Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T09:40:59.509Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dietary patterns are associated with cognitive function in the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 September 2016

Keith E. Pearson*
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition Sciences, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
Virginia G. Wadley
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
Leslie A. McClure
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
James M. Shikany
Affiliation:
Division of Preventive Medicine, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham, AL, USA
Fred W. Unverzagt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Suzanne E. Judd
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
*
*Corresponding author: K. E. Pearson, email [email protected]

Abstract

Identifying factors that contribute to the preservation of cognitive function is imperative to maintaining quality of life in advanced years. Of modifiable risk factors, diet quality has emerged as a promising candidate to make an impact on cognition. The objective of this study was to evaluate associations between empirically derived dietary patterns and cognitive function. This study included 18 080 black and white participants aged 45 years and older from the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort. Principal component analysis on data from the Block98 FFQ yielded five dietary patterns: convenience, plant-based, sweets/fats, Southern, and alcohol/salads. Incident cognitive impairment was defined as shifting from intact cognitive status (score >4) at first assessment to impaired cognitive status (score ≤4) at latest assessment, measured by the Six-Item Screener. Learning, memory and executive function were evaluated with the Word List Learning, Word List Delayed Recall, and animal fluency assessments. In fully adjusted models, greater consumption of the alcohol/salads pattern was associated with lower odds of incident cognitive impairment (highest quintile (Q5) v. lowest quintile (Q1): OR 0·68; 95 % CI 0·56, 0·84; P for trend 0·0005). Greater consumption of the alcohol/salads pattern was associated with higher scores on all domain-specific assessments and greater consumption of the plant-based pattern was associated with higher scores in learning and memory. Greater consumption of the Southern pattern was associated with lower scores on each domain-specific assessment (all P < 0·05). In conclusion, dietary patterns including plant-based foods and alcohol intake were associated with higher cognitive scores, and a pattern including fried food and processed meat typical of a Southern diet was associated with lower scores.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2016

As average life expectancy continues to increase due to progressive advances in the prevention and treatment of chronic disease( Reference Ortman, Velkoff and Hogan 1 ), Americans are enjoying the benefits of a prolonged life while simultaneously discovering the consequences of an ageing population, particularly those related to a decline in cognitive function. In the USA, where the prevalence of Alzheimer's disease and other dementias is expected to triple by 2050( 2 ), identifying modifiable risk factors that contribute to cognitive function is a growing area of research and could aid in the preservation of quality of life in older ages.

Several studies have evaluated the contributions of specific foods and nutrients to cognitive function, and some evidence suggests that regular consumption of foods such as fatty fish, nuts and berries, among others, could be related to more favourable cognitive outcomes( Reference Kalmijn, Feskens and Launer 3 Reference Devore, Kang and Breteler 8 ). Although these studies have provided valuable information, one limitation is that this type of approach does not accurately reflect the way people consume foods. Rather than individual foods or nutrients, people generally consume a combination of foods in meals that fall within an overall dietary pattern. By taking advantage of the potential interactions and collective effects of multiple foods, dietary patterns may be more predictive of cognitive function than foods or nutrients in isolation( Reference Hu 9 ).

Previous studies using investigator-defined dietary pattern analysis have demonstrated that adherence to a Mediterranean diet pattern or the Mediterranean–DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) dietary pattern was associated with a reduced risk of cognitive impairment and slower cognitive decline( Reference Psaltopoulou, Sergentanis and Panagiotakos 10 Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang 12 ). However, these dietary patterns are typically defined a priori by investigators and may not reflect true or realistic patterns of food consumption within a population. As an alternative, this study aimed to use principal component analysis (PCA) to employ an empirical approach to identify dietary patterns that may more accurately represent the dietary habits of our sample. A number of studies have used similar methodology but have possessed smaller sample sizes generalisable to only one race( Reference Akbaraly, Singh-Manoux and Marmot 13 Reference Qin, Adair and Plassman 19 ). This study utilised the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort, which consists of 30 239 black and white participants dispersed throughout continental USA. Within the REGARDS cohort, we have previously identified five dietary patterns( Reference Judd, Letter and Shikany 20 ): convenience, plant-based, sweets/fats, Southern, and alcohol/salads. The objective of this study was to examine the associations between empirically derived dietary patterns, incident cognitive impairment, and cognitive performance on three domain-specific assessments in a large cohort of black and white adults over the age of 45 years. Our hypotheses were that the convenience, sweets/fats and Southern dietary patterns would be associated with poorer cognitive outcomes and that the plant-based and alcohol/salads dietary patterns would be associated with more favourable cognitive outcomes.

Experimental methods

Study sample

The REGARDS study is a national cohort of 30 239 community-dwelling black and white participants aged 45 years and older at baseline. Participants were recruited from 2003–2007 using lists purchased from Genesys, Inc. that were selected to oversample both black Americans and residents of the region of the Southeast USA known as the stroke belt. Upon entry into the study, the full cohort of participants had a mean age of 64·8 years (ranging from 45 to 98 years) and was approximately 42 % black, 55 % female and 56 % living in the stroke belt. Exclusion criteria included belonging to a race other than white or black, currently undergoing active treatment for cancer or another medical condition that could affect long-term study participation, nursing home residence or the inability to communicate in English.

The data from this analysis were collected primarily by using computer-assisted telephone interviewing and an in-home examination. The initial telephone call collected data regarding demographics, socio-economic status and medical history. An in-home examination by a trained medical professional followed where anthropometrics, blood and urine samples, blood pressure measurements and an electrocardiogram were collected. Additionally, several self-administered questionnaires were left with the participant to complete and mail back to the REGARDS coordinating centre. Additional details of the study design have been described in depth elsewhere( Reference Howard, Cushman and Pulley 21 ). This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the institutional review boards of all participating institutions. Written informed consent was obtained by all participants included in this study.

Assessment of dietary patterns

Dietary intake of the participants was assessed using the Block98 food frequency questionnaire (Block98 FFQ), which aims to assess usual dietary intake over the past year by including questions about both frequency and portions of various foods. The Block98 FFQ assesses food frequency by asking participants how often they consume each food item, with the following possible answers: never, a few times per year, once per month, 2–3 times per month, once per week, 2 times per week, 3–4 times per week, 5–6 times per week, or every day. The Block98 FFQ additionally assesses the usual quantity of food consumed by asking the participant how much of each food item they consume, on average, each time they consume that food item. For foods consumed in individual units such as eggs, bacon and doughnuts, participants were asked to choose the number that represents the usual quantity of that food they consume (i.e. 1 egg, 2 eggs, 3 eggs, or 4 eggs). To help estimate usual quantity consumed for other items such as spinach or ice cream, participants were provided a photograph that illustrated several common portions of foods (¼ cup, ½ cup, 1 cup, or 2 cups of foods on plates or ½ cup, 1 cup, or 2 cups of foods in bowls). Block FFQ have been previously validated using multiple food records( Reference Boucher, Cotterchio and Kreiger 22 Reference Caan, Slattery and Potter 24 ). The FFQ were left with the participant during the in-home examination, mailed back by the participant to the REGARDS coordinating centre, and sent to NutritionQuest for analysis.

The dietary patterns used in these analyses were derived previously( Reference Judd, Letter and Shikany 20 ) and have been associated with incident stroke( Reference Judd, Gutierrez and Newby 25 ), incident coronary artery disease( Reference Shikany, Safford and Newby 26 ), sepsis( Reference Gutierrez, Judd and Voeks 27 ) and progression to end-stage renal disease in individuals with chronic kidney disease( Reference Gutierrez, Muntner and Rizk 28 ). The 107 food items from the FFQ were combined into fifty-six food groups for use in PCA. Using a random split-sample technique to ensure validity and replication of the patterns, PCA with varimax rotation was utilised in the first half of the sample. Factor solutions were examined for interpretability and separate PCA were conducted to test for congruence by region, sex and race. Congruence coefficients were obtained to examine whether the dietary patterns could represent the entire sample or should be derived separately for these subgroups. In the second half of the sample, a confirmatory factor analysis including only the food groups with absolute value loadings ≥0·20 was used to independently validate the results from the PCA and test for model fit. After considering the screen test using eigenvalues >1·5 and examining the congruence coefficients to achieve optimal congruence across region, sex and race subgroups, this analysis retained five factors, and a final PCA with varimax rotation was performed in the full sample. Factor scores were calculated for each participant for each dietary pattern by multiplying the factor loading of each food group by each participant's average consumption of each food group.

The five dietary patterns were named according to the types of foods that loaded highly in each of them. Factor one was named the convenience pattern and consisted of mixed dishes with meat, pizza, Chinese food and Mexican dishes; factor two was named the plant-based pattern and consisted of vegetables, fruits, fish and beans; factor three included high factor loadings for miscellaneous sugars, desserts, candy, sweetened breakfast foods and added fats and was named the sweets/fats pattern; factor four was named the Southern pattern because of its high loadings of added fats, fried food, eggs and egg dishes, organ meats, processed meats and sugar-sweetened beverages; and factor five was named the alcohol/salads pattern and loaded highly in green-leafy vegetables, tomatoes, salad dressing, wine and liquor. Full factor loadings for each pattern are shown in Supplementary Table S1. In total, the five dietary patterns explained approximately 24 % of the total variance in dietary intake in the REGARDS sample, which is similar to other dietary pattern analyses reported in the literature( Reference Newby and Tucker 29 ). The amount of variance explained by each dietary pattern is as follows: convenience, 8·7 %; plant-based, 5·9 %; sweets/fats, 3·6 %; Southern, 3·0 %; and alcohol/salads, 2·7 %.

Assessment of cognitive function

Given the large, nationwide distribution of the REGARDS study, the cognitive assessment of its participants required a brief assessment that was able to be delivered over telephone. Beginning December 2003, the Six-Item Screener (SIS)( Reference Callahan, Unverzagt and Hui 30 ) was administered during baseline telephone calls and subsequently in annual intervals. The SIS is a brief screening assessment that consists of a three-item word recall and three-items pertaining to temporal orientation. Intact cognitive function was defined as having a score of 5 or 6 correct, and incident cognitive impairment was defined as shifting from intact cognitive function on the first cognitive assessment to impaired cognitive function (a score ≤4) on the most recent cognitive assessment( Reference Callahan, Unverzagt and Hui 30 ). Using a combined endpoint of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in a diverse community-based sample, the cut-point of 4 or fewer correct on the SIS has a sensitivity and specificity of 74 and 80 %, respectively( Reference Callahan, Unverzagt and Hui 30 ).

In January 2006, a three-test battery of domain-specific assessments was administered by telephone to participants and has been subsequently administered every 2 years. To assess verbal learning and memory domains, the Word List Learning (WLL) and Word List Delayed Recall (WLDR) from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) battery( Reference Welsh, Butters and Mohs 31 ) were administered. These assessments involved a set of three learning trials of a list of ten words followed by a 5 min delay that preceded a free recall trial. For WLL, the scores from the three trials were summed and produced a score ranging from zero to 30. For the WLDR, the score reflects the number of words the participant could recall after a 5 min delay and ranges from zero to 10. For both measures, repetitions and intrusions were excluded, and a procedure was implemented to exclude non-standard performance patterns (occurring in <2 % of the sample). To assess executive function, the Animal Fluency Test (AFT)( Reference Welsh, Butters and Mohs 31 ) was administered. This test required participants to name as many animals as they could in 1 min, yielding a raw score that was then adjusted for repetitions and intrusions.

For this analysis, the primary outcome is incident cognitive impairment as measured by the SIS. Due to the limited number of participants with multiple assessments for the domain-specific cognitive measures, we will be examining cross-sectional cognitive performance by including only the first measure of the WLL, WLDR and AFT assessments for each participant who possessed dietary data and were free of stroke at baseline.

Covariate assessment

Age (continuous in years), race (dichotomous: black/white), sex (dichotomous: male/female), region of residence (categorical: stroke-belt, stroke-buckle, non-belt or buckle), income (categorical: <$20 000/year, $20 000–$34 999/year, $35 000–$74 999/year, >$75 000/year, and refused to provide income information) and education (categorical: less than high school, high school graduate, some college, college graduate) were self-reported at baseline. Total energy intake (continuous in kcal) was estimated from the FFQ administered at baseline. Height and weight were obtained from the in-home examination and used to calculate BMI (continuous in kg/m2). Physical activity defined by exercise frequency (categorical: none, 1–3 times/week, 4+ times/week) and smoking status (categorical: current, past, never) were self-reported at baseline. History of heart disease (dichotomous: yes/no) was defined as self-reported myocardial infarction, coronary artery bypass graft, angioplasty, stenting or evidence of myocardial infarction from an electrocardiogram performed during the in-home examination. Participants were defined as hypertensive (dichotomous: yes/no) if systolic blood pressure was ≥140 mmHg or diastolic blood pressure was ≥90 mmHg or if they self-reported current medication use to control blood pressure. Diabetes status (dichotomous: yes/no) was defined as having a fasting glucose ≥7 mmol/l (≥126 mg/dl) or non-fasting blood glucose ≥11 mmol/l (≥200 mg/dl) or if the participant reported taking medication or insulin for the management of diabetes. Depressive symptoms (continuous in Center for Epidemiological Studies – Depression four-item version (CESD-4) item score units) were evaluated at baseline over the telephone using the CESD-4( Reference Kohout, Berkman and Evans 32 ).

Statistical analysis

Likelihood-ratio χ2 tests and t tests were used to calculate unadjusted means of demographic characteristics by quintile of each dietary pattern. Logistic regression was utilised to examine the relationship between quintiles of dietary pattern scores and odds of incident cognitive impairment via the SIS. Three models incrementally adding covariates were evaluated in this analysis. Model 1 included adjustment for age, race, sex, region and total energy intake. Model 2 additionally adjusted for socio-economic variables previously shown to affect cognitive function: income and education. Model 3 added adjustments for other known cognitive risk factors: physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD and depressive symptoms. Participants with non-missing values for all covariates were included in each model, resulting in 0, 0·03 and 7·5 % missing data for each model, respectively. Effect modification for race and sex was examined by placing an interaction term in the model for each pattern. Tests for linear trend across quintiles of dietary patterns were evaluated by including each dietary pattern in quintiles as a continuous, ordinal variable in each model. Multiple regression was utilised to evaluate mean differences between quintiles of dietary patterns and each of the three domain-specific cognitive assessments, including all of the covariates listed previously to adjust for confounding. Analyses for the AFT also included a covariate to adjust for the participants who received assistance from someone in their home environment or was given a disallowed prompt by the interviewer (about 3·4 % of the sample).

Results

Of the 30 239 original REGARDS participants, 72 % of the cohort returned a usable FFQ. This analysis excluded participants not returning a usable FFQ (n 8603), defined as the following: did not return a FFQ (17 % of full sample), returned a blank FFQ (3 %), possessing >15 % missing data on FFQ (5 %), or estimated to consume implausible energy intakes on FFQ (3 %)( Reference Judd, Letter and Shikany 20 , Reference Shikany, Safford and Newby 26 ).

Of the dietary subsample of REGARDS, participants were excluded if they did not possess at least two SIS assessments (n 1191) or were cognitively impaired at baseline (n 1447). Participants who self-reported history of stroke at baseline or had an incident stroke prior to first cognitive assessment (n 905) were also excluded. Finally, participants lacking an in-home medical assessment were excluded from these analyses (n 13). These exclusions resulted in a final sample of 18 080 participants. Additionally, cross-sectional analysis of cognitive performance on domain-specific assessments was performed in REGARDS participants possessing at least one WLL, WLDR and AFT assessment, dietary data and no history of stroke prior to cognitive assessment (n 14 247). Participants excluded from the longitudinal analyses were more likely than included participants to be older, male, black, less educated, and have lower income. Excluded participants were also more likely to report no weekly physical activity, currently smoke, have a higher BMI, have a history of hypertension, diabetes and CVD, and exhibit more depressive symptoms.

Descriptive statistics of participants who were included in this analysis are provided in Table 1. Compared with participants in the lowest quintile (Q1), participants in the highest quintile (Q5) of consumption of the convenience pattern tended to be younger, white, male, live outside the stroke belt, and have a higher income and a higher education level. Participants in Q5 of the plant-based pattern tended to be older, a higher proportion black, female, and possess a higher education level than participants in Q1. Participants in the Q5 of the sweets/fats pattern tended to be more white, male, stroke-belt residents, with a lower income and education than participants in Q1. For the Southern pattern, Q5 participants were more likely to be black, male, residing in the stroke-belt, and possess a lower income and education level than participants in Q1. Finally, participants in Q5 of the alcohol/salads pattern tended to be more likely to be younger, white, male, residing outside the stroke-belt, with a higher income and education level.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics by quintile of dietary pattern in the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort 2003–2014

(Number of participants and percentages, mean values and standard deviations)

Q1, lowest quintile; Q5, highest quintile.

Of the 18 080 participants included in this analysis, 1486 cases of incident cognitive impairment were identified over an average follow up of 6·8 years. Odds of incident cognitive impairment by quintile of each dietary pattern are displayed in Table 2. After adjustment for demographic factors and total energy intake, participants in the highest quintile of the Southern dietary pattern had higher odds of incident cognitive impairment (Q5 v. Q1: OR 1·46; 95 % CI 1·19, 1·78; P for trend ≤ 0·0001) compared with participants in the lowest quintile. Additionally, participants in the highest quintile of the plant-based and alcohol/salads dietary patterns had lower odds of incident cognitive impairment (plant-based – Q5 v. Q1: OR 0·81; 95 % CI 0·67, 0·98; P for trend = 0·02; alcohol/salads – Q5 v. Q1: OR 0·65; 95 % CI 0·54, 0·79; P for trend ≤0·0001). After further adjustment for socio-economic status and other cognitive risk factors, the observed associations with the plant-based and Southern patterns were attenuated and no longer statistically significant, but the association with the alcohol/salads pattern remained (Q5 v. Q1: OR 0·68; 95 % CI 0·56, 0·84; P for trend = 0·0005). No significant associations between the convenience and sweets/fats dietary patterns and incident cognitive impairment were observed, and tests for interactions by race and sex were non-significant for each pattern.

Table 2. Odds of incident cognitive impairment by quintile of dietary pattern in the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study 2003–2014 (n 18 080)*

(Odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals)

CESD-4, Center for Epidemiological Studies – Depression four-item version.

* Model 1 adjusts for demographic variables (age, race, sex, region and total energy intake). Model 2 additionally adjusts for socio-economic variables (income and education). Model 3 additionally adjusts for cognitive risk factors and co-morbidities (physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD and score on the CESD-4).

In the assessments of learning, memory and executive function, participants in the highest quintile of the alcohol/salads patterns had higher scores on all measures of cognitive function compared with participants in the lowest quintile (Figs 1–3). Likewise, participants with the highest consumption of the plant-based pattern scored higher on the WLL and WLDR assessments compared with participants with the lowest consumption (Figs 1 and 2). There were no differences in scores on the AFT between the extreme quintiles of the plant-based pattern, but a significant linear trend was observed (see Fig. 3). Additionally, scoring in the highest quintile of the Southern dietary pattern was associated with significantly lower scores in the learning, memory and executive function domains. Scoring in the highest quintile of the convenience dietary pattern was also associated with higher performance on the WLL (P < 0·05). No other differences were detected on any domain-specific assessments between any of the quintiles for the convenience and sweets/fats patterns, although a significant linear trend was observed on the WLL for the convenience and sweets/fats patterns and for the convenience pattern on the AFT. Domain-specific results for the plant-based, Southern and alcohol/salads patterns are displayed in Figs 1–3, and further details are provided in Supplementary Table S2.

Fig. 1. Multivariable-adjusted mean differences and 95 % confidence intervals on the Word List Learning assessment. Adjusted for age, race, sex, region, total energy intake, income, education, physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD and depressive symptoms. Example interpretation: participants with factor scores in quintiles (Q) 3, 4 and 5 of the Southern dietary pattern scored significantly lower on the Word List Learning assessment than participants in Q1. * Mean differences were statistically significant (P < 0·05).

Fig. 2. Multivariable-adjusted mean differences and 95 % confidence intervals on the Word List Delayed Recall assessment. Adjusted for age, race, sex, region, total energy intake, income, education, physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD and depressive symptoms. Example interpretation: participants with factor scores in quintiles (Q) 4 and 5 of the Southern dietary pattern scored significantly lower on the Word List Delayed Recall assessment than participants in Q1. * Mean differences were statistically significant (P < 0·05).

Fig. 3. Multivariable-adjusted mean differences and 95 % confidence intervals on the Animal Fluency Test. Adjusted for age, race, sex, region, total energy intake, income, education, physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD, depressive symptoms and disallowed help/prompting. Example interpretation: participants with factor scores in quintiles (Q) 3, 4 and 5 of the Southern dietary pattern scored significantly lower on the Animal Fluency Test than participants in Q1. * Mean differences were statistically significant (P < 0·05).

Discussion

In this study of 18 080 black and white participants aged 45 years and older, we found that greater consumption of the alcohol/salads dietary pattern was associated with lower odds of incident cognitive impairment and higher performance on several cognitive measures assessing learning, memory and executive function. Additionally, greater consumption of a plant-based dietary pattern was associated with higher cognitive performance while greater consumption of a Southern dietary pattern was associated with lower cognitive performance on these domain-specific measures. Our findings strengthen the body of literature that collectively suggests that dietary patterns may have an impact on cognitive function, and this particular study provides a unique perspective by utilising empirically derived dietary patterns in a large, diverse sample of black and white adults living throughout the country.

As expected, greater consumption of the plant-based dietary pattern that loaded highest in many different types of vegetables, fruits and legumes was associated with higher cognitive performance on the WLL and WLDR assessments. This is consistent with previous studies, both cross-sectional and longitudinal, that have demonstrated an association between higher levels of fruit or vegetable intake and more favourable cognitive outcomes( Reference Chan, Chan and Woo 33 Reference Morris, Evans and Tangney 36 ). Many researchers have hypothesised that this observation could be related to higher intakes of fruits and vegetables contributing to higher levels of antioxidants, resulting in lower levels of oxidative stress. In a cross-sectional study of 193 healthy adults aged 45–102 years, Polidori et al.( Reference Polidori, Pratico and Mangialasche 34 ) tested this hypothesis and found that adults who reported consuming higher intakes of fruits and vegetables had higher cognitive performance, higher levels of circulating antioxidant micronutrients, and lower levels of oxidative stress biomarkers compared with adults consuming lower amounts of fruits and vegetables. Additionally, higher fruit and vegetable intake has been associated with lower blood pressure( Reference John, Ziebland and Yudkin 37 ) and CVD incidence( Reference He, Nowson and Lucas 38 ), which are both known risk factors for cognitive impairment( Reference Breteler, Claus and Grobbee 39 , Reference Kilander, Nyman and Boberg 40 ) and may be mediating these associations despite attempts to adjust for confounding.

Interestingly, greater consumption of the alcohol/salads dietary pattern was associated with lower odds of incident cognitive impairment and higher cognitive performance on all cognitive assessments analysed in this study. Although previous studies utilising similar methodology have yielded dietary patterns comparable with the plant-based and Southern dietary patterns( Reference Akbaraly, Singh-Manoux and Marmot 13 , Reference Newby and Tucker 29 , Reference Torres, Lautenschlager and Wattanapenpaiboon 41 ), the alcohol/salads pattern appears to be unique to our cohort. We believe that the size and racial diversity of REGARDS participants geographically distributed throughout the USA provides the opportunity for our analysis to yield unique patterns that may not reflect the dietary patterns previously derived in participants of smaller, less diverse cohorts. The alcohol/salads pattern loaded highest on salad dressings/sauces and green leafy vegetables, and also contained a high factor loading for tomatoes. Green leafy vegetables and tomatoes are vegetables that are particularly high in antioxidants and could be contributing to cognitive function in similar ways described for the plant-based dietary pattern. This pattern also consisted of higher intakes of both wine and liquor. Several previous epidemiological studies have demonstrated an association between moderate alcohol consumption and more favourable cognitive outcomes, most citing the potential cardiovascular benefits of moderate alcohol consumption to be contributing to the increased cognitive performance( Reference Stampfer, Kang and Chen 42 Reference McGuire, Ajani and Ford 45 ). It is also possible that the observed associations with the alcohol/salads pattern may be partially explained by reverse causation, especially the cross-sectional associations involving the domain-specific assessments. Several studies have reported that higher childhood cognitive ability is correlated with higher alcohol consumption in adulthood( Reference Hatch, Jones and Kuh 46 , Reference Batty, Deary and Schoon 47 ). Since the REGARDS study does not possess cognitive data during childhood, we cannot exclude the possibility that the observed associations between the alcohol/salads dietary pattern and more favourable cognitive outcomes could at least partially be attributed to differences in childhood cognitive ability.

The Southern dietary pattern was associated with poorer cognitive performance on the WLL, WLDR and AFT assessments in this study. This was not surprising given the pattern's high factor loadings of fried food, processed meats, sugar-sweetened beverages and refined white bread. A similar ‘processed food pattern’ was identified by Torres et al.( Reference Torres, Lautenschlager and Wattanapenpaiboon 41 ) and also consisted of fried foods, processed meat and sugar beverages in 249 people aged 65–90 years with mild cognitive impairment. In that study, the highest intake of the processed food pattern was associated with the lowest cognitive performance on a global cognitive examination.

The results of this analysis must be interpreted with consideration of the study's limitations. Three of the five dietary patterns were associated with cognitive performance on multiple domain-specific assessments, but only the alcohol/salads pattern was associated with incident cognitive impairment on the SIS. This discrepancy may reflect a higher sensitivity of the domain-specific assessments to detect cognitive differences relative to the SIS. Additionally, through our use of multivariable modelling, we attempted to minimise the influence of several confounders on the associations between dietary patterns and cognitive function. Regardless of our efforts, the possibility of residual confounding still remains. The correlation between socio-economic status and cognition is well established, and several studies have reported attenuations in associations between dietary patterns and various cognitive outcomes after the adjustment of socio-economic measures( Reference Akbaraly, Singh-Manoux and Marmot 13 , Reference Parrott, Shatenstein and Ferland 48 ). However, it is notable that many of the associations between dietary patterns and cognitive function in this analysis remained significant even after adjustment for income and education. One final limitation is the possibility for recall bias to exist in the measurement of our dietary data by FFQ. It is reasonable to suggest that participants with lower cognitive function would have more difficulty providing accurate dietary data via recall of food frequency. However, we attempted to minimise the potential of recall bias by excluding participants with cognitive impairment at baseline from the longitudinal analysis of incident cognitive impairment.

Despite these limitations, we believe this study provides a unique perspective of the diet–cognition relationship in a very large cohort of geographically dispersed black and white Americans. Utilising empirically derived dietary patterns with no pre-specification of diet quality, we identified a plant-based and alcohol/salads dietary pattern associated with higher cognitive performance and a Southern dietary pattern associated with lower cognitive performance. Findings from this study, in conjunction with previous literature, could be used to develop interventions to maintain the cognitive function of older Americans.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/jns.2016.27

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the other investigators, the staff, and the participants of the REGARDS study for their valuable contributions. A full list of participating REGARDS investigators and institutions can be found at http://www.regardsstudy.org

This research project is supported by a cooperative agreement (U01 NS041588) from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Service.

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke or the National Institutes of Health. Representatives of the funding agency have been involved in the review of the manuscript but not directly involved in the collection, management, analysis or interpretation of the data.

Additional funding was provided by the General Mills Bell Institute of Health and Nutrition for the coding of the Block FFQ.

All of the authors that contributed to this study have no disclosures to report. K. P. was responsible for study concept and design, statistical analysis and interpretation of the data, drafting/revising the manuscript and is the guarantor for the accuracy of the data analysis and the conduct of this research. V. W. contributed to the study design, interpretation of the data and provided critical revision of the manuscript. L. M. contributed to the study design, analysis and interpretation of the data and critical revision of the manuscript. J. S. provided critical revision of the manuscript. F. U. provided critical revision of the manuscript. S. J. contributed to the acquisition of the data, study design, analysis and interpretation of the data and critical revision of the manuscript.

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

1. Ortman, JM, Velkoff, VA & Hogan, H (2014) An Aging Nation: the Older Population in the United States, pp. 251140. Washington, DC: US Census Bureau.Google Scholar
2. Alzheimer's Association (2016) 2016 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement 12, 459509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3. Kalmijn, S, Feskens, EJM, Launer, LJ, et al. (1997) Polyunsaturated fatty acids, antioxidants, and cognitive function in very old men. Am J Epidemiol 145, 3341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Tangney, CC, et al. (2005) Fish consumption and cognitive decline with age in a large community study. Arch Neurol 62, 18491853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5. Pribis, P, Bailey, RN, Russell, AA, et al. (2012) Effects of walnut consumption on cognitive performance in young adults. Br J Nutr 107, 13931401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Solfrizzi, V, Panza, F & Capurso, A (2003) The role of diet in cognitive decline. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 110, 95110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Subash, S, Essa, MM, Al-Adawi, S, et al. (2014) Neuroprotective effects of berry fruits on neurodegenerative diseases. Neural Regen Res 9, 15571566.Google ScholarPubMed
8. Devore, EE, Kang, JH, Breteler, MM, et al. (2012) Dietary intakes of berries and flavonoids in relation to cognitive decline. Ann Neurol 72, 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Hu, FB (2002) Dietary pattern analysis: a new direction in nutritional epidemiology. Curr Opin Lipidol 13, 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Psaltopoulou, T, Sergentanis, TN, Panagiotakos, DB, et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet, stroke, cognitive impairment, and depression: a meta-analysis. Ann Neurol 74, 580591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Morris, MC, Tangney, CC, Wang, Y, et al. (2015) MIND diet slows cognitive decline with aging. Alzheimers Dement 11, 10151022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Morris, MC, Tangney, CC, Wang, Y, et al. (2015) MIND diet associated with reduced incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement 11, 10071014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Akbaraly, TN, Singh-Manoux, A, Marmot, MG, et al. (2009) Education attenuates the association between dietary patterns and cognition. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 27, 147154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Kesse-Guyot, E, Andreeva, VA, Jeandel, C, et al. (2012) A healthy dietary pattern at midlife is associated with subsequent cognitive performance. J Nutr 142, 909915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Ashby-Mitchell, K, Peeters, A & Anstey, KJ (2015) Role of dietary pattern analysis in determining cognitive status in elderly Australian adults. Nutrients 7, 10521067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Kim, J, Yu, A, Choi, BY, et al. (2015) Dietary patterns and cognitive function in Korean older adults. Eur J Nutr 54, 309318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Sugawara, N, Yasui-Furukori, N, Umeda, T, et al. (2015) Relationship between dietary patterns and cognitive function in a community-dwelling population in Japan. Asia Pac J Public Health 27, NP2651NP2660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18. Shakersain, B, Santoni, G, Larsson, SC, et al. (2015) Prudent diet may attenuate the adverse effects of Western diet on cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement 12, 100109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Qin, B, Adair, LS, Plassman, BL, et al. (2015) Dietary patterns and cognitive decline among Chinese older adults. Epidemiology 26, 758768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Judd, SE, Letter, AJ, Shikany, JM, et al. (2014) Dietary patterns derived using exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis are stable and generalizable across race, region, and gender subgroups in the REGARDS study. Front Nutr 1, 29.Google ScholarPubMed
21. Howard, VJ, Cushman, M, Pulley, L, et al. (2005) The REasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke study: objectives and design. Neuroepidemiology 25, 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Boucher, B, Cotterchio, M, Kreiger, N, et al. (2006) Validity and reliability of the Block98 food-frequency questionnaire in a sample of Canadian women. Public Health Nutr 9, 8493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23. Block, G, Woods, M, Potosky, A, et al. (1990) Validation of a self-administered diet history questionnaire using multiple diet records. J Clin Epidemiol 43, 13271335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Caan, BJ, Slattery, ML, Potter, J, et al. (1998) Comparison of the Block and the Willett self-administered semiquantitative food frequency questionnaires with an interviewer-administered dietary history. Am J Epidemiol 148, 11371147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Judd, SE, Gutierrez, OM, Newby, PK, et al. (2013) Dietary patterns are associated with incident stroke and contribute to excess risk of stroke in black Americans. Stroke 44, 33053311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Shikany, JM, Safford, MM, Newby, P, et al. (2015) Southern dietary pattern is associated with hazard of acute coronary heart disease in the Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study. Circulation 132, 804814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Gutierrez, OM, Judd, SE, Voeks, JH, et al. (2015) Diet patterns and risk of sepsis in community-dwelling adults: a cohort study. BMC Infect Dis 15, 231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Gutierrez, OM, Muntner, P, Rizk, DV, et al. (2014) Dietary patterns and risk of death and progression to ESRD in individuals with CKD: a cohort study. Am J Kidney Dis 64, 204213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Newby, P & Tucker, KL (2004) Empirically derived eating patterns using factor or cluster analysis: a review. Nutr Rev 62, 177203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30. Callahan, CM, Unverzagt, FW, Hui, SL, et al. (2002) Six-item screener to identify cognitive impairment among potential subjects for clinical research. Med Care 40, 771781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Welsh, KA, Butters, N, Mohs, RC, et al. (1994) The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology 44, 609614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32. Kohout, FJ, Berkman, LF, Evans, DA, et al. (1993) Two shorter forms of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression) depression symptoms index. J Aging Health 5, 179193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Chan, R, Chan, D & Woo, J (2013) A cross sectional study to examine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive impairment in older Chinese people in Hong Kong. J Nutr Health Aging 17, 757765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Polidori, MC, Pratico, D, Mangialasche, F, et al. (2009) High fruit and vegetable intake is positively correlated with antioxidant status and cognitive performance in healthy subjects. J Alzheimers Dis 17, 921927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. Kang, JH, Ascherio, A & Grodstein, F (2005) Fruit and vegetable consumption and cognitive decline in aging women. Ann Neurol 57, 713720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Tangney, CC, et al. (2006) Associations of vegetable and fruit consumption with age-related cognitive change. Neurology 67, 13701376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. John, JH, Ziebland, S, Yudkin, P, et al. (2002) Effects of fruit and vegetable consumption on plasma antioxidant concentrations and blood pressure: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 359, 19691974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. He, FJ, Nowson, CA, Lucas, M, et al. (2007) Increased consumption of fruit and vegetables is related to a reduced risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Hum Hypertens 21, 717728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. Breteler, MM, Claus, JJ, Grobbee, DE, et al. (1994) Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam Study. BMJ 308, 16041608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Kilander, L, Nyman, H, Boberg, M, et al. (1998) Hypertension is related to cognitive impairment: a 20-year follow-up of 999 men. Hypertension 31, 780786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Torres, SJ, Lautenschlager, NT, Wattanapenpaiboon, N, et al. (2012) Dietary patterns are associated with cognition among older people with mild cognitive impairment. Nutrients 4, 15421551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42. Stampfer, MJ, Kang, JH, Chen, J, et al. (2005) Effects of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive function in women. New Engl J Med 352, 245253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43. Lang, I, Wallace, RB, Huppert, FA, et al. (2007) Moderate alcohol consumption in older adults is associated with better cognition and well-being than abstinence. Age Ageing 36, 256261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. Hendrie, HC, Gao, S, Hall, KS, et al. (1996) The relationship between alcohol consumption, cognitive performance, and daily functioning in an urban sample of older black Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc 44, 11581165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
45. McGuire, LC, Ajani, UA & Ford, ES (2007) Cognitive functioning in late life: the impact of moderate alcohol consumption. Ann Epidemiol 17, 9399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46. Hatch, SL, Jones, PB, Kuh, D, et al. (2007) Childhood cognitive ability and adult mental health in the British 1946 birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 64, 22852296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47. Batty, GD, Deary, IJ, Schoon, I, et al. (2008) Childhood mental ability and adult alcohol intake and alcohol problems: the 1970 British cohort study. Am J Public Health 98, 22372243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. Parrott, MD, Shatenstein, B, Ferland, G, et al. (2013) Relationship between diet quality and cognition depends on socioeconomic position in healthy older adults. J Nutr 143, 17671773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics by quintile of dietary pattern in the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort 2003–2014(Number of participants and percentages, mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 1

Table 2. Odds of incident cognitive impairment by quintile of dietary pattern in the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study 2003–2014 (n 18 080)*(Odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals)

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Multivariable-adjusted mean differences and 95 % confidence intervals on the Word List Learning assessment. Adjusted for age, race, sex, region, total energy intake, income, education, physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD and depressive symptoms. Example interpretation: participants with factor scores in quintiles (Q) 3, 4 and 5 of the Southern dietary pattern scored significantly lower on the Word List Learning assessment than participants in Q1. * Mean differences were statistically significant (P < 0·05).

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Multivariable-adjusted mean differences and 95 % confidence intervals on the Word List Delayed Recall assessment. Adjusted for age, race, sex, region, total energy intake, income, education, physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD and depressive symptoms. Example interpretation: participants with factor scores in quintiles (Q) 4 and 5 of the Southern dietary pattern scored significantly lower on the Word List Delayed Recall assessment than participants in Q1. * Mean differences were statistically significant (P < 0·05).

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Multivariable-adjusted mean differences and 95 % confidence intervals on the Animal Fluency Test. Adjusted for age, race, sex, region, total energy intake, income, education, physical activity, smoking status, BMI, hypertensive status, diabetes status, history of CVD, depressive symptoms and disallowed help/prompting. Example interpretation: participants with factor scores in quintiles (Q) 3, 4 and 5 of the Southern dietary pattern scored significantly lower on the Animal Fluency Test than participants in Q1. * Mean differences were statistically significant (P < 0·05).

Supplementary material: PDF

Pearson supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Pearson supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 116.2 KB