Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T08:47:10.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fear, trust, and compliance with COVID-19 measures: a study of the mediating effect of trust in government on the relationship between fear and compliance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2024

Lenka Hrbková*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Social Studies, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
Aleš Kudrnáč
Affiliation:
Institute of Sociology of the Czech Academy of Sciences, Praha, Czech Republic
*
Corresponding author: Lenka Hrbková; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

As the COVID-19 pandemic became an unprecedented global threat, it was accompanied by an increase in trust in governments as well as fear among the public. Previous research suggests that both institutional trust and fear contribute to the willingness of citizens to comply with anti-pandemic measures. Moreover, fear during the contagion also increases trust in government. This article presents a test of the mediation of the effects of fear on compliance through trust. In addition, it differentiates between three different facets of COVID-19-related fear: fear of the disease, fear of economic consequences, and fear of political consequences. The results suggest that while fear of the disease increases compliance, fear of political consequences decreases compliance. Moreover, the effects of fear are mediated through trust in government. The negative impact of fear of political consequences on compliance increased between April and December 2020.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Previous research has often linked the rally-round-the-flag effect to emotions such as anger, particularly in the context of international crises or terrorist attacks. The COVID-19 pandemic, however, evoked fear as the dominant affective response, offering insights into a unique manifestation of the rally effect (Lwin et al. Reference Lwin, Lu, Sheldenkar, Schulz, Shin, Gupta and Yang2020). At the same time, the pandemic provides a unique opportunity to examine the role of institutional trust in compliance with anti-pandemic policies, since the rally effect was observed at its outset in numerous countries, including the Czech Republic (Bol et al. Reference Bol, Giani, Blais and Loewen2021; Eggers and Harding Reference Eggers and Harding2022; Falcone et al. Reference Falcone, Coli, Felletti, Sapienza, Castelfranchi and Paglieri2020; Kudrnáč and Klusáček Reference Kudrnáč and Klusáček2022).

This study contributes to the research on emotions within public policy, highlighting the importance of a refined comprehension of fear during global crises. While existing studies often adopt a generalized view of emotions, events like a pandemic elicit diverse fears, each rooted in specific aspects of the crisis. We examine the distinct fears and their impact on compliance, mediated by trust in government. To dissect these mechanisms, we utilize two online nationally representative cross-sectional surveys from the Czech Republic, conducted during the pandemic’s first (April 2020) and second (December 2020) waves.

The paper unfolds as follows: first, we present our theoretical framework, emphasizing fear and trust as key compliance drivers, and proposing a model where trust mediates fear’s effects on compliance. Subsequently, we describe the Czech Republic’s pandemic context and government responses, followed by sections on data collection, variables, and analytical strategy. We then showcase our results, followed by a discussion. We conclude by summarizing our findings and outlining avenues for future research.

Fear and response to public policy

Fear is a critical emotional driver shaping public attitudes and behaviors during crises. A pandemic crisis is primarily woven through fear (Kinsman Reference Kinsman2012), arising as a response to perceived threats when they lack a specific, identifiable source (Lerner and Keltner Reference Lerner and Keltner2001; Smith et al. Reference Smith, Cronin and Kessler2008). This type of threat aligns with the existential uncertainties of a pandemic in contrast to the more defined threat of a war or terrorist attack and can, therefore, motivate the rally effect since increased trust in political leaders and institutions helps citizens to cope with the anxieties and worries arising from the crisis (Albertson and Gadarian Reference Albertson and Gadarian2015; van der Meer et al. Reference van der Meer, Steenvoorden and Ouattara2023).

Fear also induces risk-averse behavior, making individuals cautious and predisposing them to compliance with government directives aimed at risk mitigation (Mackie et al. Reference Mackie, Devos and Smith2000). In the context of a severe national threat, fear is associated with increased risk estimates and precautionary measures. After the Madrid terrorist attack in 2005, citizens of Spain showed increased avoidant behavior (Conejero and Etxebarria Reference Conejero and Etxebarria2007). Similarly, in the USA, individuals who felt anxiety and fear post-9/11 were less supportive of military action in Afghanistan (Huddy et al. Reference Huddy, Feldman, Taber and Lahav2005).

During a health crisis, fear can prompt cautious behavior (Bish and Michie Reference Bish and Michie2010; Blendon et al. Reference Blendon, Benson, DesRoches, Raleigh and Taylor-Clark2004; Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Amlôt, Page and Wessely2009). Recent studies show that fear was positively associated with public health compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic, despite often being measured vaguely (Brouard et al. Reference Brouard, Vasilopoulos and Becher2020; Erhardt et al. Reference Erhardt, Freitag, Filsinger and Wamsler2021; Jørgensen et al. Reference Jørgensen, Bor and Bang Petersen2021). For instance, Brouard et al. (Reference Brouard, Vasilopoulos and Becher2020) asked respondents to indicate to what extent they felt fear “when thinking about the situation with COVID-19 in France” and found a strong association between fear and compliance. Similarly, Jørgensen et al. (Reference Jørgensen, Bor and Bang Petersen2021) identified a positive association between fear and compliance by asking respondents how concerned they were about the consequences of coronavirus for them personally, their families, and their close friends. Furthermore, Erhardt et al. (Reference Erhardt, Freitag, Filsinger and Wamsler2021) found a positive effect of fear on institutional trust when measuring fear and anger through a survey asking how respondents felt at the time of the interview.

To tackle the issue of vague measurements of fear in crises, our study investigates three specific dimensions of pandemic-related fear: health, economic, and political consequences. Recognizing that the pandemic triggered complex emotional responses beyond the virus itself, individuals vary in their perceptions of the threat (Renström and Bäck Reference Renström and Bäck2021). Thus, we argue that the impact of fear may depend on its source. While some fear the disease itself, others are concerned about the economic and political repercussions (Mertens et al. Reference Mertens, Gerritsen, Duijndam, Salemink and Engelhard2020). By distinguishing between fear of health risks, economic downturn, and political instability, we assess how each specific source of fear correlates with compliance and trust in government.

Fear of health risks and compliance

In addition to general fear, fear of health risks is the most studied source of fear related to attitudes and behavior during the pandemic. Available research suggests that a public health crisis such as the COVID-19 pandemic generates a fearful response in the population. Fear of the virus and contagion makes people more cautious and avoidant of risky behavior, and people who worry about the health risks of COVID-19 comply with public health measures more than others (Plohl and Musil Reference Plohl and Musil2020). Research indicates that fear of contracting a coronavirus infection and its potential lethal health outcomes is a predictor of adherence to recommended public health policies (Ahorsu et al. Reference Ahorsu, Lin, Imani, Saffari, Griffiths and Pakpour2022; Harper et al. 2020). In Italy, fear of testing positive for COVID-19 was found to mediate the effect of COVID-19 threat assessment on the intention to follow government measures (Nerini et al. Reference Nerini, Matera, Policardo and Di Gesto2021). Data from Austria and Germany also suggest a positive association between fear of contracting the virus and adherence to public health guidelines (Schnell et al. Reference Schnell, Spitzenstätter and Krampe2021). Based on previous research, we assume a positive relationship between health-related fear of COVID-19 and compliance.

Hypotheses 1: Fear of health-related consequences of the pandemic is associated with greater trust in the government and compliance with preventive measures.

In addition to the fear of the health consequences of COVID-19, we anticipate that fear of the potential economic and political consequences of the crisis may affect people’s compliance with government measures.

Fear of economic consequences and compliance

Protests against the COVID-19 lock-downs suggest that fear of the economic consequences of the measures such as curfews, closed businesses, and closed schools might make people reluctant to agree with, or even actively resist, such measures (Henley Reference Henley2020). Lockdowns in the first wave of the pandemic caused economic hardship for large social groups (Witteveen Reference Witteveen2020) and subsequent waves and restrictions often induced even greater fear of the economic consequences of the pandemic. There is a perceived trade-off between the health and economic impacts of antipandemic policy interventions (Besley and Stern Reference Besley and Stern2020), which can reduce both trust in government and adherence to public health measures among citizens who experience economic insecurity in the face of an ongoing crisis. In addition, lockdowns are associated with inflation and COVID-19-induced recession (Jaravel and O’Connell Reference Jaravel and O’Connell2020), which could trigger feelings of economic insecurity and strengthen resistance to government intervention. Therefore, our second hypothesis predicts that the fear of economic consequences may be related to a decrease in compliance with governmental measures.

Hypotheses 2: Fear of the economic consequences of the pandemic decreases compliance with public health measures.

Given the changes in the public sphere, the crisis has not only impacted national economies; it has also affected the decision-making processes of executive bodies in numerous countries, leading to concerns about the political implications of the pandemic.

Fear of political consequences and compliance

As countries around the world declared states of emergency in the midst of an increasing number of coronavirus cases, concerns over an unprecedented expansion of government power arose (The Economist 2020). In efforts to “flatten the curve,” at least 80 countries postponed elections (Asplund Reference Asplund2022), and governments prohibited political gatherings, preventing antigovernment protests (Buras Reference Buras2020; Holroyd Reference Holroyd2020). In some countries (e.g. Australia, Germany, Hungary, and Czechia), national executives strengthened their positions through new legislative frameworks to enhance their own regulatory powers (Bolleyer and Salát Reference Bolleyer and Salát2021). Some people may perceive the measures enacted by governments as a danger to key norms of liberal democracy and civil liberties. Evidence also suggests that ingroup party and expert endorsement can nudge the public towards support for measures eroding civil liberties (Arceneaux et al. Reference Arceneaux, Bakker, Hobolt and De Vries2020). The pandemic and its politicization also contributed to political polarization (Hart et al. Reference Hart, Chinn and Soroka2020). In addition, uncoordinated lockdowns and internal border closures limited freedom of movement within the EU in an unprecedented way, raising questions about its future (Wolff et al. Reference Wolff, Servent and Piquet2020). Therefore, our third hypothesis reflects on the possibility that the fear generated by COVID-19 related to shifts in political power might impact behavior in ways different than fear of the disease.

Hypotheses 3: Fear of political consequences of the pandemic decreases compliance with public health measures.

However, previous research suggests that compliance is not only associated with fear but also with trust in government. Interestingly, there is a lack of studies combining both fear and institutional trust in explaining levels of compliance, with research on the two factors running in parallel. In this study, we argue that we need to include both phenomena in one model to better understand the sources of compliance.

Trust in government and compliance with public health policies

Political trust is essential for effective public policy since it leads to greater law-abiding behavior (Marien and Hooghe Reference Marien and Hooghe2011). Trust in government and authorities has been identified as an important driver of compliance with public policy in across various issues such as tax compliance (Batrancea et al. Reference Batrancea, Nichita, Agostini, Narcizo, Forte, Neves Mamede, Roux-Cesar, Nedev, Leoš, József, Salamzadeh, Nduka, Kudła, Kopyt, Pacheco, Maldonado, Isaga, Benk and Budak2022) and environmentally friendly behavior (Bruno et al. Reference Bruno, Bianchi and Sánchez2022). The association of institutional trust with compliance with public health measures was documented in previous public health crises (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Morse and Tsai2017; Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Amlôt, Page and Wessely2009; Verger et al. Reference Verger2018), and its impact was also confirmed in the COVID-19 pandemic (Kudrnáč and Klusáček Reference Kudrnáč and Klusáček2022; Pak et al. Reference Pak, McBryde and Adegboye2021; Scandurra et al. Reference Scandurra2023; Shanka and Menebo Reference Shanka and Menebo2022). For example, Pak et al. (Reference Pak, McBryde and Adegboye2021) report that during the first wave of the pandemic, trust in government and perception of its truthfulness doubled the impact of policy restrictions on public compliance. The effects applied to both democratic and authoritarian regimes. Studies using cell phone mobility data during the COVID-19 pandemic indicate that geographical units with populations with higher levels of trust in government complied more with stay-at-home orders compared to regions with lower trust (Bargain and Aminjonov Reference Bargain and Aminjonov2020; Brodeur et al. Reference Brodeur, Grigoryeva and Kattan2021; Goldstein and Wiedemann 2021).Footnote 1

In this article, we use surveys conducted at two different phases of the pandemic, which allows us to test whether decrease in trust is related to a decrease in fear due to “normalisation” of the pandemic situation. Previous research has found that negative emotional responses to the outbreak, such as anxiety, declined over time (Gallagher et al. Reference Gallagher, Smith, Richardson and Long2022; Shuster et al. Reference Shuster, O’Brien, Luo, Berner, Perl, Heflin, Kulkarni, Chung, Na, Fiore and Gu2021), which would imply that the willingness to comply with measures and trust in government should also vary over time based on levels of fear.

Mediation of the effects of fear through trust in government

Although fear and institutional trust have been identified as two independent drivers of compliance, the rally-round-the-flag literature also implies that fear evoked by crisis leads to increased trust. Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that the pattern of relationships between fear and trust, on one hand, and compliance, on the other, may be complex. The situation related to the COVID-19 pandemic resembles previous crises in the general rise of fear and a sudden increase in trust in government (Erhardt et al. Reference Erhardt, Freitag, Filsinger and Wamsler2021).

The surge of institutional trust that followed in the wake of the start of the COVID-19 crisis was driven by collective angst caused by the increasing infection numbers (Esaiasson et al. Reference Esaiasson, Sohlberg, Ghersetti and Johansson2021; Schraff Reference Schraff2021). This effect has been documented in various countries, even those with highly unpopular political leaders (Yam et al. Reference Yam, Jackson, Barnes, Lau, Qin and Lee2020). Spikes of trust among the population in national leaders in reaction to an external threat have been explained in the literature as the rally-round-the-flag effect (Hatuel-Radoshitzky and Yarchi Reference Hatuel-Radoshitzky and Yarchi2022; Mueller Reference Mueller1970). Feelings of fear and insecurity stoked by an external crisis, such as a migration crisis, terrorist attack, or a public health crisis, increase public trust in the government and political leaders (Albertson and Gadarian Reference Albertson and Gadarian2015).

The effect of fear on trust in government was also identified in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic, and fear of infection appears to be a consistent predictor of institutional trust (Dietz et al. Reference Dietz, Roßteutscher, Scherer and Stövsand2023). Furthermore, increased institutional trust results from direct external danger posed by infection itself, not by concerns of a secondary threat, such as economic stagnation (van der Meer et al. Reference van der Meer, Steenvoorden and Ouattara2023). These findings imply that fear of the pandemic may not only lead directly to increased compliance of the public with antipandemic measures but can increase compliance through stronger trust in government.

An alternative approach by Groeniger et al. (Reference Oude Groeniger, Noordzij, van der Waal and de Koster2021) assumes that the increase in trust in the Dutch government resulted from public confidence in the lockdown measures and governmental action rather than from the public’s perception of the crisis. However, data from Sweden confirm the effect of fear in the rally-round-the flag hypothesis despite the Swedish government not having imposed strict lockdown measures (Esaiasson et al. Reference Esaiasson, Sohlberg, Ghersetti and Johansson2021), which implies that the rally effect should to a notable extent result more from public worry and fears of the threat rather than from government measures. Erhardt et al. (Reference Erhardt, Freitag, Filsinger and Wamsler2021) find a direct effect of fear on levels of trust in government. Given the evidence of the impact of fear on trust in government and the above-stated literature suggesting that increased trust in government has a positive association with compliance with authorities, we propose the following hypothesis.

Hypotheses 4: Fear related to health consequences of COVID-19 has an indirect positive effect on compliance through increased trust in the government.

Worries related to the pandemic but not based on fear of infection, such as worries about economic stagnation and social isolation, were not found to cause changes in institutional trust levels (van der Meer et al. Reference van der Meer, Steenvoorden and Ouattara2023). Therefore, we assume that fear of the pandemic’s economic and political consequences may not be mediated by trust in government in the same way as fear of the virus and health-related consequences. Since both economic and political consequences of the pandemic are not external threats but are directly related to the actions of the government during the crisis, these aspects of fear should not drive the rally effects. Economic worries seem to decrease public willingness to accept governmental measures (Rosman et al. Reference Rosman, Kerwer, Steinmetz, Chasiotis, Wedderhoff, Betsch and Bosnjak2021). Furthermore, in general economic crisis generates a decrease in political trust (Haugsgjerd Reference Haugsgjerd2018; Tormos Reference Tormos2019). Therefore, our fifth hypothesis is related to the effect of fear unrelated to health.

Hypotheses 5: Fear of consequences caused by COVID-19 unrelated to health will have an indirect negative effect on compliance through decreased trust in government.

Case study selection

Czechia was among the first countries to adopt antipandemic measures. The interventions started with the closing of schools and the introduction of a state of emergency, which was in effect between 12 March and 17 May 2020. The state of emergency enabled the government to adopt a series of measures, including the total closure of national borders, restaurants, shops, sport, and cultural facilities, a ban on social events and gatherings of more than two people, and a face mask mandate. Although some of the public agreed with such measures, the government was also criticized for its unsystematic and unclear communication of the measures.

The first wave of the pandemic was mild, and the incidence of COVID-19 in summer 2020 was low enough to withdraw public health measures. The contagion escalated in the autumn and Czechia became the country with the highest number of deaths per 100,000 people in the world (Gan et al. Reference Gan, Renton and McGee2020). In this second wave of the pandemic, the government was reluctant to impose new restrictions and decided to close schools and restaurants only on October 14. Alcoholic beverages in public spaces and gatherings of groups greater than six people were banned. The restrictions were again withdrawn before the Christmas season.

Public discourse was dominated by discussion of the consequences for business caused by the COVID-19 situation and insufficient support from the state. Public discontent with the antipandemic measures manifested itself as protests in Prague on 28 October and 6 December (irozhlas.cz 2020). Although opponents of the measures criticized the government’s interventions, there was also widespread discontent with the government’s slow reaction to slow down the spread during the second wave. Other missteps, such as the ineffectiveness of a government self-tracking smartphone app, the overloading of public health offices, and high turnover in the office of Health Minister, were also criticised. Trust in government fell between the first and second pandemic waves. Comparison of the mean scores of trust between the first and second waves shows a drop by 2.7 points on a 0–10 scale.

Variables

Compliance

Compliance with COVID-19 preventive measures was measured using a set of eight survey items. The items focused on prescriptions for social distancing and preventive behavior, such as mask wearing, hand washing, and avoiding contact with people outside the home. The wording of the items is presented in the online Appendix. Respondents were asked to report to what extent these items reflected their own behavior during the pandemic on a five-point scale (ranging from “does not reflect at all” to “reflects completely”). The variable is calculated as a mean score of the items. Cronbach’s alpha reached .77 in the first wave of data collection and .71 in the second wave, which is acceptable.

Fear

To assess fear of COVID-related threats, we used three single-item measures. Our approach was inspired by Brouard et al. (Reference Brouard, Vasilopoulos and Becher2020), who asked their respondents how they felt about the situation with COVID-19 in France. Instead of fear, anger, and hope, our three items asked about the extent to which respondents felt fear related to 1) the disease and the uncertainty of 2) political and 3) economic consequences of the pandemic. All emotional reactions were measured on scales (0–10) ranging from “no feeling at all” to “very strong feeling.”

Fear of health consequences was measured by responses to the following item: ‘Fear of one’s own health’. Fear of political consequences was measured as: “Fear of lasting restrictions on rights and freedoms.” Fear of the economic consequences was reported by the following item: Fear of deterioration of one’s own economic situation.”

Trust in government and covariates

In addition to the dependent and key variables, we also measured the mediator, trust in government, which was indicated on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 10 (completely). The item format is commonly used in comparative datasets such as the European Social Study and is considered by the OECD Guidelines for Measuring Trust as a preferable way of measuring institutional trust (OECD 2017). Additionally, we controlled for gender, age, education, and party sympathies regarding the main governmental party at the time (ANO). Women tend to comply with public health measures more than men. Older people, since they tend to be more vulnerable to COVID-19, are also more willing to comply (Brouard et al. Reference Brouard, Vasilopoulos and Becher2020). We controlled for the main governmental party sympathy, since the prime minister and the ministers associated with his party (e.g. the Minister for Health) were responsible for management of the pandemic, and we assume that partisan sympathy for ANO might confound trust in government. In the survey, the respondents indicated their sympathies for political parties ranging from strong antipathy (0) to strong sympathy (10). This standard tool for quantifying like and dislike of political parties is used, for example, in the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems. We also controlled for education, as people with higher levels of education may be more able to engage in more cautious behavior, such as working from home.

Data and analytical strategy

Our analyses are based on cross-sectional data collected during the first (6–16 April 2020, n = 1,284) and second (10–15 December 2020, n = 1,148) waves of the pandemic. Data from both waves were collected through Focus research company that used an existing online panel of more than 60,000 survey respondents. Our samples are therefore nonprobability samples based on self-selection of respondents. However, both samples are representative of the Czech adult population in terms of gender, age, education, region, and settlement size using quotas based on the 2011 Czech census data.

To examine the effects of fear and trust on compliance, we ran three models for each wave (six in total) for each dependent variable, that is, compliance with preventive measures and compliance with social distancing. In separate models, we regressed three types of fear on each of the three dependent variables at T1 and T2.

Consequently, we ran nine mediation models for each wave to test whether the relationships between fear and compliance with the three dependent variables was mediated by trust in government (see Figure 1). We used the R mediation package to estimate the direct and indirect effects of all three types of fear on compliance with policy. Due to the violation of normality assumptions in our data, as indicated by the Shapiro–Wilk tests for all mediation models, we employed nonparametric bootstrapping to robustly estimate the indirect effects in our models. As we ran multiple models on our data at both time points, we controlled the inflated probability of Type I error. In our analysis, we chose the procedure by Benjamini and Yekutieli (Reference Benjamini and Yekutieli2001). This method was preferred over traditional family-wise error rate (FWER) controlling methods, such as the Bonferroni correction, because it offers a balanced means of controlling Type I errors while maintaining statistical power – particularly pertinent given our large sample sizes. As recommended by Cribbie (Reference Cribbie2007), this method offers a more rigorous control of the false discovery rate (FDR) than the original FDR procedure by Benjamini and Hochberg (Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995), providing a stringent yet powerful approach to multiple testing correction, which is crucial for avoiding the underestimation of true effects inherent in overly conservative FWER methods.

Figure 1. Theoretical model of fear-trust in government-compliance mediation.

Results and discussion

Fear and compliance over time

Initially, we built regression models testing the effects of the three types of fear on compliance in T1 and T2 (Table 1). Fear of the health consequences of COVID-19 has a positive statistically significant effect on compliance at both time points. Furthermore, models referring to the initial and later phases of the pandemic show that people who feared the health consequences of COVID-19 were more likely to follow the measures (Hypotheses 1). This is in line with previous research suggesting that the primary response to the threat posed by the global pandemic was presumably fear arising from the novelty of the situation and the uncertainty it generated (Demirtaş-Madran Reference Demirtaş-Madran2021; Harper et al. Reference Harper, Satchell, Fido and Latzman2021; Plohl and Musil Reference Plohl and Musil2020).

Table 1. Regression results

Note: Standard errors in brackets.

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Fear of economic consequences is not associated with compliance. Fear of political consequences has a negative effect on compliance with restrictive measures at the later stage of the pandemic but not the first (Hypotheses 3 partially supported). This finding suggests that some people may perceive the political actions of the authorities during a crisis as a danger to democracy and civil liberties, which, in turn, affects their compliance with measures. Although several studies suggest that pandemic-related measures can cause economic hardship (Witteveen Reference Witteveen2020) and are associated with economic uncertainty and the threat of recession (Jaravel and O’Connell Reference Jaravel and O’Connell2020), our results do not show that fear of economic consequences is related to compliance with pandemic measures (Hypotheses 2 not supported).

Trust in government and compliance over time

Our results show that the level of trust of the respondents in the government has a consistent effect on compliance with public health measures at both times. People who trust the government are more willing to comply with all types of measures. This finding is consistent with previous research focusing on prior health crises (Blair et al. Reference Blair, Morse and Tsai2017; Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Amlôt, Page and Wessely2009; Verger et al. Reference Verger2018) as well as studies related to the COVID-19 pandemic (Brodeur et al. Reference Brodeur, Grigoryeva and Kattan2021; Goldstein and Wiedemann Reference Goldstein and Wiedemann2022; Pak et al. Reference Pak, McBryde and Adegboye2021; Scandurra et al. Reference Scandurra2023; Shanka and Manebo Reference Shanka and Menebo2022). In essence, the finding that fear of the disease is correlated to trust in government in Czechia is in agreement with previous findings from other countries (Erhardt et al. Reference Erhardt, Freitag, Filsinger and Wamsler2021).

Results of the mediation model for the first wave

We estimated a series of mediation models that follow the theoretical model depicted in Figure 1. Starting with models using data from T1 (Table 2), fear of health consequences is positively related to trust in government (Model 3A). Simultaneously, trust in government is associated with compliance with preventive measures. The mediation is partial, and about 10% of the effect of fear of health consequences on compliance with preventive measures is mediated through trust in government.

Table 2. Meditation analysis at T1

Note: Standard errors in brackets.

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

We find any no evidence of a mediation effect in the model testing fear of economic consequences (Model 3B). While trust in government is a positive and statistically significant predictor of compliance, fear of economic consequences is not related to trust in government at the start of the pandemic. More specifically, the effects of fear of economic consequences and trust in government on compliance are direct, but there is no indirect effect of fear through trust in government. This may be explained by the tendency of individuals to follow government measures while not actually trusting the government that they will come out of the situation economically unharmed. This is similar to Henley’s (Reference Henley2020) suggestion about people’s reluctance to agree with governmental measures.

Fear of political consequences is negatively related to trust in government (Model 3C). At the same time, trust in government is a positive and statistically significant predictor of compliance. The results, however, indicate no mediation of fear of political consequences on compliance through trust in government.

As the initial phase of the pandemic unfolded, compliance with anitpandemic measures was notably driven by fear of health consequences of COVID-19. This effect was partly mediated by trust in government,

Results of the mediation model for the second wave

Subsequently, we ran a series of identical mediation models according to the theoretical model depicted in Figure 1 with data from T2 (Table 3). Fear of health consequences of COVID-19 is again positively related to trust in government and trust in government is a positive and statistically significant predictor of compliance (Model 4A). The mediation is partial and about 8% of the effect of fear of health consequence of COVID-19 on compliance with preventive measures is mediated through trust in government (Hypotheses 4).

Table 3. Mediation analysis at T2

Note: Standard errors in brackets.

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Concurrently, there is a significant positive relationship between fear of economic consequences and compliance (Model 4B). Contrary to the lack of a mediation effect observed in the initial wave, both direct and indirect effects of fear regarding economic consequences are present in T2. The opposing directions of these effects suggest a competitive mediation, wherein the mediated and direct effects are present and point in opposite directions (Zhao et al. Reference Zhao, Lynch and Chen2010). In such instances, the opposing effects of the direct and indirect pathways cancel each other out, resulting in a nonsignificant total effect of economic fear on compliance behaviors. Our data do not support the assumed relationships between this type of fear, trust in government, and compliance (Hypotheses 5 not supported). However, the hypothesized negative effect on trust in government was identified in the later phase of the pandemic, possibly because the lockdowns at the start of the pandemic caused economic hardship for almost everyone (Witteveen Reference Witteveen2020).

Trust in government is also a negative predictor of compliance with preventive measures in the model that tests the effect of fear of political consequences. Fear of political consequences is negatively related to both trust in government and compliance (Model 4C). The mediation is partial and the effect of fear of political consequences is mediated by 27% on compliance with preventive measures through trust in government (Hypotheses 5). Greater fear of political consequences diminishes the likelihood of adherence to government measures, with a significant portion of this effect being mediated by reduced trust in government. This could be a result of nonstandard procedures that may be perceived as a violation of democratic norms (Bolleyer and Salát Reference Bolleyer and Salát2021). Moreover, the decision not to comply could be understood as a protest behavior demonstrating the need to defend democracy and civil liberties.

Summary of mediation analysis results

While Six et al. (Reference Six, Vadder, Glavina, Verhoest and Pepermans2023) suggest that fear was a more powerful factor of compliance than institutional trust in the first phase of the pandemic, we do not find support for such a claim. Our results (Table 4) suggest that trust in government and fears had independent direct effects on compliance. Indeed, the results of the mediation analyses mostly support Hypotheses 4, suggesting that the effect of fear of health consequences of the pandemic on compliance is partially mediated by trust in government. Trust in government partially mediates the relationship between fear and compliance at the start of the pandemic. It also partially mediates the relationship between fear of health and political consequences in the second phase of the pandemic. The overall mediation results suggest that the effects of fear of health consequences are in line with the rally-round-the flag hypothesis that assumes fear increases institutional trust (Erhardt et al. Reference Erhardt, Freitag, Filsinger and Wamsler2021; Schraff Reference Schraff2021), which in turn leads to compliance. Conversely, fear of negative political consequences related to the pandemic negatively affects trust in government and, in turn, compliance, specifically in the later phase of the pandemic. These results contradict the findings of Six et al. (Reference Six, Vadder, Glavina, Verhoest and Pepermans2023), suggesting that institutional trust was negatively related to compliance in the later stage of the pandemic.

Table 4. Results summary of fear-trust in government-compliance mediation analyses in time

Summary of results of the effects of three different types of fear

The results show that the effect of fear of the disease on trust in government was about the same in the later phase of the pandemic. Based on prior studies of anxiety (Gallagher et al. Reference Gallagher, Smith, Richardson and Long2022; Shuster et al. Reference Shuster, O’Brien, Luo, Berner, Perl, Heflin, Kulkarni, Chung, Na, Fiore and Gu2021), we expected that the power of all types of fear would decline over time, but we found the opposite. While the fear of economic consequences did not predict trust in government at the start of the pandemic, the effect became negative in the second phase. This change could be a result of people’s evaluation of the government’s performance. Similarly, the effect of fear of political consequences on trust in government stayed negative and became even stronger with time. The results of the effects of the three different types of fear based on mediation models (3A – 4C) are summarized in Table 5.

Table 5. Summary of results of the effects of three different fears in time

Note: “+”= positive stat.sig. effect, “−” = negative stat.sig. effect; “n.s”= no stat. sig. effect.

In summary, our analysis indicates that the impact of fear on compliance with public health measures varies according to the source of the fear. Fear of the disease itself heightens perceived threat severity, which consistently correlates with increased compliance. Conversely, fear of economic consequences yields incongruent results; when the effects of the other types of fear are accounted for, this fear does not significantly affect compliance over time. Meanwhile, fear of the political repercussions of the pandemic diminishes compliance in the later stages of the crisis. This influence manifests both directly and indirectly via diminished trust in government, corroborating our hypothesis that a complex crisis can elicit a spectrum of emotional reactions, including varying types of fear and concerns, each carrying different repercussions for adherence to public policy measures.

Although it could have been expected that the effects of fear of health, economic and political consequences on willingness to comply would weaken over time because of people’s progressive demotivation to follow recommended protective behaviors (Rypdal et al. Reference Rypdal, Bianchi and Rypdal2020), the positive effect of fear of the disease was of a similar strength at both times, while the effects of the fear of political consequences became more clearly and strongly negative over time.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of our analysis lie in the longitudinal assessment, distinguishing three types of fear, and testing the mediating effects of trust in government on the relationship between fear and compliance. However, this study also has some limitations. First, our data cover only two time points. It would be beneficial to cover a longer period because the emotional reactions of the population and people’s evaluations of public authorities change as a crisis unfolds. Second, our data are not a panel and therefore we cannot make claims regarding change over time, but rather observe differences between two time points. Third, the measures of emotions were designed to capture only a limited number of the possible dimensions of fear that the pandemic might trigger in people. The goal of this study is not to develop a scale of fear during a pandemic but to demonstrate that people’s responses might vary according to how they perceive the salience of threats caused by a crisis.

Conclusion

Our study sheds light on the intricate relationship between fear, trust, and policy compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic and delineates the pivotal role of fear in public compliance with government policies during a crisis. We find that fear concerning health-related consequences initially reinforces compliance with public health measures, while also enhancing trust in the authorities as the public seeks guidance and safeguarding from their leaders. This provides evidence of an emotion-driven mechanism behind the rally effect, where fear of the disease amplifies trust, which in turn, boosts compliance with crisis mitigation policy as suggested by previous research (Shanka and Menebo Reference Shanka and Menebo2022). While fear of the potential economic and political impacts of COVID-19 did not initially drive compliance, the fear of political consequences, such as the threat to individual rights and shifts in political power, had pronounced direct and indirect effects on compliance as the crisis evolved. This fear notably diminished both compliance and trust in government in the later phase of the pandemic, illustrating how the changing nature of fear can markedly sway public policy responses.

The relationship between emotions and crisis dynamics is intricate; emotional reactions vary with different crisis conditions and evolve over time. Contrary to our assumption that the impact of health-related fear would wane as the pandemic persisted, its influence on trust and compliance remained stable. Yet, in the later stages of the crisis, fears about the pandemic’s political implications potentially undermined the rally effect and the positive influence of fear on compliance.

These nuanced findings highlight the importance of sophisticated approaches to measure and address emotional reactions to complex events. The COVID-19 crisis, for instance, provoked a spectrum of worries, from concerns for loved ones’ health to fears of healthcare system overload (Mertens et al. Reference Mertens, Gerritsen, Duijndam, Salemink and Engelhard2020). Fear can propel compliance via the rally effect, but it can also have the opposite effect, reducing compliance and trust.

While this study specifically addresses the COVID-19 pandemic, the insights are applicable to other scenarios necessitating strong government action. The multifaceted nature of fear can lead to varying effects on compliance and policy support. For instance, terrorist attacks often induce immediate fear of violence, followed by concerns over increased state surveillance and potential privacy violations, which can affect trust in government and policy support differently (Citron and Gray Reference Citron and Gray2012; Richards Reference Richards2012).

We found no direct effect of economic fear on compliance, and its indirect effect later in the pandemic was negated by a corresponding decrease in trust in government. Given the cross-sectional nature of our data, we advise caution in interpreting these results and acknowledge the limitations in drawing definitive conclusions. Future research would benefit from panel data to allow for more robust inferences.

Our findings suggest directions for both scholarly research and public policy. Scholars should consider the multidimensionality of emotional responses, such as fear, as different aspects can lead to diverse outcomes. Moreover, our results can provide guidance for policymakers and political leaders in managing public emotions during crises. To enhance compliance, addressing public fears about political stability may be as crucial as alleviating health-related fears. Public officials might achieve better adherence to policies by ensuring and communicating political security, rather than solely focusing on inducing fear of the health consequences of COVID-19.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0143814X24000059

Data availability statement

Replication materials are available in the Journal of Public Policy Dataverse at Hrbková, Lenka; Kudrnáč, Aleš, 2024, ‘Replication Data For: Fear, trust, and compliance with COVID-19 measures: A study of the mediating effect of trust in government on the relationship between fear and compliance’, https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/MJMC69?fbclid=IwAR2Dg1vjS-T3fGFoJy1wUb_Uv-RF9AK4XyoCOesBQH_lfWXcxKywo5eUIkw, Harvard Dataverse.

Acknowledgment

Lenka Hrbková’s work was supported by the NPO “Systemic Risk Institute” number LX22NPO5101, funded by the European Union – Next Generation EU (Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports, NPO: EXCELES). Aleš Kudrnáč’s activity was carried out as part of the “Research of the COVID-19 and post-COVID-19 era” project involving the Institute of Contemporary History of CAS, Institute of Sociology of CAS, and the Institute of Psychology of CAS.

Footnotes

1 However, Six et al. (Reference Six, Vadder, Glavina, Verhoest and Pepermans2023) found that in the initial phases of the pandemic, fear, and perception of rule appropriateness drove public compliance with the measures rather than trust in government, and trust was actually negatively associated with compliance at a later stage of the pandemic.

References

Ahorsu, Daniel Kwasi, Lin, Chung-Ying, Imani, Vida, Saffari, Mohsen, Griffiths, Mark D., and Pakpour, Amir H.. 2022. “The Fear of COVID-19 Scale: Development and Initial Validation.” International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 20 (3): 15371545.10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertson, Bethany, and Gadarian, Shana Kushner. 2015. Anxious Politics: Democratic Citizenship in a Threatening World. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. March 19, 2023. https://www.cambridge.org/core/books/anxious-politics/04E10C404DE64C07CF9F83B5D4BD485E .CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, Kevin, Bakker, Bert N., Hobolt, Sara, and De Vries, Catherine Eunice. 2020. Is COVID-19 a Threat to Liberal Democracy? PsyArXiv. preprint. February 1, 2022. https://osf.io/8e4pa.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asplund, Erik. 2022. “Global Overview of COVID-19: Impact on Elections.” IDEA. https://www.idea.int/news-media/multimedia-reports/global-overview-covid-19-impact-elections.Google Scholar
Bargain, Olivier, and Aminjonov, Ulugbek. 2020. “Trust and Compliance to Public Health Policies in Times of COVID-19.” Journal of Public Economics 192: 104316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batrancea, Larissa M., Nichita, Anca, Agostini, Ruggero De, Narcizo, Fabricio Batista, Forte, Denis, Neves Mamede, Samuel de Paiva, Roux-Cesar, Ana Maria, Nedev, Bozhidar, Leoš, Vitek, József, Pántya, Salamzadeh, Aidin, Nduka, Eleanya, Kudła, Janusz, Kopyt, Mateusz, Pacheco, Luis, Maldonado, Isabel, Isaga, Nsubili, Benk, Serkan, and Budak, Tamer. 2022. “A Self-Employed Taxpayer Experimental Study on Trust, Power, and Tax Compliance in Eleven Countries.” Financial Innovation 8 (1): 96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Yoav, and Hochberg, Yosef. 1995. “Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 57 (1): 289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Yoav, and Yekutieli, Daniel. 2001. “The Control of the False Discovery Rate in Multiple Testing under Dependency.” The Annals of Statistics 29 (4). December 20, 2023. https://projecteuclid.org/journals/annals-of-statistics/volume-29/issue-4/The-control-of-the-false-discovery-rate-in-multiple-testing/10.1214/aos/1013699998.full.10.1214/aos/1013699998CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besley, Timothy, and Stern, Nicholas. 2020. “The Economics of Lockdown.” Fiscal Studies 41 (3): 493513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bish, Alison, and Michie, Susan. 2010. “Demographic and Attitudinal Determinants of Protective Behaviours during a Pandemic: A Review.” British Journal of Health Psychology 15 (4): 797824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, Robert A., Morse, Benjamin S., and Tsai, Lily L.. 2017. “Public Health and Public Trust: Survey Evidence from the Ebola Virus Disease Epidemic in Liberia.” Social Science & Medicine (1982) 172: 8997.10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.11.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blendon, Robert J., Benson, John M., DesRoches, Catherine M., Raleigh, E., and Taylor-Clark, Kalahn. 2004. “The Public’s Response to Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome in Toronto and the United States.” Clinical Infectious Diseases 38 (7): 925–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bol, Damien, Giani, Marco, Blais, André, and Loewen, Peter John. 2021. “The Effect of COVID-19 Lockdowns on Political Support: Some Good News for Democracy?European Journal of Political Research 60 (2): 497505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolleyer, Nicole, and Salát, Orsolya. 2021. “Parliaments in Times of Crisis: COVID-19, Populism and Executive Dominance.” West European Politics 44 (5–6): 1103–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brodeur, Abel, Grigoryeva, Idaliya, and Kattan, Lamis. 2021. “Stay-at-Home Orders, Social Distancing, and Trust.” Journal of Population Economics 34 (4): 1321–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brouard, Sylvain, Vasilopoulos, Pavlos, and Becher, Michael. 2020. “Sociodemographic and Psychological Correlates of Compliance with the COVID-19 Public Health Measures in France.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 53 (2): 253258.10.1017/S0008423920000335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruno, Juan Manuel, Bianchi, Enrique Carlos, and Sánchez, Carolina. 2022. “Determinants of Household Recycling Intention: The Acceptance of Public Policy Moderated by Habits, Social Influence, and Perceived Time Risk.” Environmental Science & Policy 136: 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buras, Piotr. 2020. “First Hungary, Now Poland. It’s Time for Europeans to Speak out against COVID-19 Power Grabs | View.” euronews. August 5, 2020. https://www.euronews.com/2020/04/15/first-hungary-now-poland-time-for-europeans-to-speak-out-against-covid-19-power-grabs-view.Google Scholar
Citron, Danielle, and Gray, David. 2012. “Addressing the Harm of Total Surveillance: A Reply to Professor Neil Richards.” Harvard Law Review (126): 262.Google Scholar
Conejero, Susana, and Etxebarria, Itziar. 2007. “The Impact of the Madrid Bombing on Personal Emotions, Emotional Atmosphere and Emotional Climate.” Journal of Social Issues 63 (2): 273–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cribbie, Robert A. 2007. “Multiplicity Control in Structural Equation Modeling.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 14 (1): 98112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demirtaş-Madran, H. Andaç. 2021. “Accepting Restrictions and Compliance with Recommended Preventive Behaviors for COVID-19: A Discussion Based on the Key Approaches and Current Research on Fear Appeals.” Frontiers in Psychology 12: 558437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietz, Melanie, Roßteutscher, Sigrid, Scherer, Philipp, and Stövsand, Lars-Christopher. 2023. “Rally Effect in the Covid-19 Pandemic: The Role of Affectedness, Fear, and Partisanship.” German Politics 32 (4): 643663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eggers, Andrew C., and Harding, Robin. 2022. “Rallying in Fear? Estimating the Effect of the UK COVID-19 Lockdown with a Natural Experiment.” European Journal of Political Research 61 (2): 586600.10.1111/1475-6765.12467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erhardt, Julian, Freitag, Markus, Filsinger, Maximilian, and Wamsler, Steffen. 2021. “The Emotional Foundations of Political Support: How Fear and Anger Affect Trust in the Government in Times of the Covid-19 Pandemic.” Swiss Political Science Review 27 (2): 339–52.10.1111/spsr.12462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esaiasson, Peter, Sohlberg, Jacob, Ghersetti, Marina, and Johansson, Bengt. 2021. “How the Coronavirus Crisis Affects Citizen Trust in Institutions and in Unknown Others: Evidence from ‘the Swedish Experiment’.” European Journal of Political Research 60 (3): 748–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falcone, Rino, Coli, Elisa, Felletti, Silvia, Sapienza, Alessandro, Castelfranchi, Cristiano, and Paglieri, Fabio. 2020. “All We Need Is Trust: How the COVID-19 Outbreak Reconfigured Trust in Italian Public Institutions.” Frontiers in Psychology 11: 2585.10.3389/fpsyg.2020.561747CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, Matthew W., Smith, Lia J., Richardson, Angela L., and Long, Laura J.. 2022. “Six Month Trajectories of COVID-19 Experiences and Associated Stress, Anxiety, Depression, and Impairment in American Adults.” Cognitive Therapy and Research 46 (3): 457–69.10.1007/s10608-021-10277-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gan, Nectar, Renton, Adam, and McGee, Luke. 2020. “October 29 Coronavirus News.” CNN. February 2, 2022. https://www.cnn.com/world/live-news/coronavirus-pandemic-10-29-20-intl/index.html.Google Scholar
Goldstein, Daniel A. N., and Wiedemann, Johannes. 2022. “Who Do You Trust? The Consequences of Partisanship and Trust for Public Responsiveness to COVID-19 Orders.” Perspectives on Politics 20 (2): 412438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harper, Craig A., Satchell, Liam P., Fido, Dean, and Latzman, Robert D.. 2021. “Functional Fear Predicts Public Health Compliance in the COVID-19 Pandemic.” International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 19: 18751888. http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s11469-020-00281-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, P. Sol, Chinn, Sedona, and Soroka, Stuart. 2020. “Politicization and Polarization in COVID-19 News Coverage.” Science Communication 42 (5): 679–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatuel-Radoshitzky, Michal, and Yarchi, Moran. 2022. “Rally ’Round the Flag Revised: External Soft Threats and Media Coverage.” Media, War & Conflict 15 (1): 6181.10.1177/1750635220917419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haugsgjerd, Atle. 2018. “Political Distrust amidst the Great Recession: The Mitigating Effect of Welfare State Effort.” Comparative European Politics 16 (4): 620–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, Jon. 2020. “Latest Coronavirus Lockdowns Spark Protests across Europe.” The Guardian. December 4, 2021. https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/nov/02/latest-coronavirus-lockdowns-spark-protests-across-europe.Google Scholar
Holroyd, Matthew. 2020. “Poland Holds Virtual Protests against Abortion Bill during Lockdown.” euronews. August 5, 2020. https://www.euronews.com/2020/04/14/poland-holds-virtual-protests-against-abortion-bill-during-covid-19-lockdown.Google Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, Feldman, Stanley, Taber, Charles, and Lahav, Gallya. 2005. “Threat, Anxiety, and Support of Antiterrorism Policies.” American Journal of Political Science 49 (3): 593608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
irozhlas.cz. 2020. “Lidé Protestovali v Centru Prahy Proti Koronavirovým Opatřením. Neměli Roušky, Nedodržovali Rozestupy.” irozhlas.cz. https://www.irozhlas.cz/zpravy-domov/koronavirus-cesko-v-cesku-cr-demonstrace-praha-opatreni_2012061509_ako.Google Scholar
Jaravel, Xavier, and O’Connell, Martin. 2020. “Real-Time Price Indices: Inflation Spike and Falling Product Variety during the Great Lockdown.” Journal of Public Economics 191: 104270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jørgensen, Frederik, Bor, Alexander, and Bang Petersen, Michael. 2021. Compliance without fear: Individual‐level protective behaviour during the first wave of the COVID‐19 pandemic. British Journal of Health Psychology, 26(2), 679-696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinsman, John. 2012. “‘A Time of Fear’: Local, National, and International Responses to a Large Ebola Outbreak in Uganda.” Globalization and Health 8 (1): 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudrnáč, Aleš, and Klusáček, Jan. 2022. “The Temporary Increase in Trust in Government and Compliance with Anti-pandemic Measures at the Start of the Covid-19 Pandemic.” Czech Sociological Review 58 (2), 119150.10.13060/csr.2022.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, Jennifer S., and Keltner, Dacher. 2001. “Fear, Anger, and Risk.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 (1): 146–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lwin, May Oo, Lu, Juahui, Sheldenkar, Anita, Schulz, Peter J., Shin, Wonsun, Gupta, Raj, and Yang, Yinping. 2020. Global sentiments surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic on Twitter: analysis of Twitter trends. JMIR public health and surveillance, 6(2), e19447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackie, Diane M., Devos, Thierry, and Smith, Eliot R.. 2000. “Intergroup Emotions: Explaining Offensive Action Tendencies in an Intergroup Context.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (4): 602–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marien, Sofie, and Hooghe, Marc. 2011. “Does Political Trust Matter? An Empirical Investigation into the Relation between Political Trust and Support for Law Compliance: Does Political Trust Matter?European Journal of Political Research 50 (2): 267–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mertens, Gaëtan, Gerritsen, Lotte, Duijndam, Stefanie, Salemink, Elske, and Engelhard, Iris M.. 2020. “Fear of the Coronavirus (COVID-19): Predictors in an Online Study Conducted in March 2020.” Journal of Anxiety Disorders 74: 102258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, John E. 1970. “Presidential Popularity from Truman to Johnson1.” American Political Science Review 64 (1): 1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nerini, Amanda, Matera, Camilla, Policardo, Giulia Rosa, and Di Gesto, Cristian. 2021. “Containment Measures against COVID-19 in Italy: The Role of Protection Motivation and Values.” Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment 32 (6): 754767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
OECD. 2017. OECD Guidelines on Measuring Trust. Paris: OECD Publishing. March 8, 2023. https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/governance/oecd-guidelines-on-measuring-trust_9789264278219-en.Google Scholar
Oude Groeniger, Joost, Noordzij, Kjell, van der Waal, Jeroen, and de Koster, Willem. 2021. “Dutch COVID-19 Lockdown Measures Increased Trust in Government and Trust in Science: A Difference-in-Differences Analysis.” Social Science & Medicine 275: 113819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pak, Anton, McBryde, Emma, and Adegboye, Oyelola A. 2021. “Does High Public Trust Amplify Compliance with Stringent COVID-19 Government Health Guidelines? A Multi-Country Analysis Using Data from 102,627 Individuals.” Risk Management and Healthcare Policy 14: 293302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plohl, Nejc, and Musil, Bojan. 2020. “Modeling Compliance with COVID-19 Prevention Guidelines: The Critical Role of Trust in Science.” Psychology, Health & Medicine 26 (1): 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renström, Emma A., and Bäck, Hanna. 2021. “Emotions during the Covid-19 Pandemic: Fear, Anxiety, and Anger as Mediators between Threats and Policy Support and Political Actions.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 51 (8): 861–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, Neil M. 2012. “The Danger of Surveillance.” Harvard Law Review (126): 1934.Google Scholar
Rosman, Tom, Kerwer, Martin, Steinmetz, Holger, Chasiotis, Anita, Wedderhoff, Oliver, Betsch, Cornelia, and Bosnjak, Michael. 2021. “Will COVID-19-Related Economic Worries Superimpose Health Worries, Reducing Nonpharmaceutical Intervention Acceptance in Germany? A Prospective Pre-Registered Study.” International Journal of Psychology 56 (4): 607–22.10.1002/ijop.12753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, G. James, Amlôt, Richard, Page, Lisa, and Wessely, Simon. 2009. “Public Perceptions, Anxiety, and Behaviour Change in Relation to the Swine Flu Outbreak: Cross Sectional Telephone Survey.” BMJ 339: b2651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rypdal, Kristoffer, Bianchi, Filippo Maria, and Rypdal, Martin. 2020. “Intervention Fatigue Is the Primary Cause of Strong Secondary Waves in the COVID-19 Pandemic.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17 (24): 9592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scandurra, Cristiano et al. 2023. “Why People Were Less Compliant with Public Health Regulations during the Second Wave of the Covid-19 Outbreak: The Role of Trust in Governmental Organizations, Future Anxiety, Fatigue, and Covid-19 Risk Perception.” Current Psychology 42 (9): 74037413.10.1007/s12144-021-02059-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanka, Mesay Sata, and Menebo, Mesay Moges. 2022. “When and How Trust in Government Leads to Compliance with COVID-19 Precautionary Measures.” Journal of Business Research 139: 1275–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shuster, Anastasia, O’Brien, Madeline, Luo, Yi, Berner, Laura A., Perl, Ofer, Heflin, Matthew, Kulkarni, Kaustubh, Chung, Dongil, Na, Soojung, Fiore, Vincenzo G., and Gu, Xiaosi. 2021. “Emotional Adaptation during a Crisis: Decline in Anxiety and Depression after the Initial Weeks of COVID-19 in the United States.” Translational Psychiatry 11: 435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnell, Tatjana, Spitzenstätter, Daniel, and Krampe, Henning. 2021. “Compliance with COVID-19 Public Health Guidelines: An Attitude-Behaviour Gap Bridged by Personal Concern and Distance to Conspiracy Ideation.” Psychology & Health 0 (0): 122.Google Scholar
Schraff, Dominik. 2021. “Political Trust during the Covid-19 Pandemic: Rally around the Flag or Lockdown Effects?European Journal of Political Research 60 (4): 1007–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Six, Frédérique, Vadder, Steven de, Glavina, Monika, Verhoest, Koen, and Pepermans, Koen. 2023. “What Drives Compliance with COVID-19 Measures over Time? Explaining Changing Impacts with Goal Framing Theory.” Regulation & Governance 17 (1): 321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Heather J., Cronin, Tracey, and Kessler, Thomas. 2008. “Anger, Fear, or Sadness: Faculty Members’ Emotional Reactions to Collective Pay Disadvantage.” Political Psychology 29 (2): 221–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Economist. 2020. “Autocrats See Opportunity in Disaster.” The Economist. August 5, 2020. https://www.economist.com/leaders/2020/04/23/autocrats-see-opportunity-in-disaster.Google Scholar
Tormos, Raül. 2019. “Measuring Personal Economic Hardship and Its Impact on Political Trust During the Great Recession.” Social Indicators Research 144 (3): 1209–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Meer, Tom, Steenvoorden, Eefje, and Ouattara, Ebe. 2023. “Fear and the COVID-19 Rally Round the Flag: A Panel Study on Political Trust.” West European Politics: 117.Google Scholar
Verger, Pierre, Bocquier, Aurélie, Vergélys, Chantal, Ward, Jeremy, and Patrick Peretti-Watel. 2018. “Flu Vaccination among Patients with Diabetes: Motives, Perceptions, Trust, and Risk Culture - a Qualitative Survey.” BMC Public Health 18: 1-10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witteveen, Dirk. 2020. “Sociodemographic Inequality in Exposure to COVID-19-Induced Economic Hardship in the United Kingdom.” Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 69: 100551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, Sarah, Servent, Ariadna Ripoll, and Piquet, Agathe. 2020. “Framing Immobility: Schengen Governance in Times of Pandemics.” Journal of European Integration 42 (8): 1127–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yam, Kai Chi, Jackson, Joshua Conrad, Barnes, Christopher M., Lau, Jenson, Qin, Xin, and Lee, Hin Yeung. 2020. “The Rise of COVID-19 Cases Is Associated with Support for World Leaders.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117(41): 25429–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Xinshu, Lynch, John G., and Chen, Qimei. 2010. “Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis.” Journal of Consumer Research 37 (2): 197206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Theoretical model of fear-trust in government-compliance mediation.

Figure 1

Table 1. Regression results

Figure 2

Table 2. Meditation analysis at T1

Figure 3

Table 3. Mediation analysis at T2

Figure 4

Table 4. Results summary of fear-trust in government-compliance mediation analyses in time

Figure 5

Table 5. Summary of results of the effects of three different fears in time

Supplementary material: File

Hrbková and Kudrnáč supplementary material

Hrbková and Kudrnáč supplementary material
Download Hrbková and Kudrnáč supplementary material(File)
File 16.3 KB