Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:31:05.414Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Risk and resilience in Syrian refugee children: A multisystem analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2023

Demelza Smeeth
Affiliation:
Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
Andrew K. May
Affiliation:
Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
Elie G. Karam
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, St Georges Hospital University Medical Center, Beirut, Lebanon
Michael J. Rieder
Affiliation:
Physiology and Pharmacology, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
Abdelbaset A. Elzagallaai
Affiliation:
Physiology and Pharmacology, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
Stan van Uum
Affiliation:
Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
Michael Pluess*
Affiliation:
Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK Department of Psychological Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
*
Corresponding Author: Michael Pluess, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Refugee children are often exposed to substantial trauma, placing them at increased risk for mental illness. However, this risk can be mitigated by a capacity for resilience, conferred from multiple ecological systems (e.g., family, community), including at an individual biological level. We examined the ability of hair cortisol concentrations and polygenic scores for mental health to predict risk and resilience in a sample of Syrian refugee children (n = 1359). Children were categorized as either at-risk or resilient depending on clinical thresholds for posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, and externalizing behavior problems. Logistic regression was used to examine main and interacting effects while controlling for covariates. Elevated hair cortisol concentrations were significantly associated with reduced resilience (odds ratio (OR)=0.58, 95%CI [0.40, 0.83]) while controlling for levels of war exposure. Polygenic scores for depression, self-harm, and neuroticism were not found to have any significant main effects. However, a significant interaction emerged between hair cortisol and polygenic scores for depression (OR=0.04, 95%CI [0.003 0.47]), suggesting that children predisposed to depression were more at risk for mental health problems when hair cortisol concentrations were high. Our results suggest that biomarkers (separately and in combination) might support early identification of refugee children at risk for mental health problems.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Exposure to war and forced displacement are significant traumata that can severely disrupt the development of refugee children (Maragel & Manachi, Reference Maragel and Manachi2018; Pieloch et al., Reference Pieloch, McCullough and Marks2016). However, not all children will respond equivalently to such atypical stressors due to interindividual differences in each child’s resilience and the resilience of the systems in which they are embedded (e.g., family, societal, political, etc.) (Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020). Although multisystemic in nature, the most proximal influences on resilience are those at the biological level (i.e., the genetic and physiological make-up of individual children), but few studies have explored putative biological markers of resilience in refugee populations or children (Stein et al., Reference Stein, Choi, Jain, Campbell-Sills, Chen, Gelernter, He, Heeringa, Maihofer, Nievergelt, Nock, Ripke, Sun, Kessler, Smoller and Ursano2019; Tol et al., Reference Tol, Song and Jordans2013). Here, we examine the main and interacting effects of hair cortisol and psychopathology-related polygenic scores in predicting risk and resilience in a large sample of Syrian refugee children.

Multisystemic resilience

Resilience has been defined as the “the capacity of a dynamic system to adapt successfully to disturbances that threaten system function, viability, or development” (Masten, Reference Masten2014). From a developmental psychology perspective, humans are the dynamic systems of interest, and resilience is gauged from the patterns of behavior and adaptation emerging in the aftermath of stress (Masten, Reference Masten2021). Much historical developmental psychology research has examined how children exposed to harmful environments (e.g., poverty, deprivation, abuse) manage to adapt toward healthy, functional adulthood, locating resilience within specific behaviors or stable traits (Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Pluess and Ungar2021). Humans, however, are comprised of subordinate systems (e.g., immune, neurological, endocrine, microbial), and are embedded in supraordinate systems (Ungar, Reference Ungar and Ungar2021b) such as those described in Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory (Bronfenbrenner, Reference Bronfenbrenner1979). Each of these systems undergo constant change and must individually adapt to stressful perturbations, but by virtue of their nested hierarchy, systems dynamically interact and influence each other creating complex patterns of interplay. Consequently, contemporary research acknowledges resilience as a multifaceted process, occurring over time, rather than a single stable factor or trait (Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Pluess and Ungar2021).

Interest in resilience research continues to gather momentum in a world currently hallmarked by rapid changes and growing uncertainties (Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020; Ungar, Reference Ungar and Ungar2021a). Of particular concern are the rising frequency of climate disasters and armed conflict (Masten, Reference Masten2021), generating a substantial global refugee population, an alarming proportion of which are children (Murray, Reference Murray2019). The Syrian civil war, for example, has been waged over the past 12 years, forcing over five million Syrian residents, of which approximately 50% are below the age of 18 (Maragel & Manachi, Reference Maragel and Manachi2018), to flee to neighboring countries. As an interim measure, many refugees are housed within informal tented settlements, but these can be variably beset by issues of overcrowding, limited food security, poor hygiene, and reduced access to education and jobs (Kazour et al., Reference Kazour, Zahreddine, Maragel, Almustafa, Soufia, Haddad and Richa2017; Murray, Reference Murray2019). Poor living conditions, war exposure, and forced displacement constitute multiple severe traumas which together demand substantial multisystemic resilience to overcome, driving the need for more comprehensive, transdisciplinary research to mitigate the plight of refugees (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019).

Childhood resilience

Elucidating resilience in children is of particular importance, given that children are at greater risk for harmful outcomes following exposure to adversity (Murray, Reference Murray2019; Sirin & Aber, Reference Sirin and Aber2018). Childhood represents a critical phase of development, in which many biological and cognitive systems are under development and calibration (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019; Murray, Reference Murray2019). Unfortunately, refugee children are often robbed of the experiences and opportunities considered fundamental for positive development. Refugee children typically experience family loss and/or separation (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Cacciatore and Klimek2012), and become disconnected from their primary cultures and communities (Cook et al., Reference Cook, Blaustein and van der Kolk2003). Many lack regular access to proper nutrition, sanitation, safe accommodation, or education (Meiqari et al., Reference Meiqari, Hoetjes, Baxter and Lenglet2018; Murray, Reference Murray2019). Amongst those fortunate enough to receive schooling, there may still be challenges with language and acculturation (Pieloch et al., Reference Pieloch, McCullough and Marks2016). At further extremes, refugee children may be subjected to rape and sexual violence (especially females), or forced marriage (Hodes et al., Reference Hodes, Jagdev, Chandra and Cunniff2008; Murray, Reference Murray2019). All considered, refugee children are exposed to complex trauma, that is, multiple traumatic events for an extended period of time (Cook et al., Reference Cook, Blaustein and van der Kolk2003).

As a result of complex trauma, children are likely to experience mental health disorders, physical disorders, and revictimization (Pieloch et al., Reference Pieloch, McCullough and Marks2016). Recent estimates indicate that 23–57% of Syrian refugee children meet the clinical diagnosis threshold when scoring posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, and/or anxiety symptoms (McEwen et al., Reference McEwen, Biazoli, Popham, Moghames, Saab, Fayyad, Karam, Bosqui and Pluess2023; Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Karam, Fayyad, Karam, Saab, Moghames and Pluess2022; Sirin & Rogers-Sirin, Reference Sirin and Rogers-Sirin2015). However, not all refugee children will succumb to mental illness, with many still retaining a reasonable likelihood of adaptively overcoming their childhood adversities (Murray, Reference Murray2019). The chances will depend on the resilience of the child, and the resilience of the systems in which the child is ultimately situated at any given time. Since resilience is an ongoing process, even children suffering significant mental health problems may still develop resilience as pertinent systems change (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019). Identifying the promotive and protective factors manifesting resilience under such severe conditions may inform future strategies for assisting refugee children.

Resilience at the biological level

Although a substantial number of studies have explored psychosocial factors promoting resilience in refugee children (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019; Pieloch et al., Reference Pieloch, McCullough and Marks2016), less is known about biological factors (Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020; Tol et al., Reference Tol, Song and Jordans2013). Studies in twins note that resilience is heritable (although to what extent remains unclear), suggesting that biological factors play an appreciable role (Amstadter et al., Reference Amstadter, Myers and Kendler2014; Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020). A number of candidate gene studies have highlighted genetic and epigenetic variation in single genes that might have bearing on resilience by virtue of their effects on cognitive (e.g. SLC6A4), immunological (e.g. FKBP5), or stress-response systems (e.g. CRHR1) but trust in candidate gene findings has diminished since the advent of (epi)genome-wide association studies (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Ostacher and Ballon2019; Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020). However, to our knowledge only one genome-wide association study directly examining resilience has been performed to date, which was not sufficiently powered to develop a meaningful polygenic score (a weighted sum of additive genetic influences) capable of predicting resilience (Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Choi, Jain, Campbell-Sills, Chen, Gelernter, He, Heeringa, Maihofer, Nievergelt, Nock, Ripke, Sun, Kessler, Smoller and Ursano2019). Nevertheless, due to biological pleiotropy (Zheutlin et al., Reference Zheutlin, Dennis, Karlsson Linnér, Moscati, Restrepo, Straub, Ruderfer, Castro, Chen, Ge, Huckins, Charney, Kirchner, Stahl, Chabris, Davis and Smoller2019), genetic variants associated with relevant phenotypes such as neuroticism, depression, and worry are likely to share association with resilience, depending on how the construct is operationalized (Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020). Polygenic scores for linked phenotypes might thus have utility for predicting resilient outcomes, especially when combined with other biomarkers, but such potential remains largely unexplored (Ahrens et al., Reference Ahrens, Neumann, von Werthern, Kranz, Kollmann, Mattes, Puhlmann, Weichert, Lutz, Basten, Fiebach, Wessa, Kalisch, Lieb, Chiocchetti, Tüscher, Reif and Plichta2022).

As a well-established risk factor for mental illnesses, stress-related hormone regulation, particularly cortisol, is likely an informative biological marker of resilience (Masten, Reference Masten2014; Maul et al., Reference Maul, Giegling, Fabbri, Corponi, Serretti and Rujescu2020). Cortisol levels tend to appreciably correlate with stress and trauma exposure, although the direction of correlation depends on numerous considerations such as age of the individual, timing of cortisol sampling (morning vs afternoon), sampled tissue, and chronicity of adversity. For example, a study of hair cortisol in girls variably exposed to the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake in China revealed elevated cortisol levels in those with greater exposure (Luo et al., Reference Luo, Hu, Liu, Ma, Guo, Qiu, Wang, Wang, Zhang, Zhang, Hannum, Zhang, Liu and Li2012), and hair cortisol was also positively correlated with war exposure in a cohort of Syrian refugee children and adolescents (Smeeth et al., Reference Smeeth, McEwen, Popham, Karam, Fayyad, Saab, Rieder, Elzagallaai, van Uum and Pluess2023). Similarly, children exposed to recent trauma were discovered to have elevated afternoon salivary cortisol levels, while children with both acute and chronic trauma exposure had lower morning salivary cortisol in addition to higher levels in the afternoon (Bevans et al., Reference Bevans, Cerbone and Overstreet2008). Children exposed to moderate levels of adversity are known to have more robust cortisol responses compared to both highly exposed and unexposed children (Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Anckarsäter, Lichtenstein, Nelson and Gustafsson2010), suggesting that cortisol regulation can be indicative of a steeling effect. While cortisol levels have been investigated in numerous psychosocial contexts, results continue to paint a complex picture, and more studies are needed that specifically consider cortisol regulation in children and adolescents demonstrating resilience in the face of adversity (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019).

Following recent exploration of different psychosocial systems and levels influencing resilience in Syrian refugee children (Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Karam, Fayyad, Karam, Saab, Moghames and Pluess2022), we set out to further explore resilience in the same sample by examining factors at the biological level. We operationalized resilience as being below clinically relevant thresholds for PTSD, externalizing and depression symptoms. At-risk children were those meeting the criteria for at least one of the three psychopathologies. We then tested for associations with hair cortisol concentration and a range of polygenic scores for phenotypes linked to mental health which we hypothesize would be associated with reduced resilience. In addition to being a robust indicator of long-term cortisol secretion, hair cortisol might also reflect changes in cortisol secretion over recent months (depending on the length of hair sample tested), making hair cortisol a potential measure of both acute stress and longer-term patterns of stress response (Ahrens et al., Reference Ahrens, Neumann, von Werthern, Kranz, Kollmann, Mattes, Puhlmann, Weichert, Lutz, Basten, Fiebach, Wessa, Kalisch, Lieb, Chiocchetti, Tüscher, Reif and Plichta2022). Consequently, we hypothesized a strong main effect association between hair cortisol and risk/resilience categorization, even after controlling for appropriate covariates. Because direct associations between resilience and polygenic scores are likely to be of small effect (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Mak and O’Reilly2020), and because resilience emerges in response to environmental stressors, we also tested for an interaction between polygenic scores and hair cortisol, hypothesizing that interaction effects would be larger.

Methods

Study participants

Participants were drawn from the biological pathways of risk and resilience in Syrian refugee children (BIOPATH) study (McEwen et al., Reference McEwen, Popham, Moghames, Smeeth, de Villiers, Saab, Karam, Fayyad, Karam and Pluess2022). Briefly, the BIOPATH study aimed to explore the psychosocial and biological factors underpinning multisystemic resilience and mental health problems in a large cohort of Syrian refugee children. Following ethical review and approval from the Institutional Review Board of the University of Balamand/Saint George Hospital University Medical Center, Lebanon (ref: IRB/O/024-16/1815), the Lebanese National Consultative Committee on Ethics, and the Ministry of Health, purposive cluster sampling was used to recruit refugee families housed in informal tented settlements in the Beqaa region of Lebanon between October 2017 and January 2018. Small-to-medium settlements were selected across seven municipalities, housing refugees classed as the most to third-most vulnerable according to the UNHCR Vulnerability Framework. The informal tented settlements comprised unplanned groups of up to 153 temporary housing structures and families tended to live in large households with a median of 7 individuals per household (IQR: 2–24). Water trucking was the predominate source of clean water in many of these settlements, and while larger settlements contained limited amenities such as a mosque or school, many did not. In each settlement the community leader (chawich) was approached for permission to conduct the study and all eligible families within the settlement were invited to participate. Child–caregiver dyads were eligible to participate if they had left Syria due to the civil war within the previous 4 years, the child was aged 8–16 years at enrollment, and a primary caregiver was available for interview. Of the 2282 families approached, 1600 (70.1%) were both eligible and willing to participate. Due to uncertainty about birth dates, some children outside the intended age range were recruited, and were retained within the sample unless there were concerns about comprehension.

Consenting child–caregiver dyads were interviewed by trained research staff, who administered a range of psychological and other instruments. Research staff were recruited from Lebanon, were required to be bilingual (Arabic and English), and to have recently completed a relevant undergraduate degree. Interview data and biological samples were checked daily by an experienced trial coordinator in Lebanon and regularly by the UK-based team to identify potential errors or problems. Children and caregivers were interviewed simultaneously, but by separate interviewers, in different rooms where possible. When only one room was available, the child and caregiver sat at opposite sides of the room, facing away from each other, and were interviewed in a low voice to maximize privacy. Children assented to providing hair and saliva samples for hair hormone and genetic analyses. The present study includes the subset of children with high quality biological samples and relevant interview data (1359 out of 1594 total participants; filtering steps described in later sections). There was little difference in characteristics between the original cohort and the sample utilized here (Table S1).

Instruments

All instrumentation in the BIOPATH study was translated into Arabic, following the protocol outlined by McEwen et al. (Reference McEwen, Moghames, Bosqui, Kyrillos, Chehade, Saad, Rahman, Popham, Saab, Karam, Karam and Pluess2021), and piloted in the same target population to ensure good psychometric performance. Modifications were made where necessary to improve comprehensibility and performance, and visual aids were often employed to assist children with the Likert scale response format.

Demographic data

Children were asked a series of questions designed to capture key demographic data about themselves and their families. Information was obtained on gender, age, nationality, and general physical health (e.g., recent injuries/illnesses, smoking behavior). Pubertal development was assessed using a shortened version of the Puberty Development Scale (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo1993; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988). As important covariates for hair hormone analyses, children were also asked about the frequency of hair washing and hair alterations (chemical straightening, dyes, etc.). Interviews with caregivers corroborated the information supplied by children. Caregivers also indicated the approximate amount of time that had passed since fleeing Syria.

Mental health outcomes

The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC) is a widely used self-report questionnaire assessing the frequency of symptoms of depression in children and adolescents (Faulstich et al., Reference Faulstich, Carey, Ruggiero, Enyart and Gresham1986). The instrument comprises 20 items, scored on a scale from 0 to 3, with higher scores indicating more frequent symptoms. Good reliability and validity have been demonstrated in numerous previous studies, and the instrument has performed well in diverse settings including Rwanda (Betancourt et al., Reference Betancourt, Scorza, Meyers-Ohki, Mushashi, Kayiteshonga, Binagwaho, Stulac and Beardslee2012) and Dubai (Ghubash et al., Reference Ghubash, Daradkeh, Al Naseri, Al Bloushi and Al Daheri2000). Following initial pilot testing for the BIOPATH study, the instrument was reduced to 10 items (each loading strongly onto a single factor) that were more understandable by Syrian refugee children. Internal consistency reliability for the remaining 10 items was high (Cronbach’s α = .88).

The Child PTSD Symptom Scale and Trauma Symptoms Checklist (CPSS) is a 17-item self-report instrument assessing the severity of DSM-IV PTSD symptoms, designed for use in children aged 8–18 (Foa et al., Reference Foa, Johnson, Feeny and Treadwell2001). Item responses are captured on a scale from 0 to 3, with higher scores indicating more frequent symptoms. In previous studies, the instrument has attained good internal consistency and test–retest reliability (>.70). The CPSS shares a .80 correlation with the Child Posttraumatic Stress Reaction Index (Pynoos et al., Reference Pynoos, Frederick, Nader, Arroyo, Steinberg, Eth, Nunez and Fairbanks1987), suggesting strong convergent validity. In the present study, the CPSS demonstrated high internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α = .91).

As a measure of externalizing behavior problems, the externalizing subscale of the parent-reported Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, Reference Goodman1997) was used, supplemented by additional items assessing oppositional defiant and conduct disorders (McEwen et al., Reference McEwen, Popham, Moghames, Smeeth, de Villiers, Saab, Karam, Fayyad, Karam and Pluess2022). The SDQ is a 25-item instrument designed to capture psychological attributes of children aged between 3 and 16 years. The instrument can be subscaled in several different ways, including a 10-item externalizing subscale comprising items on conduct and hyperactivity/inattention problems. Items were parent-rated on a three-point scale from 0 to 2, with higher scores indicating more externalizing behavior. An additional 12 items aligning to the DSM-5 criteria for conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder were also scored along the same scale. Together, the 22 items attained an internal consistency reliability of α = .80.

Clinical cut-offs were applied for each mental health scale (12 out of 51 on the CPSS, 10 out of 30 on the abridged CES-DC and 12 out of 44 on the combined externalizing scale total) to define resilience as described previously (Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Karam, Fayyad, Karam, Saab, Moghames and Pluess2022). Cut-offs were derived from validation against consensus diagnosis based on structured clinical interviews using the MINI-KID (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Sheehan, Shytle, Janavs, Bannon, Rogers, Milo, Stock and Wilkinson2010) and expert clinical judgment in a representative subsample (n = 119) of the study (McEwen et al., Reference McEwen, Moghames, Bosqui, Kyrillos, Chehade, Saad, Rahman, Popham, Saab, Karam, Karam and Pluess2021). Resilience was based on a categorical composite of these cut-offs. If participants scored below all three cut-offs, they were considered resilient, but if participants scored above the cut-off for any of the three measures, they were classed as at risk of poor mental health.

Exposure to war

The War Events Questionnaire (WEQ) was designed by the Institute for Development, Research, Advocacy and Applied Care to measure the degree of civilian exposure to war events, specifically for use in Lebanon. The instrument comprises 25 items which assess various experiences of explicit war events such as violence, injury, or kidnapping and was administered to both the child and caregiver. Because self-report may be less reliable in younger children (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Jerman, Purewal Boparai, Koita, Briner, Bucci and Harris2018), child and caregiver responses were combined such that if either one reported that the child experienced an event, the event was considered to have occurred. The WEQ has been used amongst Syrian refugee children previously, where it performed adequately. In our sample, internal consistency reliability for the instrument was high (Cronbach’s α = .88).

Hair hormones

Hair cortisol concentration was measured as described previously (Smeeth et al., Reference Smeeth, McEwen, Popham, Karam, Fayyad, Saab, Rieder, Elzagallaai, van Uum and Pluess2023). Hair sections of at least 3 mm in diameter were cut from the posterior vertex of the child’s head using clean scissors, packaged in aluminim foil with the scalp end labeled and stored in a cool place before shipping for analysis. Hair samples were taken from 1584 children. Month of collection was noted to account for potential seasonal effects.

Cortisol concentration from hair segments (0–2 cm long) were assayed according to an established ELISA-based protocol (Etwel et al., Reference Etwel, Russell, Rieder, Van Uum and Koren2014). Hair sections were weighed, washed with isopropanol, and minced. Cortisol was extracted with methanol overnight at ambient temperature with shaking and evaporated under a stream of nitrogen gas at 60° C until dry. Resulting residues were reconstituted in phosphate-buffered saline before undergoing analysis in duplicate using a modified, commercially available immunoassay (11-CORHU-E01-SLV; ALPCO Diagnostics, USA). Samples exceeding the ELISA’s reported detection range were diluted and reanalyzed. Samples below the immunoassay detection limit (1 ng/mL) were not excluded from main analyses due to their bias toward younger females (n = 227). The inter-assay coefficients of variation across plates were 6.5–9.8%. Hair cortisol concentration was normalized to sample weight and expressed as ng/g. For all analyses, hair cortisol concentrations were logged (base-10) to ensure a normal distribution. Ten samples were excluded due to outlying values of more than three standard deviations above the mean of the logged hair cortisol, resulting in a final sample of 1574.

Genotypes, imputation, and polygenic scores

Saliva samples were collected using Isohelix GeneFix collection kits. Genomic DNA was extracted and genotyped on the Illumina Infinium Global Screening Array, which types 650,181 genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphisms. Following standard quality control procedures using PLINK version 1.90 (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Chow, Tellier, Vattikuti, Purcell and Lee2015; Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira, Bender, Maller, Sklar, de Bakker, Daly and Sham2007), 28 samples were removed due to discrepancies between pedigree and genotyped sex, 68 samples were removed for an individual genotype missing rate > 2%, and a further six samples were removed for outlying heterozygosity rates (F > 0.2). Variants with a missing rate > 3%, a minor allele frequency less than 1%, and/or a Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium p-value < 1 × 10−6 were excluded, leaving 420,463 markers.

Imputation of additional genetic variants was performed using the 1000 Genomes Phase 3, Version 5 reference panel and polygenic scores were calculated by means of the Michigan Imputation Server’s Genotype Imputation and Polygenic Scores service (Beta version 1.6.10; https://imputationserver.sph.umich.edu). We selected seven psychopathology-related polygenic scores recently found to maintain an average of 72.2% predictive ability in individuals of Middle Eastern ancestry compared to those of European ancestry (Privé et al., Reference Privé, Aschard, Carmi, Folkersen, Hoggart, O’Reilly and Vilhjálmsson2022). Where possible the polygenic score derived using penalized regression was favored as this exhibited higher predictive ability than the counterpart created using LDpred2. In some cases the LDpred2 version was the only available polygenic score for that phenotype. All seven scores had 100% coverage in our study sample post-imputation (Table 1).

Table 1. Description of polygenic scores

PGS Catalogue: https://www.pgscatalog.org.

We selected two further polygenic scores for sitting and standing height derived from the same study (Privé et al., Reference Privé, Aschard, Carmi, Folkersen, Hoggart, O’Reilly and Vilhjálmsson2022) and tested their predictive ability for height within our cohort. Despite the polygenic score being based on an adult European population, there was a significant positive relationship between polygenic scores for sitting and standing height with the respective measurements in the children when controlling for age, gender, and 10 genetic principal components (B = 0.60, t (740) = 7.30, p = 7 × 10−13; B = 1.06, t (740) = 5.84, p = 8 × 10−9).

Data analysis

Analyses were performed on the subset of children with complete mental health data, valid hair cortisol concentrations, and good quality genetic data according to the filtering described previously (n = 1359). The association between predictor variables and the resilience binary outcome was investigated using logistic regression analysis in R (Version 4.2.2; R Core Team, 2013) using the glm function. All analyses controlled for age and gender, while additional covariates were selected based on their reported confounders within the literature. Analyses which utilized hair cortisol also controlled for analysis batch, pubertal stage, hair washing frequency, hair alterations, smoking, and month of collection. Analyses which utilized polygenic scores also controlled for 10 genetic principal components. Results for all included terms are reported excluding batches and principal components due to the high number of terms. Multiple testing was controlled for by applying the false discovery rate (FDR) correction and applying a threshold of 0.1. Moderation effects were investigated by including interaction terms. Significant interactions were interpreted by plotting the marginal effects of log-cortisol and polygenic scores on resilience using the ggeffects R package (Version 1.1.4; Lüdecke, Reference Lüdecke2018).

As a follow-up, the main analyses were repeated using a resilience score based solely on depression and PTSD symptoms to better understand the differences between resilience to all mental health outcomes and resilience to just internalizing disorders. In addition, they were performed only on a subset of the oldest children as previous research suggests that polygenic scores for mental health outcomes derived from adult populations are not strongly predictive in children but show improved predictive ability in adolescents (Khera et al., Reference Khera, Chaffin, Wade, Zahid, Brancale, Xia, Distefano, Senol-Cosar, Haas, Bick, Aragam, Lander, Smith, Mason-Suares, Fornage, Lebo, Timpson, Kaplan and Kathiresan2019). Therefore we filtered children according to their pubertal development stage and retained only those in late-post puberty (n = 224).

As a sensitivity analysis the same set of analyses were performed excluding genetic outliers (any individual with a principal component value more than four standard deviations from the mean, repeated iteratively until no outliers remained) and genetically related individuals (pi hat > 0.2) to ensure this was not driving associations with polygenic scores (n = 859). As an additional sensitivity analysis, they were repeated on matched groups of resilient and at-risk children as described previously in this sample (Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Karam, Fayyad, Karam, Saab, Moghames and Pluess2022). Matching was performed using nearest neighbor matching according to Mahalanobis distance in the MatchIt R package (Version 4.5.0; Ho et al., Reference Ho, Imai, King and Stuart2011). The groups (n = 249 per group) were matched according to their specific pattern of responses across all the individual war exposure items, in addition to child age, gender, and time since leaving Syria, such that each group had identical gender distributions, almost identical mean age and time since leaving Syria, and an almost identical number of children who experienced each war event (standardized mean difference between groups < 0.1 for most items and < 0.2 for all items).

Results

Sample characteristics

After excluding individuals with missing interview data, poor quality genotyping data, and outlying hair cortisol concentrations, we had a sample of 1359 individuals (Table 2). Participants were aged 6–18 years old (mean age = 11.3, SD = 2.4) and evenly split between males and females (47.2% male). As reported previously, participants had experienced significant exposure to war as well as substantial hardship in their current living situation (McEwen et al., Reference McEwen, Popham, Moghames, Smeeth, de Villiers, Saab, Karam, Fayyad, Karam and Pluess2022). Forty-seven percent of children had left Syria within the past 3 years. The majority of children reported having experienced at least one war-related event (97.2%) and on average children reported 9.6 war-related events (SD = 5.5). Many families were also experiencing inadequate living standards, with 35.8% of families reporting insufficient food and 27.0% of families reporting inadequate heating.

Table 2. Sample description

a Missing 1 (0.07%).

b Missing 6 (0.44%).

When applying the mental health scale cut-offs determined through clinical interview, 527 (38.8%) were deemed to have significant symptoms of depression, 756 (55.6%) were deemed to have significant symptoms of PTSD, and 598 (44.0%) were deemed to have significant externalizing symptoms. Comorbidity was common with 45% of individuals having significant symptoms of two or more disorders and 13% having significant symptoms of all three disorders. When taking a multidimensional approach to determining resilience, 262 (19.3%) were defined as resilient, with the remainder exhibiting significant symptoms above the cut-off of at least one mental health problem.

Main effects on resilience

We firstly aimed to confirm that war exposure was negatively impacting resilience. A logistic regression was performed to ascertain the effects of war exposure on resilience classification while controlling for age and gender. We found that those reporting a higher number of war-related events had a decreased likelihood of being resilient (odds ratio (OR) = 0.92, 95%CI [0.90, 0.95]; Table S2). Similar results were observed when considering resilience to only internalizing disorders (OR = 0.93, 95%CI [0.91, 0.95]; Table S2).

While the predominate form of trauma experienced by individuals in this cohort occurred during the war, they also experience a wide range of current stressors and adversities in their daily lives. This can be reflected in hair cortisol which acts as an average measure of HPA axis activity over the recent past. We therefore investigated whether hair cortisol, as a biomarker of cumulative stressors, was associated with resilience. We found that those with higher hair cortisol had a decreased likelihood of being resilient (OR = 0.58, 95%CI [0.40, 0.83]; Table 3). Research within this cohort has already shown that war exposure was associated with later elevated hair cortisol, therefore we also controlled for war exposure to ensure that this finding did not simply mimic that found between war exposure and resilience above. We observed a similar relationship between hair cortisol and resilience when controlling for war exposure suggesting that this relationship is at least partly due to additional factors (OR = 0.59, 95%CI [0.41, 0.86]; Table 3). Similar findings were observed for internalizing-specific resilience (Table S3).

Table 3. The relationship between cortisol and resilience

Reference levels: aMale, bNo, cNo, dOnce per week, ePre-mid puberty, fOctober. Main effects of batches not shown for clarity.

While the environment is a significant contributor to resilience, individuals also exhibit interindividual variation in resilience, some of which can be explained by genetic factors. We therefore investigated whether a genetic predisposition to various mental health-related phenotypes was associated with resilience within this cohort. Those with high polygenic scores for recent feelings of foreboding (PGS001920) had a lower likelihood of being resilient although this was only statistically significant when additionally controlling for war exposure (OR = 0.01, 95%CI [0.0001, 0.62]; Table 4) and this did not survive controlling for multiple testing. Similar observations were found when analyses were repeated with genetic outliers and related individuals excluded or when using the subset of matched resilient and at-risk individuals (Table S4).

Table 4. The relationship between polygenic scores and resilience

Reference levels: aMale. bFDR-corrected p-values for main effects of each PGS only. Main effects of PCAs not shown for clarity.

Those with high polygenic scores for ever being depressed for a whole week (PGS001898) also had a lower likelihood of being resilient although this was only statistically significant when war exposure was not controlled for (OR = 0.13, 95%CI [0.02, 0.86]; Table 4). While a stronger association was observed when using the matched sample (OR = 0.06, 95%CI [0.005, 0.86]), no evidence for an association was observed when excluding genetic outliers and related individuals (Table S4).

In contrast, genetic predisposition to phenotypes including previous feelings of depression (PGS001829), recent feelings of depression (PGS001899), depression related to a stressful or traumatic event (PGS002113), ever contemplating suicide or self-harm (PGS002222), or neuroticism symptom score (PGS001996) had no association with risk/resilience grouping in the primary analyses. These findings were similar in most sensitivity analyses although a genetic predisposition for ever contemplating suicide or self-harm (PGS002222) was associated with a greater likelihood of resilience when genetic outliers and related children were excluded (OR = 15,522, 95%CI [4.40, 54713255]).

When these analyses were repeated only considering resilience to internalizing disorders, the association with the polygenic score for recent feelings of foreboding (PGS001920) or ever being depressed for a whole week (PGS001898) were considerably weakened suggesting this finding may be more specific to externalizing symptoms, rather than resilience as a whole (OR = 0.26, 95%CI [0.01, 9.17]; OR = 0.25, 95%CI [0.05, 1.24]; Table S4). The remaining polygenic scores showed no association with resilience to internalizing symptoms.

Finally, we repeated the same analyses with only the oldest individuals as previous reports have suggested that polygenic scores derived from adult populations have higher predictive ability in adolescents compared to children. Polygenic scores for depression (PGS001829) or recent feelings of depression (PGS001899) were both associated with reduced resilience (OR = 0.01, 95%CI [0.0002, 0.31]; OR = 0.00000001, 95%CI [0, 0.07]; Table S4). In contrast, genetic predisposition to depression related to a stressful or traumatic event (PGS002113), ever being depressed for a whole week (PGS001898), ever contemplating suicide or self-harm (PGS002222), or neuroticism symptoms (PGS001996) was not associated with resilience in this older subset.

Interaction analyses

The previous set of analyses showed that while exposure to adversity or biomarkers thereof are associated with a degradation of resilience, genetic predisposition to poor mental health had little impact. However, there was some indication that when taking war exposure into account the association between the exposure of interest and resilience was altered. Therefore we next investigated the synergistic effects of the predictors on resilience.

We first investigated whether hair cortisol moderates the association between war exposure and resilience. We found no evidence for an interactive effect of war exposure and hair cortisol on resilience (OR = 0.95, 95%CI [0.89 1.01]; Table 5). Similar findings were observed when investigating resilience to just internalizing disorders and limiting analysis to those who were in the later stages of puberty (Table S5).

Table 5. The moderating relationship of cortisol and war exposure on resilience

Reference levels: aMale, bNo, cNo, dOnce per week, ePre-mid puberty, fOctober. Main effects of batches not shown for clarity.

Next, we investigated whether the polygenic scores for mental health outcomes moderate the association between war exposure and resilience. Again, we found no strong evidence for an interactive effect between war exposure and any polygenic score on resilience (Table S6). Finally we investigated whether the polygenic scores for mental health outcomes moderate the association between hair cortisol and resilience. While there was no evidence for a moderation effect involving most polygenic scores, the interaction between the polygenic score for depression (PGS001829) and hair cortisol was significantly associated with resilience (OR = 0.04, 95%CI [0.003 0.47]; Table 6). Further investigation of this interaction indicated that while those with a lower genetic predisposition for depression had little relationship between hair cortisol and resilience likelihood, those with a higher genetic predisposition had a stronger negative relationship between cortisol and likelihood of resilience (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Marginal estimates for the association between hair cortisol and probability of resilience for representative levels of polygenic risk for depression (PGS001829). Individuals one standard deviation above the mean polygenic risk for depression (PGS = 0.07; green line) were most likely to be resilient at low levels of cortisol secretion, but were the least likely to be resilient at high levels of secretion. Children with the mean polygenic risk (PGS = −0.04; blue line) displayed a more gradual decline in resilience probability with increasing cortisol secretion. Meanwhile, children one standard deviation below the mean polygenic risk (PGS = −0.16; red line) displayed a stable probability of resilience regardless of cortisol secretion. Shaded regions indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Table 6. The moderating relationship of cortisol and polygenic scores on resilience

Main effects of additional covariates not shown for clarity.

Discussion

A holistic appreciation of resilience requires an examination of the construct across multiple systems (individual, family, community, etc.), at different levels within each system (Ungar, Reference Ungar and Ungar2021b). Although psychosocial and environmental influences on resilience are well described, transdisciplinary investigations of biological-level resilience are only beginning to gather momentum (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019; Masten, Reference Masten2021). Building on our previous exploration of resilience in Syrian refugee children (Popham et al., Reference Popham, McEwen, Karam, Fayyad, Karam, Saab, Moghames and Pluess2022), we examined the potential of hair cortisol concentrations and polygenic scores for mental health outcomes to predict resilience to internalizing and externalizing mental health problems.

Regarding main effect analyses, war exposure was significantly associated with decreased resilience. Early experiences of trauma, such as war events, are known to significantly dysregulate childhood physiological development, hampering stress response and cognitive functioning, with downstream impacts on behavior regulation (De Bellis & Zisk, Reference De Bellis and Zisk2014), social competencies, attention, memory, and general academic performance (Dye, Reference Dye2018). Exposure to complex trauma compounds these issues, leading to greater symptom severity and complexity (Dye, Reference Dye2018). Such impairment at multiple physiological and cognitive levels will understandably diminish the capacity for resilience in children, particularly refugees, supporting our significant findings.

We also detected a significant association between elevated hair cortisol concentrations and decreased resilience, even when controlling for war exposure. Cortisol concentrations are a well-described proxy often used to gauge physiological stress responses (Steudte-Schmiedgen et al., Reference Steudte-Schmiedgen, Kirschbaum, Alexander and Stalder2016) and sampling hair allows the assessment of major events over the recent past (Karlén et al., Reference Karlén, Ludvigsson, Frostell, Theodorsson and Faresjö2011). Elevated cortisol levels have been reliably detected in other stressful contexts such as shift-work (Manenschijn et al., Reference Manenschijn, van Kruysbergen, de Jong, Koper and van Rossum2011), unemployment (Dettenborn et al., Reference Dettenborn, Tietze, Bruckner and Kirschbaum2010), and chronic pain (Van Uum et al., Reference Van Uum, Sauvé, Fraser, Morley-Forster, Paul and Koren2008). Similar to our findings, Ahrens et al. (Reference Ahrens, Neumann, von Werthern, Kranz, Kollmann, Mattes, Puhlmann, Weichert, Lutz, Basten, Fiebach, Wessa, Kalisch, Lieb, Chiocchetti, Tüscher, Reif and Plichta2022) discovered that elevated hair cortisol distinguished individuals suffering acute mental deterioration versus those more resilient following COVID-19 lockdown. Dajani et al. (Reference Dajani, Hadfield, van Uum, Greff and Panter-Brick2018) noted that cortisol hypersecretion characterized adolescent Syrian refugees who felt more fearful and insecure post-migration. However, to what extent hair cortisol reflects trait components (e.g., an inherited disposition toward elevated cortisol secretion) of stress response versus state influences (e.g. environmental stressors) is not fully apparent (Ahrens et al., Reference Ahrens, Neumann, von Werthern, Kranz, Kollmann, Mattes, Puhlmann, Weichert, Lutz, Basten, Fiebach, Wessa, Kalisch, Lieb, Chiocchetti, Tüscher, Reif and Plichta2022). Although evidence suggests hair cortisol is more reflective of strong trait elements (Stalder & Kirschbaum, Reference Stalder and Kirschbaum2012), concentrations remain reliably correlated to the severity of, and time since, traumatization (Steudte-Schmiedgen et al., Reference Steudte-Schmiedgen, Kirschbaum, Alexander and Stalder2016). In our study, hair cortisol did not moderate the effects of war exposure on resilience, possibly indicating less of a trait component, or one less detectable in young children. Instead, our results suggest that state influences including both earlier and current adversities may reduce childhood resilience. Nevertheless, hair cortisol levels may serve as a convenient measure that collates the effects of genetically predisposed traits with acute and chronic traumas, earmarking hair cortisol as a particularly promising biomarker (Steudte-Schmiedgen et al., Reference Steudte-Schmiedgen, Kirschbaum, Alexander and Stalder2016).

In contrast, polygenic risk for various mental health-related outcomes was not clearly associated with resilience. Although scores for recent feelings of foreboding (PGS001920) and ever feeling depressed for a whole week (PGS001898) exhibited promising links to resilience, these associations did not hold up under further scrutiny (multiple testing correction and sensitivity analyses). These findings were somewhat unsurprising, given that polygenic scores typically account for a small amount of phenotypic variation, especially for behavioral phenotypes which often have strong environmental components (Smeland & Andreassen, Reference Smeland and Andreassen2021). Moreover, polygenic scores are known to suffer deflation when applied to ethnicities other than those used to train the scoring algorithm (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Mak and O’Reilly2020). Scores used in our study (which were mostly trained on European data) were previously acknowledged to deflate by 28% on average when scoring individuals of Middle Eastern ancestry (Privé et al., Reference Privé, Aschard, Carmi, Folkersen, Hoggart, O’Reilly and Vilhjálmsson2022). Deflation is attributed to multiple issues, including differences in linkage disequilibrium patterns, allele frequencies, effect sizes, and environments.

Score deflation might have been further exacerbated in our study due to the age of participants. Only amongst an older subset of children was there some evidence that polygenic scores for selected phenotypes were associated with reduced resilience. This supports the idea that adult-based polygenic scores are not immediately valid for younger populations (Lange et al., Reference Lange, Kerr, Mansell, O’Sullivan, Burgner, Clifford, Olds, Dwyer, Wake and Saffery2022) and that genetic predisposition to poor mental health may not be identical between children and adolescents. Genotype expression likely changes in response to aging, with different genes influencing phenotypes across different age ranges (de Zeeuw et al., Reference de Zeeuw, van Beijsterveldt, Glasner, Bartels, Ehli, Davies, Hudziak, Groen-Blokhuis, Hottenga, de Geus and Boomsma2014). Given that an overwhelming majority of refugees are of non-European ancestry (Murray, Reference Murray2019), and almost half are below the age of 18, there is even greater impetus for future polygenic score development to be more inclusive of other ancestries and age ranges, especially if polygenic scores are to realize their potential for more personalized medicine and treatment (Smeland & Andreassen, Reference Smeland and Andreassen2021).

Due to the anticipated lack of main effects, we hypothesized that interaction effects between polygenic scores and environmental variables (war exposure, hair cortisol) would be more discernible. However, only one interaction, between hair cortisol and polygenic risk for depression (PGS001829), emerged as significant. Children with a higher genetic predisposition to depression were more likely to be classed as resilient at lower levels of hair cortisol, but the probability of resilience diminished appreciably as cortisol concentration increased. Associations and interactions between hair cortisol and polygenic scores are a recent area of interest in the literature (Ahrens et al., Reference Ahrens, Neumann, von Werthern, Kranz, Kollmann, Mattes, Puhlmann, Weichert, Lutz, Basten, Fiebach, Wessa, Kalisch, Lieb, Chiocchetti, Tüscher, Reif and Plichta2022; Bolhuis et al., Reference Bolhuis, Tiemeier, Jansen, Muetzel, Neumann, Hillegers, van den Akker, van Rossum, Jaddoe, Vernooij, White and Kushner2019; Rietschel et al., Reference Rietschel, Streit, Zhu, McAloney, Frank, Couvy-Duchesne, Witt, Binz, Hayward, Direk, Anderson, Huffman, Wilson, Campbell, Rudan, Wright, Hastie, Wild, Velders and Rietschel2017), driven by the appreciation that hair cortisol captures both heritable and environmentally influenced aspects of stress regulation. However, current results remain underwhelming. Rietschel et al. (Reference Rietschel, Streit, Zhu, McAloney, Frank, Couvy-Duchesne, Witt, Binz, Hayward, Direk, Anderson, Huffman, Wilson, Campbell, Rudan, Wright, Hastie, Wild, Velders and Rietschel2017) found no correlation between hair cortisol and polygenic scores for Neuroticism and Major Depressive Disorder. Similarly, there was no significant moderating effect of hair cortisol on compound sets of polygenic scores related to internalizing, psychotic, neurodevelopmental, or dysfunctional coping disorders in predicting mental health trajectories following COVID-19 lockdown (Ahrens et al., Reference Ahrens, Neumann, von Werthern, Kranz, Kollmann, Mattes, Puhlmann, Weichert, Lutz, Basten, Fiebach, Wessa, Kalisch, Lieb, Chiocchetti, Tüscher, Reif and Plichta2022). Bolhuis et al. (Reference Bolhuis, Tiemeier, Jansen, Muetzel, Neumann, Hillegers, van den Akker, van Rossum, Jaddoe, Vernooij, White and Kushner2019) did, however, identify a significant interaction between hair cortisol and a polygenic score for schizophrenia in predicting components of pre-adolescent brain structure, although this did not survive correction for multiple testing. Our results are potentially a further indication that high cortisol levels can trigger underlying genetic predispositions to mental illness, but this interaction might capture only a marginal difference in risk or resilience and awaits validation in independent studies.

Over 80% of refugee children are estimated to exhibit psychosocial problems that often stem from disrupted development (Fegert et al., Reference Fegert, Diehl, Leyendecker, Hahlweg, Prayon-Blum and Fegert2018), and these can strain the ability of supraordinate systems to confer supporting resilience. For example, caretakers may struggle to cope with the needs of traumatized children and may respond in ways that unintentionally exacerbate their weakened resilience (e.g., insensitive parenting, verbal threats, physical abuse) (Eruyar et al., Reference Eruyar, Maltby and Vostanis2018; Punamäki et al., Reference Punamäki, Qouta and Peltonen2018). The negative consequences of trauma exposure in childhood can persist well into adulthood, continuing to undermine a capacity for resilience (Dye, Reference Dye2018). Such long-term and knock-on effects across systems of resilience underpin the current emphasis on transdisciplinary investigations that can widen our understanding of promotive and predictive factors of resilience, especially within children (Masten, Reference Masten2021). Our study provides empirical evidence that elevated hair cortisol concentrations might be a useful biomarker for identifying refugee children at greater risk for mental health problems. Polygenic scores alone do not appear to be a similarly useful marker in this regard, aligning with their current lack of clinical utility (Smeland & Andreassen, Reference Smeland and Andreassen2021). However, combinations of markers such as hair cortisol and polygenic scores might hold some benefit, but further studies are required.

Limitations

Several limitations to our study should be noted. Firstly, our definition of resilience was based solely on the absence of diagnosable mental health problems (of three limited types), which simplifies a far more nuanced construct, likely limiting our power to detect associations. While we conducted follow-up analyses to disentangle the distinct associations with resilience to externalizing and internalizing problems, we acknowledge that our approach may be biasing results toward the more prevalent mental health problems within our sample. We were also unable to incorporate anxiety and other mental health problems into the resilience profiling, therefore we may be misclassifying children as resilient when in fact they might be experiencing significant mental distress or impairment.

Secondly, although our sample size was adequate for main effect analyses, it was appreciably underpowered to detect interactions, particularly those involving polygenic scores. This is likely exacerbated by the impaired predictive ability of polygenic scores derived from adult European populations in our younger Syrian cohort. The main effects of psychopathology-related polygenic scores were mostly insignificant, despite polygenic scores for height demonstrating a clear predictive ability in our sample. However, height has a large, stable genetic component (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Esko, Yang, Vedantam, Pers, Gustafsson, Chu, Estrada, Luan, Kutalik, Amin, Buchkovich, Croteau-Chonka, Day, Duan, Fall, Fehrmann, Ferreira, Jackson and Frayling2014), whereas resilience and mental health outcomes are more dynamic constructs with wider sets of environmental influences.

Lastly, due to the cross-sectional design of our analysis, no causal inferences can be made. These are critical if measures of stress responses like hair cortisol concentrations are to be used for prediction or stratification before the onset of mental health symptomology. Importantly, Dajani and colleagues (2018) found that measures of hair cortisol in highly traumatized adolescent Syrian refugees were dysregulated, showing no clear relationship across three time-points (2.8 and 11.3 months after baseline measures). The lack of correlation was ascribed to the complex change in cortisol production from hyper- to hyposecretion as stress shifts from acute to chronic – an occurrence typical for refugees. This phenomenon of "cortisol blunting" highlights the importance of longitudinal investigations, capable of delineating cortisol trajectories, that are more informative than single time-point assessments.

Conclusion

In conclusion, our study highlights the potential of biological markers (particularly hair cortisol) to augment the identification of at-risk and resilient refugee children. There remain, however, substantial complexities that need to be unpacked in future research (e.g., cortisol blunting; ethnicity-specific polygenic scores) before biological markers can serve as credible screening tools in refugee contexts. Moreover, the true utility of these markers may lie in discernible interaction effects, both between biological markers, and between biological markers and more distal systems of resilience. Early and objective identification of children’s degree of risk could ultimately improve the timing and efficacy of psychosocial interventions aimed at alleviating mental health burdens and strengthening multisystemic resilience.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000433.

Acknowledgements

The BIOPATH study was funded by the Eunice Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD; R01HD083387) and sponsored by Queen Mary University of London (QMUL). The funder and sponsor played no role in study design, in the collection, analysis or interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, or the decision to submit the article for publication. We warmly thank all participating families for their participation. We thank Fiona McEwen, Cassandra Popham, Claudinei Biazoli, and Patricia Moghames, and all other members of the BIOPATH team (https://www.qmul.ac.uk/sbbs/about-us/our-departments/psychology/global-mental-health/meet-the-team/) for their dedication, hard work, and insights. We thank the Barts and The London Genome Centre and UCL Genomics for conducting the DNA extraction and genotyping. We also thank Thu Chau and Meaghan Stolk for conducting the cortisol ELISA analysis.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Eunice Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (MP, grant number R01HD083387) and the UKRI Postdoc Guarantee Fellowship (AKM, grant number EP/X028690/1).

Competing interest

The author(s) declare none.

Footnotes

*

DS and AKM contributed equally to this paper.

References

Ahrens, K. F., Neumann, R. J., von Werthern, N. M., Kranz, T. M., Kollmann, B., Mattes, Börn, Puhlmann, L. M. C., Weichert, D., Lutz, B., Basten, U., Fiebach, C. J., Wessa, Mèle, Kalisch, R., Lieb, K., Chiocchetti, A. G., Tüscher, O., Reif, A., Plichta, M. M. (2022). Association of polygenic risk scores and hair cortisol with mental health trajectories during COVID lockdown. Translational Psychiatry, 12(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-02165-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amstadter, A. B., Myers, J. M., & Kendler, K. S. (2014). Psychiatric resilience: Longitudinal twin study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 205(4), 275280. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.130906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betancourt, T., Scorza, P., Meyers-Ohki, S., Mushashi, C., Kayiteshonga, Y., Binagwaho, A., Stulac, S., Beardslee, W. R. (2012). Validating the center for epidemiological studies depression scale for children in Rwanda. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(12), 12841292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.09.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bevans, K., Cerbone, A., & Overstreet, S. (2008). Relations between recurrent trauma exposure and recent life stress and salivary cortisol among children. Development and Psychopathology, 20(1), 257272. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, K., Tiemeier, H., Jansen, P. R., Muetzel, R. L., Neumann, A., Hillegers, M. H. J., van den Akker, E. T. L., van Rossum, E. F. C., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Vernooij, M. W., White, T., Kushner, S. A. (2019). Interaction of schizophrenia polygenic risk and cortisol level on pre-adolescent brain structure. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 101, 295303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.12.231 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctv26071r6 Google Scholar
Carlson, B. E., Cacciatore, J., & Klimek, B. (2012). A risk and resilience perspective on unaccompanied refugee minors. Social Work, 57(3), 259269. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/sws003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carskadon, M. A., & Acebo, C. (1993). A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14(3), 190195. https://doi.org/10.1016/1054-139X(93)90004-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C. C., Chow, C. C., Tellier, L. C., Vattikuti, S., Purcell, S. M., & Lee, J. J. (2015). Second-generation PLINK: Rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. GigaScience, 4(1), 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13742-015-0047-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. W., Mak, T. S.-H., & O’Reilly, P. F. (2020). Tutorial: A guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nature Protocols, 15(9), 27592772. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-020-0353-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, A., Blaustein, M., & van der Kolk, B. (2003). Complex trauma in children and adolescents. National Child Traumatic Stress Network, Retrieved from, http://www.NCTSNet.org Google Scholar
Dajani, R., Hadfield, K., van Uum, S., Greff, M., & Panter-Brick, C. (2018). Hair cortisol concentrations in war-affected adolescents: A prospective intervention trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 89(December 2017), 138146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.12.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bellis, M. D., & Zisk, A. (2014). The biological effects of childhood trauma. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(2), 185222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2014.01.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Zeeuw, E. L., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Glasner, T. J., Bartels, M., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Hudziak, J. J., Groen-Blokhuis, M. M., Hottenga, J. J., de Geus, E. J. C., Boomsma, D. I. (2014). Polygenic scores associated with educational attainment in adults predict educational achievement and ADHD symptoms in children. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 165(6), 510520. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32254 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dettenborn, L., Tietze, A., Bruckner, F., & Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Higher cortisol content in hair among long-term unemployed individuals compared to controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(9), 14041409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.04.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L. E., Ostacher, M., & Ballon, J. (2019). How genome-wide association studies (GWAS) made traditional candidate gene studies obsolete. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(9), 15181523. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-019-0389-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dye, H. (2018). The impact and long-term effects of childhood trauma. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 28(3), 381392. https://doi.org/10.1080/10911359.2018.1435328 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eruyar, S., Maltby, J., & Vostanis, P. (2018). Mental health problems of Syrian refugee children: The role of parental factors. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(4), 401409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1101-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etwel, F., Russell, E., Rieder, M. J., Van Uum, S. H., & Koren, G. (2014). Hair cortisol as a biomarker of stress in the 2011 Libyan war. Clinical and Investigative Medicine. Medecine Clinique Et Experimentale, 37(6), E403408. https://doi.org/10.25011/cim.v37i6.22245 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faulstich, M. E., Carey, M. P., Ruggiero, L., Enyart, P., & Gresham, F. (1986). Assessment of depression in childhood and adolescence: An evaluation of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC). The American Journal of Psychiatry, 143(8), 10241027. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.143.8.1024 Google Scholar
Feder, A., Fred-Torres, S., Southwick, S. M., & Charney, D. S. (2019). The biology of human resilience: Opportunities for enhancing resilience across the life span. Biological Psychiatry, 86(6), 443453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.07.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fegert, J. M., Diehl, C., Leyendecker, B., Hahlweg, K., Prayon-Blum, V., Fegert, J. M., the Scientific Advisory Council of the Federal Ministry of Family Affairs, S. C., Women and Youth (2018). Psychosocial problems in traumatized refugee families: Overview of risks and some recommendations for support services. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 12(1), 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-017-0210-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., Johnson, K. M., Feeny, N. C., & Treadwell, K. R. (2001). The child PTSD Symptom Scale: A preliminary examination of its psychometric properties. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(3), 376384. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3003_9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghubash, R., Daradkeh, T. K., Al Naseri, K. S., Al Bloushi, N. B. A., & Al Daheri, A. M. (2000). The performance of the Center for Epidemiologic Study Depression Scales (CES-D) in an Arab female community. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 46(4), 241249. https://doi.org/10.1177/002076400004600402 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafsson, P. E., Anckarsäter, H., Lichtenstein, P., Nelson, N., & Gustafsson, P. A. (2010). Does quantity have a quality all its own? Cumulative adversity and up- and down-regulation of circadian salivary cortisol levels in healthy children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(9), 14101415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, D., Imai, K., King, G., & Stuart, E. A. (2011). MatchIt: Nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. Journal of Statistical Software, 42(8), 128. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v042.i08 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodes, M., Jagdev, D., Chandra, N., & Cunniff, A. (2008). Risk and resilience for psychological distress amongst unaccompanied asylum seeking adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(7), 723732. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01912.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlén, J., Ludvigsson, J., Frostell, A., Theodorsson, E., & Faresjö, T. (2011). Cortisol in hair measured in young adults—A biomarker of major life stressors? BMC Clinical Pathology, 11(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6890-11-12 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazour, F., Zahreddine, N. R., Maragel, M. G., Almustafa, M. A., Soufia, M., Haddad, R., & Richa, S. (2017). Post-traumatic stress disorder in a sample of Syrian refugees in Lebanon. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 72, 4147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.09.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khera, A. V., Chaffin, M., Wade, K. H., Zahid, S., Brancale, J., Xia, R., Distefano, M., Senol-Cosar, O., Haas, M. E., Bick, A., Aragam, K. G., Lander, E. S., Smith, G. D., Mason-Suares, H., Fornage, M., Lebo, M., Timpson, N. J., Kaplan, L. M., Kathiresan, S. (2019). Polygenic prediction of weight and obesity trajectories from birth to adulthood. Cell, 177(3), 587596.e9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.03.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lange, K., Kerr, J. A., Mansell, T., O’Sullivan, J. M., Burgner, D. P., Clifford, S. A., Olds, T., Dwyer, T., Wake, M., Saffery, R. (2022). Can adult polygenic scores improve prediction of body mass index in childhood? International Journal of Obesity, 46(7), 13751383. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41366-022-01130-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdecke, D. (2018). ggeffects: Tidy data frames of marginal effects from regression models. The Journal of Open Source Software, 3(26), 772. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.00772 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, H., Hu, X., Liu, X., Ma, X., Guo, W., Qiu, C., Wang, Y., Wang, Q., Zhang, X., Zhang, W., Hannum, G., Zhang, K., Liu, X., Li, T. (2012). Hair cortisol level as a biomarker for altered hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in female adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder after the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake. Biological Psychiatry, 72(1), 6569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manenschijn, L., van Kruysbergen, R. G. P. M., de Jong, F. H., Koper, J. W., & van Rossum, E. F. C. (2011). Shift work at young age is associated with elevated long-term cortisol levels and body mass index. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 96(11), E1862E1865. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2011-1551 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maragel, M., & Manachi, S. (2018). The resilience of Syrian refugee children in Lebanon. In Syrian Refugee Children in the Middle East and Europe. Routledge.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2014). Global perspectives on resilience in children and youth. Child Development, 85(1), 620. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2021). Resilience in developmental systems: Principles, pathways, and protective processes in research and practice. In Multisystemic resilience: Adaptation and transformation in contexts of change (pp. 113134). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190095888.003.0007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maul, S., Giegling, I., Fabbri, C., Corponi, F., Serretti, A., & Rujescu, D. (2020). Genetics of resilience: Implications from genome-wide association studies and candidate genes of the stress response system in posttraumatic stress disorder and depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics: The Official Publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics, 183(2), 7794. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32763 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, F. M., Biazoli, C., Popham, C., Moghames, P., Saab, D., Fayyad, J., Karam, E., Bosqui, T., & Pluess, M. (2023). Prevalence and predictors of mental health problems in refugee children living in informal settlements in Lebanon. Nature Mental Health, 1, 135144. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44220-023-00017-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, F. M., Moghames, P., Bosqui, T., Kyrillos, V., Chehade, N., Saad, S., Rahman, D. A., Popham, C. M., Saab, D., Karam, G., Karam, E., & Pluess, M. (2021, June 14). Validating screening questionnaires for internalizing and externalizing disorders against clinical interviews in 8 to 17-year-old Syrian refugee children. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6zu87 Google Scholar
McEwen, F. S., Popham, C., Moghames, P., Smeeth, D., de Villiers, B., Saab, D., Karam, G., Fayyad, J., Karam, E., Pluess, M. (2022). Cohort profile: Biological pathways of risk and resilience in Syrian refugee children (BIOPATH). Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57(4), 873883. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-022-02228-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meiqari, L., Hoetjes, M., Baxter, L., & Lenglet, A. (2018). Impact of war on child health in northern Syria: The experience of Médecins Sans Frontières. European Journal of Pediatrics, 177(3), 371380. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-017-3057-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J. S. (2019). War and conflict: Addressing the psychosocial needs of child refugees. Journal of Early Childhood Teacher Education, 40(1), 318. https://doi.org/10.1080/10901027.2019.1569184 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, D. L., Jerman, P., Purewal Boparai, S. K., Koita, K., Briner, S., Bucci, M., & Harris, N. B. (2018). Review of tools for measuring exposure to adversity in children and adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 32(6), 564583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2018.04.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pieloch, K. A., McCullough, M. B., & Marks, A. K. (2016). Resilience of children with refugee statuses: A research review. Canadian Psychology / Psychologie Canadienne, 57(4), 330339. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000073 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popham, C. M., McEwen, F. S., Karam, E., Fayyad, J., Karam, G., Saab, D., Moghames, P., Pluess, M. (2022). The dynamic nature of refugee children’s resilience: A cohort study of Syrian refugees in Lebanon. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796022000191 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popham, C. M., McEwen, F. S., & Pluess, M. (2021). Psychological resilience in response to adverse experiences: An integrative developmental perspective in the context of war and displacement. In Ungar, M. (Eds.), Multisystemic resilience: Adaptation and transformation in contexts of change. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190095888.003.0022 Google Scholar
Privé, F., Aschard, H., Carmi, S., Folkersen, L., Hoggart, C., O’Reilly, P. F., & Vilhjálmsson, B. J. (2022). Portability of 245 polygenic scores when derived from the UK Biobank and applied to 9 ancestry groups from the same cohort. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 109(1), 1223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2021.11.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Punamäki, R.-L., Qouta, S. R., & Peltonen, K. (2018). Family systems approach to attachment relations, war trauma, and mental health among Palestinian children and parents. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 8(Suppl 7), 1439649. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2018.1439649 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S., Neale, B., Todd-Brown, K., Thomas, L., Ferreira, M. A. R., Bender, D., Maller, J., Sklar, P., de Bakker, P. I. W., Daly, M. J., Sham, P. C. (2007). PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81(3), 559575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pynoos, R. S., Frederick, C., Nader, K., Arroyo, W., Steinberg, A., Eth, S., Nunez, F., & Fairbanks, L. (1987). Life threat and posttraumatic stress in school-age children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44(12), 10571063. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800240031005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing 2013. http://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Rietschel, L., Streit, F., Zhu, G., McAloney, K., Frank, J., Couvy-Duchesne, B., Witt, S. H., Binz, T. M., Hayward, C., Direk, N., Anderson, A., Huffman, J., Wilson, J. F., Campbell, H., Rudan, I., Wright, A., Hastie, N., Wild, S. H., Velders, F. P., …, & Rietschel, M. (2017). Hair cortisol in twins: Heritability and genetic overlap with psychological variables and stress-system genes. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15351. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-11852-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D. V., Sheehan, K. H., Shytle, R. D., Janavs, J., Bannon, Y., Rogers, J. E., Milo, K. M., Stock, S. L., Wilkinson, B. (2010). Reliability and validity of the mini international neuropsychiatric interview for children and adolescents (MINI-KID). The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(3), 17393–326. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05305whi CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sirin, S. R., & Aber, J. L. (2018). Increasing understanding for Syrian refugee children with empirical evidence. Vulnerable Children and Youth Studies, 13(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.1080/17450128.2017.1409446 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirin, S., & Rogers-Sirin, L. (2015). The educational and mental health needs of Syrian refugee children.Google Scholar
Smeeth, D., McEwen, F. S., Popham, C. M., Karam, E. G., Fayyad, J., Saab, D., Rieder, M. J., Elzagallaai, A. A., van Uum, S., Pluess, M. (2023). War exposure, post-traumatic stress symptoms and hair cortisol concentrations in Syrian refugee children. Molecular Psychiatry, 28(2), 647656. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01859-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeland, O. B., & Andreassen, O. A. (2021). Polygenic risk scores in psychiatry – large potential but still limited clinical utility. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 51, 6870. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.05.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stalder, T., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012). Analysis of cortisol in hair – state of the art and future directions. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 26(7), 10191029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2012.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, M. B., Choi, K. W., Jain, S., Campbell-Sills, L., Chen, C-Yen, Gelernter, J., He, F., Heeringa, S. G., Maihofer, A. X., Nievergelt, C., Nock, M. K., Ripke, S., Sun, X., Kessler, R. C., Smoller, J. W., Ursano, R. J. (2019). Genome-wide analyses of psychological resilience in U.S. Army soldiers. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 180(5), 310319. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32730 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steudte-Schmiedgen, S., Kirschbaum, C., Alexander, N., & Stalder, T. (2016). An integrative model linking traumatization, cortisol dysregulation and posttraumatic stress disorder: Insight from recent hair cortisol findings. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 124135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tol, W. A., Song, S., & Jordans, M. J. D. (2013). Annual Research Review: Resilience and mental health in children and adolescents living in areas of armed conflict – a systematic review of findings in low- and middle-income countries. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(4), 445460. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12053 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungar, M. (2021a). Introduction why a volume on multisystemic resilience?. In Ungar, M. (Eds.), Multisystemic resilience: Adaptation and transformation in contexts of change. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190095888.003.0001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungar, M. (2021b). Modeling multisystemic resilience: Connecting biological, psychological, social, and ecological adaptation in contexts of adversity. In Ungar, M. (Eds.), Multisystemic resilience: Adaptation and transformation in contexts of change. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190095888.003.0002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Uum, S. H. M., Sauvé, B., Fraser, L. A., Morley-Forster, P., Paul, T. L., & Koren, G. (2008). Elevated content of cortisol in hair of patients with severe chronic pain: A novel biomarker for stress. Stress-the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 11(6), 483488. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890801887388 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, A. R., Esko, T., Yang, J., Vedantam, S., Pers, T. H., Gustafsson, S., Chu, A. Y., Estrada, K., Luan, J., Kutalik, Z., Amin, N., Buchkovich, M. L., Croteau-Chonka, D. C., Day, F. R., Duan, Y., Fall, T., Fehrmann, R., Ferreira, T., Jackson, A. U., … & Frayling, T. M. (2014). Defining the role of common variation in the genomic and biological architecture of adult human height. Nature Genetics, 46(11), 11731186. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3097 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheutlin, A. B., Dennis, J., Karlsson Linnér, R., Moscati, A., Restrepo, N., Straub, P., Ruderfer, D., Castro, V. M., Chen, C-Y., Ge, T., Huckins, L. M., Charney, A., Kirchner, H. L., Stahl, E. A., Chabris, C. F., Davis, L. K., Smoller, J. W. (2019). Penetrance and pleiotropy of polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia in 106,160 patients across four health care systems. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(10), 846855. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18091085 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Description of polygenic scores

Figure 1

Table 2. Sample description

Figure 2

Table 3. The relationship between cortisol and resilience

Figure 3

Table 4. The relationship between polygenic scores and resilience

Figure 4

Table 5. The moderating relationship of cortisol and war exposure on resilience

Figure 5

Figure 1. Marginal estimates for the association between hair cortisol and probability of resilience for representative levels of polygenic risk for depression (PGS001829). Individuals one standard deviation above the mean polygenic risk for depression (PGS = 0.07; green line) were most likely to be resilient at low levels of cortisol secretion, but were the least likely to be resilient at high levels of secretion. Children with the mean polygenic risk (PGS = −0.04; blue line) displayed a more gradual decline in resilience probability with increasing cortisol secretion. Meanwhile, children one standard deviation below the mean polygenic risk (PGS = −0.16; red line) displayed a stable probability of resilience regardless of cortisol secretion. Shaded regions indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 6

Table 6. The moderating relationship of cortisol and polygenic scores on resilience

Supplementary material: File

Smeeth et al. supplementary material

Smeeth et al. supplementary material
Download Smeeth et al. supplementary material(File)
File 41.4 KB