Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T20:47:22.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Beginning of Time

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2024

Karenleigh A. Overmann*
Affiliation:
University of Colorado Colorado Springs 1420 Austin Bluffs Pkwy Colorado Springs, CO 80918 USA Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The present analysis focuses on the material component of time, the devices used for measuring and counting it. The biological basis for subjective, experiential time is first reviewed, as are early strategies found cross-culturally for measuring and counting time objectively. These strategies include timekeeping by natural phenomena, using tallies to keep track of small periods of time, harnessing shadows for daily and annual time, and visualizing time with clocks and calendars. The conclusion then examines how such timekeeping devices might influence the conceptualization of time.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research

Perhaps the most interesting thing about time as a conceptual domain is the fact its material component is so often overlooked. As is true of space and number, time is viewed as a system of ideas created mentally, linguistically, or socially which is later externalized to material forms, rather than as concepts emerging from and through interactions with material forms to occasion explication, naming and sharing. Admittedly, the material component may tend to be somewhat more visible in time than in space and number, for two reasons: first, our concepts of time overtly resemble the natural phenomena which are their basis, and second, time as experienced often differs from time as measured by the clock, perhaps making us aware a material form has a role in the latter. Yet time is no more foreordained to be structured into hours, days, months and years than numbers are to be grouped by the quantity of our fingers or space by the size of our feet; such patternings are instead circumstantial, reflecting the material components of their conceptualization.

Time also differs in the way it emerges. Across cultures, the beginnings of space and number are perceptual and anthropomorphic: space and quantity are visually appreciable in ways which are directly comparable to the human body. Space, the distance across visible surfaces (Gibson Reference Gibson, Fraser and Lawrence1975), is compared to the lengths and widths of thumbs, hands, forearms, arm spans, feet and paces, while quantity, a property appreciable through the so-called number sense, is represented and expressed with fingers and body parts like toes. Such body-based methods suffice, at least until societies need to measure agricultural fields and monuments or count beyond the hands’ capacity and persistence.

In contrast, the body is not the first device for time, since time is neither an extent comparable to the limbs nor a quantity the digits can instantiate. Invisible, intangible and silent, time is experienced as duration (an internal awareness) and change (an external observation). Duration is highly subjective: time can fly, take forever, or briefly vanish, and what one person experiences can have little relevance to what others encounter in the identical context. Change is greatly variable: ‘pebbles roll, the leaves fall, the streams flow, and the animals scurry about. … and the sun moves across the heavens’ (Gibson Reference Gibson, Fraser and Lawrence1975, 297). Yet most things change too imperceptibly or unpredictably to be used as a basis for time. To infer time from change, that change must be recurrent and regular.

The planetary cycles have the requisite characteristics. Our planet spins on a tilted axis and revolves around the sun with an encircling moon, creating daily alternations of light and dark, the yearly round of seasons, and a dependable if more variable lunar presence. However ancient timekeeping might be, the planetary cycles which once patterned prehistoric time continue to pattern it today: months and weeks still reflect the moon's periodicity and four-phased appearance, the year is still based on the planet's orbit around the sun, and intercalary periods are still needed to keep the two in sync. Before and without cultural constructs to structure time, we experience the planetary cycles through circadian rhythms of wakefulness and sleep, changes and trends in temperature and weather, and ontogenetic trajectories spanning birth, maturation, reproduction, decline and death.

Despite our shared planetary circumstances and species capacities, not all societies structure time. For the Pirahã, an Indigenous people of Amazonian Brazil, the experience of time is summed up ‘with a simple formula: “Live here and now.” … Living in the now also fits with the fact the Pirahã don't appear to have a creation myth explaining existence. When asked, they simply reply: “Everything is the same, things always are”’ (Daniel Everett, quoted in von Bredow Reference von Bredow2006). One way to understand this is by distinguishing time as subjectively experienced (internal, lived, phenomenological, or psychological time) from time as objectively measured (external or physical time) (Degni Reference Degni and Pickren2022). The Pirahã undoubtedly experience time subjectively, but they do not measure or count it. Significantly, the Pirahã are better known for another unique trait: They lack a system of numbers, not just an inability to count but the absence of numerical expressions (Everett Reference Everett2005). While the degree to which the Pirahã are truly anumeric is debated (Gordon Reference Gordon2004), a lack of numbers relates logically to the absence of a structured concept of time: societies which do not count anything do not count time, and counting is a primary mechanism for creating and structuring concepts of time.

Assuming all humans experience subjective time, and taking the absence of objective time as our notional starting point, the present analysis examines how human societies count and measure time and the effects the devices used have on its conceptualization. Our ability to develop concepts of objective time is rooted in several biological–psychological mechanisms. From this foundation, we add, first, the ability to associate naturally occurring phenomena with time, and second, the ability to use material devices to count and measure it. We review key technologies like the gnomon, the sundial element which casts the shadow used for daily and yearly time. Through the use of such devices, time becomes objective, and its conceptualization inherits aspects of the physical structures used to represent it.

The biological–psychological foundations of subjective time

Subjective time is informed by several biological–psychological mechanisms which humans share with other species. First are the circadian rhythms, internally generated wake–sleep cycles which keep the planet's inhabitants attuned, more or less, to its cycle of days and nights (Panda et al. Reference Panda, Hogenesch and Kay2002). This system is sloppy because the sense of time it produces is highly variable; it is also sluggish because an hour as experienced is 1.12 times longer than what the clock measures (Campbell Reference Campbell and Block2014). Other mechanisms are interval timing, a sense of duration spanning fractions of seconds to hours (Merchant & de Lafuente Reference Merchant and de Lafuente2014), and temporal discrimination, the ability to discern ‘before’ and ‘after’ in stimuli separated by at least several milliseconds (Block Reference Block and Block2014). In addition, hormonal mechanisms and physiological responses inform seasonal behaviours like mating, nesting, migrating and coat change (Wingfield et al. Reference Wingfield, Jacobs and Hillgarth1997), matters to which humans are not immune (e.g. seasonal affective disorder).

Beyond these mechanisms is the common neural substrate for time, space and number in the primate parietal cortex, particularly the intraparietal sulcus (Dehaene & Brannon Reference Dehaene and Brannon2011). The right posterior parietal cortex has a critical role in the timing of stimuli from different sensory modalities (Bueti et al. Reference Bueti, Bahrami and Walsh2008). Visuospatial perception, the ability to perceive objects in terms of their physical location in relation to us and to each other which is subserved by the parietal cortex, is also important to the experience of time, as temporal judgements enable motor actions in space (Simmons Reference Simmons2003). Neurons of the intraparietal sulcus, which have also been implicated in visual attention, correlate with processes which relate to time (duration), space (distance) and number (quantity) in monkeys (Leon & Shadlen Reference Leon and Shadlen2003), species used as human proxies because they are evolutionarily close enough to provide neurological insights, while distant enough to be subjected to invasive techniques. Interruptions to visuospatial input from rapid eye movements (‘saccades’) compress the visual perception of space and interrupt the visual perception of quantity, thereby altering the perception of duration (Morrone et al. Reference Morrone, Ross and Burr2005). The similarities in these stimulus responses and attentional effects support the idea of a common neurological mechanism (Burr et al. Reference Burr, Turi and Anobile2010), as does synaesthesia, a neurological condition in which information from one sensory domain activates unrelated senses, causing them to be experienced simultaneously (Cohen Kadosh et al. Reference Cohen Kadosh, Gertner and Terhune2012; Hubbard Reference Hubbard2007).

Memory is also important. Impairments to short-term and general memory in conditions like amnesia and Alzheimer's disease decrease both accuracy and precision in the ability to estimate time's passage (Nichelli et al. Reference Nichelli, Venneri and Molinari1993). Working memory, the mental ability to hold and manipulate multiple pieces of information in conscious attention (Baddeley Reference Baddeley1986), has been implicated in both the subjective experience of time and mathematical cognition (LeFevre et al. Reference LeFevre, DeStefano, Coleman and Campbell2005). Timing and time perception rely on an internal biological clock which outputs time interval estimations into working memory (Gibbon et al. Reference Gibbon, Church and Meck1984). In addition, episodic memory, which stores personal experiences, connects our past to our present and enables us to forecast our future (Tulving Reference Tulving2002). The contents of these different forms of memory then inform decision-making processes (Wearden Reference Wearden and Helfrich2003).

Given these biological–psychological capacities, humans experience time much like other species do. However, we also engage with material forms in unique ways, and it is this engagement which enables us to conceive of time as a substance with structure and other properties.

Timekeeping by natural associations

Timekeeping, the activity of keeping or marking a steady rhythm, begins as the recognition that two things correspond—perhaps the first appearance of particular stars and a part of the year. For example, the Alaskan Alutiiq begin their year in August, naming it (as translated) ‘the Pleiades begin to come’ (Clark Reference Clark1974, 75). In southern Africa, the first appearance of the Pleiades resolves ‘any confusion over which month it might be’ (Macdonald Reference Macdonald1890, 194). Two things are in correspondence: an aspect of the natural world and ‘time’. The latter is a concept of something intangible and perhaps ineffable; the former is physical and visible, and it gains tangibility and manipulability when a device is used to represent it (Malafouris Reference Malafouris2013).

Shadows are environmental features whose recurrent, reliable change is directly related to the planetary cycles; thus, they frequently serve as a proxy for time, illustrating our ability to infer causal relations. Shadows are longest at dawn and dusk, shortest at noon, and move between these extremes throughout the day. A shadow's length and bearing from the object casting it provide a dependable if approximate time of day. Processing shadow information involves perceptual assumptions—for example, that there is a single, relatively stationary light source somewhere ‘above’—which enable contextual inferences about the location, movement, direction, trajectory, shape, depth and background of both the light source and the object casting the shadow (Casati & Cavanagh Reference Casati and Cavanagh2019). The abilities to make perceptual assumptions about shadows and infer causal relations from them are demonstrated by both human infants and non-human primates (Imura et al. Reference Imura, Tomonaga, Yagi, Matsuzawa, Tomonaga and Tanaka2006). It is a small but crucial step from such basic perceptual inferences to the other contextual inferences which are the essence of timekeeping by means of natural associations: the sun's height in the sky, the moon's cycles and phases, the recurrence of seasons and stars, the presence and behaviour of plants and animals.

Human societies are motivated to keep track of time because the planetary cycles are informative about upcoming changes in conditions and resource availability, matters essential to survival. A sense of the stakes involved comes from the names given to the months by the Alaskan Aleuts: March is ‘when straps are eaten—starvation’; April is the ‘end of eating straps’ and the ‘time for leaving houses’; and May is the ‘month of flowers’ (Veniaminov Reference Veniaminov1840, 256–8). At the opposite end of the resourcing spectrum are the names of the months in siLozi, an African Bantu language: January is the month ‘when food is so abundant that children eat too much and defecate anywhere’; February is ‘when fish can be seen swimming in small pools’; and March is ‘the time of flood waters’ (Lukusa Reference Lukusa2005, 18–19).

Such names do more than reflect, codify and predict natural conditions: They also imply the use of the sun or stars for timekeeping because one month is usually longer than the others, a typical intercalation for keeping such sequences aligned to the solar year. For the Aleut, the eleventh month was longer (Veniaminov Reference Veniaminov1840), an intercalary strategy akin to adding a thirteenth month every other year, as was practised by the Romans. The use of the moon is also likely as a plausible basis for dividing the year into 12 months. Since lunations shift across the year in the same way weeks do the modern months, a true lunar calendar soon becomes out of sync with the seasons; this suggests the divisor 12 might reflect an older lunar calendar (Nilsson Reference Nilsson and Fielden1920). Twelve could also be coincidental. Seasonally based calendars can have as few as two months; they may count seasons without combining them to produce calendrical years only half a solar year in length, and they can omit certain periods of time altogether, generally those in which little of societal interest takes place (Nilsson Reference Nilsson and Fielden1920). In any case, this type of timekeeping is inexact, relative to astronomically or mathematically based calendars. As noted for the Yuki, a North American people with a seasonally based calendar, the named month was ‘not to be taken literally—it [was] simply the informant's guess of the approximate time of year’ (Foster Reference Foster1944, 202).

Another motivation is the usefulness of time. In the Kalahari, time is used to estimate the distance covered in walking, with speed and distance contextualized by gender and purpose: A woman gathering walks more slowly and covers less distance (1–2 km/hour) than a man does hunting (6–7 km/hour) (Silberbauer Reference Silberbauer1981). Total distance is ‘expressed as the number of nights one would sleep while making the journey, and fractions of a remaining day are indicated by pointing to the position in the sky of the sun’ expected on the traveller's arrival (Silberbauer Reference Silberbauer1981, 98). Here we find both counting and natural associations: completed days are marked by the arrival of night, and the approximate time of a partial day is expressed by the sun's expected location. Such gestural indication is a tropical practice; this is unsurprising, given the variation in daylight and the sun's height from summer to winter in temperate and polar zones (Nilsson Reference Nilsson and Fielden1920).

When timekeeping might have emerged is a question on which the archaeological record is mostly silent, as the kind of natural associations used as early calendars leave few material traces. However, tools like traps and snares, whose yields occur in a future which involves remembering where they were placed in the past, imply the ability to envision past and future (Coolidge & Wynn Reference Coolidge and Wynn2018; Tulving Reference Tulving2002). Archaeological evidence suggests traps and snares were in use by 75,000 years ago (Wadley Reference Wadley2010).

Artefacts which hint at timekeeping begin to appear around 40,000 years ago as pieces of inscribed bone. Lunar timekeeping is suspected whenever the incisions total 28–32 in number, or are organized into groups in this range, as it corresponds to the moon's observed periodicity (Marshack Reference Marshack1985; Reference Marshack1991). An example is the Lebombo bone, a baboon fibula engraved with 29 marks from Border Cave, South Africa, dated to 42,000 years ago (Fig. 1). However, its lunar interpretation is doubtful, for several reasons. First, the bone is broken off at one end, raising the possibility the total number of notches was originally greater than 29 by an unknown amount. Second, the artefact is only 76 mm in length (d'Errico et al. Reference d'Errico, Backwell and Villa2012) with infinitesimal incisions (1.5–2.5 mm), sizes suggesting it likely served a personal purpose, whereas notations which serve purposes like timekeeping tends to be public (Hayden Reference Hayden2021). Third, while the artefact has a glossy surface indicative of long-term curation (d'Errico et al. Reference d'Errico, Doyon and Colagé2018), there are no physical signs it was used as a calendar. That is, to function for keeping time beyond a single lunar cycle, permanent incisions would need to involve some method of incrementation, but no traces of two likely methods, colouration and fibre wear, have been reported (Overmann Reference Overmann2023). Finally, for contemporary artefacts of similar appearance whose meaning has been interpreted by knowledgeable cultural informants, around 30 marks can mean ‘all’ of a category of items (Kelly Reference Kelly2020), rather than the number of lunar nights.

Figure 1. Engraved baboon fibula from Border Cave, South Africa (three views). (Image from d'Errico et al. Reference d'Errico, Backwell and Villa2012, supporting information, fig. 8, © PNAS and used with permission.)

Another artefact with potential relevance to prehistoric timekeeping (Fig. 2) comes from Abri Blanchard, a site near Sergeac, France. The marks on this artefact, a piece of reindeer bone 11 cm long dated to 35,000 years ago, form a serpentine figure which resembles the lunar analemma, the trace of the different heights to which the moon rises over successive nights during a period of several months (Jègues-Wolkiewiez Reference Jègues-Wolkiewiez2005). Their individual shapes also suggest the moon's phases (Marshack Reference Marshack1991). The majority (65 of 69) of marks on the artefact fit the predicted pattern; the remaining four were likely a mistake (Jègues-Wolkiewiez Reference Jègues-Wolkiewiez2005; Rappenglück Reference Rappenglück, Šprajc and Pehani2013), a reasonable hypothesis given the overall conformity. Since counting lunar nights does not require characterizing the moon's varied height or its waxing–waning change, the artefact seems not to represent timekeeping per se. Nonetheless, if the interpretation is correct, the artefact implies a dedicated skywatcher, suggesting the development of specialization and a degree of sedentism, since the observations were made from a stable location over a period of several months. Certainly, a specialist keeping watch on the moon suggests a society likely to have used the lunar cycles for timekeeping.

Figure 2. Engraved reindeer bone from Abri Blanchard, France. (Image: CC BY-SA 4.0 with added scale.)

A third hint of Palaeolithic moon-watching comes from Dolní Věstonice in the Czech Republic (Fig. 3). Dated to 26,000 years ago, this grey marlstone rod 13.0 × 1.7 × 1.0 cm in size was reconstructed from multiple fragments; 30 long and short marks form groups of 5, 7, 7, 5, 5, possibly indicating the different luminescence conditions created by the new crescent, waxing half-moon, full moon, waning half-moon and disappearing crescent (Emmerling et al. Reference Emmerling, Geer and Klíma1993). The artefact implies specialization, as the Abri Blanchard bone did, and its notational groupings suggest a concern with the availability of moonlight throughout a lunar month, perhaps in support of forecasting conditions suitable for night-time activities. The groupings also show that interpreting prehistoric markings requires astronomical insight beyond simply knowing the number of lunar nights. Nonetheless, while the interpretation is astronomically plausible, it too remains speculative.

Figure 3. Engraved marlstone rod from Dolní Věstonice, Czech Republic. (Redrawn and adapted from Emmerling et al. Reference Emmerling, Geer and Klíma1993, 153 fig. 1 & 156 table 1.)

The marks and dots which often accompany paintings in the Upper Palaeolithic caves of France and Spain may have recorded information about the lifecycles of animals used for food, particularly mating and birth (Bacon et al. Reference Bacon, Khatiri, Palmer, Freeth, Pettitt and Kentridge2023). However, in contemporary cultures, similar marks often represent hunting kills (Nilsson Reference Nilsson and Fielden1920). Further, contemporary cultures are not associated with calendars that are notational in form, seasonally organized and focused on animal lifecycles. Instead, contemporary cultures tend to count time with tallies which accumulate lunations for purposes involving human social matters like pregnancy duration, and by the time calendars are written, they are no longer seasonally based. On the other hand, uniqueness does not completely exclude the possibility prehistoric peoples tracked animal lifecycles using notations. Certainly, the proposed interpretation would be consistent with the specialization, sedentism, notational adeptness and lunar concerns implied by the Abri Blanchard and Dolní Věstonice artefacts. Nonetheless, when artefactual meaning is opaque and there are no cultural informants to provide reliable insight, such interpretations remain speculative. Additional criticisms are offered in Nowell et al. Reference Nowell, Bahn and Le Quellec2024.

These devices imply the practice of timekeeping may be quite old, but they do not establish whether or when it had emerged or the form it first took. Nonetheless, they raise the question of why human societies might incorporate such devices into the cognitive system for time. A reasonable answer is this: although the natural and celestial phenomena used for timekeeping are features of the world, they are not yet features which can be touched and manipulated in the way bones and marks can be. It is to unambiguous timekeeping by means of such devices we now turn.

Counting and measuring time

The use of tallies for timekeeping is common among contemporary societies. These devices involve correspondences in the same way natural associations did, except they represent them in a way which depends less on memory and context. A mark generally means a unit of time, often a completed day. Time marked by tallying might be used to estimate distance in travel, typically the amount of ground covered in a day's worth of walking. For example, an Australian Wathaurung messenger painted his arm with 14 stripes of mud, one for each day of his journey across the Outback (Morgan Reference Morgan1852).Footnote 1 On his return trip, he would erase a stripe each day. Numbers per se were not involved in this quantification of time: some 50 years later, the Wathaurung number system still counted no higher than three (Mathews Reference Mathews1904). However, the absence of higher numbers was no challenge, since it is difficult to imagine the stripes themselves or the days they enumerated as being individuated in a way requiring either to be named. That is, each stripe did not mean a particular day had passed or needed to be individually recalled; rather, each stripe meant another day had passed and the return journey would need to include a corresponding day (Overmann Reference Overmann2023).

Another common form of tally was noted for the Siuai of Papua New Guinea: ‘In setting a date for, say, a feast, the host sends to each of his principal guests a palm frond having a number of leaves equal to the number of days before the feast. Thereafter, the host and his guests tear off one leaf each day to mark the passage of time before the feast’ (Oliver Reference Oliver1955, 98). Like the Wathaurung example, this form of tally need not involve counting per se, since determining ‘whether two collections have the same number of terms’ does not require ‘defin[ing] what that number is’ (Russell Reference Russell1920, 15). Whether or not counting is involved, such material forms keep track of an interval of days with an accuracy and precision which natural associations and human memory cannot match. The information remains public, while becoming dispatchable. The incorporation and use of such devices occasions new relations (e.g. associations between days and device) and social behaviours (setting future dates; daily interaction with the device).

The solar year and intercalation periods

Tallying works well for solar days and lunar months, but the solar year requires different strategies. One is the horizon calendar (Fig. 4), which involves observing where the sun rises (or sets) on the horizon over the course of a year, activity which implies specialization and profound sedentism. In the northern hemisphere, the sun achieves its furthest horizonal positions north and south at the summer and winter solstices (respectively, 21 June and 21 December). Where the sun rises on the eastern horizon is compared to its major landmarks; these alignments inform agricultural activities and time annual rites and celebrations (Ezcurra et al. Reference Ezcurra, Ezcurra and Meissner2022; Forde Reference Forde1931; Zeilik Reference Zeilik1985b).

Figure 4. Horizon calendars. (Top) Basin of Mexico as viewed from Templo Mayor (19°N). Where the sun rises along the horizon shifts north and south between the solstices, with the equinoxes falling midway between the two. (Redrawn and relabelled from Ezcurra et al. Reference Ezcurra, Ezcurra and Meissner2022, 4 fig. 2c; also see Aveni Reference Aveni2001, 242–3.) (Bottom) Eastern horizon from Shungopavi, Second Mesa (36°N), as originally drawn by an Indigenous informant. The annual planting cycle corresponds to the sun's rising position as read right-to-left along the top, then left-to-right along the bottom. (Redrawn and relabelled from Forde Reference Forde1931, 386 fig. 6b; translations from McCluskey Reference McCluskey, Boutsikas, McCluskey and Steele2021 and pers. comm. 29 December 2023 and 4 January 2024.)

Close observation of the sun is essential to resolving the mismatch between the lunar and solar cycles. One lunar phase cycle, defined as one new moon to the next, is 29.5 days, a number which does not evenly divide a solar year of 365.2 days. When societies which keep time by the moon notice the mismatch, they tend to repeat or skip a lunar month to bring them back into sync (Nilsson Reference Nilsson and Fielden1920). A formal intercalation period eventually develops to keep the cycles synchronized. For example, the Babylonians and Romans periodically injected a thirteenth month to keep their lunar months aligned to the solar year (Parker & Dubberstein Reference Parker and Dubberstein1956; Warmington Reference Warmington1940). In comparison, the Maya divided the solar year into 18 (non-lunar) months of 20 days; to keep these cycles aligned, a five-day month was appended to the year's end to bring the total to 365 days (Stanzione Reference Stanzione2003).

Time changes when it is counted and measured

As numbers elaborate, societies begin using them to count things never counted before, including time (Overmann Reference Overmann2013). For example, the North American Quinault used knotted cords (a form of tally) to count lunations, menstrual cycles and the amount of time between a marriage and the birth of a couple's first child in order to establish the heir's legitimacy (Olson Reference Olson1936). Societies also begin counting human age, either characterizing it in relation to events which identify a specific year or counting it by the number of winters which have passed since birth. In comparison, the Andamans Islanders had ‘no means of counting, and no words to express gradations of time … They take no interest in the passing of years, and have no idea of their own age’ (Cipriani & Tayler Reference Cipriani and Tayler1966, 151). This does not mean people in societies which do not count time fail to notice change in, for example, their own bodies, for such things are often formalized with public rites transitioning juveniles to adulthood. Rather, it means time is unlikely to be conceived as a structured substance until it is counted.

Counting time gives structure to its conceptualization. The Pirahã, a society which lacks numbers, live in the present ‘now’ and lack an origin myth (Everett Reference Everett2005). Once time begins to be counted, a period of the past identified as ‘many ago’ emerges (Gusinde Reference Gusinde1931). As numbers elaborate and the counting of time continues, the long ago and the present eventually yield to a tripartite division: the present time, a mythic past, and an intermediate period which loosely encompasses the last few hundred years (Overmann Reference Overmann2013). However, as time passes, the intermediate period may not become longer or the mythic past more remote from the present (Gell Reference Gell2020). Nonetheless, counting time not only makes time into a substance which can be counted, it gives it an overall structure. Societies become able not merely to count their days, but also to conceive time as something structured as today, history, and myth.

Harnessing the shadow to visualize time in spatial terms

A common strategy for making the passage of time visible and measurable, as distinct from counting the recurrence of cycles, involves the shadow. Typically, visualization and measurement involve a gnomon, the shadow-casting piece in a sundial. A gnomon can be as simple as a stick set into the ground, so it easily falls within the technological repertoire of even very early societies (Tyson Reference Tyson2003). Beyond indicating the daylight hours, shadow lengths, when closely measured, enable detection of solstices, the year's longest and shortest shadows; equinoxes, equivalent midpoint shadows between the solstice extremes; and latitude, a variable informing shadow length. Solstices and equinoxes delimit the solar year, which is generally calculated from solstice to solstice or from equinox to equinox. Latitudinal variability enabled the Greek mathematician Eratosthenes to deduce the planet's circumference in the late first millennium bce (Orzel Reference Orzel2022). A gnomon can also be used to determine the four cardinal directions (Fig. 5). A circle is drawn around the gnomon, as guided by a string attached to its base, and the points where its shadow crosses the circle in the morning and afternoon are marked; a line through these points runs east–west, while a line placed perpendicular to it runs north–south (Kelley & Milone Reference Kelley and Milone2005).

Figure 5. Gnomon used for cardinal alignments. (Image: author.)

Evidence of ancient shadow use is found across the globe in the form of gnomons and sundials, textual records of shadow recordings, and cardinal alignments in monumental constructions. One of the oldest artefacts was discovered in Egypt's Valley of the Kings about a decade ago (Fig. 6). Dated to the second millennium bce, this limestone slab measures 17.8×15.2×3.3 cm and contains a hole for a gnomon surrounded by an ink semicircle divided to convert the shadow into hours (Bickel & Gautschy Reference Bickel and Gautschy2014). It was found on the floor of a workman's hut surrounded by limestone chips with sketches and short inscriptions, a context suggesting it regulated the workdays of the artisans who decorated the Valley's tombs (Bickel & Gautschy Reference Bickel and Gautschy2014). The semicircle is marked to make the hours fairly equal in duration, with the exception of a longer sixth hour (12.5 on the figure), which perhaps encompassed a midday break (Vodolazhskaya Reference Vodolazhskaya2014). The method of marking the hours, along with context of the sundial's findspot, suggest such artefacts were used to regulate daytime activities in the manner of the analogue clock, its technological descendent.

Figure 6. Vertical sundial from the Valley of the Kings. (Redrawn and relabelled from Vodolazhskaya Reference Vodolazhskaya2014, 3 fig. 2 & 6 fig. 5.)

Tables of shadow lengths for solstices and equinoxes, measurements used in astronomy and divination, are attested in Mesopotamia towards the end of the second millennium bce (Rochberg Reference Rochberg2010). Other Babylonian texts describe how to construct a sundial: a gnomon was to be set up on a stone slab; its lines were drawn according to its morning and noon shadows as the longest and shortest of the day, with the solstice shadows giving the longest (winter) and shortest (summer) shadows of the year (Rochberg Reference Rochberg2010). As reconstructed, the Babylonian sundial corresponds to something recorded by the Greek historian Herodotus: ‘the sunclock and the sundial, and the 12 divisions of the day, came to Hellas from Babylonia and not from Egypt’ (Herodotus II:109; as translated in Godley Reference Godley1922). The 12 divisions of the Babylonian day applied to the period between sunrise and sunset, with two successive sunrises comprising a 24-hour cycle—a conceptualization still in use today, albeit starting at midnight (or sundown) and with hours of equal duration. These records and their complex mathematical applications suggest Babylonian shadow use was much older than the second millennium bce.

In China, gnomonic observations ensured the imperial calendar was accurate and precise. These attributes were critical to governance, as the divine mandate of the emperor, the ‘Son of Heaven’, was considerably weakened if the moon and sun failed to obey the calendar; after all, why should peasants ‘pay taxes and serve in the army if the emperor didn't know the secrets of the heavens?’ (Aslaksen Reference Aslaksen2003, 34). The oldest Chinese gnomon found to date, a painted stick dated to 2300 bce, comes from Taosi, northern China (Li Reference Li and Ruggles2015). There is textual evidence of early gnomon use: shadow lengths were recorded in the Zhoubi suanjing (Gnomon of the Zhou Dynasty), representing observations made around 2000 bce (Li Reference Li and Ruggles2015). Errors suggest its intermediate shadow lengths were derived rather than measured, as adjusted by the cun qian li, a heuristic for correlating shadow length with latitude which was vital to synchronizing imperial timekeeping across a vast empire (Li & Sun Reference Li and Sun2009). The heuristic said the shadow of a gnomon 8 chi tall (~1.96 m) increases by 1 cun (⅒ chi) for every 1000 li (about 400 km) of movement north; it decreases by the same amount when moved south (Li & Sun Reference Li and Sun2009). In comparison, changes in longitude affect time, with each degree of movement adding (west) or subtracting (east) about four minutes, relative to the original longitude (Sobel Reference Sobel2007). As was true of Mesopotamia, these records and complex mathematical applications suggest Chinese shadow use significantly predates its appearance in artefactual and textual forms.

Among the most famous architectural alignments with the sun are the pyramids at Giza, built during the third millennium bce; the four corners of their bases point precisely east–west and north–south. Pyramidal alignments honoured the sun god Ra, ancient Egypt's most important deity, with the east–west direction marking the trajectory from birth to death which terrestrial life shared with Ra's daily transit across the sky (Wilkinson Reference Wilkinson2017). In Babylon, the four corners of the Ziggurat of Ur, an elevated brick platform used as temple and celestial observatory during the third millennium bce, were oriented towards the cardinal directions (Woolley Reference Woolley1924), as were the so-called Chinese pyramids used as royal tombs in the first millennium bce (Magli Reference Magli2018). In Europe, Stonehenge (2000–3000 bce) and Newgrange (3200 bce) were built with solar alignments: Stonehenge looks to the place on the horizon where the sun rises on the summer solstice and sets on the winter solstice, while Newgrange contains a special ‘roof-box’ which admits the sun on the winter solstice, the only sunlight to reach inside the chamber during a year (Orzel Reference Orzel2022). In the New World, architectural alignments are found at Chichén Itzá, Yucatán Peninsula, where the Pyramid of Kukulcán (1000 ce), ‘the feathered serpent god of rebirth and creativity’, produces striking equinoctial interplays of sunlight, shadows, stairs and carvings which suggest an enormous serpent descends its northern stairway (Aveni et al. Reference Aveni, Milbrath and Lope2004, 123). In Machu Picchu (1450 ce) and Cusco (1100 ce), Peru and Chaco Canyon, New Mexico (900–1150 ce), buildings and streets align with solstices and equinoxes, publicly marking the timing of annual celebrations and notable events (Aveni Reference Aveni2001; Dearborn & Schreiber Reference Dearborn and Schreiber1986; Sofaer Reference Sofaer and Lekson2007; Zeilik Reference Zeilik1985a).

The ancient astronomical traditions share the incorporation of celestial phenomena into cosmologies, culturally constructed ways of understanding how society, nature and the divine are interconnected (Iwaniszewski Reference Iwaniszewski and Rubiño-Martín2009). They also differ, first, by the effect of latitude on the sun's geometry, and second, by the presence or absence of writing. In the temperate zones, stars circle the celestial pole, so ‘either the celestial equator or the ecliptic [becomes used] … as the fundamental reference plane’ (Aveni Reference Aveni1981, 163). In the tropics, the sun passes directly overhead at certain times of year, so the astronomical frame of reference is based on zenith (the point directly overhead), nadir (its subsurface point), and the horizon (Aveni Reference Aveni1981). Novel technologies exploit the zenith: a hollow vertical tube will admit sunlight into a chamber on zenith dates; these alignments permitted a more precise estimation of the equinox, relative to shadow measurements (Aveni Reference Aveni2001). As for writing, Mesopotamia, Egypt, China and Mesoamerica were the epicentres of original writing systems where literacy and mathematics developed, enabling the recording of astronomical data and the calculation and prediction of celestial movements and events. In comparison, European and North American astronomy was performed by oral societies which preserved their celestial observations as directional alignments in monuments, architecture and roads.

The visualization of time

Clocks and calendars represent time in spatial terms to help us visualize, codify and organize it (de Smedt & de Cruz Reference de Smedt and de Cruz2011). Where clocks make visible the time of day, calendars visualize longer cycles—months and years. Cross-culturally, calendars are differentiated by the specific celestial features used as their basis, the strategies employed to keep the cycles in sync, and whether the cycles are astronomically observed or mathematically calculated. Calendars come in three basic types: solar, lunar and lunisolar. A solar calendar is based on the planet's orbit around the sun, as assessed by its effects on the solar geometry: the mechanical properties and alignments which produce the solstices and equinoxes, seasons, and annual recurrence of stars. Sub-divisions need not be lunar (e.g. the 20-day months of the Maya solar calendar). A lunar calendar is based on the moon, as marked (for example) by the first appearance of the new moon; this yields a period of 29.5 days. Since ‘a half day cannot be observed, and since weather may obscure observation’, the lunar month, as observed and recorded, can range from 28 to 32 days (Marshack Reference Marshack1985, 31). As lunations do not evenly divide the solar year, a lunar calendar is not aligned to it. A lunisolar calendar uses both solar and lunar cycles and must find a way to keep the two in sync, since there are more than 12 but fewer than 13 lunations per solar year.

Calendars can take different physical forms. A calendar can be a sequence of conditions and activities, as for example, the Aleut and Bantu months whose names describe, codify and predict recurrent aspects of the natural and social worlds. The names form an ordinal sequence, a progression of invariant elements (e.g. months of the year; days of the week; the counting numbers) subserved by a dedicated neural network (Cohen Kadosh & Gertner Reference Cohen Kadosh, Gertner, Dehaene and Brannon2011). A dedicated neural network is the kind of evolutionary change which typically occurs over a long period of time, and this suggests human societies began forming such sequences at a fairly remote time in prehistory. Ordinal sequences are also overlearned, which means they become fixed in memory in a way which makes them resist the normal processes which change memorized information and word forms (an examination of these effects in the counting numbers one through five is provided by Pagel & Meade Reference Pagel and Meade2017). These attributes not only ensure the continuity of such sequences, they also connect their present forms to their past forms and explain their persistence.

Sequences of marks like the Wathaurung mud stripes and Siuai palm fronds form calendars, with each mark (stripe or frond) meaning one day's worth of time. Made of perishable materials, these ad hoc devices synchronize activity within short, specific temporal contexts, typically days or weeks separating related social events: embarking upon and then returning from a journey; scheduling and later celebrating a ritual. Such devices could potentially be made of durable materials. As mentioned earlier, a durable calendar accumulating lunar nights implies the ability to display and predict the completion of subsequent cycles. Such employment would reasonably involve a method of incrementation similar to those used with the Wathaurung stripes and Siuai fronds.

Shadows used to indicate time of year form calendars. The apparent motion of the sun slows down at the solstices, the points where it stands still (solstitium) and then turns in the direction of its movement (e.g. Fig. 4). On a sundial, if the distance between the solstice shadows is evenly divided, the months around the solstices will be longer in their number of days, those at the equinoxes shorter. The Javanese solar calendar illustrates the effect: of the 12 months in the calendar, those centred around the solstices are 41–43 days long, those centred on the equinoxes just 23–27 days (Aveni Reference Aveni1981, 167). The same solar geometry affects daily timekeeping, as equidistant marks produce unequal hours. To make the hours equal in duration, marks must be unevenly spaced, with those centred on noon placed closer together, those towards sunrise and sunset spaced more widely apart (e.g. Fig. 6).

Landscape can be a calendar, implying a society which is sedentary. Calendars formed from the natural landscape are shown in Figure 3. Calendars formed by cultural landscapes include megaliths (Newgrange, Stonehenge), pyramids (Egypt, Mesopotamia, China), and architecture and roads (Chichén Itzá, Cusco, Machu Picchu, Chaco Canyon). The large earthen mounds raised in Cahokia, North America (c. 1100 ce) bridge the natural and cultural landscapes (Aveni Reference Aveni2001). All these structures were aligned to represent and highlight various solar, lunar and celestial phenomena in ways which were public and impressive (Aveni Reference Aveni2001).

A calendar can be a circle, a form evoking the movement of a gnomon's shadow, the curve of the natural horizon, the sun's arc through the sky, and the rotation of stars around the celestial pole. Megaliths like Stonehenge are typically circular in their layout, reflecting and perhaps representing the horizon (Aveni Reference Aveni2001). The stones at Wurdi Youang, an Australian site possibly more than 11,000 years old, are also roughly arranged in a circle (Fitzsimmons Reference Fitzsimmons2016; Hamacher & Norris Reference Hamacher, Norris and Ruggles2011). The Aztec calendar stone, an impressively carved piece of basalt some 3.6 m in diameter housed in Mexico City's Museo Nacional de Antropologia, reiterates the circular shape. The centre of the circle depicts a deity, perhaps the sun god Tonatiuh, the earth god Tlaltecuhtli, or a hybrid of the two (Villela et al. Reference Villela, Robb and Miller2010). The deity is encircled by glyphs representing the 20 days of the Aztec month, and then by rings representing eclipse markers, solar rays, full moons and the cardinal directions. These cosmological themes suggest the calendar functioned to visualize time, rather than indicate it observationally in the way the circles at Stonehenge and Wurdi Youang did.

Finally, a calendar can be written, a development necessarily following the invention of writing. A written calendar is typically a table, a form permitting specialists to organize, compare and analyse data on shadow lengths and celestial movements. Tables are found in the calendrical traditions of the Near East, China and Mesoamerica. Tables are also the direct precursor of the modern calendar. An early example is the Fasti Antiates Maiores (Fig. 7), a table 1.16×2.50 m in size painted on a wall of the ancient Roman city of Antium and dated to 84–55 bce (Hannah Reference Hannah, Seaes, Keegan and Laurence2013). It consisted of 12 months of 28, 29, or 31 days organized into eight-day weeks; a thirteenth intercalary month of 27 days was inserted every other year, producing alternating years of 355 or 377 days long (Warmington Reference Warmington1940). This table was accompanied by a list of consul names, juxtaposing the calendar's cyclical time with a linear sequence of historical events (Feeney Reference Feeney2007; Laurence & Smith Reference Laurence and Smith1996). The list reflects a developing trend towards numbering years consecutively instead of restarting their count with each new ruler's reign. Consecutive numbering would eventually become anchored by a specific event (e.g. Gregorian, Islamic and Hebrew) or cycle (e.g. Chinese and Maya).

Figure 7. Fasti Antiates Maiores (as reconstructed). The top row lists the first day of each month and the month names (January–December plus an intercalary month). Days are marked A–H (eight-day weeks); also marked are the Kalends (first day of a month), Nones (new moons), and Ides (full moons) (Warmington Reference Warmington1940, 453–65). The bottom row lists the number of days in each month. (Image: CC BY-SA 4.0.)

The material component of time and its conceptual influence

Human thinking reflects the material circumstances of its evolution. Our thoughts are not merely ‘about’ the world and its various parts and properties; our cognition is specifically adapted to the planet we live on and the kind of species we are—for example, our sensory and perceptual systems are adapted to detect and extract information from our natural and social environments. Thus far, these are not contentious positions. It is more radical to see material forms not as merely the object, context or stimulus of our thinking, but as a constitutive component of it (Malafouris Reference Malafouris2013). One way to approach this is to see our use of material forms as rearranging the world in ways which let us make different sense of it. In this approach, rather than being the passive recipients of our mental content, material devices are a domain which co-creates our thinking as it interacts with our brains, bodies and behaviours.

We are looking for signs of material influence on our concepts of time, similar to those found in numbers and space. In numbers, our concepts are fundamentally and pervasively structured by the use of the hands as the first contiguous device used for representation and expression; the structural properties which emerge from using the fingers in counting include discreteness, stable order, linearity and productive grouping (Overmann Reference Overmann2023). Complex concepts of numbers which lack these properties are almost inconceivable, not just because these properties are the reason the concepts are complex, but also because it is difficult to imagine what complex numbers would be like without such properties—if they were even possible. It is similarly difficult to imagine what sort of thing space would be if it were not measured and parsed in units like feet and metres. For time, a parallel hypothetical case would need to be independent of the planet we live on and our biological–psychological capacities for subjective time. Whatever this hypothetical construct might be, it would surely be far removed from time as we currently conceive it. The best we seem able to do is envision counterfactuals—time travel, enchanted sleep, heavenly eternity—which imagine an escape from time's parameters but do not challenge its nature as conceptualized.

We conceive of time as structured into days, months and years. These divisions are unambiguously related to the planetary cycles whose recurrent, regular change provides their foundation. Clocks and calendars enable us to visualize these cycles as past, present and future. Further, incorporating and using material devices for this purpose harnesses their power to accumulate the cognitive effort of past individuals and generations and distribute it to new individuals and future generations (Hutchins Reference Hutchins1995; Overmann Reference Overmann2023). In this way, the prehistoric gnomon directly supports the later invention of the sundial and analogue clock, just as its reliable indication of the sun's movement underpins the subsequent emergence of explicit notions of time of day and year.

Cultural conceptualizations of time endow it with further attributes: First is motion, which characterizes whether it is time itself or the perceiver which moves. This motion has direction, typically a circular or linear flow. This flow has an associated pace, which is either fast or slow; it also has an associated duration, the length of its elapse between successive events. Finally, time has a frame of reference, in which event sequencing or temporal ordering is ego-centric or absolute. These attributes are reasonably traced to the material means by which time is visualized, counted and measured. For example, when represented by a gnomon, attributes like pace and duration become objective and public, where they were once subjective and personal. Many of the phenomena used for time are circles or partial circles: the sun's disk and its daily arc across the sky, the face of the moon and its rise and set, the celestial rotation of stars, the circular sweep of shadows. Circular or cyclical conceptualization of time are common, found in ancient societies like the Babylonians and Maya and contemporary societies like the Nuer, Gawa and Hopi (Closs Reference Closs1977; Evans-Pritchard Reference Evans-Pritchard1939; King Reference King1912; Munn Reference Munn1992b; Whorf Reference Whorf1950). In this regard, the linear conceptualization of time more prevalent in Western societies stands out as unusual, although—since calendars and clocks are cyclical in nature, genealogies and king lists linear—the two constructs often coexist (Munn Reference Munn1992a).

The material forms used as proxies for time influence these conceptualizations in other ways. Counting and measuring create and delimit periods of time. Periods excluded from this process are essentially timeless: they are literally outside of time, like the (uncounted) months which separate the (counted) monsoon seasons in the Bismarck Archipelago or the (uncounted) dark nights before the new moon reappears (Nilsson Reference Nilsson and Fielden1920). Other attributes are dichotomous: time is concrete or abstract, immanent or transcendent, process-linked or homogeneous (Gell Reference Gell2020). Years identified by memorable events (e.g. catastrophes, battles, political matters) are ‘concrete’, individuated and specific to cultural groups in ways years labelled numerically are not. The sunrise on an ancestral horizon, shadow of a megalith, and city calendar painted on the forum wall tether time to place in a way quite distinct from time as represented by a wristwatch or other portable or interchangeable device. And time linked to specific agricultural and ritual events (Fig. 4) involves cultural knowledge and governs societal behaviours in ways the time used to synchronize today's global networks does not.

In these dichotomous qualities, we find another similarity between time and numbers, especially in the notion conceptualizations are either concrete or abstract. In numbers, the distinction distinguishes numbers which are attached to an object being counted (‘concrete’) from numbers which are not so attached (‘abstract’). In making this distinction, the material forms used to represent the concepts are often overlooked. As the mental representation of numbers is highly perishable and their mental manipulation is relatively limited, the necessity of using material forms for representing and manipulating keeps numbers bound to them, no matter how elaborated numbers become (Overmann Reference Overmann2023). What does change are the number and type of material forms involved in representing and manipulating; for example, numbers represented by only the fingers, as in a digit-tally system, are tied to that material form in a way numbers represented by fingers, tallies, tokens like coins, and written notations are not. As various material forms are recruited into the cognitive system, concepts become distributed over multiple forms, becoming functionally independent of any particular form (Overmann Reference Overmann2019). Elaboration of the material component of time—for example, adding sundials, obelisks, water clocks, candles, hourglasses, calendars, chronometers, grandfather clocks, pocket watches, wristwatches, stopwatches, atomic clocks and the apps on our phones—surely realizes a similar effect: time as conceptualized becomes seemingly independent of any particular technology, while depending on them as the means of its visualization and manipulation.

Since time first began to be time, we have thought of it as fixed. However, improved measurement technologies and techniques show the planetary cycles are not quite as stable as assumed. The planet's rotation changes daily by a fraction of a millisecond; that change is not invariably slower but has recently sped up, so intercalary adjustments will now require subtracting time and not just adding it, a possibility ‘never … foreseen or tested’ (CGPM 2022). The solar geometry is also changing. The moon is shrinking in size; the planet both expands and contracts (Tsuchiya et al. Reference Tsuchiya, Kawai and Maruyama2013; Watters et al. Reference Watters, Weber, Collins, Howley, Schmerr and Johnson2019). Every year, the moon moves 4 cm farther from the planet, which in turn moves 15 cm farther from the sun (Krasinsky & Brumberg Reference Krasinsky and Brumberg2004; Williams & Boggs Reference Williams and Boggs2016). Every moment the planet occupies a different position in space, relative to the solar system, galaxy and universe, so the background of stars changes over time (Aderin-Pocock Reference Aderin-Pocock2022). All this takes place in a universe which has been expanding from the moment of its birth some 14 billion years ago (Hawking Reference Hawking1995). While each of these changes is infinitesimal, they are cumulatively relevant. Time which depends on where the planet is and what it happens to be doing is a different concept of time, relative to what our ancestors thought it was.

On the other hand, this kind of materially influenced conceptual change is exactly what we should predict, if the material forms used to count and measure time indeed influence how we conceive it. The question we have asked here is whether our cultural conceptualizations of time are structured by the material forms used for its visualization and manipulation in ways similar to those found in space and numbers. We have argued the answer is ‘yes’, with the incorporation and use of material devices providing both opportunities and a mechanism for them to influence the concepts they represent and help to explicate.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Lambros Malafouris, Chris Gosden, Thomas Wynn and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on early versions of the manuscript, and Stephen McCluskey for his help with the Hopi horizon calendar (Fig. 4).

Footnotes

1. The account comes from John Morgan's Reference Morgan1852 biography of William Buckley (1780–1856), a transported convict who escaped and lived with the Wathaurung for over 30 years. While some elements of Buckley's story were fantastical and were probably intended to provoke interest, his account is held to describe Indigenous Australian life accurately (Curr Reference Curr1886; Flannery Reference Flannery and Flannery2017; Tipping Reference Tipping1966).

References

Aderin-Pocock, M., 2022. Will the constellations move and change over time? BBC Sky at Night Magazine. https://www.skyatnightmagazine.com/advice/constellations-move-change-over-time/Google Scholar
Aslaksen, H., 2003. The Mathematics of the Chinese Calendar. Singapore: National University.Google Scholar
Aveni, A.F., 1981. Tropical archeoastronomy. Science 213(4504), 161–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aveni, A.F., 2001. Skywatchers of Ancient Mexico (rev. edn). Austin (TX): University of Texas.Google Scholar
Aveni, A.F., Milbrath, S. & Lope, C.P., 2004. Chichén Itzá's legacy in the astronomically oriented architecture of Mayapán. Anthropology and Aesthetics 45, 123–43.Google Scholar
Bacon, B., Khatiri, A., Palmer, J., Freeth, T., Pettitt, P. & Kentridge, R., 2023. An Upper Palaeolithic proto-writing system and phenological calendar. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 33(3), 371–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D., 1986. Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Bickel, S. & Gautschy, R., 2014. Eine ramessidische Sonnenuhr im Tal der Könige [A Ramesside sundial in the Valley of the Kings]. Zeitschrift für Ägyptische Sprache und Altertumskunde 141(1), 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, R.A., 2014. Models of psychological time, in Cognitive Models of Psychological Time (2nd edn), ed. Block, R.A.. New York (NY): Psychology Press, 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bueti, D., Bahrami, B. & Walsh, V., 2008. Sensory and association cortex in time perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(6), 1054–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burr, D.C., Turi, M. & Anobile, G., 2010. Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. Journal of Vision 10(6), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, S.S., 2014. Circadian rhythms and human temporal experience, in Cognitive Models of Psychological Time (2nd edn), ed. Block, R.A.. New York (NY): Psychology Press, 101–18.Google Scholar
Casati, R. & Cavanagh, P., 2019. The Visual World of Shadows. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CGPM (Conférence générale des poids et mesures), 2022. Resolution 4 of the 27th CGPM (2022): On the use and future development of UTC. https://www.bipm.org/en/cgpm-2022/resolution-4Google Scholar
Cipriani, L.C. & Tayler, D., 1966. The Andaman Islanders. New York (NY): Frederick A. Praeger.Google Scholar
Clark, D.W., 1974. Koniag Prehistory. Stuttgart: W. Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Closs, M.P., 1977. The nature of the Maya chronological count. American Antiquity 42(1), 1827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R. & Gertner, L., 2011. Synesthesia, in Space, Time and Number in the Brain, eds. Dehaene, S. & Brannon, E.M.. San Diego (CA): Academic Press, 123–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen Kadosh, R., Gertner, L. & Terhune, D.B., 2012. Exceptional abilities in the spatial representation of numbers and time. Neuroscientist 18(3), 208–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coolidge, F.L. & Wynn, T., 2018. The Rise of Homo sapiens (2nd edn). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Curr, E.M., 1886. The Australian Race (Vol. 2). Melbourne: John Ferres.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Backwell, L., Villa, P., et al., 2012. Early evidence of San material culture represented by organic artifacts from Border Cave, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(33), 13214–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
d'Errico, F., Doyon, L., Colagé, I., et al., 2018. From number sense to number symbols. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 373(1740), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Smedt, J. & de Cruz, H., 2011. The role of material culture in human time representation: calendrical systems as extensions of mental time travel. Adaptive Behavior 19(1), 6376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dearborn, D. & Schreiber, K., 1986. Here comes the sun: the Cuzco-Machu Picchu connection. Archaeoastronomy 9(1–4), 114–22.Google Scholar
Degni, S., 2022. The problem and the measurement of time in psychology, 1874–1910, in The Oxford Encyclopedia of the History of Modern Psychology, ed. Pickren, W.E.. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 124.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. & Brannon, E.M. (eds), 2011. Space, Time and Number in the Brain. San Diego (CA): Academic Press.Google Scholar
Emmerling, E., Geer, H. & Klíma, B., 1993. Ein Mondkalenderstab aus Dolní Věstonice [A lunar calendar stick from Dolní Věstonice]. Quartär 43, 151–63.Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E.E., 1939. Nuer time reckoning. Africa 12(3), 189216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, D.L., 2005. Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology 46(4), 621–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ezcurra, E., Ezcurra, P. & Meissner, B., 2022. Ancient inhabitants of the Basin of Mexico kept an accurate agricultural calendar using sunrise observatories and mountain alignments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 119(51), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feeney, D., 2007. Caesar's Calendar. Berkeley (CA): University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzsimmons, H., 2016. The world's oldest observatory? How Aboriginal astronomy provides clues to ancient life. ABC News Australia. https://www.abc.net.au/news/2016-10-12/aboriginal-astronomy-provides-clues-to-ancient-life/7925024Google Scholar
Flannery, T., 2017. Introduction, in The Life and Adventures of William Buckley, ed. Flannery, T.. Melbourne: Text Publishing, 112.Google Scholar
Forde, C.D., 1931. Hopi agriculture and land ownership. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 61, 357405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, G.M., 1944. A Summary of Yuki Culture. Berkeley (CA): University of California.Google Scholar
Gell, A., 2020. The Anthropology of Time. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gibbon, J., Church, R.M. & Meck, W.H., 1984. Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 423(1), 5277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J.J., 1975. Events are perceivable but time is not, in The Study of Time II, ed. Fraser, J.T. & Lawrence, N.M.. New York (NY): Springer, 295301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godley, A.D., 1922. Herodotus, The histories. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, P., 2004. Numerical cognition without words. Science 306(5695), 496–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gusinde, M., 1931. The Fireland Indians: The Selk'nam (Vol. 1). Vienna: Anthropos-Bibliothek.Google Scholar
Hamacher, D.W. & Norris, R.P., 2011. ‘Bridging the gap’ through Australian cultural astronomy, in Proceedings of the 278th Symposium of the International Astronomical Union and ‘Oxford IX’ International Symposium on Archaeoastronomy, ed. Ruggles, C.L.N.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 282–90.Google Scholar
Hannah, R., 2013. Time in written spaces, in Written Space in the Latin West, 200 BC to AD 300, eds Seaes, G., Keegan, P. & Laurence, R.. London: Bloomsbury Academic, 83102.Google Scholar
Hawking, S.W., 1995. A Brief History of Time. Toronto: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Hayden, B., 2021. Keeping count: on interpreting record keeping in prehistory. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 63, 101304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E., 1995. Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubbard, E.M., 2007. Neurophysiology of synesthesia. Current Psychiatry Reports 9(3), 193–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imura, T., Tomonaga, M. & Yagi, A., 2006. Processing of shadow information in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and human (Homo sapiens) infants, in Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees, eds Matsuzawa, T., Tomonaga, M. & Tanaka, M.. Tokyo: Springer, 305–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwaniszewski, S., 2009. Did I say cosmology? On modern cosmologies and ancient world-views, in Proceedings of a Workshop Held at Parque de Las Ciencias, Granada, Spain, eds Rubiño-Martín, J.A. et al. San Francisco (CA): Astronomical Society of the Pacific, 100106.Google Scholar
Jègues-Wolkiewiez, C., 2005. Aux racines de l'astronomie, ou l'ordre caché d'une œuvre paléolithique [At the roots of astronomy, or the hidden order of a Palaeolithic work]. Antiquités Nationales 37, 4362.
Kelley, D.H. & Milone, E.F., 2005. Exploring Ancient Skies. New York (NY): Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, P., 2020. Australian message sticks: old questions, new directions. Journal of Material Culture 25(2), 133–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, L.W., 1912. Cuneiform Texts from Babylonian Tablets in the British Museum: Part XXXIII. London: British Museum.Google Scholar
Krasinsky, G.A. & Brumberg, V.A., 2004. Secular increase of astronomical unit from analysis of the major planet motions, and its interpretation. Celestial Mechanics and Dynamical Astronomy 90(3–4), 267–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laurence, R. & Smith, C.J., 1996. Ritual, time, and power in ancient Rome. Accordia Research Journal 6, 133–51.Google Scholar
LeFevre, J.-A., DeStefano, D., Coleman, B., et al., 2005. Mathematical cognition and working memory, in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, ed. Campbell, J.I.D.. New York (NY): Psychology Press, 361–77.Google Scholar
Leon, M.I. & Shadlen, M.N., 2003. Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron 38(2), 317–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, G., 2015. Gnomons in ancient China, in Handbook of Archaeoastronomy and Ethnoastronomy, ed. Ruggles, C.L.N.. New York (NY): Springer, 2095–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y. & Sun, X.-C., 2009. Gnomon shadow lengths recorded in the Zhoubi Suanjing. Research in Astronomy and Astrophysics 9(12), 1377–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukusa, S.T.M., 2005. The meaning of the names of the months in Bantu languages. Journal of the Linguistics Association for Southern African Development Community Universities 2, 1421.Google Scholar
Macdonald, J., 1890. Light in Africa. London: Hodder & Stoughton.Google Scholar
Magli, G., 2018. Royal mausoleums of the western Han and of the Song Chinese dynasties: a satellite imagery analysis. Archaeological Research in Asia 15, 4554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malafouris, L., 2013. How Things Shape the Mind. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshack, A., 1985. A lunar-solar year calendar stick from North America. American Antiquity 50(1), 2751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshack, A., 1991. The Roots of Civilization (rev. edn). New York (NY): Moyer Bell.Google Scholar
Mathews, R.H., 1904. Language of the Wuddyāwūrru Tribe, Victoria. Zeitschrift für Ethnologie 36(6), 729–34.Google Scholar
McCluskey, S.C., 2021. The Hopi calendar and some archaeological correlates of horizon markers, in Advancing Cultural Astronomy, eds Boutsikas, E., McCluskey, S.C. & Steele, J.. Cham: Springer, 5170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merchant, H. & de Lafuente, V. (eds), 2014. Neurobiology of Interval Timing. New York (NY): Springer.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, J., 1852. The Life and Adventures of William Buckley (19th edn). Adelaide: Griffin Press.Google Scholar
Morrone, M.C., Ross, J. & Burr, D., 2005. Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space. Nature Neuroscience 8(7), 950–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munn, N.D., 1992a. The cultural anthropology of time: a critical essay. Annual Review of Anthropology 21(1), 93123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munn, N.D., 1992b. The Fame of Gawa. Durham (NC): Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Nichelli, P., Venneri, A., Molinari, M., et al., 1993. Precision and accuracy of subjective time estimation in different memory disorders. Cognitive Brain Research 1(2), 8793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsson, M.P., [1920] 1960. Primitive Time-reckoning (trans. Fielden, F.J.; 2nd edn). Lund: C.W.K. Gleerup.Google Scholar
Nowell, A., Bahn, P. & Le Quellec, J.-L., 2024. Evaluating the evidence for lunar calendars in Upper Palaeolithic parietal art. Cambridge Archaeological Journal. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774324000155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliver, D.L., 1955. A Solomon Island Society: Kinship and leadership among the Siuai of Bougainville. Cambridge (MA): Harvard College.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, R.L., 1936. The Quinault Indians. Seattle (WA): University of Washington.Google Scholar
Orzel, C., 2022. A Brief History of Timekeeping. Dallas (TX): BenBella.Google Scholar
Overmann, K.A., 2013. Material scaffolds in numbers and time. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 23(1), 1939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overmann, K.A., 2019. Concepts and how they get that way. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 18(1), 153–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overmann, K.A., 2023. The Materiality of Numbers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pagel, M. & Meade, A., 2017. The deep history of the number words. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373(1740), 20160517.Google ScholarPubMed
Panda, S., Hogenesch, J.B. & Kay, S.A., 2002. Circadian rhythms from flies to human. Nature 417(6886), 329–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, R.A. & Dubberstein, W.H., 1956. Babylonian Chronology: 626 B.C.–A.D. 75. Providence (RI): Brown University.Google Scholar
Rappenglück, M.A., 2013. Palaeolithic stargazers and today's astro maniacs: methodological concepts of cultural astronomy focused on case studies of earlier prehistory, in Proceedings of the 20th Conference of the European Society for Astronomy in Culture, eds. Šprajc, I. & Pehani, P.. Ljubljana: Slovene Anthropological Society, 85102.Google Scholar
Rochberg, F., 2010. In the Path of the Moon: Babylonian celestial divination and its legacy. Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B., 1920. Introduction to Mathematical Philosophy (2nd edn). London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Silberbauer, G.B., 1981. Hunter and Habitat in the Central Kalahari Desert. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Simmons, A., 2003. Spatial perception from a Cartesian point of view. Philosophical Topics 31(1/2), 395423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, D., 2007. Longitude. New York (NY): Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Sofaer, A., 2007. The primary architecture of the Chacoan culture, in Architecture of Chaco Canyon, New Mexico, ed. Lekson, S.. Salt Lake City (UT): University of Utah, 227–54.Google Scholar
Stanzione, V.J., 2003. Rituals of Sacrifice. Albuquerque (NM): University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
Tipping, M.J., 1966. Buckley, William (1780–1856). Australian Dictionary of Biography. https://adb.anu.edu.au/biography/buckley-william-1844/text2133Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, T., Kawai, K. & Maruyama, S., 2013. Expanding-contracting earth. Geoscience Frontiers 4, 341–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., 2002. Episodic memory. Annual Review of Psychology 53(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyson, N. deGrasse, 2003. Stick-in-the-mud astronomy. Natural History Magazine 3, 32–6.Google Scholar
Veniaminov, I., 1840. Записки объ Островахъ Уналашкинскаго отдѣла. часть вторая [Notes on the Islands of the Unalashka Department. part two]. St Petersburg: Russian-American Company.Google Scholar
Villela, K.D., Robb, M.H. & Miller, M.E., 2010. Introduction, in The Aztec Calendar Stone. Los Angeles (CA): Getty Research Institute, 1–41.Google Scholar
Vodolazhskaya, L.N., 2014. Reconstruction of ancient Egyptian sundials. Archaeoastronomy and Ancient Technologies 2(2), 118.Google Scholar
Wadley, L., 2010. Were snares and traps used in the Middle Stone Age and does it matter? A review and a case study from Sibudu, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution 58(2), 179–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warmington, E.H., 1940. Remains of Old Latin, Vol. IV. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University.Google Scholar
Watters, T.R., Weber, R.C., Collins, G.C., Howley, I.J., Schmerr, N.C. & Johnson, C.L., 2019. Shallow seismic activity and young thrust faults on the Moon. Nature Geoscience 12, 411–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wearden, J.H., 2003. Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology, in Time and Mind II: Information processing perspective, ed. Helfrich, H.. Ashland (OH): Hogrefe & Huber, 2139.Google Scholar
Whorf, B.L., 1950. An American Indian model of the universe. International Journal of American Linguistics 16(2), 6772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, R.H., 2017. The Complete Gods and Goddesses of Ancient Egypt. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Williams, J.G. & Boggs, D.H., 2016. Secular tidal changes in lunar orbit and earth rotation. Celestial Mechanics and Dynamical Astronomy 126, 89129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, J.C., Jacobs, J. & Hillgarth, N., 1997. Ecological constraints and the evolution of hormone-behavior interrelationships. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 9–8, 2241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolley, C.L., 1924. The ziggurat of Ur. Museum Journal 15(2), 107–14.Google Scholar
Zeilik, M., 1985a. A reassessment of the Fajada Butte solar marker. Journal for the History of Astronomy 16(9), S69S85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeilik, M., 1985b. The ethnoastronomy of the historic pueblos, I: calendrical sun watching. Journal for the History of Astronomy 16, S1S24.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Engraved baboon fibula from Border Cave, South Africa (three views). (Image from d'Errico et al.2012, supporting information, fig. 8, © PNAS and used with permission.)

Figure 1

Figure 2. Engraved reindeer bone from Abri Blanchard, France. (Image: CC BY-SA 4.0 with added scale.)

Figure 2

Figure 3. Engraved marlstone rod from Dolní Věstonice, Czech Republic. (Redrawn and adapted from Emmerling et al.1993, 153 fig. 1 & 156 table 1.)

Figure 3

Figure 4. Horizon calendars. (Top) Basin of Mexico as viewed from Templo Mayor (19°N). Where the sun rises along the horizon shifts north and south between the solstices, with the equinoxes falling midway between the two. (Redrawn and relabelled from Ezcurra et al.2022, 4 fig. 2c; also see Aveni 2001, 242–3.) (Bottom) Eastern horizon from Shungopavi, Second Mesa (36°N), as originally drawn by an Indigenous informant. The annual planting cycle corresponds to the sun's rising position as read right-to-left along the top, then left-to-right along the bottom. (Redrawn and relabelled from Forde 1931, 386 fig. 6b; translations from McCluskey 2021 and pers. comm. 29 December 2023 and 4 January 2024.)

Figure 4

Figure 5. Gnomon used for cardinal alignments. (Image: author.)

Figure 5

Figure 6. Vertical sundial from the Valley of the Kings. (Redrawn and relabelled from Vodolazhskaya 2014, 3 fig. 2 & 6 fig. 5.)

Figure 6

Figure 7. Fasti Antiates Maiores (as reconstructed). The top row lists the first day of each month and the month names (January–December plus an intercalary month). Days are marked A–H (eight-day weeks); also marked are the Kalends (first day of a month), Nones (new moons), and Ides (full moons) (Warmington 1940, 453–65). The bottom row lists the number of days in each month. (Image: CC BY-SA 4.0.)