year of life. Journal of Child Language, 48(4), 737764. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2018). Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617742725CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185205. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L., Pan, B. A., & Ayoub, C. (2005). Predictors of variation in maternal talk to children: A longitudinal study of low-income families. Parenting, 5(3), 259283. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327922par0503_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senn, T. E., Walsh, J. L., & Carey, M. P. (2014). The mediating roles of perceived stress and health behaviors in the relation between objective, subjective, and neighborhood socioeconomic status and perceived health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 48(2), 215224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9591-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, D. E., Sperry, L. L., & Miller, P. J. (2019). Reexamining the verbal environments of children From different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development, 90(4), 13031318. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13072CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StataCorp. (2019). Stata statistical software: Release 16. StataCorp LLC.Google Scholar
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Hart, E. R., Sperber, J. F., Fox, N. A., & Noble, K. G. (2022). Associations among stress and language and socioemotional development in a low-income sample. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 597605. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001759CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ursache, A., Noble, K. G., & Blair, C. (2015). Socioeconomic status, subjective social status, and perceived stress: Associations with stress physiology and executive functioning. Behavioral Medicine, 41(3), 145154. https://doi.org/10.1080/08964289.2015.1024604CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohr, B. R., Topol, D., Watson, V., St Pierre, L., & Tucker, R. (2014). The importance of language in the home for school-age children with permanent hearing loss. Acta Paediatrica, 103(1), 6269. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12441CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Z., Pan, X., Miller, K. F., & Cortina, K. S. (2014). Automatic classification of activities in classroom discourse. Computers & Education, 78, 115123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2014.05.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 21432152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptives for Studies 1, 2, and 3

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlation Matrix- All Studies

Figure 2

Table 3. Associations Between Stress and Home Language Environment

Supplementary material: PDF

Hart et al. supplementary material

Hart et al. supplementary material

Download Hart et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 258.5 KB