Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T14:04:19.951Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional neglect and parents’ adverse childhood events

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2023

Laura Ylitervo*
Affiliation:
Research Unit of Clinical Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Centre Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland
Juha Veijola
Affiliation:
Research Unit of Clinical Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Centre Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland
Anu-Helmi Halt
Affiliation:
Research Unit of Clinical Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Medical Research Centre Oulu, Oulu University Hospital and University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Department of Psychiatry, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland
*
Corresponding author: Laura Ylitervo; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Introduction

Emotional neglect means that the child’s emotional and developmental needs are not fulfilled by the parents or other caregivers. Adverse childhood events (ACEs) are a risk factor for mental health problems and impaired parenting skills. The objective here was to examine whether parents’ ACEs increase the child’s risk of experiencing emotional neglect.

Methods

The participants in the present study were members of the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 (NFBC1986). Emotional neglect experiences were measured in 190 members of this cohort by means of the Trauma and Distress Scale (TADS), and ACEs in both parents were measured with a specific questionnaire. A linear regression model was used to examine the association between parents’ ACEs and the children’s emotional neglect scores.

Results

The children’s mean emotional neglect score was 8.11 on a scale from 5 to 25. There was no significant difference between males (mean 8.01) and females (mean 8.19). Only father’s ACEs were associated with child’s emotional neglect score. In the linear regression model, the children’s emotional neglect scores increased by 0.3 points for father’s ACE.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that father’s ACEs may increase the child’s risk of experiencing emotional neglect. It seems that childhood adversities are transferred from parents to children, but larger samples would be needed to confirm these findings.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Emotional neglect means that the child’s emotional and developmental needs are continuously ignored by the parents, so that the experiencing of emotional neglect may be said to be associated with the type of the relationship between the child and his or her caregivers. Probably, one of the most widely recognized models for these relationships is Bowlby’s attachment theory, first introduced in 1958 [Reference Bowlby1]. According to this theory, the type of child–parent relationship can be determined by examining how the child reacts in stressful situations with respect to the caregiver. This again depends on how the child views the caregivers: that is, whether they are able to provide the nurture and support that the child needs. Emotional neglect comes into the picture when the caregiver cannot fulfill the child’s basic emotional needs, because of impaired parenting skills and responsiveness, possibly due to mental health problems [Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2] or substance use [Reference Meulewaeter, De Pauw and Vanderplasschen3], for example. Emotional neglect may lead to an insecure or anxious attachment style, which in turn could increase the risk of anxiety and other mental health problems [Reference Schimmenti and Bifulco4].

Emotional neglect is one type of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs), but there is a lack of research on its prevalence in general population. For example, Stoltenborgh et al. (2013) concluded a meta-analysis on emotional neglect, and they pointed out that the 13 suitable studies they found about emotional neglect is very little compared to other ACE domains, such as sexual abuse [Reference Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn5]. Mathews et al. (2020) found 23 studies in their meta-analysis, which still falls short in comparison to other ACEs [Reference Mathews, Pacella, Dunne, Simunovic and Marston6]. There is no consistent and established questionnaire to measure emotional neglect, which leads to difficulties comparing prevalence numbers across studies.

It seems that childhood adversities are transferred from parents to children at least in some form. ACEs may influence mental health even at a young age, but also in adulthood [Reference Chapman, Whitfield, Felitti, Dube, Edwards and Anda7Reference Turner and Butler9]. Emotional and physical neglect and abuse experienced in the childhood are risk factors for depression and anxiety [Reference Moffitt, Caspi, Harrington, Milne, Melchior and Goldberg10, Reference Salokangas, Schultze-Lutter, Schmidt, Pesonen, Luutonen and Patterson11] and for lifelong alcohol dependence [Reference Pilowsky, Keyes and Hasin12]. Taken together, if parents have experienced ACEs, they will have a higher risk of mental health problems, and since having a depressed parent, for example, is considered an adverse childhood event, the child of such a parent will be at risk, leading to a vicious cycle in which adversities can be passed on for generations.

Another example of how this cycle may be initiated is post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Muzik et al. found that mothers with PTSD have a higher risk for impaired bonding with their children [Reference Muzik, Bocknek, Broderick, Richardson, Rosenblum and Thelen13]. According to previous literature, parents with PTSD are more prone to aggressive or hostile behavior toward their children [Reference Christie, Hamilton-Giachritsis, Alves-Costa, Tomlinson and Halligan14, Reference van Ee, Kleber and Jongmans15], are emotionally less available [Reference van Ee, Kleber and Jongmans15], perceive their child more negatively [Reference van Ee, Kleber and Jongmans15] and are less satisfied with their parenting [Reference Christie, Hamilton-Giachritsis, Alves-Costa, Tomlinson and Halligan14, Reference van Ee, Kleber and Jongmans15]. Children learn emotional regulation from their parents, and parents’ PTSD has been found to be associated with child’s internalizing and externalizing behavior as well as emotional regulation [Reference Samuelson, Wilson, Padrón, Lee and Gavron16, Reference Hartzell, Stenson, van Rooij, Kim, Vance and Hinrichs17]. On the other hand, Greene et al. found that if a parent with PTSD is capable of responsive parenting behavior, the adverse effects could be avoided [Reference Greene, McCarthy, Estabrook, Wakschlag and Briggs-Gowan18].

In addition to other psychiatric conditions, substance use disorders can potentially lead to emotional neglect. Meulewaeter et al. [Reference Meulewaeter, De Pauw and Vanderplasschen3] studied mothers with substance use disorders and how they reflected their own parenting experiences and bonding with their children. It became apparent from the interviews that these mothers had noticed how their substance use had impacted their parenting skills and how they had emotionally neglected their children. They also described having had similar experiences in their own childhood concerning their relations with their mothers.

This brings us back to the cycle of adverse childhood events and the phenomenon of the transfer of traumatic experiences across generations. Leifer et al. [Reference Leifer, Kilbane, Jacobsen and Grossman19] conducted an intergenerational study to explore transmission of the risk of sexual abuse. Their material included three generations, children and their mothers and grandmothers, and they found that the mothers of children who had experienced sexual abuse themselves had a history of childhood abuse and neglect and had more often had conflicting relationships with their own mothers [Reference Leifer, Kilbane, Jacobsen and Grossman19]. Taken together, these previous research findings suggest that if the first-generation mother is traumatized or otherwise has impaired parenting skills, this could cause mental health problems for her children, and furthermore, that if the second-generation parents have mental health problems, then the third generation could also be at risk, and so on.

We examined here a sample of 190 cohort members and their parents to see whether the parents’ ACEs increased the risk of their children experiencing emotional neglect. We hypothesized that the more ACEs the parents had experienced, the more emotional neglect their children would experience.

Methods

Study setting

The NFBC1986 material

This study is based on the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 (NFBC1986), which is a prospective longitudinal research program covering 99% of all births in Northern Finland with their calculated term between July 1, 1985, and June 30, 1986 (n = 9,479, live born 9,432) [20]. Data on the participants have been collected from health registers, questionnaires, and clinical examinations for a period extending from prenatal care to the present day. The NFBC1986 data give a representative sample of the general population in Finland and represent one of the largest birth cohort studies with high genetic and ethnic homogeneity. The design and population of the NFBC1986 are described in full in Järvelin et al. [Reference Järvelin, Hartikainen-Sorri and Rantakallio21].

Population

Out of the 1,396 members of the NFBC1986 cohort invited to participate in this study, 471 (34%) accepted and answered the Trauma and Distress Scale (TADS) questionnaire at the age of around 26 years [Reference Björnholm, Nikkinen, Kiviniemi, Niemelä, Drakesmith and Evans22]. These participants will for clarity be referred to below as “children” even though they were adults at the time of answering the TADS questionnaire.

The participants’ parents were also invited to join the study and to fill in a questionnaire concerning their adverse childhood experiences (ACEs). We then excluded those participants for whom both parents refused to answer the ACEs questionnaire (n = 279, 59%) as well as those who had themselves not answered the TADS questions concerning emotional neglect (n = 2, 0.4%). This gave a total series of 190 children and their parents (i.e., 190 mothers and 190 fathers). Participant selection is illustrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Participant selection.

The participants gave their consent for their parents to be contacted and for their own data to be linked to their parents’ data. Conversely, all the parents consented to their data being linked to their child’s data and to the data of the other biological parent. The protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Northern Ostrobothnia Hospital District.

Instruments

Trauma and Distress Scale

The Trauma and Distress Scale (TADS) questionnaire that was used here to measure childhood adversities is a self-reported instrument developed by Patterson et al. [Reference Patterson, Skeate, Schultze-Lutter, Graf von Reventlow, Wieneke and Ruhrmann23] and translated into Finnish by Salokangas et al. [Reference Salokangas, Schultze-Lutter, Patterson, von Reventlow, Heinimaa and From24]. It consists of 43 items and divides childhood adversities into five domains: emotional neglect, emotional abuse, physical neglect, physical abuse, and sexual abuse.

We focused on the five questions concerning emotional neglect: 5) When I was young, I felt valued or important, 8) My family were emotionally warm and loving, 13) When I was young, my family looked after each other, 21) I respect myself, and 40) My family was supportive and encouraging when I was young. These questions were answered on a scale from 1 to 5, where 1 denoted “never” and 5 “almost always.”

Because of the phrasing of the other questions in TADS, the points from the questions concerning emotional neglect had to be reversed. Thus, the more points a subject scores on these five questions (range 5–25), the more emotional neglect that person experienced in childhood.

Questionnaire for the parents

The parents of the NFBC1986 members were invited to answer a questionnaire concerning their childhood, personality, and substance use that formed part of the Health 2000 study [Reference Pirkola, Isometsä, Aro, Kestilä, Hämäläinen and Veijola25]. Only the questions that outlined the parents’ childhood and relationship with their own parents (i.e., the grandparents of the NFBC1986 members) were considered here. The questionnaire included 13 items related to adverse childhood events, such as being seriously ill, bullied, or assaulted, having a parent with mental illness or problems with alcohol, or divorce of the parents. These questions were answered on a scale from 1 to 3, where 1 denoted “no,” 2 “cannot say,” and 3 “yes.” Only the “yes” answers were accepted for calculating the number of ACEs for each parent. The original questionnaire is available in Supplementary Material.

Confounders

The age and education of the parents and the child’s gender were accepted as confounders in the statistical analysis. The parents’ age was their age at the time when their child was born, as calculated from their dates of birth. Education of the parents and the children was assessed in terms of four levels: 1 = 9 years of school and/or vocational training, 2 = general or vocational upper secondary school, 3 = university of applied sciences, and 4 = university.

Statistical analysis

The children’s emotional neglect scores and the numbers of parents’ ACEs were analyzed to investigate the possible association between emotional neglect and parents’ adverse childhood events. The statistical analysis was conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics. Assumption of independence of all variables was tested and met.

Due to skewed distribution of child’s emotional neglect score, Spearman’s correlation coefficients were used to see which variables were associated with the emotional neglect score. We also used Kruskal–Wallis test to see if the amount of parents’ ACEs had a different effect on child’s emotional neglect score. For this test, parents’ ACEs were divided into four classes: 0 = no ACEs, I = one ACE, II = two or three ACEs, and III = four or more ACEs (a similar categorization to that used by Kananen et al. [Reference Kananen, Surakka, Pirkola, Suvisaari, Lönnqvist and Peltonen26]).

For linear regression model, assumptions of linear association, heteroscedasticity, no multicollinearity and normality of residuals were tested and met. Normality of residuals was tested with Normal Probability Plot.

Results

Basic demographic information concerning the children and their parents is shown in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2. Supplementary Table 1 also shows results of the drop-out analysis, which revealed that the mean emotional neglect score of the excluded participants (n = 279) was higher that included participant’s emotional neglect score (9.23 and 8.11, p = 0.001). Other variables did not significantly differ between the included and excluded participants (see Supplementary Table 1).

The numbers of adverse childhood events reported by the parents are outlined in the Table 1 in four ACE groups. According to these categories, 8.9% of mothers and 5.3% of fathers had four or more ACEs (see Table 1).

Table 1. Parents’ ACEs divided into four classes

Abbreviation: ACEs, adverse childhood events.

We examined children’s emotional neglect scores in these four groups and found an ascending trend (see Figures 2 and 3). The Kruskal–Wallis test found no significant difference between the four classes (p = 0.089 for father and p = 0.109 for mother).

Figure 2. Box-plot figure of the association between mother’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score.

Figure 3. Box-plot figure of the association between father’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score.

We also tested if any of the parents’ individual ACE domains were associated with the child’s emotional neglect score. For mothers, experience of financial challenges of the family (p = 0.008) and divorced parents (p = 0.009) were associated with child’s emotional neglect score. For fathers, experience of mother’s alcohol abuse (p = 0.011) was associated with the child’s emotional neglect score.

Table 2 shows the correlations between child’s emotional neglect score, parents’ ACEs, and the confounders. Spearman’s correlation analysis showed a minimally weak correlation between the number of father’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score (r = 0.150, p = 0.039). Mother’s ACEs were not associated with the child’s emotional neglect score (p = 0.406). Father’s education had a negative correlation to child’s emotional neglect score (r = −0.176, p = 0.017; see Table 2).

Table 2. Correlations between child’s TADS-score, parents’ ACEs, and confounders

Abbreviations: ACEs, adverse childhood events; TADS, Trauma and Distress Scale.

* 1 = boy, 2 = girl.

Bolded = statistically significant p-value

As only father’s ACEs and education level were associated with the child’s emotional neglect score, the linear regression model was conducted only for father. The results of the linear regression model are shown in Table 3. The model is a multilinear analysis with father’s education and age as confounders. This regression model points to a statistically significant correlation between the father’s ACEs and the child’s emotional neglect scores (p = 0.045). In this model, the confounders were not statistically significant. According to the model, child’s predicted emotional neglect score is 6.3 + 0.343 (father’s ACEs) points; in other words, the child’s emotional neglect score increased by 0.343 points for each father’s ACE (see Table 3).

Table 3. Linear regression between father’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score

Abbreviations: ACEs, adverse childhood events

Note: Adj. R 2 0.028.

Discussion

Our findings suggest a correlation between the number of father’s ACEs and the child’s emotional neglect score, which implies that if the father has experienced adverse childhood events their children will have a higher risk of experiencing emotional neglect. The results partially confirm our hypothesis.

The results suggest that the association of parental ACEs to emotional neglect experience in the offspring is small, but detectable. Although the Kruskal–Wallis test did not find a statistically significant difference between the ACEs and emotional neglect scores, the box-plot figure did show a small ascending trend. In addition, the Spearman’s correlation coefficient was minute, yet statistically significant. Only father’s ACEs and father’s education had a statistically significant correlation to child’s emotional neglect score. According to the linear regression model, the child’s emotional neglect score increased 0.3 points for each father’s ACE, which means that, as the emotional neglect score is measured in integers, it would take three ACEs to increase the child’s emotional neglect score by one point. Also, as the scale for the emotional neglect scores was 5–25, this would still be a very small increment. Nevertheless, in the light of these results parental traumas do have some effect on the children.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first investigation into the association between parents’ ACEs and emotional neglect experienced by their children. Although it was focused specifically on ACEs and emotional neglect, its results are consistent with others obtained in slightly different settings. Christie et al. [Reference Christie, Hamilton-Giachritsis, Alves-Costa, Tomlinson and Halligan14] and van Ee et al. [Reference van Ee, Kleber and Jongmans15] found in their review articles that PTSD can affect parenting behavior, which could lead to emotional neglect. Their material, however, represented extreme cases of parents with ACEs, as PTSD is a consequence of an extremely adverse (childhood) event.

One explanatory factor behind the weak correlation between parents’ ACEs and their children’s experience of emotional neglect might be resilience. This is a multidimensional concept which basically refers to a capability for positive adaptation to challenging conditions and recovery from adverse or potentially traumatic events [Reference Fletcher and Sarkar27]. As stated earlier, ACEs are a risk factor for mental health problems such as depression [Reference Chapman, Whitfield, Felitti, Dube, Edwards and Anda7], but even so, not everyone who has experienced ACEs will develop depressive disorders, and this might be because of resilience [Reference Poole, Dobson and Pusch28]. Even though a parent had experienced adverse childhood events, it might still be possible, if they are resilient, to maintain a relatively stable state of mind and thus provide a good childhood for their own children. Resilience could be one way of arresting the transfer of traumas through several generations.

The setting for our study imposed some limitations. It relies on a measure of retrospective recall, as the cohort members were answering the TADS questionnaire at the age of 26 and their parents were doing so at an even greater age. This might cause some bias in the results [Reference Hardt and Rutter29]. Another source of bias may be the current mood of the subject at the moment of recall, as it might affect how that person remembers events such as ACEs that occurred in the past [Reference Eich, Macaulay and Ryan30]. In addition, self-reported data might be influenced by the mindset of the participant. ACEs, especially emotional neglect, are associated with the perception of a negative attitude in others [Reference Salokangas, From, Luutonen and Hietala31], and if we perceive that other people have a negative attitude toward us, we may deprecate our relationships with them. This phenomenon may have had some effect in the present instance.

This study was based on the NFBC1986, which gives a comprehensive sample of the general population in Finland. Although the source population has many strengths, our study population consisted of only 190 participants. Thus, selection bias is possible, and the results might not be generalizable at large. In addition, the distributions of children’s emotional neglect score and parents’ ACEs were quite skewed. Both parents and children had relatively low number of ACEs, as the mean score for the parents’ ACEs was 1.81 (on a scale 0–13) and the children’s mean emotional neglect score was 8.11 (on a scale 5–25). We do not know if this is due to selection bias or is this really the average level of ACEs in the NFBC1986 children and their parents. Of course, a larger population would have minimized the selection bias and provided more reliable results. Testing our hypothesis on a population with higher scores or larger deviation would also add further to the information provided by our results. Although the present work does shed some light on the possibility of parents’ ACEs affecting their children, larger samples would be needed in future to confirm these findings.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2023.2420.

Data availability statement

The NFBC data are available from the University of Oulu, Infrastructure for Population Studies. Permission to use the data can be applied for research purposes via the electronic material request portal. Our use of the data follows the EU general data protection regulation (679/2016) and the Finnish Data Protection Act. Use of the personal data is based on each participant’s written informed consent given for the latest follow-up study, which may lead to limitations on its use. For more information, please contact the NFBC project center () and visit the cohort website (www.oulu.fi/nfbc).

Acknowledgments

We thank all the cohort members, their parents, and the researchers who participated in this study. We also wish to acknowledge the work done by the NFBC project center.

Financial support

Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986 project was supported by the EU QLG1-CT-2000-01643 (EUROBLCS; Grant No. E51560), NorFA (Grant Nos. 731, 20056, and 30167), and USA/NIH 2000 G DF682 (Grant No. 50945). A.-H.H. has received funding from the Terttu Foundation. The sources of funding had no role in the design or conduct of the study, the collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data, the preparation, review or approval of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Competing interest

The authors have no competing interests to disclose with respect to this research or the authorship and/or publication of this article.

References

Bowlby, J. The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. Int J Psychoanal. 1958;39:350–71.Google ScholarPubMed
Goodman, SH, Rouse, MH, Connell, AM, Broth, MR, Hall, CM, Heyward, D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14(1):127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meulewaeter, F, De Pauw, SSW, Vanderplasschen, W. Mothering, substance use disorders and intergenerational trauma transmission: an attachment-based perspective. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmenti, A, Bifulco, A. Linking lack of care in childhood to anxiety disorders in emerging adulthood: the role of attachment styles. Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2015;20:41–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoltenborgh, M, Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, van IJzendoorn, MH. The neglect of child neglect: a meta-analytic review of the prevalence of neglect. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2013;48(3):345–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, B, Pacella, R, Dunne, MP, Simunovic, M, Marston, C. Improving measurement of child abuse and neglect: a systematic review and analysis of national prevalence studies. PloS One. 2020;15(1):e0227884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, DP, Whitfield, CL, Felitti, VJ, Dube, SR, Edwards, VJ, Anda, RF. Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. J Affect Disord. 2004;82(2):217–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, JG, McLaughlin, KA, Berglund, PA, Gruber, MJ, Sampson, NA, Zaslavsky, AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):113–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, HA, Butler, MJ. Direct and indirect effects of childhood adversity on depressive symptoms in young adults. J Youth Adolescence. 2003;32:89103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T, Caspi, A, Harrington, H, Milne, B, Melchior, M, Goldberg, D, et al. Generalized anxiety disorder and depression: childhood risk factors in a birth cohort followed to age 32. Psychol Med. 2007;37(3):441–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salokangas, RKR, Schultze-Lutter, F, Schmidt, SJ, Pesonen, H, Luutonen, S, Patterson, P, et al. Childhood physical abuse and emotional neglect are specifically associated with adult mental disorders. J Ment Health. 2020;20(4):376–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilowsky, DJ, Keyes, KM, Hasin, DS. Adverse childhood events and lifetime alcohol dependence. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(2):258–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muzik, M, Bocknek, EL, Broderick, A, Richardson, P, Rosenblum, KL, Thelen, K, et al. Mother–infant bonding impairment across the first 6 months postpartum: the primacy of psychopathology in women with childhood abuse and neglect histories. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2013;16:2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christie, H, Hamilton-Giachritsis, C, Alves-Costa, F, Tomlinson, M, Halligan, SL. The impact of parental posttraumatic stress disorder on parenting: a systematic review. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2019;10(1):1550345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Ee, E, Kleber, RJ, Jongmans, MJ. Relational patterns between caregivers with PTSD and their nonexposed children: a review. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2016;17(2):186203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuelson, KW, Wilson, CK, Padrón, E, Lee, S, Gavron, L. Maternal PTSD and children’s adjustment: parenting stress and emotional availability as proposed mediators. J Clin Psychol. 2017;73(6):693706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartzell, G, Stenson, AF, van Rooij, SJH, Kim, YJ, Vance, LA, Hinrichs, R, et al. Intergenerational effects of maternal PTSD: roles of parenting stress and child sex. Psychol Trauma. 2022;14(7):1089–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, CA, McCarthy, KJ, Estabrook, R, Wakschlag, LS, Briggs-Gowan, MJ. Responsive parenting buffers the impact of maternal PTSD on young children. Parent Sci Pract. 2020;20(2):141–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leifer, M, Kilbane, T, Jacobsen, T, Grossman, G. A three-generational study of transmission of risk for sexual abuse. J Clin Child Adolesc. 2004;33(4):662–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
University of Oulu. Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1986. University of Oulu. http://urn.fi/urn:nbn:fi:att:f5c10eef-3d25-4bd0-beb8-f2d59df95b8e (accessed June 17, 2023).Google Scholar
Järvelin, MR, Hartikainen-Sorri, AL, Rantakallio, P. Labour induction policy in hospitals of different levels of specialisation. Br J Obstet Gynaecol. 1993;100(4):310–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Björnholm, L, Nikkinen, J, Kiviniemi, V, Niemelä, S, Drakesmith, M, Evans, JC, et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal cigarette smoking and structural properties of the human corpus callosum. Neuroimage. 2020;1(209):116477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, P, Skeate, A, Schultze-Lutter, F, Graf von Reventlow, H, Wieneke, A, Ruhrmann, S, et al. The Trauma and Distress Scale. Birmingham: University of Birmingham; 2002.Google Scholar
Salokangas, RKR, Schultze-Lutter, F, Patterson, P, von Reventlow, HG, Heinimaa, M, From, T, et al. Psychometric properties of the Trauma and Distress Scale, TADS, in an adult community sample in Finland. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2016;7(1):30062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pirkola, S, Isometsä, E, Aro, H, Kestilä, L, Hämäläinen, J, Veijola, J, et al. Childhood adversities as risk factors for adult mental disorders: results from the Health 2000 study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40(10):769–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kananen, L, Surakka, I, Pirkola, S, Suvisaari, J, Lönnqvist, J, Peltonen, L, et al. Childhood adversities are associated with shorter telomere length at adult age both in individuals with an anxiety disorder and controls. PloS One. 2010;5(5):e10826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, D, Sarkar, M. Psychological resilience: a review and critique of definitions, concepts, and theory. European Psychologist. 2013;18(1):1223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poole, JC, Dobson, KS, Pusch, D. Childhood adversity and adult depression: the protective role of psychological resilience. Child Abuse Negl. 2016;64:89100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardt, J, Rutter, M. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:260–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eich, E, Macaulay, D, Ryan, L. Mood dependent memory for events of the personal past. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1994;123(2):201–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salokangas, RKR, From, T, Luutonen, S, Hietala, J. Adverse childhood experiences leads to perceived negative attitude of others and the effect of adverse childhood experiences on depression in adulthood is mediated via negative attitude of others. Eur Psychiatry. 2018;54:2734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Participant selection.

Figure 1

Table 1. Parents’ ACEs divided into four classes

Figure 2

Figure 2. Box-plot figure of the association between mother’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Box-plot figure of the association between father’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score.

Figure 4

Table 2. Correlations between child’s TADS-score, parents’ ACEs, and confounders

Figure 5

Table 3. Linear regression between father’s ACEs and child’s emotional neglect score

Supplementary material: File

Ylitervo et al. supplementary material

Ylitervo et al. supplementary material 1

Download Ylitervo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Ylitervo et al. supplementary material

Ylitervo et al. supplementary material 2

Download Ylitervo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.1 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Ylitervo et al. supplementary material

Ylitervo et al. supplementary material 3

Download Ylitervo et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 66.6 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.