Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T19:08:54.817Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Academic Performance and Social Competence of Adolescents: Predictions based on Effortful Control and Empathy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2013

Juan P. Zorza
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Julián Marino
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Córdoba (Argentina)
Soledad de Lemus
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Alberto Acosta Mesas*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alberto Acosta Mesas. Departamento de Psicología Experimental. Facultad de Psicología. Campus Universitario de Cartuja, s/n. 18071. Granada (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study explored the predictive power of effortful control (EC) on empathy, academic performance, and social competence in adolescents. We obtained self-report measures of EC and dispositional empathy in 359 students (197 girls and 162 boys) aged between 12 and 14 years. Each student provided information about the prosocial behavior of the rest of his/her classmates and completed a sociogram. At the end of the school year, we calculated the mean grade of each student and the teacher responsible for each class completed a questionnaire on the academic skills of his/her students. The study confirmed the existence of a structural equation model (SEM) in which EC directly predicted academic performance and social competence. Additionally, empathic concern partially mediated the effect of EC on social competence. Finally, social competence significantly predicted academic performance. The article discusses the practical applications of the model proposed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2013 

In Western countries, academic performance and school cohabitation are two major concerns of scientists, citizens, and politicians. Reports are regularly published on students’ knowledge at the end of compulsory education (PISA, 2010) and school cohabitation (Díaz-Aguado, Martín, & Martínez, Reference Díaz-Aguado, Martín and Martínez2010). These topics have been explored from heterogeneous and multidisciplinary approaches. In the areas of psychology and neuroscience, cognitive development and socio-emotional development have been associated with cognitive control processes (Posner & Rothbart, Reference Posner and Rothbart2005), emotional self-regulation, and empathy (Decety & Svetlova, Reference Decety and Svetlova2012; Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010). There is empirical evidence that individual differences in attentional control influence students’ adaptation to both academic and social school demands (Checa, Rodríguez-Bailón, & Rueda, Reference Checa, Rodríguez-Bailón and Rueda2008) and that academic performance and socio-emotional adjustment can be improved by providing training in control and self-regulation skills (Rueda, Checa, & Rothbart, Reference Rueda, Checa and Rothbart2010).

Since children start attending school, they are continually expected to reach curricular goals and develop a broad range of skills that cannot be achieved without the mediation of the control processes that are part of the executive function. At the same time, in the context of interpersonal relationships with teachers, schoolmates, and parents, students experience many affective and emotional states. Such states must be properly managed and regulated according to the demands of the time and the normative frameworks of the group (Acosta, Reference Acosta and Soler Villalobos2008a). The transition from childhood to adolescence and later to maturity is characterized by the consolidation of personal autonomy and independence. All this progress is mediated by the strengthening of control skills (Crone, Reference Crone2009).

Many theorists and researchers have found a positive relationship between effortful control (EC) on one side and academic performance and good social adjustment on the other (see reviews by Liew, Reference Liew2012; Rueda et al., Reference Rueda, Checa and Rothbart2010). EC is a basic dimension of temperament that mediates between voluntary control of behavior and regulation of emotional reactivity (Derryberry & Rothbart, Reference Derryberry and Rothbart1997). It is related to the efficiency of executive attention in changing and focusing attention, inhibiting inappropriate behaviors (inhibitory control), activating or performing an action when there is a strong tendency to avoid it (activation control), and with information integration and action planning (Eisenberg, Valiente & Eggum, Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010). The processes involved in EC partially overlap with those involved in the executive function (Liew, Reference Liew2012). In fact, EC seems to be mainly supported by some regions of the prefrontal cortex — particularly the anterior cingulate cortex (Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Tang, Reference Posner, Rothbart, Sheese and Tang2007) — that are also part of the neural network that supports executive control.

Some studies have directly predicted academic performance and social behavior from EC. Liew, McTigue, Barrois, and Hughes (Reference Liew, McTigue, Barrois and Hughes2008) obtained measures of effortful control and academic performance from 6-year-old children in a longitudinal study performed during three consecutive years. They found positive correlations between control measures and literacy and math achievement in the first year. Checa et al. (Reference Checa, Rodríguez-Bailón and Rueda2008) reported that adolescents with better control resources also had higher academic performance, particularly in math, and were preferred by their classmates in sociometric tests. Cermakova, Moneta, and Spada (Reference Cermakova, Moneta and Spada2010) reported a relationship between self-reported attentional control measures and the learning strategies of a group of college students. They found that students with more control resources organized their time better and used a more elaborate and comprehensive strategy.

Other studies have also explored the possibility that direct relationships between EC and academic performance may be mediated by social behavior. Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson, and Reiser (Reference Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson and Reiser2008) assessed the social skills of children aged between 7 and 12 years based on their participation in class and their relationship with their teachers. They found that children’s social skills partially mediated the relationship between children’s EC and their mean academic grades at the beginning and the end of the school year. In a subsequent longitudinal study, Valiente et al. (Reference Valiente, Eisenberg, Haugen, Spinrad, Hofer and Liew2011) obtained data from a children sample at three different points in time. At the age of 6 years, they assessed children’s EC using parents’ reports and inhibitory control using a behavioral task. Two years later, they assessed children’s social functioning based on reports from parents and teachers. Finally, at the age of 12 years, they included academic performance in their assessments. Using a structural equation model, the authors confirmed that social functioning fully mediated the relationship between EC and academic performance. This relationship was not explained by any other variables such as sex, age, or socio-economic level.

All this evidence highlights the importance of control processes in academic performance and social competence throughout children’s development from childhood to late adolescence. However, other dispositions or skills are also particularly relevant in this type of predictions, particularly regarding social behavior. On the one hand, EC has also been associated with empathy (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Michalik, Spinrad, Hofer, Kupfer and Valiente2007; Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Cumberland and Losoya2004). On the other hand, numerous studies have associated empathy with the display of prosocial behavior and the maintenance of a good network of interpersonal relationships (De Waal, Reference De Waal2008; Rimé, Reference Rimé2009).

Empathy refers to individuals’ ability to understand and respond to the affective experiences of others (Decety & Jackson, Reference Decety and Jackson2004) and plays a central role in social relationships. It allows two people – or a group of people – to share their affects, understand each other’s feelings, show interest in each other’s concerns, and often help each other (Singer & Lamm, Reference Singer and Lamm2009). It involves a great deal of interrelated cognitive and affective processes (Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, Reference Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz and Perry2009). The most important of these are 1) empathic concern, which is probably shared with other species (Decety & Svetlova, Reference Decety and Svetlova2012) and mediates affective activation, and 2) perspective taking, which makes it possible to consider someone else’s point of view. All these processes are deployed when human beings establish links with other people, whether they are strangers, friends, classmates, colleagues, parents, or intimate partners, among others (Singer, Reference Singer, Paul, Colin, Ernst, Russell and Poldrack2009). Some of these processes are closely linked to cognitive control (Barkley, Reference Barkley2001). In an empathic situation, the distinction between ‘self’ and ‘other’ is maintained and evoked emotions are regulated to prevent emotional overreaction (Decety & Jackson, Reference Decety and Jackson2006). From a neuroanatomical point of view, empathic regulation processes are supported by connections among the orbitofrontal cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Decety, Reference Decety2011), which are also mobilized in other circumstances that require cognitive control.

Some studies have explored whether empathy is supported by cognitive control and self-regulation skills. Valiente et al. (Reference Valiente, Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Cumberland and Losoya2004) found a relationship between EC, dispositional empathy, and situational empathy and discomfort in children aged between 4 and 8 years exposed to video segments. They found that EC was negatively correlated with personal discomfort and positively correlated with children’s dispositional empathy. Eisenberg et al. (Reference Eisenberg, Michalik, Spinrad, Hofer, Kupfer and Valiente2007) obtained similar results in a longitudinal study with 6-year-old children assessed every two years, five times in total. In that study, high levels of EC were positively correlated with participants’ empathic responses and negatively correlated with participants’ impulsiveness.

There is abundant empirical evidence of the relationship between empathy, prosocial behavior, and social skills (Eisenberg, Eggum, & Di Giunta, Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010). This relationship has been found to persist throughout individuals’ development. Eisenberg et al. (Reference Eisenberg, Guthrie, Murphy, Shepard, Cumberland and Carlo1999) published a study in which participants were assessed from the ages of 4 to 22 years, approximately. At early ages, assessments were based on the observation of participants’ prosocial behaviors with their classmates and information provided by parents about such behaviors and children’s empathic skills. At later ages, the authors administered self-report questionnaires and obtained reports from classmates. Results showed a strong continuity in prosocial behavior over time and a close relationship between such behavior and empathy. Spinrad and Eisenberg (Reference Spinrad, Eisenberg, Gilman, Huebner and Furlong2009) also found that students who showed concern about the emotions of others and exhibited prosocial behaviors were better accepted by their schoolmates and more positively rated by adults.

There is currently a growing interest to identify whether the various components of empathy differentially influence social behavior (Batanova & Loukas, Reference Batanova and Loukas2011). Caravita, Di Blasio, and Salmivalli (Reference Caravita, Di Blasio and Salmivalli2009) found that the empathic concern of primary and secondary education students was positively correlated with helping behaviors toward victims of bullying, particularly among friends. By contrast, perspective taking in high-status students was associated in some cases with intimidating behaviors toward others. Jolliffe and Farrington (Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2006) conducted a study with secondary education students. They found that affective empathy, a construct that is equivalent to empathic concern but not to cognitive perspective taking, differentiated between abusive and non-abusive adolescents, regardless of sex. In that study, affective empathy was higher in non-abusive adolescents. Recently, Batanova and Loukas (Reference Batanova and Loukas2011) found that, in 14-year-old adolescents, high levels of empathic concern were negatively correlated with aggressive behaviors exhibited by them one year later. Perspective taking was positively correlated with subtle strategies of control and interpersonal dominance.

The studies described above suggest the existence of close relationships between EC on one side and empathy, academic performance, and social competence on the other. They also highlight the importance of empathic processes in social behavior. However, much less is known about the possible relationships between EC and empathy in the simultaneous prediction of academic performance and interpersonal relationships. EC-related skills are likely to have a general effect on empathy. Yet, the relationship between empathy and academic performance and the mediating effect of EC predictions on interpersonal relationships are not clear.

Eisenberg et al. (Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010) proposed two pathways of influence of control processes on academic performance. One pathway links them directly, while the other postulates that academic performance is indirectly influenced through social competence (see Figure 1 part A). On the one hand, effective control processes guarantee a good achievement of academic demands. On the other hand, relationships among peers and between students and teachers and the motivation to participate in school activities also lead to better academic performance when such cognitive control resources are present. Although empirical data (Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Eisenberg, Haugen, Spinrad, Hofer and Liew2011) confirmed the model in a longitudinal study with children aged between 6 and 12 years, it would be good to corroborate it at older ages or higher educational levels. The first years of secondary school play a key role in adolescents’ socialization and future academic performance (Wentzel & Caldwell, Reference Wentzel and Caldwell1997). Between the age of 10 and 12 years, interpersonal relationships increasingly revolve around the peer group and often prevail over family relations. At these ages, a great deal of emotional support comes from peers and personal identity becomes consolidated in the framework of these relationships (Parker & Asher, Reference Parker and Asher1993). During adolescence, the relationships between EC, academic performance, and social competence may be different from those observed at earlier ages, particularly the influence of social competence on academic performance. The links between peer relationships and academic performance may be closer during childhood than at later stages.

Figura 1. Proposal adapted from Eisenberg, Valiente, and Eggum (Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010) on the positive relationships between effortful control, social competence, and academic performance (part A). Model verified in this study (part B) including dispositional empathy as a mediator of the influence of effortful control on social competence.

Given the close relationships between EC and empathy as well as between empathy and prosocial behaviors and good interpersonal relationships, it seems appropriate to include empathy as another relevant construct in the prediction of such relationships. It is important to clarify whether the effects of EC on social competence require the presence of a high level of dispositional empathy or not. EC resources alone are likely to be insufficient to successfully predict social competence without the presence of empathy to link them to the interpersonal sphere.

The aim of this study was to test a predictive model of academic performance during adolescence (see Figure 1 part B). Based on the proposal made by Eisenberg, Spinrad et al. (Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010), we expected EC to predict academic performance both directly and through the mediation of social competence. Our main goal was to verify whether peer judgments of both interpersonal preference and prosocial behaviors are positively correlated with academic performance during adolescence, as has been observed at earlier ages. We also hypothesized that the influence of EC on social competence is mediated by adolescents’ dispositional empathy. Yet, as explained above, this relationship has been found to differ according to the affective and cognitive components of empathy (Batanova & Loukas, Reference Batanova and Loukas2011; Caravita et al., Reference Caravita, Di Blasio and Salmivalli2009; Jolliffe & Farrington, Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2006). We expected empathic concern to have greater predictive power than perspective taking. The following measures were obtained to test the model: self-report measures of EC and empathy, measures of social competence provided by classmates, and academic performance, computed from teachers’ reports and participants’ mean grades at the end of the school year (eight months after assessing EC and empathy).

It was decided to test the fit of the model with structural equation modeling (SEM). This method was expected to reveal the functional relationships among the variables and assess the equivalence between the covariance measures derived from the model and those obtained from the data (Bentler, Reference Bentler, Nesselroade and Catell1988). The aim was not to use such results to draw causal relationships among the variables. Instead, we intended to use the pattern of relationships confirmed among the variables to learn more about the constructs present in the model and to formulate recommendations aimed at improving educational intervention.

Method

Participants

The sample consisted of 359 students (197 girls and 162 boys) attending four public secondary education schools in the city of Granada, Spain. Participant age ranged between 12 and 14 years. Mean age was 12.47 years (SD = .73) for girls and 12.50 years (SD = .74) for boys. Students came from families of medium to low socio-economic and cultural status. Participation was voluntary, with the consent of parents.

Questionnaires and measures

Effortful control. EC was evaluated using the Early Adolescence Temperament Questionnaire – Revised Self Report (EATQ-R self report; Ellis & Rothbarth, Reference Ellis and Rothbart2001), translated into Spanish by Checa et al. (Reference Checa, Rodríguez-Bailón and Rueda2008). In the literature, this scale has been used with adolescent populations from the age of 10 (Ellis & Rothbart, Reference Ellis and Rothbart2001). The EATQ-R evaluates four factors of temperament in adolescents: ‘effortful control’, ‘extraversion/surgency’, ‘negative affect’, and ‘affiliation’. In this study, we used the total score of the 16 items that collected information about ‘effortful control’ (α = .69), composed of three sub-factors: ‘activation control’ (e.g., “If I have a hard assignment to do, I get started right away), ‘attention’ (e.g., “It is easy for me to really concentrate on homework problems”), and ‘inhibitory control’ (e.g., “I can stick with my plans and goals”). Items were rated on a Likert scale from 1 (almost always untrue of you) to 5 (almost always true of you). In the present study, internal consistency (α = .73) was similar to that reported in previous studies (Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson and Reiser2008).

Empathy. Empathy was assessed with the Spanish adaptation (Mestre, Frías, & Samper, Reference Mestre, Frías and Samper2004) of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis, Reference Davis1983). This is one of the scales most widely used in the literature as a measure of dispositional empathy. It includes 28 items grouped into four sub-factors: empathic concern, perspective taking, fantasy, and personal distress. As in previous studies (Batanova & Loukas, Reference Batanova and Loukas2011; Gini, Albiero, Benelli, & Altoè, Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoè2007), the present study included scores of perspective taking and empathic concern, the two dimensions that provide the most accurate and direct measurement of empathic resources. The factors fantasy and personal distress have sometimes led to ambiguous interpretations and show low correlations with behavioral and brain activation measures (Decety & Lamm, Reference Decety, Lamm, Decety and Ickes2009). Empathic concern evaluates whether an individual responds affectively to the emotional experience of others (e.g., “I often have tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me”). Perspective taking provides information about individuals’ cognitive ability to see something from someone else’s perspective in everyday situations (e.g., “I try to look at everybody’s side of a disagreement before I make a decision”). Items were rated on a Likert scale from 1 (does not describe me well) to 5 (describes me very well). In the Spanish version, Mestre et al. (Reference Mestre, Frías and Samper2004) confirmed the factor structure reported by Davis (Reference Davis1983), with internal consistency indices (Empathic Concern: α = .65; Perspective Taking: α = .56) that were similar to those of the present study (Empathic Concern: α = .59; Perspective Taking: α = .63).

Prosocial behaviors. Following the proposal made by López (Reference López2006), an adapted sociogram was used to obtain information about students’ prosocial behaviors. Each participant evaluated the extent to which his/her classmates helped him/her in various situations (i.e., schoolwork, sadness or concern, conflict with other schoolmates). Responses were provided on a Likert scale from 1 (not helpful) to 5 (very helpful).

Social preference. This variable was assessed using the following sociometric procedure: from their class list, students selected the three classmates they would most like to carry out various school or leisure activities with and the three classmates they would least like to carry out these activities with. Following the method proposed by Newcomb, Bukowski, and Patte (Reference Newcomb, Bukowski and Pattee1993), each participant’s number of positive and negative nominations was divided by the number of students in his/her class. The index of social preference was obtained by subtracting the adjusted score of negative nominations from that of positive nominations. To reduce the number of decimals, 10 was added to the result.

Academic skills. Teachers completed the Escala de Valoración del Profesor I-S (I-S Teacher Assessment Scale) (Clemente, Albiñana, & Doménech, Reference Clemente, Albiñana and Doménech1999), composed of 24 items assessing students’ socialization, respect for authority, and intelligence. Each teacher rated his/her students on a scale from 1 (minimum) to 10 (maximum). This study only includes the measures of the sub-factor intelligence, which assesses teachers’ perception of their students’ academic skills and school readiness (e.g., “their ability to implement what has been learned”). The internal consistency of this factor was very high both in the study sample (α = .98) and that initially used for the original validation of the questionnaire.

Mean grades. Academic performance was measured by calculating each student’s mean grades in June, at the end of the school year. The remaining measures were obtained about eight months earlier during the same school year. Footnote 1

Procedure

Participants were given a booklet in which the self-report questionnaires were presented in random order. They were informed that the data would be treated confidentially and were given verbal and written instructions on how to complete each questionnaire. The booklets were completed during school hours in a group session that lasted about 40 to 50 minutes in the first term of the school year (October and early November). At the end of the school year, the school management provided a list the final grades of each participant and the teacher responsible for each class completed the scale that assessed students’ academic skills.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Participants’ means and standard deviations for each variable are shown in Table 1. Results of the correlation analysis among them are shown in Table 2. EC was positively correlated with all the other variables, although the highest correlation was found with academic performance and the lowest correlation was found with the index of social preference. Empathy scores also showed positive relationships with academic performance and peer judgments of prosocial behavior. Such judgments and social preference scores were also positively correlated with academic performance scores. As expected, a positive high correlation was found between students’ mean grades and their academic skills as rated by their teachers. These correlation patterns suggested that it was appropriate to test the mediation relationships proposed in the model.

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of the measures obtained in the study as a function of gender

Table 2. Pearson correlations among the measures obtained in the study

* p < .05.**p < .01.

Evaluation of the models

The predictions of the model were tested with structural equation modeling (SEM) using the AMOS statistical package, version 18.0. The high positive correlations found between students’ mean grades and their academic skills as rated by their teachers justified the creation of a latent variable for academic performance. Applying the same criterion, another latent variable was created for social competence, composed of peer judgments of prosocial behavior and the index of social preference. Moreover, given that perspective taking and empathic concern are related factors of empathy, a single multidimensional construct, and their measure errors may be related, we added a covariance between the error terms of these variables. The Kolmogorov Smirnov (K-S) test was performed for each of the variables in the model to determine whether the assumption of normality was met. All values were non-significant, which indicates that the assumption was met.

Results of the analysis of Model 1 indicated adequate fit, χFootnote 2 = 98.826, df = 10, p = .001; χ2 /df = 9.887; RMSEA = .158 (PCLOSE = .000), CFI = .902, NFI = .893. As expected, a significant relationship was found between the error terms of perspective taking and empathic concern. As hypothesized, EC positively and significantly predicted academic performance (β = .33, p < .001), perspective taking (β = .31, p < .001; R 2 = .10), and empathic concern (β = .20, p < .001; R 2 = .04). The prediction that social competence would influence academic performance was confirmed (β = .43, p < .001). Students whose prosocial behaviors were positively rated by their peers and who were chosen by them to perform various school-related activities had higher academic performance. In addition, the predictive relationships of the model revealed the influence of empathy on social competence. social competence was predicted by students’ empathic concern (β = .20, p < .005) but not by perspective taking, the cognitive dimension of empathy (β = .11). In Model A, EC and dispositional empathy predicted 7% of the variance in social competence, which in turn predicted 32% of the variance in academic performance.

The evaluation of the model’s modification indices suggested a possible direct relationship between EC and the components of the latent variable social competence. EC is likely to influence social competence not only indirectly through the mediation of empathy but also directly. As explained in the introduction, numerous studies have provided evidence of the positive relationship between EC on one side and social skills and a decrease of behavioral problems among children and adolescents on the other (Eisenberg, Spinrad et al., Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010). Students with high levels of EC are likely to have better school adjustment and be more positively rated by their peers. For these reasons, a parameter considering the direct influence of EC on social competence was included in Model 2. Results of the analysis confirmed that EC directly predicted social competence (β = .19, p < .005), empathic concern (β = .20, p < .001; R 2 = .04), and perspective taking (β = .31, p < .001; R 2 = .04), and academic performance was still influenced by EC (β = .31, p < .001) and social competence (β = .42, p < .001) (see Figure 2). In addition, they showed a better fit of the model, χ Footnote 2 = 6.034, df = 9, p = .736; χ2 /df = .670; RMSEA = .000 (PCLOSE = .972), CFI = 1.000, NFI = .993, which explained 10% of the variance in social competence and 34% of the variance in academic performance. A comparison of the models using the chi-square difference test, ∆χFootnote 2 = 10.674, p < .001, confirmed that Model 2 had better goodness-of-fit indices. Both models were also compared using the Akaike information criterion (AIC, Akaike, Reference Acosta and Soler Villalobos1974). The second had a lower AIC than the first one, which shows it had a better fit. Footnote 2

Figura 2. Verified structural equation model considering the influence of effortful control, empathic concern, perspective taking, and social competence on academic performance. χ Footnote 2 = 6.03, df = 9, p = .74; χ 2 /df = .67; RMSEA = .000 (PCLOSE = .97), CFI = 1.000, NFI = .99. The model explained 34 % of the variance in Academic Performance.

Pr: Peer judgments of prosocial behavior. Pre: Index of social preference. Sk: Academic skills assessed by the teacher. Mg: Mean grade of all the subjects taken by the student.

* p < 0.05.**p < 0.01.

Finally, given the direct influence of EC on social competence and the low predictive power of the latter on perspective taking and empathic concern, we wanted to verify whether the predictive power and fit of the model were adequate when the relation between EC and dispositional empathy was not considered. Consequently, the correlation parameters between EC and the measures of dispositional empathy were removed from Model 3. Results of the analysis showed inadequate fit indices, χ Footnote 2 = 42.755, df = 11, p = .001; χ2 /df = 3.887; RMSEA = .090 (PCLOSE = .010), CFI = .965, NFI = .954, AIC = 90.755, confirming the need to maintain the relationship between EC and dispositional empathy.

In short, Model 2 (see Table 3) had the best fit indices. The hypothesis that EC has a positive influence on academic performance both directly and indirectly through the mediation of social competence was confirmed in this model. In addition, EC seems to influence social competence both directly and through the mediation of empathic concern (see Figure 2). To assess the mediation of empathic concern, we assessed the indirect conditional effect of EC on social competence using the bias corrected and accelerated (BCa) Bootstrapping method with 2000 resamples (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2008). The effect of EC on empathic concern (a path) was β =.64, p < .001, t = 3.811 (p < .001, df = 350), the direct effect of empathic concern on social competence (b path) was β = .03, p < .001, t = 3.115 (p < .005, df = 350), the total effect of EC on social competence (c path) was β = .12, p < .001, t = 3.660 (p < .0005, df = 350), and the direct effect of EC on social competence (c’ path) was β = .09, p < .005, t = 3.009 (p < .005, df = 350). The Sobel test, which assumes normality for indirect effects, indicated significant values for the ab path, z = 2.45, p < 0.01, whereas the BCa method obtained a significant confidence interval, between 0.009 and 0.04 (95%). These results suggest the existence of partial mediation. Although the total effect was low, it remained significant when controlled by the mediator. This confirms that EC has direct and indirect effects (mediated by empathic concern) on the prediction of social competence in adolescents.

Table 3. Fit indices for each model assessed with structural equation modeling

Note

df = Degrees of freedom; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; NFI = Bentler-Bonett Normed Fit Index; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; AIC = Akaike information criterion. Δχ2 decrease in Model 2 compared to models 1 and 3, respectively.

* p < .05.

A multi-group SEM was performed to assess whether gender was a moderator in Model 2. This was done because some differences were found between the mean scores of boys and girls and there is controversy about the influence of gender on academic performance (Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson and Reiser2008). We computed another model where we forced the loadings of each factor, regressions, and covariates so that they would be the same in both groups. There is no moderation if an equivalence is found between boys and girls (Byrne, Reference Byrne2001). The resulting model showed adequate fit, χ2 = 31.280, df = 28, p = .305; χ 2 /df = 1.117; RMSEA = .018 (PCLOSE = .974), CFI = .996, NFI = .965. The difference in χ2 (15.711), with 10 degrees of freedom, was not significant (p = .108), which shows that the proposed model is fit both for girls and boys between 12 and 14 years.

Discussion

Results confirmed the influence of cognitive control processes on academic skills and performance (Eisenberg, Valiente, & Eggum, Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010; Rueda et al., Reference Rueda, Checa and Rothbart2010). Most research in this area has focused on children or has taken the form of longitudinal studies with children from 6 to 12 years (Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Eisenberg, Haugen, Spinrad, Hofer and Liew2011), neglecting older ages or higher education levels. One of our main objectives was to find out whether EC remains a direct determining factor on the academic performance of adolescents from 12 to 14 years attending secondary education. This was confirmed by our results. We also wanted to explore whether the indirect influence of EC through social competence is mediated by dispositional empathy. This hypothesis was also confirmed.

The relationship between voluntary control of behavior and good academic performance in adolescents may be the result of two factors: EC promotes the skills needed for good learning but also encourages behaviors that facilitate an adequate response to academic demands. Attentional control, one of the components of EC, promotes the cognitive processes that are necessary for good learning. Students’ ability to focus on the relevant aspects of academic contents as well as their flexibility when manipulating mental representations and performing complex reasoning tasks is likely to facilitate their understanding and performance at school (Rueda et al., Reference Rueda, Checa and Rothbart2010).

Attentional control is known to influence cognitive processes that are key for learning. In addition, we also believe adolescents high in EC have good academic performance because they have good academic habits and ongoing motivation for achievement. It should be noted that the present study not only assessed academic performance using students’ mean grades but included a measure of the teacher’s opinion on each student’s academic skills. This ensured a broader and multi-dimensional assessment of academic performance, as recommended in this type of studies (Eisenberg, Valiente et al., Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010). In the questionnaire, the teacher reported whether the student paid attention in class, did his/her homework, and had a good disposition for learning, among other issues. Results showed a positive correlation between EC and all these behaviors. Activation control and inhibitory control, two components of EC, are key for students to be able to organize, plan, and maintain the behaviors that contribute to good learning and performance, and to inhibit their tendency to exhibit distractive behaviors. The literature has reported that students with adequate EC levels respond positively to school demands, use better learning strategies (Cermakova et al., Reference Cermakova, Moneta and Spada2010), participate in class, and are more committed to school (Eisenberg, Valiente et al., Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010; Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson and Reiser2008). By contrast, students low in EC often cannot adapt to the rules and functioning of schools, increase the negative perception of their performance, lose motivation, and exhibit disruptive behaviors (Eisenberg, Valiente et al., Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010).

As hypothesized, supporting the model proposed by Eisenberg et al. (Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010), we obtained evidence that the effect of EC on academic performance is not only direct but also indirect and mediated by social competence. Recent studies with children have confirmed that EC leads to better academic performance by improving students’ interpersonal relationships with peers and teachers and increasing their participation in class (Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson and Reiser2008). Our study obtained evidence that, during early adolescence, EC also predicts prosocial behaviors and peer acceptance and social competence also plays a relevant role in the prediction of academic performance. Control resources are likely to facilitate the compliance of relationship rules of the peer group and the school itself, contributing to maintaining the links between adolescents and academic interest. Our results agree with those of other studies (Wentzel, Reference Wentzel2003) that have shown a better performance in students who are accepted and feel they are members of the peer group than those who are ignored or rejected. Group exclusion or problems with peer relationships can have a negative influence on students’ interest and participation in class, reducing their academic motivation.

Our results partially confirm the prediction that dispositional empathy mediates the effect of EC on social behavior. The model with the best fit showed a direct influence of EC on social competence and an indirect influence mediated by empathy. The fit disappeared when the relationship between EC and empathy was not considered (Model 3). This shows the need to consider the relationship between EC and dispositional empathy in the prediction of students’ social behavior. This relationship confirms that control and empathic processes are jointly mobilized in the interpersonal domain (Decety & Svetlova, Reference Decety and Svetlova2012; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Michalik, Spinrad, Hofer, Kupfer and Valiente2007; Lamm, Batson, & Decety, Reference Lamm, Batson and Decety2007). Emotional self-regulation may also be of great relevance in this framework of relationships (Eisenberg & Eggum, Reference Eisenberg, Eggum, Decety and Ickes2009; Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Batson and Decety2007). Future studies should provide more insight on this matter.

As hypothesized and in accordance with previous studies (Caravita et al., Reference Caravita, Di Blasio and Salmivalli2009; Gini et al., Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoè2007), empathic concern was found to significantly predict social competence, whereas perspective taking did not. The ability to adopt the perspective of others and understand their affects does not necessarily lead to prosocial behaviors (Jolliffe & Farrington, Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2006; Batanova & Loukas, Reference Batanova and Loukas2011). These results should be used when developing intervention strategies to prevent violence and bullying. Promoting only the cognitive elements of empathy without getting students to experiment emotional states does not seem to be enough to foster non-violent or prosocial behaviors.

Results indicate that predictive relationships are not moderated by gender. Data on EC were similar to those obtained by Valiente et al. (Reference Valiente, Eisenberg, Haugen, Spinrad, Hofer and Liew2011). These authors also found that the effect of EC on academic performance and social competence was independent of student gender. As regards empathy, the literature has often attributed greater empathic resources and a closer relationship between dispositional empathy and prosocial behavior to older girls (Wentzel, Baker, & Russell, Reference Wentzel, Baker, Russell, Gilman and Furlong2009). This data pattern was also observed in our sample. Girls obtained slightly higher scores in empathy and prosocial behavior than boys. However, the model was not moderated by this variable. Relationships among empathy, interpersonal behavior, and gender are complex (Decety & Svetlova, Reference Decety and Svetlova2012). Different results have been reported depending on the age of participants and the method use to assess empathy (Garaigordobil, Reference Garaigordobil2009) or the relevant interpersonal behavior measured. Jolliffe and Farrington (Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2006) found that adolescent girls with low empathy scores exhibited indirect bullying behaviors, but this relationship was not observed in boys. In boys, lower affective empathy resources were associated with direct physical or verbal abuse. From a practical perspective, this finding suggests that interventions aimed at strengthening positive interpersonal relationships among adolescents should be based on promoting empathy, particularly its affective elements, but also general behavioral control skills. Among boys, who usually have more limited empathic resources than girls, activities and habits aimed at consolidating these control processes would be particularly valuable.

One of the limitations of our study is the fact that the EC and empathy measures were only assessed with self-reports. As far as possible, these processes and skills should be assessed with behavioral measures and information provided by peers, family members, and/or teachers to ensure they have greater validity and robustness. In any case, the correlation patterns among these measures and between them and measures of academic performance and social competence as well as the results of the models compared granted them considerable robustness.

Results of this study can be useful to design intervention programs aimed at improving academic performance and school cohabitation. Given the relevance of EC in the prediction of academic performance, we recommend that students with attention difficulties receive training to strengthen attentional control and executive functions. Various studies have found improvements in concentration and intelligence after such interventions (Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg, Reference Klingberg, Forssberg and Westerberg2002). Moreover, training should begin at an early age (Rueda et al., Reference Rueda, Fan, McCandliss, Halparin, Gruber and Lercari2004). As discussed by Posner and Rothbart (Reference Posner and Rothbart2005), the executive attention network plays a key role in school activities and demands. Yet, this relationship often goes unnoticed. Families and teachers should systematize and adjust their demands to ensure that, from an early age, children start to develop the habits and skills that make up the executive functions and provide individuals with the necessary resources to tackle school demands successfully.

Furthermore, the relationship between EC and empathy and the influence of both on social competence and of the latter on academic performance suggest that interventions that address these three areas are better than those restricted to one of them. Self-regulation should be promoted not only from an intra-individual approach but also in social situations that allow including empathic processes and interpersonal demands (Wentzel et al., Reference Wentzel, Baker, Russell, Gilman and Furlong2009). Social behavior and personal objectives gradually develop around the acquisition of solid and stable moral criteria based on the ability to understand one’s own emotions and thoughts as well as those of others (Eisenberg, Reference Eisenberg2000). Experiences of blame and shame and their good management in the interpersonal domain and play a key role in regulation of social behavior, compliance with regulatory frameworks, and the prevention of violent behaviors (Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, Reference Tangney, Stuewig and Mashek2007) and require empathic processes. Individuals’ ability to manage emotional and behavioral responses in harmony with the needs of others and the compromises of being part of a group are necessary to achieve the “moral reciprocity” required for a peaceful coexistence (Ortega, Reference Ortega2007).

Programs specifically aimed at promoting empathy and self-regulation (Solomon, Battistich, Watson, Schaps, & Lewis, Reference Solomon, Battistich, Watson, Schaps and Lewis2000; Spinrad & Eisenberg, Reference Spinrad, Eisenberg, Gilman, Huebner and Furlong2009) have sometimes been opposed to those that recommend more comprehensive educational actions addressing these issues as well as academic ones (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Eggum and Di Giunta2010). Our results suggest that a framework of positive interpersonal relationships and a good atmosphere in class and at school contribute to good academic performance. Schools should systematically try to ensure the academic curriculum is met and promote a good social climate that facilitates its achievement. It seems unnecessary to focus concerns exclusively on progress in declarative knowledge about the various subjects. It is also important to provide spaces and resources for socio-emotional learning. This can accommodate recent concerns about the acquisition of emotion understanding and regulating skills as well as more traditional activities dealing with academic motivation, democratic management of rules and values, and regulation of interpersonal conflict (Acosta, Reference Acosta and Acosta2008b).

Finally, the results of the study support the development of educational actions promoting the use of autonomy, independence, and self-control resources to maintain a positive framework of interpersonal relationships and social climate in the classroom and promote good academic performance as well.

Footnotes

This research was funded by the Junta de Andalucía (Spain) through a research project (P07-SEJ-03299) awarded to the last author.

1 To verify that the mean grade was a consistent index of performance that did not fluctuate depending on the school or the teachers, direct scores in each subject were transformed into a z scale and the mean grade was obtained in this scale. Correlations between the mean of the direct grades used in the study and the mean grades on the z scale were .843. Moreover, the correlations between grades in some subjects such as Natural Science (.889), Social Science (.866), Spanish (.869) and Math (.875) transformed into the z scale and direct mean grades of all subjects were also very high.

2 In this model, predictions of Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern on Social Competence were independent. These two measures had moderate reliability, partly because of the formative nature of the scales. Therefore, to ensure the consistency of the prediction of Empathy on Social Competence, a latent variable was created for empathy, composed of these two measures. The results of the analysis also confirmed the adequate fit of the model, χ 2 = 10,58, df = 10, p = .39; χ2 /df = 1.05; RMSEA = .01 (PCLOSE = .88), CFI = .99, NFI = .98.

References

Acosta, A. (2008a). La educación de los sentimientos y la convivencia escolar [Education of feelings and school life]. In Soler Villalobos, M. (Ed.), De la Educación Socioemocional a la Educación en Valores [From socioemotional education to ethic education] (pp. 3152). Madrid, Spain: Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia.Google Scholar
Acosta, A. (2008b). La educación emocional y la convivencia en el aula [Emotional education and coexistence in the clasroom]. In Acosta, A. (Coord.), Educación emocional y convivencia en el aula [Socioemotional education and cohabitation in classroom] (pp. 1130). Madrid, Spain: Ministerio de Educación.Google Scholar
Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19, 716723. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705 Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1009085417776 Google Scholar
Batanova, M. D., & Loukas, A. (2011). Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents: Examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 15341543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10964-011-9634-x Google Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1988). Causal modeling via structural equation systems. In Nesselroade, J. R. & Catell, R. B. (Eds.), Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology, (2 nd ed. pp. 317335). New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, B. (2001). Structural equation modeling with Amos: Basic concepts, applications and programming. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Social Development, 18, 140163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00465.x Google Scholar
Cermakova, L., Moneta, G. B., & Spada, M. M. (2010). Dispositional flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying during academic examination preparation. Educational Psychology, 30, 495511.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080%2F01443411003777697 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clemente, A., Albiñana, P., & Doménech, F. (1999). Escala de Valoración del Profesor I-S [Teacher Assessment Scale I-S]. Anales de Psicología, 15, 233238.Google Scholar
Crone, E. A. (2009). Executive functions in adolescence: Inferences from brain and behavior. Developmental Science, 12, 825830. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00918.x Google Scholar
Checa, P., Rodríguez-Bailón, R., & Rueda, M. R. (2008). Neurocognitive and temperamental systems of self-regulation and early adolescents’ social and academic outcomes. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2, 177187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2008.00052.x Google Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2F0022-3514.44.1.113 Google Scholar
De Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625 Google Scholar
Decety, J. (2011). Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emotion Review, 3, 92108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073910374662 Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews, 3, 71100.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187 Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2006). A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 5458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2009). Empathy versus personal distress: Recent evidence from social neuroscience. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 199215). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Svetlova, M. (2012). Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003 Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633652.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017%2FS0954579497001375 Google Scholar
Díaz-Aguado, M., Martín, J., & Martínez, R. (2010). Estudio estatal sobre la convivencia escolar en la educación secundaria obligatoria. [National study on school cohabitation in secondary schools]. Madrid, Spain: Ministerio de Educación. Retrieved from http://sede.educacion.gob.es/publiventa/detalle.action?cod=13567 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146%2Fannurev.psych.51.1.665 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Eggum, N. D. (2009). Empathic responding: Sympathy and personal distress. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., & Di Giunta, L. (2010). Empathy-related responding: Associations with prosocial behavior, aggression, and intergroup relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 4, 143180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1751-2409.2010.01020.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of prosocial dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 70, 13601372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111%2F1467-8624.00100 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Michalik, N., Spinrad, T. L., Hofer, C., Kupfer, A., & Valiente, C. (2007). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children's sympathy: A longitudinal study. Cognitive Development, 22, 544567. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cogdev.2007.08.003 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Self-regulation and school readiness. Early Education and Development, 21, 681698.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080%2F10409289.2010.497451 Google Scholar
Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001, October). Revision of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire. Poster presented at the 2001 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoè, G. (2007). Does empathy predict adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20204 Google Scholar
Garaigordobil, M. (2009). A comparative analysis of empathy in childhood and adolescence: Gender differences and associated socio-emotional variables. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 217235.Google Scholar
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2006). Examining the relationship between low empathy and bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 540550. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20154 Google Scholar
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 781791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076%2Fjcen.24.6.781.8395 Google Scholar
Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 4258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42 Google Scholar
Liew, J. (2012). Effortful control, executive functions, and education: Bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 105111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x Google Scholar
Liew, J., McTigue, E. M., Barrois, L., & Hughes, J. N. (2008). Adaptive and effortful control and academic self-efficacy beliefs on achievement: A longitudinal study of 1st through 3rd graders. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 515526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, F. (2006). El bienestar personal y social y la prevención del malestar y la violencia [Personal and social wellbeing and prevention of discomfort and violence] . Madrid, Spain: Pirámide.Google Scholar
Mestre, V., Frías, M. D., & Samper, P. (2004). La medida de la empatía: Análisis del interpersonal reactivity index [Measuring empathy: Analysis of the interpersonal reactivity index], Psicothema, 16, 255260.Google Scholar
Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children's peer relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 99128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2F0033-2909.113.1.99 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortega, R. (2007). La convivencia: Un regalo de la cultura a la escuela [Cohabitation: A gift from culture to schools]. Revista de Educación de Castilla la Mancha, 4, 5054.Google Scholar
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 611621. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.29.4.611 Google Scholar
PISA. OECD. (2010). Programa para la evaluación internacional de los alumnos [Programme for International Student Assessment]. Spanish Repport. Madrid, Spain: Ministerio de Educación. Retrieved from http://iaqse.caib.es/documents/aval2009-10/pisa2009-informe-espanol.pdf Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). Influencing brain networks: Implications for education. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 99103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.tics.2005.01.007 Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Tang, Y. (2007). The anterior cingulate gyrus and the mechanism of self-regulation. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 391395. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758%2FCABN.7.4.391 Google Scholar
Preacher, K., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/brm.40.3.879 Google Scholar
Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1, 6085. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073908097189 Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Checa, P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2010). Contributions of attentional control to socioemotional and academic development. Early Education and Development, 21, 744764.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2010.510055 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., & Lercari, L. P. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 10291040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012 Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132, 617627. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn279 Google Scholar
Singer, T. (2009). Understanding others: Brain mechanisms of theory of mind and empathy. In Paul, W. G., Colin, F. C., Ernst, F., Russell, P., & Poldrack, A. (Eds.), Neuroeconomics (pp. 251268). London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 8196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, D., Battistich, V., Watson, M., Schaps, E., & Lewis, C. (2000). A six-district study of educational change: Direct and mediated effects of the child development project. Social Psychology of Education, 4, 351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1009609606692 Google Scholar
Spinrad, T., & Eisenberg, N. (2009). Empathy, prosocial behavior, and positive development in schools. In Gilman, R., Huebner, E., & Furlong, M. (Eds.), Handboof of Positive Psychology in Schools (pp. 119131). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146%2Fannurev.psych.56.091103.070145 Google Scholar
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A., & Losoya, S. H. (2004). Prediction of children's empathy-related responding from their effortful control and parents' expressivity. Developmental Psychology, 40, 911926. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.40.6.911 Google Scholar
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Haugen, R., Spinrad, T. L., Hofer, C., & Liew, J. (2011). Children's effortful control and academic achievement: Mediation through social functioning. Early Education and Development, 22, 411433. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2010.505259 Google Scholar
Valiente, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Swanson, J., & Reiser, M. (2008). Prediction of children's cademic competence from their effortful control, relationships, and classroom participation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 6777. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.67 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wentzel, K. (2003). Sociometric status and adjustment in middle school: A longitudinal study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431602239128 Google Scholar
Wentzel, K., Baker, S., & Russell, S. (2009). Peer relationships and positive adjustment at school. In Gilman, R. & Furlong, M. J. (Eds.) Routledge (Ed.), Handbook of positive psychology in schools (pp. 229243). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wentzel, K., & Caldwell, K. (1997). Friendships, peer acceptance, and group membership: Relations to academic achievement in middle school. Child Development, 68, 11981209. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307%2F1132301 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figura 1. Proposal adapted from Eisenberg, Valiente, and Eggum (2010) on the positive relationships between effortful control, social competence, and academic performance (part A). Model verified in this study (part B) including dispositional empathy as a mediator of the influence of effortful control on social competence.

Figure 1

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of the measures obtained in the study as a function of gender

Figure 2

Table 2. Pearson correlations among the measures obtained in the study

Figure 3

Figura 2. Verified structural equation model considering the influence of effortful control, empathic concern, perspective taking, and social competence on academic performance. χ2 = 6.03, df = 9, p = .74; χ2/df = .67; RMSEA = .000 (PCLOSE = .97), CFI = 1.000, NFI = .99. The model explained 34 % of the variance in Academic Performance.Pr: Peer judgments of prosocial behavior. Pre: Index of social preference. Sk: Academic skills assessed by the teacher. Mg: Mean grade of all the subjects taken by the student.* p < 0.05.**p < 0.01.

Figure 4

Table 3. Fit indices for each model assessed with structural equation modeling