Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T08:37:41.704Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The affective neuroscience of socioeconomic status: implications for mental health

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2020

Yu Hao*
Affiliation:
Center for Neuroscience & Society, University of Pennsylvania, USA
Martha J. Farah
Affiliation:
Center for Neuroscience & Society, University of Pennsylvania, USA
*
Correspondence to Yu Hao ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

We review basic science research on neural mechanisms underlying emotional processing in individuals of differing socioeconomic status (SES). We summarise SES differences in response to positive and negative stimuli in limbic and cortical regions associated with emotion and emotion regulation. We discuss the possible relevance of neuroscience to understanding the link between mental health and SES. We hope to provide insights into future neuroscience research on the etiology and pathophysiology of mental disorders relating to SES.

Type
Special Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2020

Low socioeconomic status (SES) is a potent risk factor for mental disorders, particularly mood and anxiety disorders.Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters1Reference McLaughlin, Costello, Leblanc, Sampson and Kessler3 Life's challenges weigh more heavily on people with fewer financial resources and less education, and the impact of SES on emotional well-being is substantial. For example, Guhn et alReference Guhn, Emerson, Mahdaviani and Gadermann4 found an adjusted odds ratio for mental health conditions 25–39% higher for children of low-income families compared with others. A Canadian government survey5 found that disabling mental health problems are twice as common among those without a high school diploma compared with college graduates.

In this article we review what is known about the affective neuroscience of SES and sketch some of the implications this field of science might have for mental health. We begin with basic definitions and distinctions. ‘Affective neuroscience’ is the study of neural mechanisms underlying the experience, expression and regulation of emotion. Given that the brain is shaped by a combination of genes and environment, explanations in terms of the brain and the environment are not mutually exclusive. SES is a complex construct, or more accurately an interrelated set of constructs, that captures differences in material and social wherewithal. It has been measured in terms of financial attributes (e.g. income, wealth), neighbourhood characteristics (e.g. rates of unemployment and crime near one's home), educational and occupational background and self-reported social standing, among other approaches. Although these measures are all distinct from one another,Reference Braveman, Cubbin, Egerter, Chideya, Marchi and Metzler6 they are normally moderately correlated.Reference Chen and Paterson7 Here we will seek broad generalisations within this relatively small literature, referring therefore to SES in general rather than making distinctions among more specific measures. Similarly, we will generalise across life stages at which participants’ SES was measured. All of these distinctions undoubtedly have scientific and clinical relevance, which should also be examined. Nevertheless, for a preliminary review, we choose to begin by aggregating findings as much as possible.

Many useful insights into the aetiology and pathophysiology of mental disorders have come by studying the brain. Here we explore the possible relevance of neuroscience to understanding the link between mental health and SES. Recent attempts to link SES and psychopathology through the brain have adopted different concepts from psychology and neuroscience as explanatory frameworks. These include the effects of stress on the brain,Reference Kim, Evans, Chen, Miller, Seeman, Halfon, Forrest, Lerner and Faustman8 the role of self-regulation in mental healthReference Palacios-Barrios and Hanson9 and the distinction between adversities consisting primarily of deprivation (prevalent in low SES) and those consisting primarily of threat (such as accompanies abuseReference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert10). These frameworks are not mutually exclusive, and the goal of our review is neither to adjudicate between them nor to develop a fourth alternative. Rather, we aim to gather the most comprehensive collection to date of research findings related to SES, brain and affect, and attempt a provisional integration that will enable empirical generalisations and highlight consistencies and inconsistencies.

The neuroscience studies reviewed here are basic science research, carried out with normal people of varying SES. Note that in some studies SES is not the focus of the study but included as a covariate. In some of these cases the range of SES is restricted and the power to detect SES effects is therefore attenuated. Figure 1 depicts the systematic review process.

Fig. 1 Flowchart for the systematic review. Relevant studies were identified through searches of the database PubMed throughout 13 March 2020. The search required that studies used at least one of the following socioeconomic status (SES) keywords in the full paper: socioeconomic status, poverty, income, neighbourhood quality, neighbourhood disadvantage, educational attainment, social class, social standing. Identified studies also used at least one of the following social and affect-related keywords in the entire paper: emotion, threat, fear, angry, sad, happy, reward, social interaction, hostility, rejection. In addition, the keyword of neuroimaging method was included: fMRI. This search identified 166 results, which were screened for the inclusion criteria.

The resulting functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies manipulated emotional states experimentally inside the scanner with emotionally evocative stimuli or tasks. The most common methods involve photographs of faces expressing different emotions or of affectively valenced objects and scenes, and signals that money has been won or lost in a game. Less frequently, studies have used other kinds of stimuli or experiences to evoke emotion and these will also be described.

Subcortical and cortical bases of negative affect

Textbook coverage of affective neuroscience invariably begins with subcortical systems, particularly the amygdala and ventral striatum. These appear to respond automatically, without the need for conscious awareness of the emotion (e.g.Reference Whalen, Bush, McNally, Wilhelm, McInerney and Jenike11,Reference Pessiglione, Petrovic, Daunizeau, Palminteri, Dolan and Frith12 ), and play crucial roles in the experience and expression of emotion. Many other regions, cortical and subcortical, also participate in emotion and there is no simple mapping of specific emotions to specific brain areas.Reference Lindquist, Wager, Kober, Bliss-Moreau and Barrett13 Emotion is best thought of as a construction of the brain as a whole.Reference Barrett14 Nevertheless, many studies take the amygdala as a starting point in the investigation of emotion and the brain, especially negative emotion.

Brain responses to negative facial emotions

Although the amygdala plays a role in a range of emotions, its most consistent role is in negative emotions. The amygdala is most readily activated by viewing facial expressions of fear and anger. Our review identified a substantial number of studies that have compared low- and high-SES individuals under these experimental conditions. In the majority of studies, lower SES by at least one measure was associated with stronger amygdala reactivity to negative facial emotions. Young adults whose parents were of lower SES showed greater amygdala activation when viewing angry faces compared with control conditions in two studies.Reference Gianaros, Horenstein, Hariri, Sheu, Manuck and Matthews15,Reference Muscatell, Morelli, Falk, Way, Pfeifer and Galinsky16 In another study, negative facial expressions elicited greater amygdala reactivity for young adults from lower-SES families, taking into account parenting quality and maternal mental health.Reference Gard, Waller, Shaw, Forbes, Hariri and Hyde17 Emotional faces more generally evoked greater right amygdala activation in adults of lower SES.Reference Demenescu, Stan, Kortekaas, van der Wee, Veltman and Aleman18 The same study showed that SES was associated with strength of coupling between right amygdala and right insula.Reference Demenescu, Stan, Kortekaas, van der Wee, Veltman and Aleman18 In a study of first-time mothers viewing the faces of either happy or distressed infants, the right amygdala was more active to depictions of distress in mothers of lower SES.Reference Kim, Capistrano, Erhart, Gray-Schiff and Xu19

Three other studies yielded partial support for a relationship between amygdala reactivity to negative facial emotions and SES. The support from these findings was qualified by participant gender in one case (only for womenReference Javanbakht, Kim, Swain, Evans, Luan Phan and Liberzon20), ethnicity of depicted face in another (only for Black emotional faces in a study with Mexican American participantsReference Muscatell, McCormick and Telzer21) and history of violence exposure in a third (only in non-exposed participantsReference White, Voss, Chiang, Wang, McLaughlin and Miller22). In only one study testing the relationship between amygdala reactivity and SES was a significant relationship not found, and this study had relatively little variance in SES among the participants (all were recruited through an antipoverty programmeReference Choe, Shaw and Forbes23). Finally, in a memory study with emotional faces as retrieval cues, testing the hippocampus as a region of interest (ROI), there was less activity in the posterior hippocampus in lower-SES individuals when they watched angry faces.Reference Lambert, Sheridan, Sambrook, Rosen, Askren and McLaughlin24

Brain responses to other negative stimuli

Apart from facial emotional cues, other stimuli that have been used to evoke negative emotions include unpleasant sounds, social rejection and loss of money. In one study unpleasantly loud auditory stimuli were used to evoke emotional states in participants of varying SES, violence exposure and ethnicity, generally with a warning tone but occasionally without.Reference Harnett, Wheelock, Wood, Goodman, Mrug and Elliott25 When trials without a warning cue were considered, family income was negatively associated with hippocampal activity, consistent with greater effect of unexpected negative stimuli with lower SES. SES showed the opposite effect with cued noise, with higher neighbourhood deprivation (i.e. lower SES) showing lower activity in the hippocampi and amygdalae, perhaps related to reduced preparatory regulatory effects.

Two studies have used the sound of babies crying, contrasted with white noise, with participants of varying SES. The crying was rated as more annoying,Reference Kim, Ho, Evans, Liberzon and Swain26 but effect of SES on neural response to the cry in non-parent adults was complex and difficult to interpret; there was generally more activation in the insula and several other regions in women from low-SES backgrounds, with the opposite pattern in men. Another study of mothers hearing baby cries found less activation associated with lower SES in frontal and temporal cortical regions, but differences in classic emotion-related areas were not found.Reference Kim, Capistrano and Congleton27 Although baby cries communicate urgency and distress, they may function less as generic signals of threat or harm and more as triggers for complex, evolved systems of parenting behaviour.Reference Swain28

Social rejection is another kind of experience that evokes negative emotion. The so-called ‘social pain’ that results from rejection is not typically associated with amygdala activity, but has a more distinctive functional anatomy including the dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate, anterior insula and also the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and orbital cortex regions (see meta-analysesReference Vijayakumar, Cheng and Pfeifer29Reference Cacioppo, Frum, Asp, Weiss, Lewis and Cacioppo31). Gonzalez et alReference Gonzalez, Beckes, Chango, Allen and Coan32 assessed responses to exclusion in the classic ‘cyberball’ rejection task in young adults of varying neighbourhood SES, and tested whether SES was associated with dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and insula responses. The insula was not reliably activated by exclusion in this study overall, nor as a function of SES, but lower SES was associated with a larger dACC response. Related to social rejection is negative social evaluation. Muscatell et alReference Muscatell, Dedovic, Slavich, Jarcho, Breen and Bower33 had participants read negative versus neutral or positive personal assessments of their performance in an interview, and a priori regions of interest were the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and amygdala. Although amygdala activity did not differ with subjective social status (and it was not stated whether the task evoked amygdala activity in this condition for participants in general), dmPFC activity was evoked by negative evaluations and was higher in lower-SES participants.

Finally, the aversive experience of losing money, a secondary reinforcer, which differs in some ways from primary aversive stimuli such as pain,Reference Delgado, Jou and Phelps34 has been assessed during fMRI in two studies with participants of varying SES, neither of which found effects of SES.Reference Quevedo, Waters, Scott, Roisman, Shaw and Forbes35,Reference Gianaros, Manuck, Sheu, Kuan, Votruba-Drzal and Craig36

Summary

There is a trend, across various forms of negative emotional state, for lower-SES individuals to have more brain activity in at least some emotion-related areas. This includes evidence from tasks using emotional facial expressions, loud noises and social rejection. This generalisation, although reasonably broad, does not extend to all of the literature. In particular, two studies using infant cry sounds show SES effects, but these effects are complex and cannot be interpreted as simply more activity in emotion-related areas, and two studies of monetary loss failed to show effects of SES at all.

Subcortical and cortical bases of positive affect

The anatomy of positive affect overlaps with some of the areas mentioned above in connection with negative affect, consistent with the complex, emergent nature of emotion in the brain. Studies of positive affect use depictions of happy faces and scenes and the occurrence of desirable outcomes such as the winning of money or points in games. The region most often associated with positive affect is the ventral striatum, which consists primarily of the nucleus accumbens and part of the caudate nucleus, although other cortical regions are also engaged, including the medial and orbital frontal cortex, cingulate cortex and anterior insula.Reference Bartra, McGuire and Kable37

The literature on the neural correlates of positive emotion and SES is relatively small. One study, already mentioned in connection with negative emotion, is also relevant to positive emotion. In this study, mothers viewed happy as well as unhappy baby faces, and some of the areas activated by the happy faces, including the left amygdala and the right insula, differed by SES and specifically were less active in the lower-SES mothers.Reference Kim, Capistrano, Erhart, Gray-Schiff and Xu19

Silverman et alReference Silverman, Muennig, Liu, Rosen and Goldstein38 exposed participants to affectively valenced pictures of people, objects and scenes and contrasted neural responses to positive images (e.g. an amusement park) relative to neutral images (e.g. furniture). They found lower activity in response to the happy pictures in lower-SES participants in a variety of areas, including the striatum.

Other studies have induced positive emotional states with monetary gains during simple games. The focus of most studies on SES and reward has been on reward anticipation, rather than the receipt of the reward itself. Reward anticipation is a motivational state sometimes associated with ‘wanting’, in contrast to the response of ‘liking’.Reference Berridge, Robinson and Aldridge39 Response to receipt of a reward has either not been shown to differ by SESReference Quevedo, Waters, Scott, Roisman, Shaw and Forbes35,Reference Romens, Casement, McAloon, Keenan, Hipwell and Guyer40 or the study design has not allowed reward receipt to be examined separate from reward anticipation because of block rather than event-related design.Reference Gianaros, Manuck, Sheu, Kuan, Votruba-Drzal and Craig36 In contrast, reward anticipation generally evokes more activity for lower-SES participants. Romens et alReference Romens, Casement, McAloon, Keenan, Hipwell and Guyer40 found heightened response to the anticipation of reward in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in lower-SES girls, and no locations of reduced response. Gonzalez et alReference Gonzalez, Allen and Coan41 found a similar relationship in striatal and other regions. Quevedo et alReference Quevedo, Waters, Scott, Roisman, Shaw and Forbes35 covaried SES in a study of the effect of attachment style on reward, and although the range of SES was relatively narrow, they found that maternal unemployment and lower family income during childhood were associated with higher striatal activity and amygdala activity respectively, during reward anticipation. When anticipating a larger but lower-probability reward, lower-SES adolescents show more mPFC activation.Reference Shad, Bidesi, Chen, Thomas, Ernst and Rao42

Summary

Positive stimuli may evoke smaller responses in people of lower SES, although the evidence is limited, while reward anticipation may be accompanied by greater activation.

Networks for emotion and emotion regulation

Emotion regulation refers to self-induced changes in intensity and duration of emotional experience, typically for the purpose of reducing negative experience. These changes can be accomplished by either conscious, explicit strategies or automatic, implicit processes.Reference Gross43 One of the most effective explicit emotion regulation strategies is cognitive reappraisal, by which we volitionally reinterpret the meaning of stimuli in order to alleviate negative feelings. A recent review suggests that explicit emotion regulation engages the dorsolateral, ventrolateral and dorsomedial frontal and parietal cortex.Reference Etkin, Büchel and Gross44 Kim et alReference Kim, Evans, Angstadt, Ho, Sripada and Swain45 showed disturbing pictures to participants of varying SES and instructed them to reduce negative emotion through cognitive reappraisal, for example viewing a picture of a bruised and beaten woman and reappraising it as a picture of an actress playing the role of a violence victim. They found that individuals of low SES recruited less prefrontal activation than their higher-SES counterparts and showed less reduction in amygdala activity during reappraisal, consistent with this emotion regulation strategy being used less effectively by these participants. However, gender seems to moderate the effect of SES on prefrontal activity related to emotion regulation:Reference Javanbakht, Kim, Swain, Evans, Luan Phan and Liberzon20 in males but not females when considered separately, activation in dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortical regions (dlPFC and vlPFC) during cognitive reappraisal was positively correlated with SES.

Another form of emotion regulation is implicit, involuntary emotion regulation, which does not require effortful use of a strategy or conscious monitoring of emotional state, but is simply evoked automatically.Reference Mauss, Bunge and Gross46 Implicit emotion regulation is omnipresent in our encounters with emotional stimuli, with ventral ACC and vmPFC engaging automatically to modulate subcortical activity.Reference Etkin, Büchel and Gross44 By its nature, implicit regulation is not carried out following instructions, so it can be difficult to determine in any given task whether these ventral anterior activations represent regulatory activity. In any case, less functional coupling between the amygdala and vmPFC has been found in low-SES individuals when processing negative emotion.Reference Javanbakht, King, Evans, Swain, Angstadt and Luan Phan47 In the same intensively studied group of participants (see alsoReference Javanbakht, Kim, Swain, Evans, Luan Phan and Liberzon20,Reference Kim, Evans, Angstadt, Ho, Sripada and Swain45,Reference Javanbakht, King, Evans, Swain, Angstadt and Luan Phan47 ), Liberzon and colleaguesReference Liberzon, Ma, Okada, Shaun Ho, Swain and Evans48 found less prefrontal activity in lower-SES participants in a task designed to evoke implicit emotion regulation, although in this task the finding was localised to lateral rather than medial regions.

Studies of participants at rest provide additional evidence concerning limbic–cortical interactions. Functional connectivity between the amygdala and the vmPFC was found to be weaker in participants of lower SES, and this difference in brain activity accounted for SES disparities in vulnerability to stressful life events.Reference Hanson, Albert, Skinner, Shen, Dodge and Lansford49 Connectivity of the amygdala and hippocampus to prefrontal regions was also reduced in lower-SES children, and these differences fully mediated the relationship between SES and later depression.Reference Barch, Pagliaccio, Belden, Harms, Gaffrey and Sylvester50 Finally, connectivity between the ventral striatum and ventral PFC is reduced in low SES, and this fully mediated the relationhip between SES and anxiety.Reference Marshall, Marusak, Sala-Hamrick, Crespo, Rabinak and Thomason51

Conclusions

We offer this preliminary review of the literature as a starting point, to be refined as the literature grows and our understanding of SES and affective neuroscience advances. It is limited in part by the small size of the literature. Our search method uncovered only 27 studies, and many of these involved small samples (n < 50 for half of the studies) or a restricted range of SES. In addition, affect and SES are both complex constructs, and each has been operationalised in numerous different ways in the studies reviewed here. Is it sensible to group the sight of a frightened face, the sound of a crying baby and the loss of small sums of money into a common category of ‘negative emotion?’ We did so here provisionally, to help organise our review at a very general level, recognising that important differences may be glossed over. The studies reported here were also heterogeneous in terms of participants’ ages. Finally, the studies measured SES in different ways, for example in childhood or adulthood, and according to income, educational attainment or neighbourhood deprivation. In attempting this first broad review of SES and the neural bases of emotion, we do not distinguish between different measures of SES, and we report findings as positive if any measure of SES shows a statistically significant effect.

For the reasons just mentioned, any conclusions from this review must be considered very provisional. Nevertheless, some trends can be discerned, and these may be relevant to the SES gradient in mood and anxiety disorders. Socioeconomic disadvantage shapes the brain's response to emotional stimuli, such that negative stimuli appear to evoke a stronger response and positive stimuli may possibly evoke a weaker response. This amounts to an overall bias towards the negative and away from the positive for lower SES, which would be expected to indicate a greater susceptibility among low-SES individuals to depression and anxiety. In contrast, anticipation of reward appears to evoke more activity in people of lower SES, which in one study mediated the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage and adolescent depression symptoms.Reference Romens, Casement, McAloon, Keenan, Hipwell and Guyer40 Greater reactivity to the promise of reward may contribute to disorders of impulse control.Reference Dalley and Robbins52 Finally, in at least a few studies functional networks that may serve to regulate emotion are weaker in people of lower SES, and these differences too have been found to mediate risk for psychopathology.

Given the disproportionate mental health burden borne by those of low SES, it makes sense to deploy the full range of approaches to understanding and addressing this inequity, from the sociological to the neuroscientific. High priorities for future research will be to expand the evidentiary base relating SES, brain function and psychological symptoms, and to establish how social and economic factors external to the individual may give rise to the neural and psychological vulnerabilities reviewed here. In aiming to understand the interrelations among psychology, biology and social context, it should be possible for mental health and well-being to be more widely enjoyed throughout society.

About the authors

Yu Hao, PhD, is a postdoctoral researcher at the Center for Neuroscience & Society, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA. Martha J. Farah, PhD, is Walter H. Annenberg Professor in the Natural Sciences and Director of the Center for Neuroscience & Society, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA.

Author contributions

Literature reviewed and interpreted by both authors; first draft written by Y.H.; revisions by both authors.

Declaration of interest

None.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjb.2020.69.

References

Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, Jin, R, Merikangas, KR, Walters, EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 593602.10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorant, V, Deliège, D, Eaton, W, Robert, A, Philippot, P, Ansseau, M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol 2003; 157: 98112.10.1093/aje/kwf182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, KA, Costello, EJ, Leblanc, W, Sampson, NA, Kessler, RC. Socioeconomic status and adolescent mental disorders. Am J Public Health 2012; 102: 1742–50.10.2105/AJPH.2011.300477CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guhn, M, Emerson, SD, Mahdaviani, D, Gadermann, AM. Associations of birth factors and socio-economic status with indicators of early emotional development and mental health in childhood: a population-based linkage study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2020; 51: 8093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. Mental health-related disabilities among Canadians aged 15 years and older, 2012. Statistics Canada, 2012 (https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/pub/89-654-x/89-654-x2014002-eng.htm).Google Scholar
Braveman, PA, Cubbin, C, Egerter, S, Chideya, S, Marchi, KS, Metzler, M, et al. Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. J Am Med Assoc 2005; 294: 2879–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, E, Paterson, LQ. Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: how do they relate to adolescent health? Health Psychol 2006; 25: 704–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P, Evans, GW, Chen, E, Miller, G, Seeman, T. How socioeconomic disadvantages get under the skin and into the brain to influence health development across the lifespan. In Handbook of Life Course Health Development (eds Halfon, N, Forrest, CB, Lerner, RM, Faustman, EM): 463–98. Springer, 2017.Google ScholarPubMed
Palacios-Barrios, EE, Hanson, JL. Poverty and self-regulation: connecting psychosocial processes, neurobiology, and the risk for psychopathology. Compr Psychiatry 2019; 90: 5264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, KA, Sheridan, MA, Lambert, HK. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 47: 578–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, PJ, Bush, G, McNally, RJ, Wilhelm, S, McInerney, SC, Jenike, MA, et al. The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44: 1219–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessiglione, M, Petrovic, P, Daunizeau, J, Palminteri, S, Dolan, RJ, Frith, CD. Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 2008; 59: 561–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, KA, Wager, TD, Kober, H, Bliss-Moreau, E, Barrett, LF. The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav Brain Sci 2012; 35: 121–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, LF. How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. Picador, 2017.Google Scholar
Gianaros, PJ, Horenstein, JA, Hariri, AR, Sheu, LK, Manuck, SB, Matthews, KA, et al. Potential neural embedding of parental social standing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2008; 3: 91–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Muscatell, KA, Morelli, SA, Falk, EB, Way, BM, Pfeifer, JH, Galinsky, AD, et al. Social status modulates neural activity in the mentalizing network. Neuroimage 2012; 60: 1771–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Gard, AM, Waller, R, Shaw, DS, Forbes, EE, Hariri, AR, Hyde, LW. The long reach of early adversity: parenting, stress, and neural pathways to antisocial behavior in adulthood. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2017; 2: 582–90.Google Scholar
Demenescu, LR, Stan, A, Kortekaas, R, van der Wee, NJA, Veltman, DJ, Aleman, A. On the connection between level of education and the neural circuitry of emotion perception. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8: 866.Google ScholarPubMed
Kim, P, Capistrano, CG, Erhart, A, Gray-Schiff, R, Xu, N. Socioeconomic disadvantage, neural responses to infant emotions, and emotional availability among first-time new mothers. Behav Brain Res 2017; 325: 188–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Javanbakht, A, Kim, P, Swain, JE, Evans, GW, Luan Phan, K, Liberzon, I. Sex-specific effects of childhood poverty on neurocircuitry of processing of emotional cues: a neuroimaging study. Behav Sci (Basel) 2016; 6: 28.Google ScholarPubMed
Muscatell, KA, McCormick, E, Telzer, EH. Subjective social status and neural processing of race in Mexican American adolescents. Dev Psychopathol 2018; 30: 1837–48.10.1017/S0954579418000949CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, SF, Voss, JL, Chiang, JJ, Wang, L, McLaughlin, KA, Miller, GE. Exposure to violence and low family income are associated with heightened amygdala responsiveness to threat among adolescents. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2019; 40: 100709.Google ScholarPubMed
Choe, DE, Shaw, DS, Forbes, EE. Maladaptive social information processing in childhood predicts young men's atypical amygdala reactivity to threat. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 2015; 56: 549–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, HK, Sheridan, MA, Sambrook, KA, Rosen, ML, Askren, MK, McLaughlin, KA. Hippocampal contribution to context encoding across development is disrupted following early-life adversity. J Neurosci 2017; 37: 1925–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harnett, NG, Wheelock, MD, Wood, KH, Goodman, AM, Mrug, S, Elliott, MN, et al. Negative life experiences contribute to racial differences in the neural response to threat. Neuroimage 2019; 202: 116086.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P, Ho, SS, Evans, GW, Liberzon, I, Swain, JE. Childhood social inequalities influences neural processes in young adult caregiving. Dev Psychobiol 2015; 57: 948–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P, Capistrano, C, Congleton, C. Socioeconomic disadvantages and neural sensitivity to infant cry: Role of maternal distress. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016; 11: 1597–607.10.1093/scan/nsw063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swain, JE. The human parental brain: in vivo neuroimaging. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011; 35: 1242–54.10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vijayakumar, N, Cheng, TW, Pfeifer, JH. Neural correlates of social exclusion across ages: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional MRI studies. NeuroImage 2017; 153: 359–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotge, JY, Lemogne, C, Hinfray, S, Huguet, P, Grynszpan, O, Tartour, E, et al. A meta-analysis of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 10: 1927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, S, Frum, C, Asp, E, Weiss, RM, Lewis, JW, Cacioppo, JT. A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of social rejection. Sci Rep 2013; 3: 2027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, MZ, Beckes, L, Chango, J, Allen, JP, Coan, JA. Adolescent neighborhood quality predicts adult dACC response to social exclusion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10: 921–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muscatell, KA, Dedovic, K, Slavich, GM, Jarcho, MR, Breen, EC, Bower, JE, et al. Neural mechanisms linking social status and inflammatory responses to social stress. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016: 11: 915–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, MR, Jou, RL, Phelps, EA. Neural systems underlying aversive conditioning in humans with primary and secondary reinforcers. Front Neurosci 2011; 5: 71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quevedo, K, Waters, TEA, Scott, H, Roisman, GI, Shaw, DS, Forbes, EE. Brain activity and infant attachment history in young men during loss and reward processing. Dev Psychopathol 2017; 29: 465–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gianaros, PJ, Manuck, SB, Sheu, LK, Kuan, DCH, Votruba-Drzal, E, Craig, AE, et al. Parental education predicts corticostriatal functionality in adulthood. Cereb Cortex 2011; 21: 896910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartra, O, McGuire, JT, Kable, JW. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage 2013; 76: 412–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, ME, Muennig, P, Liu, X, Rosen, Z, Goldstein, MA. The impact of socioeconomic status on the neural substrates associated with pleasure. Open Neuroimag J 2009; 3: 5863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, KC, Robinson, TE, Aldridge, JW. Dissecting components of reward: “liking”, “wanting”, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2009; 9: 6573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romens, SE, Casement, MD, McAloon, R, Keenan, K, Hipwell, AE, Guyer, AE, et al. Adolescent girls’ neural response to reward mediates the relation between childhood financial disadvantage and depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 2015; 56: 1177–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, MZ, Allen, JP, Coan, JA. Lower neighborhood quality in adolescence predicts higher mesolimbic sensitivity to reward anticipation in adulthood. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2016; 22: 4857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shad, MU, Bidesi, AS, Chen, LA, Thomas, BP, Ernst, M, Rao, U. Neurobiology of decision-making in adolescents. Behav Brain Res 2011; 217: 6776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol 1998; 2: 271–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etkin, A, Büchel, C, Gross, JJ. The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015; 16: 693700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P, Evans, GW, Angstadt, M, Ho, SS, Sripada, CS, Swain, JE, et al. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2013; 110: 18442–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mauss, IB, Bunge, SA, Gross, JJ. Automatic emotion regulation. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 2007; 1: 146–67.Google Scholar
Javanbakht, A, King, AP, Evans, GW, Swain, JE, Angstadt, M, Luan Phan, K, et al. Childhood poverty predicts adult amygdala and frontal activity and connectivity in response to emotional faces. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9: 154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberzon, I, Ma, ST, Okada, G, Shaun Ho, S, Swain, JE, Evans, GW. Childhood poverty and recruitment of adult emotion regulatory neurocircuitry. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10: 1596–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, JL, Albert, WD, Skinner, AT, Shen, SH, Dodge, KA, Lansford, JE. Resting state coupling between the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex is related to household income in childhood and indexes future psychological vulnerability to stress. Dev Psychopathol 2019; 31: 1053–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D, Pagliaccio, D, Belden, A, Harms, MP, Gaffrey, M, Sylvester, CM, et al. Effect of hippocampal and amygdala connectivity on the relationship between preschool poverty and school-age depression. Am J Psychiatry 2016; 173: 625–34.Google ScholarPubMed
Marshall, NA, Marusak, HA, Sala-Hamrick, KJ, Crespo, LM, Rabinak, CA, Thomason, ME. Socioeconomic disadvantage and altered corticostriatal circuitry in urban youth. Hum Brain Mapp 2018; 39: 1982–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalley, JW, Robbins, TW. Fractionating impulsivity: neuropsychiatric implications. Nat Rev Neurosci 2017 18: 158–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flowchart for the systematic review. Relevant studies were identified through searches of the database PubMed throughout 13 March 2020. The search required that studies used at least one of the following socioeconomic status (SES) keywords in the full paper: socioeconomic status, poverty, income, neighbourhood quality, neighbourhood disadvantage, educational attainment, social class, social standing. Identified studies also used at least one of the following social and affect-related keywords in the entire paper: emotion, threat, fear, angry, sad, happy, reward, social interaction, hostility, rejection. In addition, the keyword of neuroimaging method was included: fMRI. This search identified 166 results, which were screened for the inclusion criteria.

Supplementary material: PDF

Hao and Farah et al. supplementary material

Hao and Farah et al. supplementary material 1

Download Hao and Farah et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.3 MB
Supplementary material: PDF

Hao and Farah et al. supplementary material

Hao and Farah et al. supplementary material 2

Download Hao and Farah et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.3 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.