Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-lvwk9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-07T03:23:23.229Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mapping the Racial Bias of the White Male Capital Juror: Jury Composition and the “Empathic Divide”

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2024

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article examines the nature of racial bias in the death sentencing process. After reviewing the various general explanations for the continued significance of race in capital cases, we report the results of an empirical study in which some aspects of racially biased death sentencing are examined in depth. Specifically, in a simulated capital penalty-phase trial setting where participants were assigned to small group “juries” and given an opportunity to deliberate, white male jurors were significantly more likely to sentence black defendants to death than were women and nonwhite jurors. This racialized pattern was explained in part by the differential evaluation of the case facts and the perceptions of the defendant that were made by the white male jurors. We discuss these findings in light of social psychological theories of contemporary racism, and we conclude that the demonstrated bias in capital jury settings should be understood as an interaction of several factors, including individual juror characteristics, group-level demographic composition, and group deliberation processes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2011 Law and Society Association.

Racial bias within legal institutions is a long-standing concern among sociolegal scholars (Reference TrubekTrubek 1990). This scholarship has made clear that racial factors shape legal outcomes through a complex interaction of individual-level, group-level, situational, and structural forces (Reference Haney LópezHaney López 2006; Reference WardWard et al. 2009). Within criminal law, a large and methodologically diverse body of research indicates that racial and ethnic bias against nonwhite defendants continues to affect criminal case outcomes in multiple and complicated ways (see, e.g., Reference Everett and WojtkiewiczEverett & Wojtkiewicz 2002; Reference KauttKautt 2009; Reference SommersSommers 2007; Reference SteenSteen et al. 2005; Reference Steffensmeier and DemuthSteffensmeier & Demuth 2000). These biases are especially problematic in death penalty cases, where jurors are exclusively empowered to render life and death sentencing verdicts. Although some of the racial disparity in how death sentences are meted out is the product of prosecutorial decisionmaking in seeking the death penalty (Reference Baldus, Woodworth and PulaskiBaldus, Woodworth, & Pulaski 1990; Reference Paternoster and BramePaternoster & Brame 2003; Reference Radelet and PierceRadelet & Pierce 1985), studies continue to demonstrate that jurors' death sentencing behavior is significantly affected by the race of the defendant and the race of the victim in the case.

The present article addresses some of the reasons that race continues to matter in the death sentencing process. We first frame the problem of racialized death sentencing as in part the product of larger structural forces that culminate in—but are not restricted to—the capital trial process. In addition, there are a number of features of capital punishment that operate to amplify or exacerbate racial animus, which we address in detail here. Finally, we acknowledge that the effects of these broader social and structural forces devolve to the behavior of individual jurors and, in particular, to white male jurors, in ways that produce racially disparate outcomes. We focus on some of the psychological mechanisms by which white male jurors help bring these outcomes about.

Social Scientific Analyses of Contemporary Racism

The various theoretical approaches to understanding racial bias can be arranged along a continuum that ranges from an emphasis on broad, social structural influences, to a focus on dynamic social and group-based processes, to more individualistic and intrapsychic explanations. At one end of this continuum, sociological theorists underscore the broader structural settings in which racism occurs and argue that these macro variables are virtually always implicated in the creation and perpetuation of racialized individual behaviors. Thus, Reference Bonilla-SilvaBonilla-Silva (1997) argues that understanding the instances, attitudes, or ideologies that constitute “racism” requires an examination of the many social systems that are based in part on racial hierarchies, one that acknowledges the various ways that these hierarchies shape social relations among groups that are situated differently within them.

In the case of race relations in the United States, in particular, the historical pattern of white domination has ensured inequitable distributions of resources and opportunities. Yet the methods by which these inequalities are preserved have changed shape over time, making them difficult to identify and even harder to rectify. Thus, racial hierarchies and inequalities, which give rise to prejudice and discrimination, are operated in seemingly even-handed, “neutral” ways (e.g., Reference Haney and HurtadoHaney & Hurtado 1994) and are produced and reproduced at a systemic level that makes them appear “natural” and the explicit “fault” of no particular individual (see also Reference MasseyMassey 2007; Reference Haney LópezHaney López 2000; Reference WacquantWacquant 2000).

At the group level, a number of social psychologists have examined forms of intergroup discrimination, focusing on the role of social identity, as a way to understand contemporary racism. This research has acknowledged the dynamic nature of prejudice and identifies individuals' salient group membership(s), which can be multiple, contradictory, and fluid over time and across situations, as the source of in-group favoritism (e.g., Reference ScheepersScheepers et al. 2006). However, because some group identities are more stable than others, certain demographic characteristics (such as gender and racial or ethnic identity), which tend to be less malleable, are more centrally related to prejudiced beliefs. This line of research suggests that in-group bias is not necessarily manifested through the amplification of negative attributions directed at out-groups, but through the assignment of more positive attributes for in-group members (Reference Dovidio, Blanchard and CrosbyDovidio et al. 1989), explicit expressions of in-group favoritism (Reference TajfelTajfel 1982), and the withholding of empathy or positive affect for out-group members (Reference Pettigrew and MeertensPettigrew & Meertens 1995).

In addition, social group stereotyping in particular is understood to be shaped by sociohistorical factors, which accounts for the persistence of widely known (if not widely accepted) culturally specific stereotypes about different groups in any given society (Reference DevineDevine 1989; Reference Devine and ElliotDevine & Elliot 1995). As a result, racial and cultural stereotypes are generally highly accessible to most members of a given culture and can shape views and perceptions about different groups in a subtle yet pervasive manner.

At a more individual level, social psychologists have emphasized the role of certain cognitive processes—including the human tendency to rely on stereotypes and other heuristics to simplify a complex social world—that may contribute to contemporary racism. Cognitive shortcuts, such as stereotyping, allow for more efficient information processing, memory, and retrieval of the complicated stimuli with which we are bombarded on a day-to-day basis (Reference Fiske and TaylorFiske & Taylor 1991). Unfortunately, of course, these very shortcuts often produce overgeneralized, inaccurate knowledge about the social world (Reference KriegerKrieger 1995). Some scholarship on stereotyping distinguishes prejudice, which is characterized as more affective and motivational, from the more cognitive and somewhat automatic and nonconscious processes that are sometimes at the basis of the stereotyping process (Reference Amodio and DevineAmodio & Devine 2006). Nonetheless, both prejudice and stereotyping can contribute to biased assessments and, consequently, lead to discriminatory behavior.

Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and GaertnerGaertner and Dovidio's (1986) theory of aversive racism integrates some of these perspectives, joining the cognitive and affective aspects of stereotyping with an appreciation of the situational and structural factors that help activate forms of racial discrimination. They argue that although blatant, intentional racism has become less socially acceptable in the post–civil rights era, a more subtle, unconscious form of prejudice has become increasingly prevalent. Indeed, they suggest that, unlike like other forms of racism that have been associated with political conservatism, aversive racism is particularly prevalent among “well intentioned, liberal, and well educated individuals” who espouse egalitarian beliefs (Reference Hodson and ChinHodson et al. 2004:120). Aversive racists are more likely to engage in forms of prejudiced or discriminatory behavior when their behavior can be justified on nonracial grounds (Reference DovidioDovidio 2001), and they often express their prejudice by showing bias in favor of their own in-group rather than outward bias against a group of disfavored others (Reference Hodson and ChinHodson et al. 2004).

In addition to its intrapsychic dimensions, aversive racism is thought to be triggered by certain aspects of the context or situation in which it occurs. Thus, this particular form of racism is more likely to be activated in settings where the prevailing social norms are conflicting or ambiguous, or in situations where the persons making judgments do not have adequate time or sufficient cognitive resources to fully process the available information (Reference DevineDevine 1989; Reference KawakamiKawakami et al. 1998). Conversely, the activation of aversive racism is less likely in situations where race itself is made salient, or where there are no race-neutral reasons available to explain disparate treatment (Reference Gaertner and DovidioGaertner & Dovidio 2005; Reference SommersSommers 2007).

Racism and Death Sentencing

There are a number of reasons why racial animus might be expected to operate with greater force in the criminal justice system—where the explicit mandate is to administer punishment to wrongdoers—than in the society at large. For one, it is easier to punish persons more harshly if they have already been demonized, are perceived as somehow less than fully human, or are regarded as fundamentally “other” (e.g., Reference Bandura and ReichBandura 1989). The derogated status of criminals in general lowers the social inhibition against subjecting them to harsh punishment, helping in part to justify—psychologically as well as legally—the treatment they receive. At the same time, it may be easier to believe that people from already disfavored groups—here, those who are racially stigmatized—have done bad things and, therefore, deserve punishment. This is especially true if, as is the case with racial stereotypes in our society, belief in their inherent “badness” is part of their stigma.

In addition, belief in the inherent, pre-existing badness of certain groups may make it easier for dominant group members to ignore flaws in the legal processes by which those derogated others are officially blamed and punished for their transgressions. Similarly—because they are more feared and despised to begin with—it may be easier to exaggerate the seriousness of the things that members of already disfavored groups have been found guilty of doing. That is, it may be easier to believe that whatever crimes these “others” have committed are per se more heinous (whereas if they were committed by members of one's own group, they would be less so). Finally, because it is more difficult to identify with persons perceived as “other,” dominant group members can more easily distance themselves from the pains of whatever punishment these “others” receive, notwithstanding the possibility that such punishment may be unjustly administered or excessive in amount.

Within this larger criminal justice system context, death penalty trials are in many ways ideal settings for contemporary racism to flourish. For one, because the death qualification process systematically excludes persons on the basis of their strong death penalty views from the jury, the demographic makeup of the capital jury is distinctive and problematic.Footnote 1 That is, compared to juries seated in nondeath cases, death-qualified jury pools are disproportionately white, male, older, and more religiously and politically conservative (Reference Filkins and TindaleFilkins et al. 1998; Reference GrossGross 1998; Reference HaneyHaney et al. 1994; Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2000). These demographic characteristics tend to correlate with measures of subtle racism (Reference BarnesBarnes 1997; Reference EmersonEmerson et al. 1999; Reference Meertens and PettigrewMeertens & Pettigrew 1997). Furthermore, survey research suggests that support for the death penalty among whites is highly correlated with measures of anti-black racial prejudice and stereotyping (Reference ArthurArthur 1998; Reference Barkan and CohnBarkan & Cohn 1994; Reference Bobo and JohnsonBobo & Johnson 2004; Reference SossSoss et al. 2003; Reference Unnever and CullenUnnever & Cullen 2007).

Second, the kinds of crimes that give rise to capital prosecutions not only provoke heightened levels of anger and fear among jurors but also are more likely to activate cultural stereotypes about minority ethnic and racial groups. In this regard, Reference Quillian and PagerQuillian and Pager (2001) have argued that the stereotype of blacks as violent and criminally inclined is one of the most pervasive, well-known, and persistent stereotypes in American culture. While other negative cultural stereotypes about blacks have significantly diminished, this one has remained strong and influential, particularly among whites (see also Reference Devine and ElliotDevine & Elliot 1995; Reference Peffley and HurwitzPeffley & Hurwitz 2002; Reference Hurwitz and PeffleyHurwitz & Peffley 1997).

Moreover, research suggests that persons tend to judge those defendants charged with stereotype-consistent offenses more harshly than those charged with stereotype-inconsistent crimes (Reference GordonGordon 1990; Reference Jones and KaplanJones & Kaplan 2003; Reference Mazzella and FeingoldMazzella & Feingold 1994). In the specific context of capital cases, Reference Fleury-SteinerFleury-Steiner (2004) found that stereotypes among capital jurors about minority defendants and their propensity toward violence shaped deliberations and outcomes. Research also indicates that there is variable discrimination against black capital defendants who kill white victims as a function of the stereotypicality of their appearance (Reference EberhardtEberhardt et al. 2006). Using defendant photographs from actual capital cases tried in Philadelphia, Eberhardt and colleagues found that the more “stereotypically black” the capital defendant appeared to independent raters, the more likely it was that the defendant had received a death sentence. Even among criminal justice professionals, racial stereotypes, when activated, have been shown to have an adverse effect on assessments of offenders' culpability and the severity of punishment deserved (Reference Graham and LoweryGraham & Lowery 2004).

Additional aspects of the context in which capital cases are decided render them especially susceptible to the influence of race. Capital jurors are placed in a unique and unfamiliar situation where the decisionmaking norms and the values that should be brought to bear on the task at hand are particularly unclear. Key elements of the death penalty decision—including making judgments about moral culpability and blameworthiness, gauging the heinousness of the capital crime, and weighing past acts of violence against the defendant's worth or value as a person—are inherently subjective assessments whose parameters are both unfamiliar and emotionally wrought. Research suggests that racial animus may be more readily mobilized and acted upon in a setting where decisionmaking norms are so ambiguous.

In addition, capital jury sentencing instructions are notoriously difficult for jurors to understand and apply (Reference Eisenberg and WellsEisenberg & Wells 1993; Reference Haney and LynchHaney & Lynch 1994, Reference Haney and Lynch1997; Reference Luginbuhl and BurkheadLuginbuhl & Burkhead 1994; Reference TiersmaTiersma 1995; Reference WienerWiener et al. 1995), which increases the likelihood that their judgments will be shaped by pre-existing biases. Indeed, prior research indicates that when persons rely on rules or guidelines that are unclear, or difficult to understand and follow, their judgments are more likely to be influenced by race (Reference BodenhausenBodenhausen 1988; Reference Bodenhausen and LichtensteinBodenhausen & Lichtenstein 1987; Reference Bodenhausen and WyerBodenhausen & Wyer 1985; Reference Hill and PfeiferHill & Pfeifer 1992; Reference Pfeifer and OgloffPfeifer & Ogloff 1991; Reference RectorRector et al. 1993). In fact, researchers have found that poor juror comprehension of capital sentencing instructions is related to racially biased death sentencing (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney, 2000; Reference Shaked-SchroerShaked-Schroer et al. 2008).

Moreover, because the creation of empathy for the defendant—usually through the presentation of a mitigating narrative in the penalty phase of the case—is important in convincing jurors to vote for life over death (Reference HaneyHaney 2004), black defendants may be especially disadvantaged at the hands of typically white jurors. We know that empathy is more likely to be felt for those who are perceived as similar to one's self (Reference LinderLinder 1996), and for this reason it is less likely to be extended to defendants who are perceived as fundamentally “other,” not only because of what they have been convicted of doing but also because of their racial characteristics (Reference EisenbergEisenberg et al. 1996; Reference HaneyHaney 2004, Reference Haney2005).

Finally, and in a related way, the racial divisions that plague American society operate to subject black capital defendants to what has been called “biographical racism”—the “accumulation of race-based obstacles, indignities, and criminogenic influences that characterizes the life histories of so many African American capital defendants” (Reference HaneyHaney 2004:1557). The fact that they are more often exposed to criminogenic forces and factors over the course of their lives helps account for their presence as defendants in capital cases. Yet most white jurors have no framework for understanding the operation of these forces. Thus, black capital defendants may be doubly disadvantaged—first by being subjected to social structural forces that shape their lives in problematic and even destructive ways, and second by having the nature and value of those lives judged finally by persons ill equipped to understand their structural determinants and mitigating aspects.

Thus, all of these forces and factors—demographic and attitudinal features of capital jurors, the characteristics of capital cases, the parameters of the penalty-phase decisionmaking process, and the larger social and institutional structure that facilitates systemic racism—likely play important roles in the activation and expression of racial bias in capital jury decisionmaking.

Evidence of Persistent Race-Based Death Sentencing

In light of the many reasons to expect that racial factors would operate with particular force and effect in the administration of capital punishment, it is not surprising to find a breadth of evidence documenting the extent to which they have and do. Indeed, in the long history of the death penalty in the United States, blacks have been consistently sentenced to die in numbers that greatly exceed their percentages in the population at large. Even before the American Revolution, the Southern colonies managed their captive slave workforces in part by relying on “ever-increasing lists of capital statutes” that were applied to blacks and not whites (Reference BannerBanner 2002:8). The legacy of this early system persisted to modern times. For example, when the U.S. Supreme Court examined the operation of the nation's system of death sentencing in Furman v. Georgia (1972), Justice Thurgood Marshall noted that a total of 405 blacks had been executed for the crime of rape over the preceding 40 years, compared to 48 whites (1972:365, n. 147).

Although the problem of racialized death sentencing was discussed at some length in Furman, it was the standardless discretion exercised by capital juries that the Court found decisive in declaring state death penalty statutes unconstitutional as applied. Following the Court's reinstatement of capital punishment four years later (Gregg v. Georgia 1976), evidence that race still played an important role in the imposition of the death penalty continued to mount. Studies indicated that blacks were overpunished as defendants or undervalued as victims, or both, in capital cases (e.g., Reference Bowers and PierceBowers & Pierce 1980; Reference Gross and MauroGross & Mauro 1984, Reference Gross and Mauro1989; Reference ZieselZiesel 1981). By the time the Court considered the issue of racially discriminatory death sentencing directly, in McCleskey v. Kemp (1987), careful data analyses that controlled for a host of potentially confounding variables (e.g., Reference Baldus, Woodworth and PulaskiBaldus, Woodworth, & Pulaski 1990) revealed many of the same disparities.

Although the McCleskey Court ruled that statistical patterns of racial disparities in death sentencing alone lacked constitutional significance, those patterns were repeatedly documented in the social science literature. In a congressionally mandated summary and review of some 28 studies of the topic, a Government Accounting Office report found that the race of the victim significantly influenced death sentencing in about four out of five of the studies, and that the race of the defendant significantly influenced it in about half of them (U.S. Government Accounting Office 1990). Thus, despite some variation in the outcomes of the studies, racial factors continued to play a significant role in the death sentencing process, sometimes strikingly so, as when a black capital defendant had been convicted of a potentially capital murder in which the victim was white.

More recently, studies of capital jury sentencing verdicts rendered during the 1980s and 1990s in the state of New Jersey and in the city of Philadelphia found that juries were significantly more likely to sentence black defendants to death in comparison to all other defendants (Reference Baldus, Woodworth and ZuckermanBaldus, Woodworth, Zuckerman, et al. 1998).Footnote 2 The Philadelphia study examined all potentially capital cases from 1983 to 1993 and indicated that a black capital defendant was 9.3 times more likely to be sentenced to death by the jury than a similarly situated defendant of another race or ethnicity. While these studies demonstrated a race of victim effect as well—cases involving black victims produced fewer death sentences than others—this effect was weaker than the influence of race of defendant. Similarly, an examination of capital case outcomes in the state of Maryland found that, compared to all other combinations of victims and offenders, blacks who killed white victims were significantly more likely to have death notices filed against them, proceed to trial as capital cases, and receive death sentences at the penalty phase of their case in comparison to otherwise similarly situated defendants (Reference Paternoster and BramePaternoster & Brame 2003).

The results of these archival studies parallel findings obtained from experimental or “simulation” research on race and sentencing. This approach permits greater control over extraneous variables, so that only the race of the defendant and/or the race of the victim can be systematically varied. In one early meta-analysis of existing experimental studies on race-of-defendant effects, Reference Sweeney and HaneySweeney and Haney reported that, despite some inconsistent findings in individual studies, there was a significant overall race-of-defendant effect such that black defendants tended to be sentenced more harshly than others. In addition, they found that “studies demonstrating greater methodological rigor more consistently uncovered racial bias in sentencing decisions” (1992:191).

In later research using an experimental or simulation design, the present authors found significant race-of-defendant effects in individual-level death penalty sentencing verdicts (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2000). In that study, a total of 348 jury-eligible adult participants individually viewed one of four identical versions of a staged capital penalty trial that varied only by the race of the defendant and the victim (black or white). Participants then determined the sentence—either life without possibility of parole or death. Our findings indicated that the black defendant was significantly more likely to be sentenced to death than the white defendant and that the discrepancy in sentence outcome was particularly pronounced in the cross-racial conditions.Footnote 3

Even more recently, we replicated this initial study in another jurisdiction and modified the design to include a group deliberation component (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009). More than 500 jury-eligible, death-qualified adults were randomly assigned to four- to seven-person “juries” that viewed one of the four versions of the penalty trial tape and deliberated to determine the sentence. We found that the black defendant was again more likely to be sentenced to death than the white defendant, and this effect was amplified by the race of the victim (such that, for example, the white defendant convicted of killing a black victim received the fewest death verdicts). Notably, we also found that the race-of-defendant effect was significantly strengthened by the deliberation process, which tended to increase our juror-participants' support for a death sentence overall and especially so in the black defendant conditions.

Capital Juror Characteristics and Racialized Death Sentencing

Obviously, these broad patterns of racially discriminatory death sentencing result from the aggregate thoughts and actions of individual decision makers—capital jurors—who make judgments and render verdicts in tandem with others. Reference Sweeney and HaneySweeney and Haney concluded their meta-analysis of experimental research on racially discriminatory sentencing by emphasizing precisely this interplay between defendant, victim, and juror characteristics: “Thus sentencing discrimination is not generic bias against Black defendants. Rather, it appears to result from a specific punitive reaction to crimes committed by members of another race against persons of one's own” (1992:192). In recent years, increased attention has been focused on the specific characteristics of capital jurors that may help explain the racialized nature of their specific punitive reactions.

The work of the Capital Jury Project (CJP) on this topic is especially noteworthy. CJP is a multistate study in which a large sample of actual capital jurors who had previously served through the penalty phase of a death penalty trial are systematically interviewed about their experiences as jurors. In one CJP study that analyzed juror data from 14 different states, Reference BowersBowers et al. (2001) found what they termed a “white male dominance” effect in cases where black defendants were accused of killing white victims. Specifically, when the jury included five or more white men, the jury was significantly more likely to sentence the defendant to death than when it included four or fewer white men (71 percent versus 30 percent ending in death sentences). Conversely, the same study also uncovered what the researchers termed a “black male presence” effect, such that the presence of one or more black men on the jury significantly reduced the likelihood of a death sentence in the case. In another CJP study utilizing data from capital cases tried in South Carolina, Reference GarveyGarvey found that black jurors were much more likely to “keep the sin separate from the sinner” (2000:47) in both black and white defendant cases and had more overall positive feelings toward the defendant than did white jurors.

With the CJP insights about the importance of considering the specific racial makeup of the capital jury in mind, we returned to our latest study of discriminatory death sentencing (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009), and reanalyzed a significant subset of our data to determine the role of these jury-based interpersonal dynamics in producing our results. We also sought to extend understandings of the psychological processes by which the white male dominance effect comes about. That is, how and why do white male jurors reach the racially tinged sentencing verdicts that they do, and what are the patterns by which their private judgments influence the views of their fellow jurors?

The Present Research

In the remainder of this article, we specifically focus on our findings regarding the interactions between juror characteristics and case factors, as mediated by the deliberation process.Footnote 4 As noted above, we found that white male jurors differed from all others (i.e., women and nonwhite men) in their sentencing decisions as a function of defendant race (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009). Our goal here is to explore in much more depth the ways in which the two demographic groups of jurors (white men and nonwhites/women) differed from each other, and within their own groups, in the way they processed the evidence that was presented in the penalty trial, and in their perceptions of the defendant, as a function of the racial dynamics that were present in the trial they viewed. Notwithstanding the obvious limitations of simulated trial studies in terms of the reduced gravity, emotionality, and complexity that can be created in an experimental setting, this method offers the best way to control for literally all extraneous factors that may shape juror decisionmaking and confound potential race effects. It also allows us to measure a number of aspects of the decisionmaking process that would otherwise be impossible to do in a real capital case, including the deliberation process, individual participants' perceptions, cognitions and emotions in relation to the case, and pre- and postdeliberation assessments.

We first describe the specific elements of our methodological design and then detail our findings in several subsections. We begin with descriptive data to provide an overview of our participants and their characteristics, including the demographic and attitudinal differences between our participant subgroups of interest. We follow with a delineation of jury group-level findings, including how the demographic composition of those small groups appeared to interact with racial characteristics of the case in shaping verdicts. We then provide a detailed examination of how white men differed from nonwhites and women in their individual sentencing decisions, as a function of the race of the defendant, followed by our sets of findings on how these two subgroups interpreted specific evidence in the case and how they viewed the defendant's attributional and motivational “profile.” Following our results sections, we interrogate whether, in light of these and other findings about the pervasiveness of racial bias in the administration of capital punishment, a fair and equitable death penalty system is achievable, and we consider how our findings provide additional insight into our understanding of contemporary racism.

Overview of the Method and Design

The study itself was designed to minimize some of the weaknesses that are often found in jury simulation studies. Specifically, we used jury-eligible nonstudent participants, relied on audio and visual stimulus materials (rather than paper transcripts or summaries), and, most important, included a group deliberation component which took place in a non-university setting that mimicked more closely a jury room. The experiment was conducted in a large, relatively urban county in California with nonstudent adults who were screened in advance for their jury eligibility and death qualification status.

A total of 539 participants were selected and assigned to one of 100 small group (four- to seven-person) juries. We aimed for six-person jury units by assigning seven people to any given jury slot. If fewer than four people showed up for a time slot, the session was canceled, the participants who came were paid, and they were then offered an opportunity to sign up for a different session. Each jury unit viewed a video of one of four versions of a capital penalty trial, which were identical in all aspects except that the race of the defendant (either black or white) and the race of the victim (black or white) were varied to create the four experimental conditions.

The simulated capital penalty trial included witness testimony, opening statements and final arguments from the defense and prosecution, and the judge's reading of the set of California pattern instructions to be used in sentence determination. The robbery-murder case facts were based on an actual California capital penalty trial, chosen because it contained a number of features of a “typical” capital murder and also because pre-testing had shown that it contained “mid-range” case facts—ones that were neither so aggravated nor so mitigated that participants would vote overwhelmingly for one or another sentencing verdict. The prosecution's penalty-phase aggravation consisted of the circumstances of the crime, victim impact testimony, evidence that the defendant had been accused of a prior similar crime, and the defendant's apparent lack of remorse for the present crime. Defense mitigation included evidence that the defendant had been severely abused as a child, had suffered from alcohol and drug abuse at the time of the crime, and had pre-existing psychiatric problems that may have impaired the defendant's mental state, and testimony that the defendant was still loved and needed by his family. We pre-tested the case to make sure it was perceived as neither so aggravated that it nearly always ended in death, nor so mitigated that it primarily ended in a life verdict.

After jurors finished viewing the videotape, but before they had an opportunity to discuss the case, they were asked to confidentially and individually make a written verdict choice and to indicate their confidence in that choice. Participants were told that this was a nonbinding “straw vote” that they were free to change during or after deliberation. Each group of jurors was asked to select a foreperson and then to deliberate to reach a unanimous sentencing verdict of either life without the possibility of parole or the death penalty. The jury was also asked to indicate its collective confidence in the decision on a 7-point scale. Each jury was given copies of the judicial instructions that had been read by the judge and a verdict form on which to record the sentencing decision.

A video camera recorded deliberations, and the jurors were left alone in the jury room to deliberate. The deliberation period was limited to a maximum of one and a half hours. If the jury was unable to reach a unanimous verdict within that time limit it was declared deadlocked, and the nature of the split was noted on a “mistrial voting form,” including each individual juror's verdict and confidence rating. When the deliberation was completed and the instructions and the verdict forms had been collected, each individual juror was asked to complete a series of questionnaires, including a “juror perceptions” questionnaire that contained several open-ended questions about the decisionmaking process as well as a series of closed-ended questions that asked for evaluations of the defendant, attorneys, witnesses, penalty trial evidence, and arguments presented, and perceptions about why the defendant committed the capital crime. In addition, participants completed an instructional comprehension questionnaire and a demographics questionnaire.

Results

General Descriptive Data

The group of 539 participants was composed of 61 percent women and 39 percent men, with a mean age of about 48 years old. Self-report race/ethnicity data indicated that approximately 82 percent of our participants identified as white, 8 percent identified as Latino/a, 4 percent as Asian American, and 4 percent as black. The participant group included somewhat more whites and correspondingly fewer and Latino/as and people of Asian descent than the general county population. This was likely in part a product of the jury eligibility criteria and the effect of the death qualification screening process. Four out of five (81 percent) of the participants had at least some college education, and about 40 percent had at least a four-year degree, which is roughly equivalent to the higher degree attainment for the county as a whole. As would be expected among a death-qualified group, three-quarters (76 percent) of our participants were somewhat or strongly in favor of the death penalty, 20 percent identified themselves as somewhat opposed, and only 4 percent reported that they were strongly opposed.

The white male group totaled 32 percent of our sample (N=172). In a subset of the following analyses, we examine within-group differences as a function of defendant race for this subgroup of our participants. We also report on some between-groups analyses, in which we compare our target group of interest (the white men) to the remaining 68 percent of our participants (N=367). Before bifurcating the group in this manner, we examined white women and nonwhites as two distinct subgroups to see whether any within-group race effects were present. Neither subgroup demonstrated within-group race effects (the sentencing rates were consistent across conditions, indicating no bias for or against either the black or white defendant). The white women and nonwhites did, however, significantly differ from each other in overall death sentencing rates across defendant race conditions.Footnote 5 Nonetheless, because our interest in this article is to explore in more detail the underlying factors of the demonstrated racial bias against black defendant, which stems from our white male participants and not from our women or participants of color, we isolated the white male participants as a distinct group for our analyses. In other words, our goal is to look at this specific subgroup of our participants to understand the contours and extent of their demonstrated bias.

In order to be able to control for their effects in subsequent between groups analyses, we compared the two groups—white men and the others—along several important demographic and attitudinal dimensions. These analyses indicated no significant differences between the two groups in their overall attitudes about the death penalty, general criminal justice beliefs, their political perspective, prior jury experience, employment status, or educational attainment. There were, however, two ways in which the white male jurors differed from the other participants: they were slightly older (the white men's mean age was approximately 50 years versus 47 years for the other participants), and they had slightly (but statistically significantly) higher comprehension of the jury instructions (15.7 out 35 possible points for the white men versus 14.2 for the others). While the age of the participants did not significantly correlate with sentence outcome in our data, there is literature to suggest that age is related to support for capital punishment and to racial attitudes, so we controlled for it anyway. On the other hand, in regard to the differences in comprehension, our previous research indicated that greater comprehension should have the effect of mitigating racial bias in outcomes, so if anything, this between-groups difference should have dampened the degree of divergence between the white men and the others. Nonetheless, we controlled for it in our analyses here.

Jury Unit Demographics, Race of Defendant, and Sentence Decision

This study was designed to examine individual-level effects, nested within jury groups, so it did not include the large number of jury groups that would be needed to achieve the required statistical power when using the jury as a single unit of analysis.Footnote 6 So although most of our analyses rely on individual-level measures (i.e., demographics, perceptions, case assessments, instructional comprehension, etc.), we report group-level data for their descriptive value. Thus, we note first that the majority of the jury groups favored a death sentence—65 of the 100 jury units voted unanimously or in the majority for death after deliberation. The remainder were either evenly split (6) or unanimously or majority/unanimously in favor of a life sentence (29). This preference for death was not necessarily predictable based on the straw vote ballots. Of the 100 groups, only 47 percent went into deliberations with a majority of the group favoring death. As prior research on jury group dynamics has indicated, majority at first ballot typically predicts final verdict (Reference HansHans 2007; Reference Sandys and DillehaySandys & Dillehay 1995). Moreover, there were not significant differences in the straw vote majority compositions as a function of defendant race; in fact, only 44 percent of the black defendant cases favored death at the straw ballot, whereas 50 percent of the white defendant cases had a majority favoring death at the same stage.

As Figure 1 illustrates, there were differences in final case outcomes as a function of defendant race and ratio of white men on the jury. We created a dichotomous variable that split the juries at the median, so those with 33 percent or more white males were considered “high” white male juries (this designated a proportion of white male representation that, at its lowest, was just above their overall representation among our participants). In practical terms for our four- to seven-person small groups, this meant that a minimum of two white men were necessary to qualify as having a high concentration. Our logic for making such a split is based on Reference BowersBowers et al.'s (2001) implication that there may be a nonmajority tipping point of white male concentration on the capital jury that triggers biased influence on the group.

Figure 1. Percentage of Majority or Unanimous Death Sentence Outcomes by Defendant Race and White Male Composition of Jury.

Looking only at the cases where there was a unanimous or majority verdict (N=94), we found that the juries with a high concentration of white men were much more death-prone toward black defendants than were juries with a low concentration of white men, or ones that viewed the white defendant. As is illustrated in Figure 1, fully 86 percent of those heavily white male juries in the black defendant condition favored death verdicts. Otherwise, the death percentage ranged from 60 to 68 percent. Independent samples t-tests comparing high and low white male group composition revealed that mean differences for group verdict in the black defendant condition were marginally significant, favoring a death verdict (t=−1.76, p<0.09), whereas in the white defendant condition, there were no differences between high and low white male jury groups. We also looked at whether the ratio of white men on the jury predicted verdict severity for the black defendant and the white defendant, and found that it had no impact for the white defendant (β=−0.123; t=−0.857; R 2=0.01). White male ratio did, however, predict sentence severity for the black defendant to a marginally significant degree (β=0.271; t=1.95, p=0.057; R 2=0.07).

White men were disproportionately likely to become forepersons, which may have resulted in an even more substantial influence during deliberation. Specifically, although white men made up 32 percent of the participants, they were forepersons on 45 of the 100 mock juries. This disproportion is more striking in light of the fact that 18 of the mock juries had no white men at all (in contrast, only one jury was fully constituted by white men).Footnote 7 Put somewhat differently, then, on the 81 juries in which white men had the opportunity but were not compelled to be the foreperson, they represented only 37 percent of participants but 54 percent of the forepersons. Of the 16 juries that had both a white male foreperson and a black defendant, only two (12.5 percent) favored life after deliberation. In contrast, 31 percent of the white defendant cases with white men as forepersons favored a life sentence after deliberation.

Race and Gender of Participant, Race of Defendant, and Sentence Decision

We conducted a number of individual-level analyses and found that (among other findings) both gender and race/ethnicity of our participants interacted with the defendant race independent variable in terms of sentence outcomes, and in terms of how case evidence was evaluated. As in our previous experiment, no main race-of-victim main effect on sentence outcome was obtained in this study across our participants or as an interaction effect with gender/race of juror.

Overall, at the individual level, approximately 55 percent of our participants indicated a preference for death in their straw votes,Footnote 8 and after deliberation, about 66 percent favored death, indicating that a punitive shift resulted from the deliberation process. Men were significantly more likely to vote for death in their straw votes than women (60 percent for men versus 51 percent for women; Chi-square (1)=3.94, p<0.05); that gender difference disappeared for final votes (68 versus 65 percent for death). White participants were also significantly more likely to cast straw votes and final verdicts for death than nonwhites, although the gap shrank after deliberation (58 versus 39 percent at the straw vote; Chi-square (1)=10.69; p=0.001, and 69 versus 54 percent at the final vote; Chi-square (1)=8.17, p<0.01). White men as a distinct subgroup were significantly more likely to vote for death at the straw vote than the others (64 versus 50 percent; Chi-square (1)=8.71, p<0.01); however, that gap narrowed to marginal significance by the final vote (72 versus 64 percent; Chi-square (1)=3.15; p<0.10). These data indirectly suggest that white men persuaded the other jurors to move closer to their own death-leaning position during the deliberation process.

We also examined whether and how our two distinct groups of jurors made decisions as a function of the race of the defendant. We found that white male participants assigned a significantly higher percentage of death sentences than the women and nonwhite participants to the black defendant at the straw vote (Chi-square=7.48, p<0.01) and in their final verdict (Chi-square=9.50, p<0.01), but not to the white defendant at either judgment point (see Figure 2). In terms of within-group race-of-defendant effects, we also found that white men were significantly more likely to select a death sentence as the final verdict for the black defendant than for the white defendant, whereas women and nonwhite participants (both as distinct groups and as a single combined group) did not demonstrate significant differences across race conditions in either vote (Chi-square for defendant race × final verdict for white men=11.91, p=0.001).

Figure 2. Verdict Choice, Pre- and Post-Deliberation: White Men vs. Others.

In order to control for the jury unit group effects, we conducted the remainder of our analyses of postdeliberation dependent variables using hierarchical linear mixed models with individual juror nested within jury unit, controlling for individual instruction score and age in our analyses between the groups. First we looked at the impact of defendant race and white male status on juror final verdict, then final verdict certain certainty. The analysis of jurors' verdict, nested within jury, indicates a significant main race-of-defendant effect (t(170.0)=2.08, p<0.05), main white male juror effect (t(446.1)=3.08, p<0.01), and race of defendant × juror race/gender interaction effect (t(443.4)=−2.07, p<0.05). Similarly, there were significant effects when using verdict certainty as the dependent measure (race of defendant t(172.6)=−2.81, p<0.01, white male juror t(447.1)=−3.82, p<0.001, and race of defendant × juror race/gender t(444.4)=2.47, p<0.05).

When jurors (nested in jury units) were analyzed as two distinct groups (either white male or not), again, it was clear that the race effects for sentence outcomes were driven by the white male participant subgroup. Specifically, the white male jurors demonstrated significant racial bias against the black defendant relative to the white defendant in final verdict (t(79.30)=3.19, p<0.01) and verdict certainty (t(79.2)=−3.59, p=0.001), whereas the nonwhite and female groups demonstrated no within-group differences in sentence outcomes on either measure as a function of defendant race.

This differential in sentence outcome appears to be substantially the product of the deliberation process. Thus, as illustrated in Figure 2, although the mock jurors on the whole moved toward death after deliberation, the shift toward death was most extreme for the white male jurors in the black defendant condition. To measure this within-subject change, we created a variable that measured shift in verdict certainty scores from straw vote to final verdict and found that the white male participants shifted their certainty toward death from the straw vote to the final vote to a significantly greater degree in the black defendant conditions than in the white defendant conditions (t(88.29)=−2.65, p=0.01). Indeed, these participants moved very slightly toward life for the white defendant (mean=−0.06), whereas they moved a full point toward death on a 7-point scale for the black defendant (mean=1.03). The white men's resistance against a death verdict for the white defendant represented the only circumstance under which there was not a shift toward death in the study. There was no significant difference as a function of defendant race for within-group shift in verdict certainty by defendant race for the women and nonwhite participant group. Again, because of the split indicated on the straw ballots, this shift toward death in general, and particularly for the black defendant, cannot be explained by a pre-existing majority for death.

Race and Gender of Participant, Race of Defendant, and the Evaluation of Evidence

In order to determine more precisely why and how our white male participants treated the black defendant more punitively than the white defendant, both within group and as compared to their female and nonwhite peers, we examined how this group perceived various aspects of the case in the two race-of-defendant conditions. First, we looked at how they judged the evidence and other case elements, and then we looked at how they viewed the potential reasons why the defendant committed the capital offense, as a function of defendant race. Specifically, we analyzed a series of measures designed to assess internal versus external causal attributions that might support either a mitigated or an aggravated sentence.

As we found in previous work (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2000) and as we reported previously on this data set (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009), the disparate treatment of defendants as a function of their race appears to be explained by the way the jurors differentially assessed the penalty-phase evidence, especially the mitigation evidence. This differentiation, though, was mostly isolated to the white men. Specifically, all four pieces of mitigating evidence and two out of four pieces of aggravating evidence were significantly influenced by the race of the defendant for the white male participants. The white men were significantly less likely to consider all of the mitigating evidence in favor of life for the black defendant compared to the white defendant. They also weighed the aggravating evidence about the murder itself and the testimony that the defendant did not express any remorse for the crime significantly more in favor of death for the black defendant. In contrast, within-group analyses of the women and nonwhite jurors revealed no differences in the way that the evidence was weighed as a function of defendant race.

We also compared the white men to the other participants in how they weighed the case evidence. In the black defendant conditions, the white men gave significantly less weight to the mitigating evidence of a history of childhood abuse and evidence that the defendant was still loved and needed by the defendant's family. They also weighed the circumstances of the murder and the evidence that the defendant lacked remorse significantly more in favor of death compared to the other participants, but only in the black defendant conditions. In the white defendant conditions, the white men differed from the others in two evaluations of the mitigating evidence. In evaluating the evidence of childhood abuse and the evidence that the defendant had substance abuse issues, the white men weighed the evidence significantly more in favor of life than did the other participants.

The Defendant's Profile and Juror Characteristics

Unlike the other participants, the white male jurors also viewed the defendant's criminal “profile” differentially as a function of the defendant's race. Although they did not see the black defendant as more dangerous than the white defendant,Footnote 9 they did view him as less remorseful for the crime (t(68.85)=2.35, p<0.05), more cold-hearted (t(62.08)=2.95, p<0.01), and, to a marginally significant degree, as more likely to reoffend (t(86.49)=−1.75, p<0.10).

Participants also answered a series of questions about the defendant's possible motivations for committing the capital crime and were provided options from which to select that were either internal or external in attributional nature, as well as ones that were either potentially aggravating or potentially mitigating. As is illustrated in Table 1, white male participants were significantly more likely to agree with internal, aggravating attributions for the black defendant compared to the white defendant, and significantly less likely to agree with several of the mitigating attributions for the black defendant, particularly those central to this case: that the defendant committed the crime due to a traumatic childhood and that he committed the crime because he suffered from mental illness.Footnote 10 As Table 1 indicates, the nonwhite and women participants generally did not give significantly different attributions based on defendant race.

Table 1. Attributions About Defendant's Behavior by Defendant Race: White Men Participants vs. Other Participants

Notes: N=169 for white men; N=370 for nonwhites and women.

** p<0.01; *p<0.05; +p<0.1, marginally significant, using linear mixed model hierarchical analysis, with juror nested within jury. Lower number indicates more agreement with that attribution on a 4-point scale.

Our white male jurors not only demonstrated within-group differences in their attributions as a function of defendant race, but they also differed from the other participants in attributing causality, but only in the black defendant condition. Specifically, they were significantly more likely to agree that the black defendant liked to harm others (t(257.6)=3.0, p<0.01) and that he was violent by nature (t(267.8)=1.98, p<0.05) when compared to the remainder of the participants. They were also marginally less likely to attribute the black defendant's crime to a traumatic childhood (t(258.2.64)=−1.90, p<0.06) and marginally more likely to attribute it to the defendant's greed (t(258.1)=1.91, p<0.06) and desperation for money (t(259.6)=1.95, p<0.06) than the other jurors. The two groups of participants did not significantly differ from each other in any of their attributions for the white defendant.

Discussion

The jurors in this experimental study were exposed to exactly the same case and evidence—each word of testimony and argument was identical—in each of the conditions that we employed. Yet the white men who participated as jurors in our study appear to have heard and judged a different case as a function of the race of the defendant. When they judged a black defendant—and essentially only then—they diverged significantly from their peers, both in terms of how they constructed the defendant's blameworthiness and motivation, and on whether they believed he deserved to be allowed to continue to live. Thus, the white male jurors in this study judged a black defendant whom they tended to view as driven by the defendant's own depraved predispositions, and as someone whose criminal behavior they were reluctant to interpret as the product of the defendant's dysfunctional and psychologically damaging background.

As a consequence, these jurors gave significantly less weight to the mitigation that had been offered in order to place the defendant's behavior in a broader and more sympathetic context. White male jurors also saw the black defendant as less redeemable, in that he was viewed as more cold-hearted and remorseless, and as someone who would be more likely to re-offend if given the chance. Furthermore, these effects were evident despite the fact that White male participants had significantly higher comprehension of the capital jury sentencing instructions, something our prior research indicated should have, if anything, moderated racially disparate outcomes and suppressed the between group differences (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2000). In addition, the significance of the race effects we uncovered in this study is likely somewhat muted, relative to the real world. That is, because we matched our defendants on a number of physical appearance dimensions, the punitive effect of racial stereotypicality in appearance, which has been demonstrated in prior research (Reference EberhardtEberhardt et al. 2006), is suppressed by our methodological design.

Especially problematic, and contrary to our expectations—in light of the mandate to seek consensus in a jury deliberation setting—was that the divergence between the way black and white defendants were perceived and treated grew wider after the deliberation process. By the final verdict, the white male participants stood apart in their extreme punitiveness toward the black defendant whose sentence they decided. Conversely, when considering the white defendant's fate, they were remarkably similar to the other participants, especially once they had deliberated with them. Thus, the race-based punitive tendencies of the white male jurors in this study were amplified rather than muted by the presence of others and the opportunity to publicly discuss their views. This seems to indicate that a form of aversive racism (Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and GaertnerGaertner & Dovidio 1986) may have been activated by the deliberation process itself. It also indirectly indicates that many white male jurors were resistant to alternative perspectives or points of view when they were on juries where the fate of a black defendant was being decided. Moreover, it appeared that these jurors in particular had disproportionate influence on the others during deliberations, moving the other jurors more toward death overall, especially in the black defendant condition.

The racial differences in the evaluation of mitigating evidence (and the importance of these differentials for death sentencing) seem to stem from the fact that white male participants were less able or inclined to empathize with—or strive to understand the plight of—the black defendant. This represents an exacerbation of an already sizable “empathic divide” between the typical juror and defendant in a capital case (Reference HaneyHaney 2004). Yet the jury's genuine understanding of a capital defendant's background and social history and their willingness to grasp that evidence's mitigating significance is what determines whether they will choose life over death (Reference HaneyHaney 1995, Reference Haney2008).

In this sense, the present research represents an initial attempt to map the size and dimensions of this problematic empathic divide, especially the various ways in which race appears to increase its size and effect. Consistent with Reference BowersBowers et al.'s (2001) work, we found that those capital juries that were composed of a significant number (i.e., a third or more) of white men were particularly resistant to the defense penalty-phase case that was presented on behalf of a black defendant. Moreover, this resistance was both amplified and solidified by the deliberation process, where the white male jurors also appeared especially influential in bringing other jurors to their punitive point of view.

These findings appear to have important implications for our understanding of white prejudice against black defendants, and the daunting challenge of creating racial fairness in these most serious and consequential criminal cases. This research reinforces previous conclusions reached by a number of scholars about the way that the race of the defendant shapes criminal justice outcomes. As they have suggested, examining only global outcomes—across decision makers and/or across types of crimes, case characteristics, and offender categories—often masks the multiple ways that race interacts with individual, situational, and/or structural factors to produce and perpetuate inequality (see, e.g., Reference Bushway and PiehlBushway & Piehl 2001; Reference MitchellMitchell 2005; Reference SommersSommers 2007). In the present study, we were able to tease out and analyze the key interactions between juror demographics and defendant race in how the case elements were conceived, weighed, and decided upon, thereby adding to a more contextualized understanding of how and why a defendant's race matters in some cases but not in others.

In addition, this experimental study adds methodological diversity to existing scholarship on race and the death penalty. Our findings converge with studies that employ other methodological designs to examine race effects in capital cases, including quantitative and qualitative analyses of archival and interview data (e.g., Reference Baldus, Woodworth and ZuckermanBaldus, Woodworth, Zuckerman, et al. 1998; Reference BowersBowers et al. 2001). Notwithstanding the sacrifice in mundane realism that jury simulations almost always entail, experimental data such as these add considerable explanatory power to the proposition that race matters greatly in these contexts.

Taken together, all these studies raise important questions about whether and how the death sentencing process can be made racially fair. The death penalty is not meted out in a race-neutral manner, nor is it imposed on persons purely on the basis of their moral culpability. Instead, a variety of impermissible but potent “extralegal” factors (including defendants' and victims' class, gender, and race; local politics; and jurisdictional resources and quality of representation) appear to shape the ultimate outcome of a capital case. Beyond that, other aspects of the overall system of death sentencing compound these problems, serving to exacerbate rather than ameliorate the capital jury's problematic decisionmaking tendencies (Reference HaneyHaney 2005). For example, the death qualification process increases the likelihood that biased jurors will be seated and racially discriminatory sentences produced. Moreover, it appears that the race-based decisionmaking that occurs in capital cases is not simply a product either of individuals or the situations in which they act. Person-situation interactions—demographically related tendencies and the demands of the setting in which these morally complex judgments are made—combine to produce these pernicious outcomes. What remains to be answered is why this situational context, at least as demonstrated by this study, particularly exacerbates racial bias among white men, and not others.

More broadly, these findings complicate social-psychological theorizing about the nature and mechanisms of contemporary racism in that they suggest an interactive process by which racial bias plays out in sociolegal judgment settings. Thus, an integrated theory that takes into account three levels of contributory factors—individual demographic factors, group composition factors, and the influence of decisionmaking processes—may be warranted to understand the conditions under which racism is especially likely to be influential (see Diagram 1 for an illustration of the capital penalty trial scenario). Attending to multiple levels of factors, and considering their interactions, has the potential to be fruitful in explaining why racially disparate outcomes in capital cases persist even in situations where there is no overt racism present, and where individual-level pre-deliberation responses may not necessarily predict racially biased outcomes. As such, these findings converge with recent work by Reference WardWard et al. (2009) indicating the value of looking at how group-level diversity within a criminal justice setting has a significant and independent impact on equitable justice outcomes.

Diagram 1

In addition, these findings strengthen support for theories that highlight differences in positive emotional and cognitive responses in explaining contemporary racism. Indeed, it appears that the majority of the racial disparity is due to a differential assessment of the sympathetic and positive aspects of the defendant and varying levels of empathy for the defendant by our white male participants. These participants not only were less generous in considering mitigating evidence than the others for the Black defendant, but they were also significantly more generous in this regard for the white defendant. Yet because these effects were limited to the white male participant subgroup, such perceptual and affective responses must be understood as at least partly filtered through a specifically raced and gendered worldview or life experience.

Finally, our findings lend further support to the notion that situational and procedural factors matter in the activation of racial animus, as Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and GaertnerGaertner and Dovidio's concept of “aversive racism” postulates (1986). In this case, the deliberation process appeared to trigger the activation of such racism, resulting in a significant shift toward a death verdict for the black defendant and a resistance to such a shift among white men when considering the fate of the white defendant.Footnote 11 Yet contrary to Gaertner and Dovidio's hypothesizing, this dynamic did not cut across all white participants; it was, again, isolated among the white men.

Conclusion

Although the U.S. Supreme Court has recently affirmed and solidified its commitment to ensuring that blacks are not excluded from participation on capital juries by prosecutors who use racially targeted peremptory challenges (Miller-El v. Dretke 2005; Snyder v. Louisiana 2008), issues of genuine racial fairness are much broader, more complicated, and seemingly elusive. As we have noted, for example, a concentration of white male jurors sitting in a black defendant's penalty trial appears to undermine the jury's willingness to consider the case for mitigation. Consequently, ensuring some representation for nonwhite capital jurors may not be sufficient to deliver a truly fair, diverse, and impartial jury. Because the process of minority exclusion is a cumulative one—resulting from the way in which venire pools are constructed, jury eligibility is determined, and death qualification and jury selection are conducted—there is still a high probability that the juries that are finally impaneled to hear and decide a capital case will be susceptible to the kind of white male juror bias that we have uncovered in this study.

There are some additional interventions that could be instituted to mitigate such bias. Thus, particularly in capital cases, where racially diverse juries are especially difficult to seat, courts could adopt policies of “affirmative jury selection” (e.g., Reference FukuraiFukurai et al. 1993; Reference Sommers and Michael NortonSommers & Norton 2008) whereby racial minorities are oversampled in the pool to ensure more substantial representation in the venire and a greater likelihood of significant representation on the jury itself. Another potential strategy might be to use targeted voir dire, designed to make the problem of racial bias salient to potential jurors in a manner that would inhibit the activation of aversive racism (e.g., Reference Sommers and EllsworthSommers & Ellsworth 2001). In addition, strategies may be utilized to more intensely probe white male potential jurors' attitudes and viewpoints during death qualification in cases involving nonwhite defendants to uncover individual-level biases. Finally, any efforts to simplify and clarify the guidelines given to capital juries have the potential to mitigate some racial bias, although it appears that incomprehension has less explanatory value for bias after deliberation (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009).

Because white male jurors are more likely to be the beneficiaries of structural privilege and less likely to have personal experience with oppression than their nonwhite and female counterparts, additional steps may need to be taken to present them with persuasive and effective mitigating evidence. That is, even greater effort and special insight may need to be brought to bear in capital cases that involve black defendants, especially if there is a cross-racial dynamic at issue in the case itself. Thus, it seems especially important for defense attorneys, investigators, and experts to recognize the nature of the challenge they confront in such cases, and to meet it through the careful development, thoughtful analysis, and effective presentation of the compelling mitigating narrative that explains the client's life in social contextual terms.

However, we concede that implementing the full panoply of these and other creative strategies may not be enough to entirely overcome racially unfair death sentencing in the United States. For many of the reasons we have discussed here, the post-Furman v. Georgia (1972) reforms that were intended to address standardless decisionmaking on the part of capital juries have not done so. Furthermore, tinkering with the final stage of a process that disproportionately impacts poor and minority defendants at each prior stage will only have limited impact on racially disparate outcomes. To the extent that “biographical racism” (Reference HaneyHaney 2004) contributes to violence; that death-eligible offenses continue to be defined in ways that target poorer, minority defendants; that prosecutors use their considerable discretion to seek death in ways that overvalue white victims and overpenalize nonwhite offenders; and that defendants in the most death-prone jurisdictions often have the poorest quality representation, bias and discrimination will continue to shape the nation's system of capital punishment.

Footnotes

This research was generously supported by the National Science Foundation's Law and Social Science Program. We are indebted to the Law & Society Review editors and to the anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments and invaluable suggestions.

1 Those who feel so strongly in favor of the death penalty that their ability would be so hampered are also excluded, yet research indicates that these individuals are not nearly as well screened and are less likely to be excluded than are opponents. See Reference Sandys, Scott McClelland and AckerSandys and McClelland (1998).

2 The bulk of the capital racial disparities research, like this work, has addressed differences in outcomes for black versus white defendants, with only limited attention given to Hispanic/Latino defendants.

3 We offer an extended explanation of our relatively muted race-of-victim effect (we found no main effect of the race of the victim in either study, but we did find that it amplified effects in the cross-racial conditions) in Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch and Haney (2000, Reference Lynch and Haney2009). There is both empirical and logical support for the notion that the race of the defendant becomes relatively more important at the penalty trial stage.

4 We have reported elsewhere (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009) on an additional set of findings from this experiment, primarily addressing how the results of the replication with the deliberation component differed (or not) from our original study (Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2000). In that 2009 article, we described the main race effects that were demonstrated, the role of instructional comprehension in decisionmaking, and differences in how evidence was evaluated as a function of verdict type and race condition. We reported briefly on the interaction of juror characteristics and race conditions in that article, but here we aim to provide a more detailed analysis of that phenomenon (see Reference Lynch and HaneyLynch & Haney 2009 for details on these other findings). We have also reported previously on some analyses of the qualitative data from both experiments, specifically how they speak to racial bias in capital sentencing (see Reference Lynch, Sarat and OgletreeLynch 2006).

5 Sixty-seven percent of the white women opted for a death sentence; 54 percent of the nonwhites did so (Chi-square (1)=5.16; p<0.05). Defendant race did not independently predict differences between these two subgroups.

6 Our power analysis with the predicted effect size indicated that we would have needed 400 total juries to reliably uncover main race effects at the group level. Given the high costs associated with using nonstudent participants with group-level units of analysis in an experiment of this complexity, this simply was not possible.

7 This was a black defendant case that moved from being evenly split between life and death at the straw vote to being unanimous for death after deliberation.

8 The apparent incongruity between the number of death majority juries at the straw vote stage and the number of individuals who opted for death in their straw votes is an artifact of the differing small group sizes and voting distributions of the jury units. To analogize to a sporting scenario, a baseball team, for example, could win four out of seven games in a World Series despite scoring fewer overall runs in those seven games by winning four games by 2–1 scores and losing three by 1–3 scores.

9 This is likely due at least in part to our matching strategy, in which we pre-tested possible defendants to find near-perfect matches between our black and white defendants on a number of dimensions, including age, attractiveness, appearing “scary,” angry, dangerous, etc.

10 The white men were also significantly more likely to attribute the black defendant's behavior to being desperate for money, which can be read in a couple of different ways. It may be that this reason would help mitigate the gravity of the crime in that it might imply less premeditation, thus benefitting the defendant, or it may play into stereotypes about race and poverty (Reference Fleury-SteinerFleury-Steiner 2004), which would not necessarily mitigate culpability.

11 This aspect of our findings is quite reminiscent of Reference BernardBernard's (1979) findings that deliberation exacerbated racially disparate outcomes.

References

References

Amodio, David, & Devine, Patricia (2006) “Stereotyping and Evaluation in Implicit Race Bias: Evidence for Independent Constructs and Unique Effects on Behavior,” 91 J. of Personality and Social Psychology 652–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arthur, John A. (1998) “Racial Attitudes and Opinions About Capital Punishment: Preliminary Findings,” 22 International J. of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice 131–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldus, David, Woodworth, George, & Pulaski, Charles (1990) Equal Justice and the Death Penalty: A Legal Empirical Analysis. Boston: Northeastern Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Baldus, David, Woodworth, George, Zuckerman, David, et al. (1998) “Racial Discrimination and the Death Penalty in the Post-Furman Era: An Empirical and Legal Overview, With Recent Findings From Philadelphia,” 83 Cornell Law Rev. 1638–770.Google Scholar
Bandura, Albert (1989) “Mechanisms of Moral Disengagement,” in Reich, W., ed., Origins of Terrorism: Psychologies, Ideologies, Theologies, States of Mind. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Banner, Stuart (2002) The Death Penalty: An American History. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkan, Steven E., & Cohn, Steven (1994) “Racial Prejudice and Support for the Death Penalty by Whites,” 31 J. of Research in Crime and Delinquency 202–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, Sandra L. (1997) “Practicing What You Preach: An Analysis of Racial Attitudes of Two Christian Churches,” 21 Western J. of Black Studies 111.Google Scholar
Bernard, J. L. (1979) “Interaction Between Race of Defendant and That of Jurors in Determining Verdicts,” 5 Law and Psychology Rev. 103–11.Google Scholar
Bobo, Lawrence D., & Johnson, Devon (2004) “A Taste for Punishment: Black and White Americans' Views on the Death Penalty and the War on Drugs,” 1 Du Bois Rev.: Social Science Research on Race 151–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodenhausen, Galen (1988) “Stereotypic Biases in Social Decision Making and Memory: Testing Process Models of Stereotypic Use,” 55 J. of Personality and Social Psychology 726–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodenhausen, Galen, & Lichtenstein, Meryl (1987) “Social Stereotypes and Information Processing Strategies: The Impact of Task Complexity,” 52 J. of Personality and Social Psychology 871–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodenhausen, Galen, & Wyer, Robert (1985) “Effects of Stereotypes on Decision-Making and Information-Processing Strategies,” 48 J. of Personality and Social Psychology 267–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonilla-Silva, Eduardo (1997) “Rethinking Racism: Toward a Structural Interpretation,” 62 American Sociological Rev. 465–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, William, & Pierce, Glenn (1980) “Arbitrariness and Discrimination Under post-Furman Capital Statutes,” 26 Crime & Delinquency 563635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, William, et al. (2001) “Death Sentencing in Black and White: An Empirical Analysis of the Role of Jurors' Race and Jury Racial Composition,” 3 University of Pennsylvania J. of Constitutional Law 171274.Google Scholar
Bushway, Shawn, & Piehl, Anne Morrison (2001) “Judging Judicial Discretion: Legal Factors and Racial Discrimination in Sentencing,” 35 Law & Society Rev. 733–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, Patricia (1989) “Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components,” 56 J. of Personality and Social Psychology 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, Patricia, & Elliot, Andrew (1995) “Are Racial Stereotypes Really Fading? The Princeton Trilogy Revisited,” 21 Personality and Social Psychology Bull. 1139–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, John (2001) “On the Nature of Contemporary Prejudice: The Third Wave,” 57 J. of Social Issues 821–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, John, et al. (1989) “Resistance to Affirmative Action: The Implication of Aversive Racism,” in Blanchard, F. & Crosby, F., eds., Affirmative Action in Perspective. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Eberhardt, Jennifer, et al. (2006) “Looking Deathworthy: Perceived Stereotypicality of Black Defendants Predicts Capital Sentencing Outcomes,” 17 Psychological Science 383–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, Theodore, & Wells, Martin (1993) “Deadly Confusion: Juror Instructions in Capital Cases,” 79 Cornell Law Rev. 152.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, Theodore, et al. (1996) “Jury Responsibility in Capital Sentencing: An Empirical Study,” 44 Buffalo Law Rev. 339–81.Google Scholar
Emerson, Michael, et al. (1999) “Equal in Christ, but Not in the World: White Conservative Protestants and Explanations of Black-White Inequality,” 46 Social Problems 398417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, Ronald, & Wojtkiewicz, Roger (2002) “Difference, Disparity, and Race/Ethnic Bias in Federal Sentencing,” 18 J. of Quantitative Criminology 189211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filkins, Joseph, et al. (1998) “An Evaluation of the Biasing Effects of Death Qualification: A Meta-Analytic/Computer Simulation Approach,” in Tindale, R. S. et al., eds., Theory and Research on Small Groups. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Fiske, Susan, & Taylor, Shelley (1991) Social Cognition, 2d ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Fleury-Steiner, Benjamin (2004) Jurors' Stories of Death: How America's Death Penalty Invests in Inequality. Ann Arbor: Univ. of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fukurai, Hiroshi, et al. (1993) Race in the Jury Box: Affirmative Action in Jury Selection. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, Samuel & Dovidio, John (1986) “The Aversive Form of Racism,” in Dovidio, J. & Gaertner, S., eds., Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism: Theory and Research. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gaertner, Samuel & Dovidio, John (2005) “Understanding and Addressing Contemporary Racism: From Aversive Racism to the Common Ingroup Identity Model,” 61 J. of Social Issues 615–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garvey, Stephen (2000) “The Emotional Economy of Capital Sentencing,” 75 New York University Law Rev. 2673.Google Scholar
Gordon, Randall (1990) “Attributions for Blue-Collar and White-Collar Crime: The Effects of Subject and Defendant Race on Simulated Juror Decisions,” 20 J. of Applied Social Psychology 971–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, Sandra, & Lowery, Brian (2004) “Priming Unconscious Racial Stereotypes About Adolescent Offenders,” 28 Law and Human Behavior 483504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, Samuel (1998) “Update: American Public Opinion on the Death Penalty—It's Getting Personal,” 83 Cornell Law Rev. 1448–75.Google Scholar
Gross, Samuel, & Mauro, Robert (1984) “Patterns of Death: An Analysis of Racial Disparities in Capital Sentencing and Homicide Victimization,” 37 Stanford Law Rev. 27153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, Samuel, & Mauro, Robert (1989) Death and Discrimination: Racial Disparities in Capital Sentencing. Boston: Northeastern Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Haney, Craig (1995) “The Social Context of Capital Murder: Social Histories and the Logic of Capital Mitigation,” 35 Santa Clara Law Rev. 547609.Google Scholar
Haney, Craig (2004) “Condemning the Other in Death Penalty Trials: Biographical Racism, Structural Mitigation, and the Empathic Divide,” 53 DePaul Law Rev. 1557–89.Google Scholar
Haney, Craig (2005) Death by Design: Capital Punishment as a Social Psychological System. New York: Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Haney, Craig (2008) “Evolving Standards of Decency: Advancing the Nature and Logic of Capital Mitigation,” 36 Hofstra Law Rev. 835–82.Google Scholar
Haney, Craig, & Hurtado, Aida (1994) “The Jurisprudence of Race and Meritocracy: Standardized Testing and ‘Race Neutral’ Racism in the Workplace,” 18 Law and Human Behavior 223–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haney, Craig, & Lynch, Mona (1994) “Comprehending Life and Death Matters: A Preliminary Study of California's Capital Penalty Instructions,” 18 Law and Human Behavior 411–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haney, Craig, & Lynch, Mona (1997) “Clarifying Life and Death Matters: An Analysis of Instructional Comprehension and Penalty Phase Closing Arguments,” 21 Law and Human Behavior 575–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haney, Craig, et al. (1994) “‘Modern’ Death Qualification: New Data on Its Biasing Effects,” 18 Law and Human Behavior 619–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haney López, Ian (2000) “Institutional Racism: Judicial Conduct and a New Theory of Racial Discrimination,” 109 Yale Law Rev. 1717–884.Google Scholar
Haney López, Ian, ed. (2006) Race, Law and Society. Surrey, UK: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Hans, Valerie (2007) “Deliberation and Dissent: ‘12 Angry Men’ versus the Empirical Reality of Juries,” 82 Chicago-Kent Law Rev. 579–89.Google Scholar
Hill, Erick, & Pfeifer, Jeffrey (1992) “Nullification Instructions and Juror Guilt Ratings: An Examination of Modern Racism,” 16 Contemporary Social Psychology 610.Google Scholar
Hodson, Gordon, et al. (2004) “The Aversive Form of Racism,” in Chin, J., ed., The Psychology of Prejudice and Racism: Racism in America. Santa Barbara, CA: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Hurwitz, Jon, & Peffley, Mark (1997) “Public Perceptions of Race and Crime: The Role of Racial Stereotypes,” 41 American J. of Political Science 375401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Christopher S., & Kaplan, Martin (2003) “The Effects of Racially Stereotypical Crimes on Juror Decision-Making and Information-Processing Strategies,” 25 Basic and Applied Social Psychology 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kautt, Paula (2009) “Heuristic Influences Over Offense Seriousness Calculations: A Multilevel Investigation of Racial Disparity Under Sentencing Guidelines,” 11 Punishment and Society 191218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawakami, Kerry, et al. (1998) “Racial Prejudice and Stereotype Activation,” 24 Personality and Social Psychology Bull. 407–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krieger, Linda (1995) “The Content of Our Categories: A Cognitive Bias Approach to Discrimination and Equal Employment Opportunity,” 47 Stanford Law Rev. 1161–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linder, Douglas (1996) “Juror Empathy and Race,” 63 Tennessee Law Rev. 887916.Google Scholar
Luginbuhl, James, & Burkhead, Michael (1994) “Sources of Bias and Arbitrariness in the Capital Trial,” 50 J. of Social Issues 103–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, Mona (2006) “Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Life and Death Decision-Making: Lessons From Laypersons in an Experimental Setting,” in Sarat, A. & Ogletree, C., eds., From Lynch Mobs to the Killing State: Race and the Death Penalty. New York: New York Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Lynch, Mona, & Haney, Craig (2000) “Discrimination and Instructional Comprehension: Guided Discretion, Racial Bias, and the Death Penalty,” 24 Law and Human Behavior 337–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, Mona, & Haney, Craig (2009) “Capital Jury Deliberation: Effects on Death Sentencing, Comprehension, and Discrimination,” 33 Law and Human Behavior 481–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massey, Douglas S. (2007) Categorically Unequal. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Mazzella, Ronald, & Feingold, Alan (1994) “The Effects of Physical Attractiveness, Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Gender of Defendants and Victims on Judgments of Mock Jurors: A Meta-Analysis,” 24 J. of Applied Social Psychology 1315–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meertens, Roel, & Pettigrew, Thomas (1997) “Is Subtle Prejudice Really Prejudice?61 Public Opinion Q. 5471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, Ojmarrh (2005) “A Meta-Analysis of Race and Sentencing Research: Explaining the Inconsistencies,” 21 J. of Quantitative Criminology 439–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paternoster, Raymond, & Brame, Robert (2003) “An Empirical Analysis of Maryland's Death Sentencing System With Respect to the Influence of Race and Legal Jurisdiction: Final Report,” http://www.newsdesk.umd.edu/pdf/finalrep.pdf (accessed 15 May 2009).Google Scholar
Peffley, Mark, & Hurwitz, Jon (2002) “The Racial Components of “Race-neutral” Crime Policy Attitudes,” 23 Political Psychology 5975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, Thomas, & Meertens, Roel (1995) “Subtle and Blatant Prejudice in Western Europe,” 25 European J. of Social Psychology 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfeifer, Jeffrey, & Ogloff, James (1991) “Ambiguity and Guilt Determinations: A Modern Racism Perspective,” 21 J. of Applied Social Psychology 1713–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quillian, Lincoln, & Pager, Devah (2001) “Black Neighbors, Higher Crime? The Role of Racial Stereotypes in Evaluations of Neighborhood Crime,” 107 American J. of Sociology 717–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radelet, Michael, & Pierce, Glenn (1985) “Race and Prosecutorial Discretion in Homicide Cases,” 19 Law & Society Rev. 587622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rector, Neil, et al. (1993) “The Effect of Prejudice and Judicial Ambiguity on Defendant Guilt Ratings,” 133 J. of Social Psychology 651–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandys, Marla, & Dillehay, Ronald (1995) “First Ballot Votes, Predeliberation Dispositions, and Final Verdicts in Jury Trials,” 19 Law and Human Behavior 175–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandys, Marla, & Scott McClelland, M. (1998) “Stacking the Deck for Guilt and Death: The Failure of Death Qualification to Ensure Impartiality,” in Acker, J. et al., eds., America's Experiment With Capital Punishment: Reflections on the Past, Present, and Future of the Ultimate Penal Sanction. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press.Google Scholar
Scheepers, Daan, et al. (2006) “Diversity in In-Group Bias: Structural Factors, Situational Features, and Social Functions,” 90 J. of Personality and Social Psychology 944–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaked-Schroer, Netta, et al. (2008) “Reducing Racial Bias in the Penalty Phase of Capital Trials,” 26 Behavioral Sciences and the Law 603–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommers, Samuel (2007) “Race and the Decision Making of Juries,” 12 Legal and Criminological Psychology 171–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, Samuel, & Ellsworth, Phoebe (2001) “White Racial Bias: An Investigation of Prejudice Against Black Defendants in the American Courtroom,” 7 Psychology, Public Policy and Law 201–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, Samuel, & Michael Norton, M. (2008) “Race and Jury Selection: Psychological Perspectives on the Peremptory Challenge Debate,” 63 American Psychologist 527–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soss, Joe, et al. (2003) “Why Do White Americans Support the Death Penalty?65 J. of Politics 397421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steen, Sara, et al. (2005) “Images of Danger and Culpability: Racial Stereotyping, Case Processing and Criminal Sentencing,” 43 Criminology 435–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffensmeier, Darrell, & Demuth, Stephen (2000) “Ethnicity and Sentencing Outcomes in US Federal Courts: Who Is Punished More Harshly?65 American Sociological Rev. 705–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, Laura, & Haney, Craig (1992) “The Influence of Race on Sentencing: A Meta-Analytic Review of Experimental Studies,” 10 Behavioral Science and the Law 179–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, Henri (1982) “Social Psychology of Inter-Group Relations,” 33 Annual Rev. of Social Psychology 139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiersma, Peter (1995) “Dictionaries and Death: Do Capital Jurors Understand Mitigation?1995 Utah Law Rev. 149.Google Scholar
Trubek, David (1990) “Back to the Future: The Short, Happy Life of the Law and Society Movement,” 18 Florida State University Law Rev. 155.Google Scholar
Unnever, James D., & Cullen, Francis T. (2007) “The Racial Divide in Support for the Death Penalty: Does White Racism Matter?85 Social Forces 1281–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
U.S. Government Accounting Office (1990) Death Penalty Research Indicates Pattern of Racial Discrimination. Washington, DC: United States General Accounting Office (February), http://archive.gao.gov/t2pbat11/140845.pdf (accessed 6 Dec. 2010).Google Scholar
Wacquant, Loic (2000) “The New ‘Peculiar Institution’: On the Prison as Surrogate Ghetto,” 4 Theoretical Criminology 377–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, Geoff, et al. (2009) “Does Racial Balance in Workforce Representation Yield Equal Justice? Race Relations of Sentencing in Federal Court Organizations,” 43 Law & Society Rev. 757806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, Richard, et al. (1995) “Comprehensibility of Approved Jury Instructions in Capital Murder Cases,” 80 J. of Applied Psychology 455–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziesel, Hans (1981) “Race Bias in the Administration of the Death Penalty: The Florida Experience,” 95 Harvard Law Rev. 456–68.Google Scholar

Cases Cited

Furman v. Georgia, 408 U.S. 238 (1972).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregg v. Georgia, 428 U.S. 153 (1976).Google Scholar
McCleskey v. Kemp, 481 U.S. 279 (1987).Google Scholar
Miller-El v. Dretke, 545 U.S. 231 (2005).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder v. Louisiana, 128 S. Ct. 1203 (2008).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Percentage of Majority or Unanimous Death Sentence Outcomes by Defendant Race and White Male Composition of Jury.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Verdict Choice, Pre- and Post-Deliberation: White Men vs. Others.

Figure 2

Table 1. Attributions About Defendant's Behavior by Defendant Race: White Men Participants vs. Other Participants

Figure 3

Diagram 1