Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T12:50:26.959Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Body image concerns in patients with persecutory delusions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2022

Felicity Waite*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Rowan Diamond
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Nicola Collett
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Emily Bold
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Eleanor Chadwick
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Daniel Freeman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Felicity Waite, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Persecutory fears build on feelings of vulnerability that arise from negative views of the self. Body image concerns have the potential to be a powerful driver of feelings of vulnerability. Body image concerns are likely raised in patients with psychosis given the frequent weight gain. We examined for the first-time body esteem – the self-evaluation of appearance – in relation to symptom and psychological correlates in patients with current persecutory delusions.

Methods

One-hundred and fifteen patients with persecutory delusions in the context of non-affective psychosis completed assessments of body image, self-esteem, body mass index (BMI), psychiatric symptoms and well-being. Body esteem was also assessed in 200 individuals from the general population.

Results

Levels of body esteem were much lower in patients with psychosis than non-clinical controls (d = 1.2, p < 0.001). In patients, body esteem was lower in women than men, and in the overweight or obese BMI categories than the normal weight range. Body image concerns were associated with higher levels of depression (r = 0.55, p < 0.001), negative self-beliefs (r = −0.52, p < 0.001), paranoia (r = −0.25, p = 0.006) and hallucinations (r = −0.21, p = 0.025). Body image concerns were associated with lower levels of psychological wellbeing (r = 0.41, p < 0.001), positive self-beliefs (r = 0.40, p < 0.001), quality of life (r = 0.23, p = 0.015) and overall health (r = 0.31, p = 0.001).

Conclusions

Patients with current persecutory delusions have low body esteem. Body image concerns are associated with poorer physical and mental health, including more severe psychotic experiences. Improving body image for patients with psychosis is a plausible target of intervention, with the potential to result in a wide range of benefits.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

More than half of patients with psychosis meet criteria to be classified as having obesity (Annamalai, Kosir, & Tek, Reference Annamalai, Kosir and Tek2017). Body image concerns are likely present but seldom asked about in clinical practice. Moreover, body image concerns may contribute directly and indirectly to the maintenance of psychotic experiences. Paranoia flourishes when people feel vulnerable due to feeling inferior, odd and apart (Freeman, Reference Freeman2016). Body image concerns likely raise the sense of vulnerability and thus directly contribute to the occurrence of paranoia. Appearance concerns may also contribute to the withdrawal that reaches agoraphobic levels in two-thirds of patients with psychosis (Freeman, Taylor, Molodynski, & Waite, Reference Freeman, Taylor, Molodynski and Waite2019b) and thus indirectly contribute to a worsening of mental health. In this paper, we report for the first time the extent of body image concerns in patients with current psychotic experiences.

Concerns about body image have been shown to be associated with paranoia in the general population (Waite & Freeman, Reference Waite and Freeman2017). In two nationally representative datasets of adults and adolescents, totalling over 15 000 individuals, concerns about weight were significantly associated with higher levels of paranoia (Waite & Freeman, Reference Waite and Freeman2017). The associations remained significant after controlling for gender and body mass index (BMI). These results indicate that negative body image and paranoia are associated in the general population. Yet this association has not been tested with patients with current persecutory delusions.

Rapid weight gain in the initial phase of antipsychotic medication use is common (Pillinger et al., Reference Pillinger, McCutcheon, Vano, Mizuno, Arumuham, Hindley and Howes2020). Qualitative research has identified that patients with psychosis often experience a negative impact from unwanted and ‘uncontrollable’ weight gain (Marshall, Freeman, & Waite, Reference Marshall, Freeman and Waite2019; Waite et al., Reference Waite, Langman, Mulhall, Glogowska, Hartmann‐Boyce, Aveyard and Freeman2022). Patients have described how rapid weight gain, following antipsychotic medication use, compounds a loss of confidence and self-worth, and appearance concerns arise, with further consequences on mood, activity, persecutory fears, content of voices and even episodes of deliberate self-harm and suicidal ideation (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Freeman and Waite2019; Waite et al., Reference Waite, Langman, Mulhall, Glogowska, Hartmann‐Boyce, Aveyard and Freeman2022). Indeed, this rapid weight gain has been described as a ‘double whammy’ of consequences, reflecting impact on both physical and mental health (Haracz, Hazelton, & James, Reference Haracz, Hazelton and James2018).

In patients with psychosis, excess weight is associated with low self-esteem, poor quality of life, social isolation and medication non-adherence (de Hert et al., Reference de Hert, Peuskens, van Winkel, Kalnicka, Hanssens, van Eyck and Peuskens2006; Mccloughen & Foster, Reference Mccloughen and Foster2011). In this study, we set out to provide the first quantitative investigation of body image in patients with persecutory delusions in the context of non-affective psychosis. The primary question was whether body image concerns are likely to be prevalent in this patient population. We also tested whether body image concerns relate to the severity of psychotic experiences, and a range of other psychological, psychiatric and functioning domains.

Method

Participants

The participants were 115 patients with persistent persecutory delusions who had received a non-affective psychosis diagnosis. The patients were recruited as part of the Feeling Safe clinical trial for the treatment of persecutory delusions (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Emsley, Diamond, Collett, Bold, Chadwick and Waite2021) from three NHS mental health trusts: Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Berkshire Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust and Northamptonshire Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust. Ethical approval was received from an NHS Research Ethics Committee (South Central – Oxford B Research Ethics Committee; reference 15/SC/0508).

The inclusion criteria for the trial were: a current, persistent (at least 3 months) persecutory delusion (as defined by Freeman & Garety, Reference Freeman and Garety2000), held with at least 60% conviction; a primary diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum psychosis (non-affective psychosis); aged 16 years or above; and willing and able to give informed consent for participation in the trial. The exclusion criteria were: current receipt of another psychological therapy; insufficient comprehension of English; primary diagnosis of alcohol, drug or personality disorder; receiving treatment in a forensic service; organic syndrome; or learning disability.

Adults from the local population were recruited for a non-clinical control group. The inclusion criteria were: aged 18 years or older; ability to understand and communicate in English; and willing and able to give informed consent for participation in the study. The exclusion criterion was current receipt of help for a mental health problem or in contact with mental health services. The non-clinical group was recruited via public advertising including flyers to local postcodes and adverts on social media and local radio. A brief telephone or email screen was conducted to ensure prospective participants met the inclusion criteria. Questionnaires were completed at an assessment session at the University of Oxford. The study received ethical approval from the University of Oxford Central University Research Ethics Committee (reference R47799/RE001). Written informed consent was received from all participants.

Measures

Body image

Body-Esteem Scale for Adolescents and Adults (BESAA) (Mendelson, Mendelson, & White, Reference Mendelson, Mendelson and White2001). The BESAA is a 23-item self-report scale assessing self-evaluation of appearance. Each item (e.g. ‘I like what I look like in pictures’) is rated on a 0 (never) to 4 (always) scale. Nine items are negatively worded; these items are reverse scored. There are three subscales: ‘appearance’ includes general feelings regarding appearance (10 items) (e.g. ‘I like what I see when I look in the mirror’, ‘I feel ashamed of how I look’), ‘attribution’ includes evaluations attributed to others about one's appearance (five items) (e.g. ‘Other people consider me good looking’) and ‘weight’ relates to satisfaction with weight (eight items) (e.g. ‘I am satisfied with my weight’, ‘My weight makes me unhappy’). Higher scores indicate greater levels of positive body-esteem. In the current study, the internal consistency of the BESAA for the total sample was high for the total scale score (Cronbach's α = 0.94, n = 302) and subscale scores: appearance subscale (Cronbach's α = 0.91, n = 312), attribution subscale (Cronbach's α = 0.80, n  = 307) and weight subscale (Cronbach's α = 0.92, n = 312).

SCOFF Questionnaire – control item (Morgan, Reid, & Lacey, Reference Morgan, Reid and Lacey1999). We used the single loss of control item from the questionnaire: ‘do you worry you have lost control over how much you eat?’. This item requires a dichotomous yes/no response.

Body Mass Index (BMI). This is a value derived from an individual's weight and height. It is calculated by dividing an individual's weight in kilograms by their height in meters. BMI scores can be categorised as underweight (<18.5), normal weight (18.5–24.99), overweight (25–29.99) or obese (>30). The obese range has subcategories: class I (30–34.99), class II (35–39.99) and severe (>40). To calculate BMI, participants' height and weight were measured during the assessment.

Psychotic experiences

Revised Green et al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (RGPTS) (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2019a). This 18-item scale measures paranoid thinking over the past month. Part A assesses ideas of reference (e.g. ‘It was hard to stop thinking about people talking about me behind my back’) and Part B assesses ideas of persecution (e.g. ‘I was convinced there was a conspiracy against me’). Each item is rated on a five-point scale. Higher scores indicate greater levels of paranoia.

Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS) (Bell, Halligan, & Ellis, Reference Bell, Halligan and Ellis2006). Five items assessing voice hearing were used from the scale. Each item (e.g. ‘Hear voices commenting on what you're thinking or doing’, ‘Hear noise or sounds when there is nothing about to explain them’, ‘Hear two or more unexplained voices talking to each other’) is rated on a 0 (not at all) to 5 (daily) scale. Higher scores indicate greater levels of hallucinatory experiences.

The Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (TEPS) (Gard, Gard, Kring, & John, Reference Gard, Gard, Kring and John2006). The 10-item anticipatory pleasure scale was used as a marker of anhedonia. Each item (e.g. ‘When I think about eating my favourite food, I can almost taste how good it is’) is rated on a six-point scale (1, very false for me to 6, very true for me) over the past month. Higher scores indicate lower levels of anticipatory pleasure.

Negative affect and related processes

Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS) (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Freeman, Smith, Kuipers, Bebbington, Bashforth and Garety2006). The BCSS comprises 24 items assessing negative and positive beliefs about the self and others over the past week. Each item is rated on a five-point scale (0–4). Four subscale scores are obtained: negative self (e.g. ‘I am unloved’), positive self (e.g. ‘I am respected’), negative other (e.g. ‘Other people are hostile’), positive other (e.g. ‘Other people are fair’). Higher scores indicate greater endorsement of items.

Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI) (Beck, Steer, & Brown, Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996). The BDI-II is a self-report 21-item scale, with each item rated on a four-point scale (0–3), for the assessment of depression over the past fortnight. Higher scores indicate higher levels of depression.

Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C-SSRS) (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Brown, Stanley, Brent, Yershova, Oquendo and Mann2011). The C-SSRS is an interviewer-rated measure assessing suicidal ideation and behaviour over the past month. We have used the single severity of suicidal ideation scale in our analyses, this includes a single rating from 0 (not at all) to 5 (suicidal intent with plan). Higher scores indicate more severe suicidal ideation.

Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ) (Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990). This 16-item scale measures trait worry over the past fortnight. Each item is rated on a five-point scale. Higher scores indicate a greater tendency to worry.

Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) (Bastien, Vallières, & Morin, Reference Bastien, Vallières and Morin2001). The ISI is a seven-item self-report questionnaire assessing insomnia symptoms over the past fortnight. Each item is rated on a 0–4 scale. Higher scores indicate the presence of symptoms of insomnia.

Safety Behaviours Questionnaire – Persecutory Delusions (SBQ) (Freeman, Garety, & Kuipers, Reference Freeman, Garety and Kuipers2001). The SBQ is a semi-structured interview assessing safety behaviours used in the last month. An action is deemed a safety behaviour if the interviewee reports that it has been carried out with the intention of reducing persecutory threat. A distinction is made between avoidance of situations and in-situation safety behaviours (e.g. not making eye contact). The number and frequency of safety behaviours are calculated to produce a total score. Frequency is rated on a four-point scale from 1 (behaviour definitely occurred on at least one occasion) to 4 (present more or less continuously/at least every day). Higher scores indicate a greater number and frequency of safety behaviours.

Quality of life

Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS) (Tennant et al., Reference Tennant, Hiller, Fishwick, Platt, Joseph, Weich and Stewart-Brown2007). The WEMWBS is a 14-item scale assessing well-being over the past fortnight. Each item is rated on a 1 (none of the time) to 5 (all of the time) scale, and therefore the total score can range from 14 to 70, with higher scores indicating a greater level of well-being.

EuroQoL (EQ-5D-5) (Herdman et al., Reference Herdman, Gudex, Lloyd, Janssen, Kind, Parkin and Badia2011). We used two scores from the questionnaire: quality of life and overall health. Quality of life is reported using the EQ-5D crosswalk index value. This is a dimensional score between 0 and 1 calculated using EuroQol index data for five items each scored 1–5. Higher scores indicate better quality of life. For overall health, a rating scale (0–100) is used to indicate ‘how good is your health today’. Higher scores indicate better overall health.

Time budget (TB) (Jolley et al., Reference Jolley, Garety, Ellett, Kuipers, Freeman, Bebbington and Dunn2006). The time budget, completed during a structured interview, assesses meaningful activity levels over the past week, with four time-blocks for each day rated from 0 to 4. The rating scale is: 0 = nothing, 1 = predominantly passive activity, 2 = an independent activity requiring some planning and motivation, 3 = several two-rated activities completely filling a time period or a more complex and demanding, but shorter, activity, 4 = time period filled with a variety of demanding independent activities. Higher scores indicate higher levels of meaningful activity.

Analyses

To test group differences on the body image measure, t tests were employed. A Cohen's d calculation was used to determine effect sizes. Associations between body image and the symptom and psychological variables within the clinical group were tested using Pearson correlations. To test differences between gender and BMI categories, t tests, χ2 tests and ANOVAs were conducted. All statistical testing was two-tailed and carried out with SPSS Version 27.0 (IBM, 2020).

Results

Demographic and clinical information

Clinical group

The average age of the patients was 41.8 years (s.d. = 11.8; range 17–62). There were slightly more men (n = 69, 60%) than women (n = 46, 40%). The ethnicities were: White (n = 98, 85.2%), Black Caribbean (n = 7, 6.1%), Indian (n = 3, 2.6%), Pakistani (n = 2, 2.6%), Black African (n = 2, 1.7%), Chinese (n = 1, 0.9%) and other (n = 1, 0.9%). Most participants in the clinical group were single (n = 81, 70.4%), with others married or in a civil partnership (n = 22, 19.1%), co-habiting (n = 2, 1.7%) or divorced (n = 10, 8.7%). The majority were unemployed (n = 91, 79%). Three-quarters (74.1%) of patients had a BMI in the overweight (n = 31, 27.7%) or obese (n = 52, 46.4%) range. No patients had a BMI in the underweight range. The clinical diagnoses were schizophrenia (n = 71, 61.7%), schizoaffective disorder (n = 21, 18.3%), delusional disorder (n = 3, 2.6%) and psychosis NOS (n = 20, 17.4%). All but four of the patients (n = 111, 96.5%) were currently prescribed anti-psychotic medication. The mean antipsychotic defined daily dose was 1.50 (s.d. = 0.79) and the mean chlorpromazine equivalent dose was 473.97 (s.d. = 396.04). Almost all participants were outpatients (n = 111, 96.5%) at the time of participation. Table 1 reports the mean scores for all clinical measures.

Table 1. Mean scores on the measures in the clinical group

Non-clinical control group

The general population group comprised 100 men and 100 women. The mean age was 39.2 years (s.d. = 13.4; range 18–76). The mean BMI was 26.7 (s.d. = 5.80, range 15–60.5). The majority of participants (58.5%) had a BMI in the overweight (n = 72, 36%) or obese (n = 45, 23%) range. Seventy-eight participants (39%) had a BMI in the healthy weight range, and only four participants (2%) had a BMI in the underweight range. There was missing BMI data for one participant. The reported ethnicities were: White (n = 186, 93%), Asian (n = 7, 3.5%), Black African (n = 1, 0.5%) and other (n = 6, 3.0%). The majority of participants (n = 166, 83%) were working either full-time (n = 99, 49.5%), part-time (n = 36, 18%), or self-employed (n = 31, 15.5%). With others in full-time education (n = 18, 9%), unemployed (n = 9, 4.5%), retired (n = 5, 2.5%) or a househusband/housewife (n = 2, 1%). Most participants in the general population group were married or in a civil partnership (n = 78, 39%), single (n = 64, 32%) or co-habiting (n = 38, 19%), with others divorced or widowed (n = 20, 10%).

There was no statistically significant difference in the mean age of the two groups: [t(312) = −1.73, p = 0.84]. There was a non-significant higher proportion of men in the clinical group (n = 69, 60%) than the non-clinical group (n = 100, 50%), χ(1) = 2.936, p = 0.087. The clinical group had a higher mean BMI [t(309) = 4.86, p < 0.001], with a mean difference of 3.58 (CI 2.13–5.03). There was a significant difference between the groups for employment status, with significantly fewer participants in the clinical group in employment [χ(1) = 149.561, p < 0.001].

Body image in patients with psychosis

Table 2 summarises the group scores on the body esteem scale and subscales. In comparison to the control group, levels of body esteem were low in patients with psychosis. Differences were seen on all three subscales, with patients reporting lower body esteem in relation to appearance, weight and attribution than participants in the non-clinical control group. There were large effect size differences in total score, and on the appearance and attribution subscales, with a medium effect size on the weight subscale. These differences remained, though with slightly reduced effect sizes, when comparing a subsample of participants with a BMI in the overweight category from the patient group (n = 52) and non-clinical group (n = 117), for the total body esteem score [t(167) = −7.723, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.66, CI −1.640 to −0.931], and the appearance [t(165) = −6.400, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.77, CI −1.431 to −0.729], weight [t(165) = −5.574, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.74, CI −1.87 to −0.594] and attribution [t(163) = −6.630, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.71, CI −1.493 to −0.777] subscales.

Table 2. Comparison of body esteem in clinical and non-clinical samples: means, standard deviations, t values, confidence intervals, p values and effect sizes for body esteem

The frequency of endorsement for each scale item, for both the patient and non-clinical participants, is shown in Table 3. The patterns of endorsement indicate clear differences between the two groups, for example, on item 17, ‘I feel ashamed of how I look’ around 80% of participants in the non-clinical group endorse this item never or seldom, whereas the reverse pattern is seen in the patient group, with most (around 70%) endorsing this item sometimes, often or always. There are also distinct differences in particular items which indicate the impact of body image concerns, for example, item 13 ‘My looks upset me’ highlights the emotional toll of body image concerns for patients. Three-quarters of participants in the non-clinical group endorse this item never or seldom. In contrast, three-quarters of participants in the clinical group endorse this item sometimes, often or always.

Table 3. Item endorsement (n, %) on the BESAA

*Negative items, which must be recoded for scoring by reversing the scale (i.e. 0 = 4, 1 = 3, 2 = 2, 3 = 1, 4 = 0). Three subscales: appearance (1, 6, 7*, 9*, 11*, 13*, 15, 17*, 21*, 23); weight (3, 4*, 8, 10, 16, 18*, 19*, 22); and attribution (2, 5, 12, 14, 20).

In comparison to other published studies using the BESAA, the patients with psychosis scored lower on the total and subscale means in contrast to: studies with student (Mendelson, Mendelson, & Andrews, Reference Mendelson, Mendelson and Andrews2000), middle-aged (McLaren & Kuh, Reference McLaren and Kuh2004) and young adolescent (Cragun, DeBate, Ata, & Thompson, Reference Cragun, DeBate, Ata and Thompson2013) participants, studies comparing sex differences in body esteem (Brennan, Lalonde, & Bain, Reference Brennan, Lalonde and Bain2010), and studies conducted in different countries [e.g. Sweden (Ivarsson, Svalander, Litlere, & Nevonen, Reference Ivarsson, Svalander, Litlere and Nevonen2006) or Korea (Jun & Choi, Reference Jun and Choi2014)]. A table of comparative studies to allow benchmarking is provided in the online Supplementary materials (online Supplementary Table S1).

Gender

In the patient group, there were significant differences between male (M = 34.25, s.d. = 15.73) and female (M = 24.08, s.d. = 15.11) participants in total body esteem [t(113) = 3.45, p = 0.001] and for the appearance [t(110) = 3.37, p = 0.001] and weight [t(110) = 4.04, p < 0.001] subscales, but not the attribution subscale [t(105) = 0.45, p = 0.66]. In contrast, there were no significant differences between male (M = 50.45, s.d. = 14.66) and female (M = 48.05, s.d. = 16.35) participants in total body esteem [t(198) = 1.094, p = 0.275] or on the appearance [t(198) = 1.272, p = 0.205], weight [t(198) = 1.906, p = 0.058] and attribution [t(198) = −1.543, p = 0.275] subscales in the non-clinical group. This is distinct from previous findings using the BESAA in which gender differences are commonly observed (Cragun et al., Reference Cragun, DeBate, Ata and Thompson2013).

BMI categories

There were significant differences between BMI categories in the level of body esteem in both the clinical [F(2,109) = 8.199, p < 0.001] and non-clinical [F (3,195) = 7.407, p < 0.001] groups. A Tukey post hoc test revealed that in the clinical group, levels of body esteem were statistically significantly lower in the overweight (1.29 ± 0.69, p = 0.022) and obese (1.13 ± 0.64, p < 0.001) BMI groups than the normal weight (1.74 ± 0.65) category. There was no statistically significant difference between the overweight and obese BMI category groups (p = 0.552). Similarly, in the non-clinical group, a Tukey post hoc test revealed that levels of body esteem were statistically significantly lower in the overweight (2.06 ± 0.72, p = 0.010) and obese (1.86 ± 0.57, p < 0.001) BMI groups than the normal weight (2.40 ± 0.57) category. There were no statistically significant differences between the underweight BMI category with any other category, nor between the overweight and obese BMI category groups (p = 0.345). Table 4 reports the total mean scores on the BESAA by BMI category (including obese subcategories) for the clinical group.

Table 4. Mean scores on the BESAA by BMI category in the clinical group

Eating habits

Fifty-eight (51.8%) patients reported a loss of control over eating. There were significant differences in body esteem [t(110) = 4.30, p < 0.001] between those reporting a loss of control over eating (M  = 1.06, s.d. = 0.66) and those who did not (M = 1.59, s.d. = 0.64). Loss of control was more commonly reported by patients with a BMI in the obese category than the normal weight category: χ(1) = 5.567, p = 0.018. However, there was no significant difference in loss of control over eating between the overweight and obese [χ(1) = 1.791, p = 0.181] or normal and overweight [χ(1) = 0.907, p = 0.341] BMI categories.

Correlates of body esteem

Table 5 reports the correlations between body esteem and the symptom variables and psychological constructs.

Table 5. Correlates of body image in patients with persecutory delusions

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Body esteem had large negative associations with depression and negative self-beliefs (low self-esteem). There were medium effect size negative correlations between body esteem and worry, safety-seeking behaviours, anhedonia, suicidal ideation and insomnia. There were significant positive correlations of a medium effect size between body esteem and wellbeing, positive beliefs about the self (self-esteem) and overall health status. There were small effect size associations between body esteem and psychotic experiences: ideas of reference, persecutory ideation and hallucinations. All associations with psychotic experiences remained significant after controlling for antipsychotic medication dose (defined daily dose and chlorpromazine equivalent). No correlation (r < 0.1) was found between body esteem and meaningful activity as measured on the time budget. However, a small effect size association was found with quality of life as measured on the EQ5D.

Discussion

Body image concerns are common in patients experiencing current persecutory delusions. They are associated with depression, negative self-views, suicidal ideation, worry, anhedonia, insomnia and excess weight. As might be expected, body image concerns were most strongly associated with depression and negative self-concept. Of particular interest are the significant associations with psychotic experiences: we found that body image concerns were associated with higher levels of paranoia and voices. This fits with our view that body image concerns may feed into the vulnerability underlying paranoia and into the content of voices. Body image concerns may be a meaningful treatment target in patients with persecutory delusions.

Levels of body esteem were low in patients with psychosis compared to data reported in studies from the general population with participants across a range of age, ethnicity and locations (Brennan et al., Reference Brennan, Lalonde and Bain2010; Cragun et al., Reference Cragun, DeBate, Ata and Thompson2013; Ivarsson et al., Reference Ivarsson, Svalander, Litlere and Nevonen2006; Jun & Choi, Reference Jun and Choi2014; McLaren & Kuh, Reference McLaren and Kuh2004; Mendelson et al., Reference Mendelson, Mendelson and Andrews2000).

In our study, there were clear gender differences in the clinical group, with women reporting lower body esteem. This is a common pattern in studies of body image, yet it may reflect a measurement issue. The items on the body esteem scale do not include muscular strength or build, features often considered important in male body image (Cragun et al., Reference Cragun, DeBate, Ata and Thompson2013). Body esteem was lower in those with a BMI in the overweight or obese categories. It was notable in the current study that body image concerns were higher in patients with psychosis even compared to non-clinical individuals in the same BMI categories. Given the elevated rates of obesity in patients with psychosis (Annamalai et al., Reference Annamalai, Kosir and Tek2017), tackling excess weight may be important for both physical and mental health (Firth et al., Reference Firth, Siddiqi, Koyanagi, Siskind, Rosenbaum, Galletly and Stubbs2019). Indeed, body image concerns differ between patients with and without metabolic syndrome (de Hert et al., Reference de Hert, Peuskens, van Winkel, Kalnicka, Hanssens, van Eyck and Peuskens2006). Yet the potential contribution of poor physical health to mental health problems has, to date, been overlooked. The patterns of endorsement on the individual scale items highlight differences between patients and the non-clinical control group in specific cognitions about appearance as well as the emotional impact of appearance concerns (e.g. ‘I feel ashamed of how I look’, ‘My looks upset me’). These cognitions and emotional responses could be treatment targets within a cognitive therapy intervention.

In this study, we found that in patients with psychosis, as seen in the general population (Waite & Freeman, Reference Waite and Freeman2017), body image concerns were associated with paranoia. Consistent with qualitative studies of patient accounts (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Freeman and Waite2019; Waite et al., Reference Waite, Langman, Mulhall, Glogowska, Hartmann‐Boyce, Aveyard and Freeman2022), we found that body image concerns were also associated with negative self-concept, suicidal ideation and hearing voices. Preliminary research indicates that body image concerns are a common feature in the content of voices (Waite et al., Reference Waite, Diamond, Collett, Chadwick, Bold, Teale and Freeman2019). In a study with 60 patients with psychosis, 90% reported hearing voices comment negatively on their appearance, and 50% heard negative comments about appearance on a daily basis. The most commonly endorsed item was ‘the voices tell me that I am ugly’. Whilst concerns about appearance might lead to additional safety-seeking or defence behaviours, also reported in the general population (McLaren & Kuh, Reference McLaren and Kuh2004), in the current study, the relationship with functioning was less clear. These findings indicate multiple routes through which appearance concerns may contribute to the occurrence of psychotic experiences. For example, within the threat-anticipation model of persecutory beliefs (Freeman, Reference Freeman2016), concerns about appearance may be a feature of negative self-concept, a source of worry or a cause for employing safety-seeking or defence behaviours, each of which contribute to the occurrence of threat beliefs. As body image concerns are associated with psychotic experiences, as well as putative psychological causal mechanisms including low self-esteem, depression and worry (Freeman, Reference Freeman2016), it may provide a novel target for treatment in patients with psychosis.

Limitations

There are a number of limitations to the current study. Firstly, it is unknown how representative the current patient participants are of the wider population of patients with non-affective psychosis. The non-clinical control group was recruited via local advertising and will not have been representative of the general population, nor was this group matched to the patient group. Secondly, in this exploratory study, with many conceptually-related variables, there was no correction for multiple testing, which raises the likelihood of Type 1 errors (false positives). Thirdly, measures of body image are commonly developed and validated with students, with high proportions of female respondents under the age of 25 years. This may result in a degree of measurement error, for example, potentially underestimating body image concerns in male participants. Reviews of body image measures, both self-report and those using other methods such as figural drawings, highlight limitations in cultural sensitivity and use across different populations (Gardner & Brown, Reference Gardner and Brown2010; Kling et al., Reference Kling, Kwakkenbos, Diedrichs, Rumsey, Frisén, Brandão and Fitzgerald2019). This is particularly important given the intersection of body image with gender, culture, sexuality and other factors. The only questionnaire validated in this patient group (Awad & Voruganti, Reference Awad and Voruganti2004) focuses on body weight rather than the multidimensional construct of body image (Arbour-Nicitopoulos, Faulkner, & Cohn, Reference Arbour-Nicitopoulos, Faulkner and Cohn2010). Although BMI was reported, other measures of physical health such as metabolic parameters or waist circumference were not included in this study. Finally, given the cross-sectional design of the study, it is not possible to determine directions of effect or the potential impact of confounding variables. Interventionist-causal designs (Kendler & Campbell, Reference Kendler and Campbell2009) are now required to determine the effect of improving body image on mental and physical health.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722000800

Acknowledgements

This study was developed in collaboration with the Oxford Cognitive Approaches to Psychosis Patient Advisory Group facilitated by the McPin Foundation.

Author contributions

FW and DF designed the study. RD, NC, EB and EC completed data collection. FW undertook the statistical analysis and drafted a first version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Financial support

The study was funded by an NIHR Research Professorship awarded to DF (NIHR-RP-2014-05-003). It was also supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre (BRC-1215-20005). DF is an NIHR Senior Investigator. This paper presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. FW is funded by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Doctoral Fellowship (102176/B/13/Z).

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Annamalai, A., Kosir, U., & Tek, C. (2017). Prevalence of obesity and diabetes in patients with schizophrenia. World Journal of Diabetes, 8(8), 390. https://doi.org/10.4239/wjd.v8.i8.390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbour-Nicitopoulos, K. P., Faulkner, G. E., & Cohn, T. A. (2010). Body image in individuals with schizophrenia: Examination of the B-WISE® questionnaire. Schizophrenia Research, 118, 307308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.01.034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Awad, A. G., & Voruganti, L. N. P. (2004). Body weight, image and self-esteem evaluation questionnaire: Development and validation of a new scale. Schizophrenia Research, 70(1), 6367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2(4), 297307. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11438246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI-II Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bell, V., Halligan, P. W., & Ellis, H. D. (2006). The Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS): A new validated measure of anomalous perceptual experience. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 366377. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennan, M. A., Lalonde, C. E., & Bain, J. L. (2010). Body image perceptions: Do gender differences exist? Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 15(3), 130138. https://doi.org/10.24839/1089-4136.JN15.3.130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cragun, D., DeBate, R. D., Ata, R. N., & Thompson, J. K. (2013). Psychometric properties of the Body Esteem Scale for Adolescents and Adults in an early adolescent sample. Eating and Weight Disorders – Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 18(3), 275282. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-013-0031-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Hert, M., Peuskens, B., van Winkel, R., Kalnicka, D., Hanssens, L., van Eyck, D., … Peuskens, J. (2006). Body weight and self-esteem in patients with schizophrenia evaluated with B-WISE®. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1–3), 222226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firth, J., Siddiqi, N., Koyanagi, A., Siskind, D., Rosenbaum, S., Galletly, C., … Stubbs, B. (2019). The Lancet Psychiatry Commission: A blueprint for protecting physical health in people with mental illness. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(8), 675712. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30132-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, D., Freeman, D., Smith, B., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., Bashforth, H., … Garety, P. (2006). The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS): Psychometric properties and associations with paranoia and grandiosity in non-clinical and psychosis samples. Psychological Medicine, 36, 749759. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706007355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D. (2016). Persecutory delusions: A cognitive perspective on understanding and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(7), 685692. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00066-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Emsley, R., Diamond, R., Collett, N., Bold, E., Chadwick, E., … Waite, F. (2021). Comparison of a theoretically driven cognitive therapy (the Feeling Safe Programme) with befriending for the treatment of persistent persecutory delusions: A parallel, single-blind, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 8(8), 696707. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00158-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., & Garety, P. A. (2000). Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: Does the definition need clarification? The British Journal of Clinical Psychology/the British Psychological Society, 39(Pt 4), 407414. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466500163400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., & Kuipers, E. (2001). Persecutory delusions: Developing the understanding of belief maintenance and emotional distress. Psychological Medicine, 31(7), 12931306. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329170100455X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Loe, B. S., Kingdon, D., Startup, H., Molodynski, A., Rosebrock, L., … Bird, J. C. (2019a). The revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS): Psychometric properties, severity ranges, and clinical cut-offs. Psychological Medicine, 51, 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719003155.Google Scholar
Freeman, D., Taylor, K. M., Molodynski, A., & Waite, F. (2019b). Treatable clinical intervention targets for patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 211, 4450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.07.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Kring, A. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 10861102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. M., & Brown, D. L. (2010). Body image assessment: A review of figural drawing scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 107111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haracz, K., Hazelton, M., & James, C. (2018). The “double whammy”: Women's experiences of weight gain after diagnosis and treatment for schizophrenia spectrum disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 206(5), 303309. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herdman, M., Gudex, C., Lloyd, A., Janssen, M. F., Kind, P., Parkin, D., … Badia, X. (2011). Development and preliminary testing of the new five-level version of EQ-5D (EQ-5D-5L). Quality of Life Research, 20(10), 17271736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-011-9903-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IBM. (2020). SPSS Statistics Version 27. Release 27.0.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corporation.Google Scholar
Ivarsson, T., Svalander, P., Litlere, O., & Nevonen, L. (2006). Weight concerns, body image, depression and anxiety in Swedish adolescents. Eating Behaviors, 7(2), 161175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.08.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolley, S., Garety, P. A., Ellett, L., Kuipers, E., Freeman, D., Bebbington, P. E., … Dunn, G. (2006). A validation of a new measure of activity in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 85(1–3), 288295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jun, E. M., & Choi, S. B. (2014). Obesity, body image, depression, and weight-control behaviour among female university students in Korea. Journal of Cancer Prevention, 19(3), 240246. https://doi.org/10.15430/JCP.2014.19.3.240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Campbell, J. (2009). Interventionist causal models in psychiatry: Repositioning the mind-body problem. Psychological Medicine, 39(6), 881887. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708004467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kling, J., Kwakkenbos, L., Diedrichs, P. C., Rumsey, N., Frisén, A., Brandão, M. P., … Fitzgerald, A. (2019). Systematic review of body image measures. Body Image, 30, 170211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.06.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, E., Freeman, D., & Waite, F. (2019). The experience of body image concerns in patients with persecutory delusions: ‘People don't want to sit next to me’. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 93, 639655. https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mccloughen, A., & Foster, K. (2011). Weight gain associated with taking psychotropic medication: An integrative review. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 20(3), 202222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1447-0349.2010.00721.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaren, L., & Kuh, D. (2004). Body dissatisfaction in midlife women. Journal of Women & Aging, 16(1–2), 3554. https://doi.org/10.1300/J074v16n01_04.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendelson, B. K., Mendelson, M. J., & White, D. R. (2001). Body-esteem scale for adolescents and adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 76, 90106. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7601_6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendelson, M. J., Mendelson, B. K., & Andrews, J. (2000). Body-mass in late adolescence: Is a competence ϫ importance model needed? Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 249266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487495. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, J. F., Reid, F., & Lacey, J. H. (1999). The SCOFF questionnaire: Assessment of a new screening tool for eating disorders. BMJ, 319(7223), 14671468. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.319.7223.1467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillinger, T., McCutcheon, R. A., Vano, L., Mizuno, Y., Arumuham, A., Hindley, G., … Howes, O. D. (2020). Comparative effects of 18 antipsychotics on metabolic function in patients with schizophrenia, predictors of metabolic dysregulation, and association with psychopathology: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(1), 6477. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30416-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, K., Brown, G. K., Stanley, B., Brent, D. A., Yershova, K. V., Oquendo, M. A., … Mann, J. J. (2011). The Columbia-suicide severity rating scale: Initial validity and internal consistency findings from three multisite studies with adolescents and adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(12), 12661277. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennant, R., Hiller, L., Fishwick, R., Platt, S., Joseph, S., Weich, S., … Stewart-Brown, S. (2007). The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): Development and UK validation. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 5, 63. https://doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-5-63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waite, F., Diamond, R., Collett, N., Chadwick, E., Bold, E., Teale, A.-L., … Freeman, D. (2019). The comments of voices on the appearance of patients with psychosis: ‘the voices tell me that I am ugly’. BJPsych Open, 5(5), e86. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2019.66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waite, F., & Freeman, D. (2017). Body image and paranoia. Psychiatry Research, 258, 136140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.10.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waite, F., Langman, A., Mulhall, S., Glogowska, M., Hartmann‐Boyce, J., Aveyard, P., … Freeman, D. (2022). The psychological journey of weight gain in psychosis. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/papt.12386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean scores on the measures in the clinical group

Figure 1

Table 2. Comparison of body esteem in clinical and non-clinical samples: means, standard deviations, t values, confidence intervals, p values and effect sizes for body esteem

Figure 2

Table 3. Item endorsement (n, %) on the BESAA

Figure 3

Table 4. Mean scores on the BESAA by BMI category in the clinical group

Figure 4

Table 5. Correlates of body image in patients with persecutory delusions

Supplementary material: File

Waite et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Waite et al. supplementary material(File)
File 31.6 KB