Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-xtvcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T05:41:26.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

EMBODIED SIGNS: READING GESTURE AND POSTURE IN CLASSIC MAYA DANCE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2018

Mark Wright
Affiliation:
Department of Ancient Scripture, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT 84602, USA ([email protected])
Justine Lemos
Affiliation:
Independent Scholar, PO Box 944, Mendocino, CA 95460, USA ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this article, we couple Peircean semiotic theory with Laban movement analysis (LMA) to interpret scenes of Classic period Maya dance. We focus primarily on depictions found on polychrome ceramics that feature the dances of the Maize God and contrast them with those featuring the wahy creatures that inhabit the underworld. We interpret their inner emotional states based on their postural and gestural vocabularies using LMA, developed for that very purpose. The body can be considered a semiotic sign, and is therefore capable of simultaneously conveying iconic, symbolic, and indexical meanings. Maya dance has typically been interpreted at the iconic or symbolic levels, which reveal its mimetic or representational qualities. We explore the indexical qualities of the bodies of the dancers, and propose that shifting our attention to the indicative mode enables us to gain yet more insight into their embodied states.

En este artículo combinamos la teoría semiótica peirceana con el Análisis del Movimiento Laban (LMA) para interpretar escenas de la danza Maya del período Clásico. Nos enfocamos principalmente en las imágenes de las cerámicas policromadas que representan las danzas del Dios del Maíz y las contrastamos con las imágenes de las criaturas wahy que habitan el inframundo. Interpretamos sus estados emocionales, basándonos en su vocabulario postural y gestual a través del LMA, lo cual se desarrolló precisamente para ese propósito. El cuerpo mismo se podría considerar como un signo semiótico, y como tal, podría expresar los significados icónicos, simbólicos e indiciales simultáneamente. La danza Maya se ha interpretado típicamente a nivel icónico o simbólico para revelar sus cualidades miméticas o representacionales. Con este estudio exploramos las características indiciales de los cuerpos de los bailarines, y sugerimos un enfoque más centrado en el modo indicial para obtener una mejor perspectiva de sus estados personificados.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2018 by the Society for American Archaeology 

Anthropological interpretation of archeological materials can be a tricky business, particularly when such interpretations attempt to vicariously access the emotional states and embodied experiences of individuals who lived in cultural and temporal contexts far removed from our own. Nevertheless, in recent years a variety of analytical approaches have demonstrated their effectiveness in offering nuanced glimpses into the lived experience of ancient peoples (Houston Reference Houston2001; Joyce Reference Joyce2005; Lock and Farquhar Reference Lock and Farquhar2007; Tiesler Reference Tiesler2014). In that vein, we argue that the postures and gestures of dancing figures depicted in Classic Maya art offer entry into their innermost emotional states when interpreted as semiotic indices.

Our analysis focuses on the postural and gestural vocabularies that characterize the dances of the Maize God and those of the wahy beings that inhabit the underworld. Methodologically, our examination rests primarily on iconographic depictions of dance found on Late Classic polychrome ceramics originating in the Maya Lowlands (Boot Reference Boot2003; Looper Reference Looper2009; Reents-Budet Reference Reents-Budet, Costin and Wright1994; Robicsek and Hales Reference Robicsek and Hales1981). We use Laban movement analysis (LMA) as a methodological platform to describe various postural attitudes depicted in scenes of dance, then turn to Peircean semiotic theory to explore their potential indexical qualities. Because neither of these particular analytical tools have gained widespread traction among students of the ancient Maya, we begin with a brief overview of each before proceeding with our own analysis.

Peircean Semiotic Theory

When discussing semiotic theory, clarification is needed as to precisely which branch of semiotics is referenced. Confusingly, two unrelated forms of semiotic theory arose in the nineteenth century, and both have maintained a foothold in the fields of archaeology and anthropology. The first came courtesy of the Swiss linguist Ferdinand de Saussure, who established the field of semiology (sémiologie), “a science that studies the life of signs within society” (Saussure Reference Saussure, Bally, Sechehaye and Baskin1966:16 [1916]). The second was developed independently by the American polymath Charles Sanders Peirce, which he dubbed “a science of semeiotic” (Peirce Reference Peirce1998:482). The Peircean model presents a sweeping phenomenological approach that is rooted in categories of signs, which is conducive to anthropological interpretation. Preucel (Reference Preucel2006:3–4) highlighted the inadequacies of the Saussurian model for adducing meaning from material culture and strongly encouraged archaeologists to embrace a Peircean semiotic approach instead.

In Peirce's (Reference Peirce1992 [1894]) taxonomy of sign-forms there are three predominant modes of signs: icon, index, and symbol. These classes of signs overlap and are not mutually exclusive. A single sign, for example, might be an icon, a symbol, and an index simultaneously (Chandler Reference Chandler2002:43). Icons function by resembling that which is represented. Indices refer to their object via a relationship in time and space. Symbols are arbitrary; they refer to their object by means of convention, such as flags or traffic lights. To illustrate, an icon of a mouse would be a drawing meant to resemble a mouse. A symbol of mouse would be the word “mouse.” An index of a mouse would be the “Eek!” sound emitted by a person startled by seeing one. An indexical sign reflects a causal relationship, one of indication, such as smoke for fire, or a broad smile indicating someone is happy. It is the index, as a sign type, with which we are most concerned throughout this paper.

The application of Peircean semiotics to the interpretation of archaeological materials is a fairly recent phenomenon, but it has proven effective and enlightening (Hutson et al. Reference Hutson, Magnoni and Stanton2012; Preucel Reference Preucel2006; Preucel and Bauer Reference Preucel and Bauer2001). Carballo (Reference Carballo, Carballo and Levine2014:197) highlights the utility of semiotics in archaeology but notes that the notion of indices is “particularly valuable for studying ritual as a form of differential communication saturated with signs and generated through performance.” In Mesoamerica generally and among the Maya specifically, the body itself “is a key symbol and a transmitter of social information through its artistic depiction, spatial positioning, and metaphorical reference” (Palka Reference Palka2002:420). Such culturally specific metaphors have the power to shape the way individuals experience the world in very real ways (Lakoff and Johnson Reference Lakoff and Johnson1980). Maya art often uses a “non-verbal vocabulary of gesture” that enables emotions and attitudes to be expressed without words (Eisner Reference Eisner1990:100). Although some elements of this gestural vocabulary transcend cultural barriers, others appear to be specific to the Classic Maya (Wichmann and Nielsen Reference Wichmann, Nielsen and Cohn2016:301).

Comparatively few archaeologists have embraced semiotic interpretations, which Hutson and colleagues (Reference Hutson, Magnoni and Stanton2012:298) suggest may be due to wariness in the field of what may seem like yet another jargon-filled theoretical trend (see also Chippindale Reference Chippindale, Yoffee and Sherratt1993). We echo the authors’ assertion that semiotic theory is neither a passing trend nor an uneasy fit between “exotic theory and local data” (Hutson et al. Reference Hutson, Magnoni and Stanton2012:298), but rather a powerful and broadly applicable interpretive tool.

Although the artistic conventions surrounding Classic Maya dance may serve as indexical markers of the context in which a particular scene was produced (Looper Reference Looper2009:104), the nature of signs is polysemous–they may point to more than one meaning at a time. Our particular focus on indexicality follows the lead of a handful of Mesoamerican scholars. To highlight just a few, Hutson et al. (Reference Hutson, Magnoni and Stanton2012:207) interpret sacbes in the Yucatan as indices of inclusion by virtue of the physical connectedness they create between center and periphery. Baron's (Reference Baron2016) recent book explores, among other things, the indexical linkages between patron gods and local identity formation. Hendon (Reference Hendon2009:224–225) argues that the hachas and yokes curated in a noble home at Late Classic Copan (Group 9N-8 Patio A) served as indexical markers of “foreignness,” even if those who donned them were in fact local and only assumed a “foreign” identity for ritual purposes. Hendon and coauthors (Reference Hendon, Joyce and Lopiparo2014:47) explore the iconic aspects of animal figures that commonly appear on polychrome vessels in the Ulúa Valley, which also served as indexical markers of local identities when preferences for specific animals arose in different subregions; furthermore, they note that the general uniformity of Ulúa Polychromes served to index the shared knowledge of a larger cultural tradition (Hendon et al. Reference Hendon, Joyce and Lopiparo2014). Joyce's (Reference Joyce, Orr and Looper2014:17) semiotic analysis of the Playa de los Muertos figurines from Middle Formative Honduras suggests the twined and woven textiles represented on the figurines “would have indexically invoked assessments of the skill and labor required to make objects such as fine plain-weave cloth and complex twined textiles.”

Laban Movement Analysis

Rudolf Laban (1879–1958) developed his theories and descriptive systems of movement out of an express desire to help spectators not only understand but truly experience the same emotional state as the performers they observe (Burton et al. Reference Burton, Samadini, Gorbet, Kulic, Laumond and Abe2016:28; Laban Reference Laban1972). His system enables a close reading of the posture, gesture, and movement vocabularies of the human body. Bartenieff and Lewis (Reference Bartenieff and Lewis1980:16) suggested three ways in which Laban analysis could be used: the study of “body structure and morphology;” the pathways and “spatial tension” of movements; and the study of EffortFootnote 1, which refers to the postural attitudes of an individual “in relation to space, weight, time, and flow.” Each of the Effort elements are defined diametrically: Space Effort is either Direct or Indirect, Weight Effort is Strong or Light, Time Effort can be Sudden or Sustained, and Flow Effort is either Free or Bound. In Laban's calculus, when two of these Effort factors are combined they constitute a state Footnote 2. When three of the Effort factors combine, they constitute a drive Footnote 3.

It is the Effort qualities that are of particular interest to this study. Bartenieff and Lewis (Reference Bartenieff and Lewis1980:59) note that “a variety of inner states can be described in the movement manifestations of two Effort combinations.” That is, the states and drives of LMA, by design, reveal inner emotional states. For example, there is little difference in the qualities of Space and Time between someone violently punching their hand forward vs. lightly “fist bumping” a friend—each is of the same duration and moves in the same direction—but the Effort quality of the two motions is dramatically different. One is Strong, the other Light. The former might register as an expression of anger or frustration, but the latter is understood to be a friendly affirmation or greeting. We argue that such Effort qualities can be detected in Classic period depictions of dance.

Anthropological studies of dance have generally examined movement, posture, and gesture as icons (Downey Reference Downey2005; Ness Reference Ness, Thomas and Ahmed2004; Srinivasan Reference Srinivasan2007). Among the Maya, for example, scholars have suggested that dances are mimetic in that they replicate “the sway of maize” (Looper Reference Looper1991:54), the “motions of animals” (Houston et al. Reference Houston, Stuart and Taube2006:255), or the flight of birds (Taube Reference Taube, Le Fort, Gardiol, Matteo and Helmke2009:46). Although proper identification of the iconic mode of these dances is crucial to our interpretations, the study of other types of sign making in dance processes can amplify our overall understanding of them (Lemos Reference Lemos2012). Ness (Reference Ness1992) pioneered the application of Peircean semiotic theory to the field of dance ethnography. She found that semiotic indices pointed to “something else also present or evident in the performance context” (Ness Reference Ness, Noland and Ann Ness2008:8), which might include signs of trance (indexed by shaking, shivering, eye rolling), or a drunk person's weaving walk—an index of the fact that the person is inebriated. Not only is dance representative, it is also indicative.

We are mindful of the objections that may be raised in our effort to adduce meaning from idealized, static, two-dimensional images of dance produced by artists who are far removed from us temporally and culturally (see Looper Reference Looper2009:103–106). Complicating the matter further, we lack provenience for a large percentage of these Classic period ceramics because they were wrested from their original context by looters, leaving us to cobble together clues from disparate datasets (Reents-Budet Reference Reents-Budet1998:82). Art historians caution that there may be a disconnect between the intended meaning as expressed by a particular performer and the message as conveyed by the artist depicting the performance, and yet another disconnect between the artist's depiction and meaning ascribed by observers who view artwork through different cultural lenses (Gombrich Reference Gombrich1999:272). Although this certainly holds true for us in our examination of ancient Maya art, such divisions between performer, artist, and viewer may have been less pronounced in their original context. Late Classic polychrome ceramics were indisputably elite objects: elite artists depicted elite performers on vessels created exclusively for elite consumption (Reents-Budet Reference Reents-Budet1998:78; Rice Reference Rice2009; see also Wichmann and Nielsen Reference Wichmann, Nielsen and Cohn2016:307). Hidden from the gaze of the commoners, these relatively small works would have required close proximity for interpretation (see Joyce Reference Joyce, Orr and Looper2014:74), much like the scenes of dance carved into the panels and lintels in the walls of temples and palaces, with access limited to elites.

Concerning our ability to detect emotional states from ancient works of art, Houston (Reference Houston2001:207) noted that Classic Maya depictions of the body are “unusually expressive, with a degree of verisimilitude or ‘naturalism’ that is deceptively transparent to Western gaze.” He pointed to the detailed depictions of both facial expressions and bodily postures that convey nuanced emotional states such as torment, fear, despair, lust, and grief. Although there were unquestionably distinctive beliefs and practices between, and even within, different Classic period polities, there appears to have been something of a “codification of bodily movement and position” across the Lowlands that provided the Classic Maya with a “shared, connective communicative identity (perhaps even representing a concept of elite practice), while simultaneously emphasizing difference and inequality between individuals” (Jackson Reference Jackson2009:75–76). In sum, the messages Maya elites hoped to convey through their postural attitudes, both actual and those idealized in their artwork, likely came through loud and clear to their intended audiences.

Emotional states are easily identified when there is congruence between an individual's facial expression, body language, and voice, but surprisingly, recent studies have revealed that the body alone is capable of conveying certain human emotions to virtually the same degree as isolated facial or vocal expressions (Coulson Reference Coulson2004; de Gelder Reference de Gelder2009). This holds true even in static images with limited vantage points (Coulson Reference Coulson2004:132), or when postural attitudes are conveyed by faceless humanoid avatars (Kleinsmith et al. Reference Kleinsmith, Bianchi-Berthouze and Steed2011). On that note, LMA has rather unexpectedly been thrust into the twenty-first century by STEM fields (science, technology, engineering, and mathematics) in their quest to imbue human-like attributes into everything from video game avatars to artificially intelligent robots (Burton et al. Reference Burton, Samadini, Gorbet, Kulic, Laumond and Abe2016; Chi et al. Reference Chi, Costa, Zhao and Badler2000; Zacharatos et al. Reference Zacharatos, Gatzoulis and Chrysanthou2014).

Accurate identification of emotional states communicated via physical expressions, be they human, robot, or computer-generated humanoid avatar, is further enhanced when the viewer understands the context in which the expression in question is elicited (Carroll and Russell Reference Carroll and Russell1996; Zhang and Sharkey Reference Zhang, Sharkey, Groß, Alboul, Melhuish, Witkowski, Prescott and Penders2011; see also Gombrich Reference Gombrich1982; Lakoff and Johnson Reference Lakoff and Johnson1980). There are, of course, both similarities and differences in the meanings associated with distinct bodily posture (Kleinsmith et al. Reference Kleinsmith, Ravindra De Silva and Bianchi-Berthouze2006), but intracultural identification of certain emotional states based on an individual's posture is quite consistent (Elfenbein and Ambady Reference Elfenbein and Ambady2003), so much so that, in what may sound to some like the seeds of a dystopian future, artificially intelligent robots are being programmed with LMA “to create reciprocal interactive behaviors that appear natural and are not restricted to simple imitation” (Lourens et al. Reference Lourens, Van Berkel and Barakova2010:1263; see also McColl et al. Reference McColl, Hong, Hatakeyama, Nejat and Benhabib2016).

Although most of this type of research involving LMA is being driven by future-oriented fields, many of their findings are equally applicable to ancient Maya art. Combined with our emerging understanding of the consistency of elite messaging practices during the Classic period, we feel confident that LMA is an appropriate, if not optimal, tool for interpreting the idealized two-dimensional static depictions of Maya dance.

Classic Maya Dance

Dancing was one of the most common rituals performed by Classic Maya rulers throughout their reigns. Grube (Reference Grube1992) was the first to translate the T516 glyph as ‘to dance.’ Phonetically, the glyph is read as ahk'ot and may be semantically related to the term for “give,” ahk’ (Macri and Looper Reference Macri and Looper2003:206). Ahk’ also serves as the root for yahk'a(w), “he/she gives (it),” which is used in the context of offerings made to the gods (Looper Reference Looper2009:17). In essence, the dances may be seen as offerings or tributes to the gods, and dance continues to be an acceptable form of prayer among traditional Mesoamerican communities (Houston Reference Houston, Inomata and Coben2006:144; Looper Reference Looper2009:18).

Dances were associated with a wide variety of events: deity impersonation rituals, sacrifices, heir designation and other dynastic events, warfare, and visits by overlords (Looper Reference Looper2009:5). Dances and public spectacles need not be seen as purely religious events, as they also served sociopolitical functions in legitimizing the power of a ruler (Houston Reference Houston, Inomata and Coben2006; Looper Reference Looper2009:5; Schele and Miller Reference Schele and Miller1986). As Sahagún (1950–Reference Sahagún, Anderson and Dibble1982 [1575–1578]:8:150) noted, among the Aztec, dancing was one of the primary responsibilities of the ruler and was done “in order to hearten and console all the peers, the noblemen, the lords, the brave warriors, and all the common folk and vassals.” Among the Classic Maya, public ceremonies were sometimes marked with the glyphic expression cha'nil (literally, “something being watched”; Tokovinine Reference Tokovinine2003:3). The expression occasionally occurs in conjunction with glyphic references to dance, which suggests that certain royal dances were intended to be performed in front of an audience (Looper Reference Looper2009:18; Tokovinine Reference Tokovinine2003).

One of the earliest iconographic depictions of dance in the Maya area comes from the San Bartolo west wall mural (ca. 100 BC). The Maize God dances inside the quatrefoil carapace of the earth turtle while tapping his turtle shell drum pectoral in preparation for his resurrection (Taube et al. Reference Taube, Saturno, Stuart and Hurst2010: Figure 46). Curiously, over seven centuries pass before the first known textual reference to dance occurs. The earliest use of the dance glyph, found on Altar L at Quirigua, dates to AD 653 (Looper Reference Looper1991:91). Other early references (AD 668–733) come from Dos Pilas, Naranjo, Piedras Negras, and La Corona. From AD 752–780, most dance references are limited to the Yaxchilan region. After about AD 780, glyphic references to dance are found scattered throughout the Maya area (Looper Reference Looper2009:18).

Proskouriakoff was the first to propose that the raised-heel motif was indicative of formal scenes of dance, noting that the dancers’ motions always appeared “restrained” (Reference Proskouriakoff1950:28). Building on Proskouriakoff, Miller (Reference Miller1981) noted that this pose includes bent knees, horizontal “turn-out” of the feet and hips, and a single foot placed on “three-quarter” pointe. Looper (Reference Looper and Koontz2001:118) suggests the raised-heel motif is better understood as a “conventional reference” to dance rather than a literal depiction of specific dance movements, noting that even in instances when glyphic references to dance accompany the scene, the dancer typically maintains a “highly rigid pose.”

Specific hand gestures are often used in conjunction with the raised-heel motif in scenes of dance, but identical gestures are sometimes used by non-dancers as well (Ancona-Ha et al. Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000), suggesting that hand gestures alone do not indicate dance (Miller Reference Miller1981:133–134). One of the more common gestures involves the hand bent back sharply at the wrists with the palm facing outward and the fingers slightly flexed (Miller Reference Miller1981:134). In addition to the standard lifted-heel pose, many of the dancers are depicted bending at the waist (Miller Reference Miller1981:133), the significance of which we will return to below.

Our attempt to read the bodies of dancers as multilayered repositories of semiotic information aims to complement previous studies of Maya dance that have explored dance performance contexts as well as the iconic aspects of posture, gesture, and costuming (Houston Reference Houston, Inomata and Coben2006; Houston et al. Reference Houston, Stuart and Taube2006; Looper Reference Looper and Koontz2001, Reference Looper2009; Taube Reference Taube and Fields1985, Reference Taube, Le Fort, Gardiol, Matteo and Helmke2009). Following Naerebout (Reference Naerebout1997:234, cited in Looper Reference Looper2009:103), we resist any attempt to reconstruct the dances themselves. Our arguments rest on iconography, not choreography. What follows is an analysis of two of the most common categories of Classic Maya dance—those of the Maize God and those of the wahy beings—filtered through the lenses of both LMA and Peircean semiotics.

The Refined Body of the Maize God Dancer

One of the most widespread motifs in Maya art is the dancing Maize God (Looper Reference Looper2009:149; Taube Reference Taube, Le Fort, Gardiol, Matteo and Helmke2009:42). It is especially prominent on Holmul-style vessels (Looper Reference Looper2008, Reference Looper2009) and on the “Tikal Dancer Plates” (Boot Reference Boot2003). Such scenes are so common that Taube suggested, “if there is a Classic Maya god of dance, it would be the maize god” (Taube Reference Taube, Le Fort, Gardiol, Matteo and Helmke2009:42). The Maize God was one of the fundamental components of the “shared vocabulary” that transcended distinctive identities formed by both political boundaries and social classes across the Maya lowlands (Inomata Reference Inomata, Fiad and Hruby2007; Jackson Reference Jackson2009; Reents-Budet Reference Reents-Budet1998).

As depicted on Maya polychrome ceramics, Maize God dancers almost always have a distinctly vertical spinal alignment (K0097, K0621, K0622, K0633, K0703, K1271, K1837, K3388, K4464, K4619, K5648, K5941, K7434, K7720, K8088, K8190, K8533). Kurath and Marti (Reference Kurath and Marti1964:140) were the first to highlight the “erect posture” of a Maize God dancer “with feet in classic ballet ‘third position’—one foot half across the other—probably in a jump, with the dancer flipping his feet, arms extended, hands down from the wrists,” depicted on one of the Tikal Dancer plates (Figure 1). They use Labanotation to provide a detailed description of the Maize God's specific posture and gesture, which forms a basis from which we can begin formulating qualitative statements about the depictions. How is the body of the dancer positioned within Space? What type of Effort is conveyed by the dancer's posture and gesture? What meanings might these postural attitudes convey to the viewer?

Figure 1. Example of Tikal Dancer Plate. Excavated from Uaxactún Burial a3 (painting by M. Louise Baker, courtesy of the University of Pennsylvania Museum [Image 165116]).

Meanings can often be adduced from the visual perspectives that artists chose when depicting their subjects, particularly those relating to frontal versus profile views of the head or body (Klein Reference Klein1976; Palka Reference Palka2002; Schapiro Reference Schapiro1973:37–49). In two-dimensional Classic Maya art, however, the heads of all full-length figures are almost unfailingly depicted in profile view. Although frontality of the torso often denotes “peak figures” and establishes hierarchical relationships in Classic Maya art (Palka Reference Palka2002), shifting perspectives of the Maize God dancers’ bodies may simply represent different moments from the same dance. Indeed, both frontal and profile perspectives of the Maize God's dance are sometimes depicted on opposite sides of the same vase (K517, K621, K3400, K5977). Significant variation exists within and between regional styles regarding the frontality of Maize God dancers (Looper Reference Looper2009:117–131). Long-established conventions may have governed such artistic decisions. As Looper (Reference Looper2009:92) noted, “the representation of dance poses in Maya art is very much determined by the need to avoid foreshortening and oblique views of the body.” Unlike frontality, however, their verticality remained remarkably consistent, which forms the foundation of our analysis here.

Vertical posture is often used cross-culturally as an indexical marker of training, technique, poise, and regality (Miettinen Reference Miettinen1992:99). Semiotic indices and icons are not mutually exclusive, however, so the Maize God's postural verticality may also be an iconic sign that is representational of his identification with the axis mundi. Among the Classic Maya, royal bodies in general tend to be depicted with this same erect posture, and indeed, the body of the ruler is also conceptualized as a living axis mundi (Houston et al. Reference Houston, Stuart and Taube2006; Schele and Miller Reference Schele and Miller1986:77; Taube Reference Taube1996, Reference Taube and Houston1998). The depiction of Pakal on the lid of his own sarcophagus provides one of the rare examples of a ruler in the guise of the Maize God whose posture is neither rigid nor vertical. To the contrary, his pose is that of a newborn infant. In this depiction, Pakal dons the garb of the Maize God not to recreate the god's dance, but rather to mimetically enact his rebirth (Martin Reference Martin, Tiesler Blos, Cobos and Green Robertson2002; Taube Reference Taube and Kerr1994). Importantly, from Pakal's resurrecting body springs the World Tree. In Classic Maya cosmology, the Maize God's resurrection as the World Tree brings order to the chaos, centers the cosmos, and provides new life to humanity (Martin Reference Martin and McNeil2006:179). As such, the vertical posture of the Maize God, and that of the rulers who dance in his guise, functions as an iconic sign representing the axis mundi, “the place of transformation, mediation, and balance” (Stross Reference Stross1992:102).

In addition to their verticality, the bodies of Maize God dancers are also focused and directional. There is little ambiguity in their attention to Space; their gestural focus is specific. Their arms appear to have a one-dimensional, sagittal, Spoke-like movement process, forward from the core of the body to the periphery of their kinesphere, pinpointing a specific spatial goal with the gesturing arm. Their gaze, like their bodies, is typically focused and directional, although sometimes they appear to gaze into the distance, far beyond their gesturing arm. Although the spine of the dancer remains vertically aligned, the distal parts of the body (particularly the arms and hands) are typically positioned at points within an octahedral pattern (Figure 2). This pattern is found on nearly all Holmul Maize God dancer vessels (see, for example, K0633, K3388, K4619, K5977, K7434, K7720, K7814, K8088, K8190). Such spatial clarity is one of the defining characteristics of the Maize God's dance.

Figure 2. Octahedral Movement Constellation (illustration by Timothy Turner).

Determining Space qualities from two-dimensional art has obvious limitations, so we are fortunate to have at least one well-preserved, fully in-the-round bust of the Maize God to analyze (Figure 3). Burdick (Reference Burdick2010:93) notes that such three-dimensional figural sculptures from across the Maya area “exemplify the energy and vitality that artists aimed to evoke in two-dimensional works.” The bust (that originally adorned Structure 10L-22 at Copan) furnishes us with a dynamic, three-dimensional rendering of the Maize God's peripheral spatial tension (defining the boundary around his kinesphere), as well as his engagement in an octahedral movement constellation.

Figure 3. Maize God sculpture from Copan (note the peripheral spatial tension and controlled gestures). Drawing by Linda Schele, reproduced courtesy of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art [SD-3518].

The hand gestures of the Maize God dancers exhibit peripheral spatial tension by calling attention to the distance between his core and the edge of his kinesphere. Although the fingers are deployed with delicacy and refinement, they nevertheless convey a sense of bound, contained energy (see especially K8533, K9190, K8088, K7814, K7720, K7434, K7013, K5977, K5648, K4619, K4464, K3389, K1837, K0633). Chi and colleagues (Reference Chi, Costa, Zhao and Badler2000) note that isolated gestures in the limbs may lack impact, but “when its Effort and Shape characteristics spread to the whole body, a person appears to project full involvement, conviction, and sincerity.” This is evident in the hand and arm movements of the Holmul Maize God dancers, which are always engaged in forward (sagittal) space, either directly forward or on the forward-high diagonal. Nevertheless, the Tikal Dancers often show the arms positioned on the high-side diagonal, suggesting a vertical rather than sagittal movement process. Nevertheless, either pattern exhibits an attention to an octahedral Space Harmony progression.

The Maize God dancers typically display a distinct wrist flexion and form spatially clear, exacting hand gestures. Catalogued using LMA, this wrist flexion shows a strongly defined kinesphere, peripheral spatial tension, and Bound Effort. Maya scribes, both human and supernatural, often display this same type of distinctive hand gesture (Figure 4). The scribal arts would have required similar levels of discipline, and thus their distinctive hand gestures are likewise indexical markers of their own training and proprioceptive control (see Herring Reference Herring2005:24). By way of contrast, the individual being held up by his companions in K1092 (Figure 5) has slack hands, and his lack of distinct, spatially precise gestures stand in contrast to other figures in the scene. He also lacks an upright vertical dimensional posture. His attention to Space is neither direct nor focused. His knees appear weak, as if he cannot hold himself upright. His posture and gestures are easily recognizable indexical signs that he is drunk.

Figure 4. Supernatural scribe forming “Gesture 14” (drawing by Jorge Pérez de Lara and Mark Van Stone).

Figure 5. Scene of drunkenness from K1092 (drawing by Emily Davis-Hale).

The two most common gestures formed by the Maize God dancers are what Ancona-Ha and coauthors (Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000:1075, 1081) have designated “Gesture 1” and “Gesture 14.” In Gesture 1 (Figure 6), the palm faces outward with the fingertips pointing down. It is commonly found in supernatural palace scenes featuring the god Itzamnaaj, where he appears to use the gesture as a salutation or greeting as he sits upon his throne, although high-ranking human elites occasionally mimic it in palace scenes as well (Ancona-Ha et al. Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000:1075). With Gesture 14 (see Figure 4), the thumb either touches or almost touches the tip of one of the fingers (Ancona-Ha et al. Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000:1081). Although rarely found on the Tikal Dancer plates, it is by far the most commonly used by the Holmul Maize God dancers (Looper Reference Looper2009:125). The gesture is never sloppy or arbitrary. Typically, a single finger delicately touches the thumb (K0633, K1837, K3389, K4464, K4619, K5648, K5977, K7013, K7434, K7720, K7814, K8088, K9190, K8533). Because Gesture 14 is used in such a wide variety of situations and by a broad range of characters, it is difficult to offer any precise interpretation of either its iconic or symbolic meanings (Ancona-Ha et al. Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000:1082). Nevertheless, we may still glean semiotic information from it by exploring the potential meanings it carries as an indexical sign.

Figure 6. Itzamnaaj using “Gesture 1” as an apparent greeting in a supernatural palace scene. Such controlled gestures serve as indexical markers of proprioceptive refinement among both gods and elites (drawing by Jorge Pérez de Lara and Mark Van Stone, in Ancona-Ha et al. Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000:1075).

Distinctly crystallized gestures such as these often serve as indexical signs of proprioceptive refinement (Kendon Reference Kendon and Poyatos1988; McNeill Reference McNeill1992). Our analysis finds that the dance of the Maize God is neither improvisational nor loose. The entire dance appears controlled, regal, and civil. The clearly defined hand gestures of the Maize God dancers and cultivated sense of refinement make clear that they are exhibiting trained, choreographed dance behavior. Among the Classic Maya, “refinement was read into bodily equipoise,” and “civility lent a particular appearance and feel to entire performative situations and physical conditions” (Herring Reference Herring2005:79). Verticality, upright stance, and posture are aligned to present the Maize God dancers in a sublimely human or metahuman state. Their embodied schema exhibit a rarified sense of movement. This crystallization of vertical alignment and directly focused Light/Bound gesture is found in many styles of dance cross-culturally and often used to portray a sense of “withheld strength” (Miettinen Reference Miettinen1992:99). This sense is augmented by the lower bodies of many Maize God dancers, which exhibit a Strong Weight Effort. They appear to be strongly rooted into the ground, with the weight center of the body flowing downward into the earth, while the upper body floats upward, supported by the base.

The Maize God dancers’ combination of Weight-Effort with Bound Flow and Direct Attention to Space leads us to postulate that they are engaged in what Laban analysts call “Spell Drive,” a compelling, nearly hypnotic combination of movement efforts. As Bartenieff and Lewis (Reference Bartenieff and Lewis1980:61) note, ”Without Time's sense of urgency or delay to loosen the stability, the steadfastness of Space/Weight becomes inescapable when Flow's ‘goingness’ is added to it, and a spell-like intensity is created.” Classic Maya elites entered into this state through dances performed during costumed deity impersonation rituals, wherein they believed they merged with, or even embodied, the supernatural being in a very real way (Houston et al. Reference Houston, Stuart and Taube2006: 270; Taube Reference Taube, Le Fort, Gardiol, Matteo and Helmke2009:45). As Danielsson (Reference Danielsson, Hamilakis, Pluciennik and Tarlow2002:181) noted, “masks enable embodiment of disembodied states” in transformative situations. Although it can be difficult at times to determine if a scene is intended to depict an actual supernatural being or merely a human impersonator (Freidel et al. Reference Freidel, Schele and Parker1993:262), our interpretive approach remains unaffected due to the perceived ontological unity that exists between the dancer and the gods during such ritual moments (Houston et al. Reference Houston, Stuart and Taube2006:270; see also Lakoff and Johnson Reference Lakoff and Johnson1980). The Maize God, of course, was the most commonly impersonated deity during such ritual dances (Taube Reference Taube and Carrasco2001:306).

Previous analyses of the posture and gestures of the Maize God dancers have focused on their iconic aspects by noting that they are imitative of the maize plant. Complemented by an indexical analysis, those same scenes also reveal their posture and gestures to be indicative of the dancers’ training in specific movement technique. This movement technique is in turn indexical of a refined sense of harmony and equipoise. The bodies of the Maize God dancers are thus made “literate,” fluent in the language of refined movement that is choreographed, specific, and meaning-making. It was up to the artists to make them legible to the viewer, however. Houston (Reference Houston2001:215, expounding on the work of Levy [Reference Levy, Shweder and LeVine1984]) emphasizes that affect—the subjective states attributed by one person to another—was “hypercognated” or “elaborated and explicitly highlighted” in Classic Maya art, noting that “by their very appearance in painting and sculpture, affect, fear and despair, drunkenness, lustful abandon and grief must have been subject to comment and dissection. In a word, they were hypercognated by the Classic Maya. But the absence of such affect must also have been hypercognated, as part of an idealized mode of self-constraint and concealment of emotion behind stylized gesture.” Filtered through our semiotic lens, that means we may read the controlled, refined affect of the Maize God dancers not only for what they are, but also in terms of what they are not: they are expressly not wild or unrestrained, a point to which we will return.

We speculate that vestiges of this dance may still be manifest in traditional Maya communities today. Among the Tzutujil Maya of Santiago Atitlan, Guatemala, for example, a powerful priest known as a nab'eysil performs a ritual dance which Christenson (Reference Christenson, Guernsey and Kent Reilly2006) describes as being “stately,” “slow,” “restrained,” and “purposeful.” Significantly, the priest dances while holding the sacred bundle of Martín, the patron of the maize harvest. The dance “consists mostly of small, purposeful steps with the knees slightly bent, the arms held downward and away from the body, and rhythmically leaning his body from side to side” (Christenson Reference Christenson, Guernsey and Kent Reilly2006:240–241). At the end of the dance, the nab'eysil holds his arms outward and slightly down in a “crucifixion-like pose.” One of the Tzutujil men explained that the pose represented “a maize plant as it grows out of the earth with its leaves extending outward away from the stalk” (Christenson Reference Christenson, Guernsey and Kent Reilly2006:241–242), vividly illustrating the concept that bodily posture and gesture function as iconic signs. Although this ethnographic description is tantalizingly similar to those offered by iconographers concerning the Classic period Maize God dancers, caution warrants we present it here by way of analogy rather than as a rubric for interpretation (Taube Reference Taube and Markman2014; see also Bricker Reference Bricker1981:130–133).

Gestural Vocabulary of the Wahy Creatures

In dramatic contrast to the refined gestures that typify the Maize God are those of the strange wahy creatures that populate the underworldFootnote 4. The wahy are a common type of supernatural being, although they are not technically considered gods. Not only are they never granted the divine epithet k'uh (sacred, holy), they are also never shown in direct interaction with such sacred beings (Grube Reference Grube, Graña-Behrens, Grube, Prager, Sachse, Teufel and Wagner2004:74). They are generally depicted as fantastical or frightening creatures that combine anthropomorphic, zoomorphic, or skeletal features (Calvin Reference Calvin, Kerr and Kerr1997; Grube Reference Grube, Graña-Behrens, Grube, Prager, Sachse, Teufel and Wagner2004:215). Although not completely understood, some Classic period wahy entities may have been personifications of disease, illnesses, or sorcery (Grube Reference Grube, Graña-Behrens, Grube, Prager, Sachse, Teufel and Wagner2004; Helmke and Nielsen Reference Helmke and Nielsen2009; Houston and Inomata Reference Houston and Inomata2009:208; Stuart Reference Stuart2012; Zender Reference Zender2004:72–77).

As with the Maize God, elites commonly impersonated these entities by wearing particular masks and costume elements during their ritual performances. Scenes of dancing wahy creatures are extremely common on Classic period ceramic vessels (Grube Reference Grube1992:214), and as with the Maize God dancers, their postural and gestural characteristics are ripe with semiotic meaning. To illustrate, we turn first to an elegantly executed Ik’-style vessel, the Altar de Sacrificios Vase (Figure 7), painted by the same artist who created K791. The artist was sponsored by the ruler K'ihnich Lamaw Ek’, and we know of at least two other vases painted by the same hand, which likewise feature wahy creatures (Halperin and Foias Reference Halperin and Foias2010; Reents-Budet et al. Reference Reents-Budet, Foias, Bishop, Blackman, Guenter, Pedro Laporte, Arroyo and Mejía2007). On both the Altar Vase and K791, the wahy beings dance with a downward, internal focus, yet their bodies appear light and luminous. Their kinesphere has shrunk. The dancers look away from their hands. Looper (Reference Looper2009:224) describes their dance postures as “strongly hunched or agitated.” Their eyes focus downward, as if concentrating on inner processes or visions, which find physical expression in private, personal movement processes. The internal focus of these dancers seems indicative of a remote state. They appear to be experiencing an otherworldly reality, one of trance or ecstasy. Unlike the vertical position of the Maize God dancers, the wahy dancers bend at the waist, and their torsos appear fluid and loose, engaged in a process of free-flowing energy. Their dance is wholly unconcerned with spatial precision or articulation. Their movement is not Bound or contained. Their legs and feet step lightly on the ground. In stark contrast to the Strong Weight displayed by the Maize God dancers, the wahy creatures do not appear rooted into the earth. To the contrary, they seem to almost float away from it, unconstrained by gravity, perhaps indexical of an out-of-body experience or a “dreamlike vision” (Werness-Rude and Spencer Reference Werness-Rude, Spencer, Werness-Rude and Spencer2015:19).

Figure 7. Dancing wahy being. Note the downward focus, Light Weight, and Free Flow efforts. Detail from the Altar de Sacrificios vase. Museo Nacional de Arqueología y Etnología, Guatemala City. Excavated from Burial 96 of Structure A-III (Adams Reference Adams1971. Drawing by Linda Schele, courtesy of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art [SD-5504].

Discussion

The gestural vocabularies of the dances of the Maize God dancers and those of the wahy are qualitatively and indexically far removed from each other. Every Effort quality (Space, Weight, Time, and Flow) associated with the wahy creatures stands in polar opposition to those of the Maize God. The Effort qualities of the Maize God's dance involve Direct Space, Strong Weight, Sustained Time, and Bound Flow. Those of the wahy, in contrast, involve Indirect Space, Light Weight, Sudden Time, and Free Flow Efforts. Recalling Houston's (Reference Houston2001) discussion of hypercognition, we can define each of their affects both in terms of what they are and what they are not. Regardless of the culture-specific meanings that may be associated with these two types of dance, the message is still clear: they are diametrically opposed to each other.

The dramatically different movement vocabularies of the Maize God dancers and the wahy beings has previously been recognized as representing the concepts of order and chaos, respectively (Looper Reference Looper2009:226). The tension between these opposing forces continues to frame the worldview of modern Maya peoples. Among the Yucatec Maya, for example, the word toh literally means “straight” or “truth,” but it also connotes moral rectitude and ethical correctness (as do its cognates in several other Mayan languages; Taube Reference Taube, Gomez-Pompa, Fedick and Jimenez-Osornio2003:465). The majority of the depictions of the dancing Maize God show him engaging in straight-line movement processes, both in the core (spine and torso) and distal parts (arms and legs) of his body. Iconically, the postural vocabulary of the Maize God is cultivated, ordered, and bounded. In this sense it resembles the milpa, which itself is a microcosm of the ordered universe.

In contrast, the wahy dancers are unconcerned with the sort of proprioceptive control or refinement exhibited by the Maize God dancers. Out of control, the wahy demons are often depicted in positions of extreme contortion and with bizarre proportions, often to humorous effect (Taube Reference Taube and Kerr1989:484; Wichmann and Nielsen Reference Wichmann, Nielsen and Cohn2016:302). The scenes are typically set in the underworld, which is conceptually linked to places like caves that lack “internal order and spatial divisions” (Stone Reference Stone1995:16–18) or the wild, untamed forest (Taube Reference Taube, Gomez-Pompa, Fedick and Jimenez-Osornio2003:467–468). Thick, knotted forest growth is referred to as lob’ in Yucatec, a word that is likewise used to denote that which is evil and dark in other Mayan languages (Barrera Vásquez et al. Reference Barrera Vásquez, Manzano, Sansores, Salas, Góngora and Dzul Pot1980:219, 454–456; Hofling and Tesucun Reference Hofllng and Tesucun1997:416; Taube Reference Taube, Gomez-Pompa, Fedick and Jimenez-Osornio2003:466).

Although there are many depictions of wahy creatures on Maya polychrome ceramics, which we did not address in this short analysis, our survey indicates that bodily contortion and distortion of perspective are intentionally emphasized in such scenes. Unlike the extremely rarified upright dance of the Maize God, the wahy move with little or no attention to space. Indeed, their posture often appears to defy the normal limits of bodily capability. It is as if they threaten to spill outside the boundaries of their own skin (as on K5010 and K1379). The wahy dancers exhibit little attention to the direction their bodies move in space and effectively embody the chaotic, undefined boundaries of the “forest” in contrast to the “carefully delineated world of humans” (Taube Reference Taube, Gomez-Pompa, Fedick and Jimenez-Osornio2003:469).

Signs are, by their nature, both polysemous and imbricative (Preucel Reference Preucel2006:155). We suggest the bodily postures of the Maize God dancers and those of the wahy creatures are themselves signs that convey multiple meanings. The body of the Maize God dancer may be iconic of multiple things at once: the maize plant itself, the milpa, the axis mundi, even the order of the cosmos. Concomitantly, their controlled, refined dances serve as indexical markers of their individual training and skill. As noted previously, Joyce (Reference Joyce, Orr and Looper2014:75) emphasized that iconographic depictions of textiles on Mesoamerican figures were capable of “indexing labor, skill, and social personhood,” not only of the artists who made the figurines, but also of those who made the textiles that were represented on the figurines. Our indexical analysis of Late Classic polychrome ceramics leads us to draw similar conclusions about Maya dancers and the artists who depicted them.

Conclusions

Some fifty years ago, Kurath and Marti (Reference Kurath and Marti1964:26) suggested that the movements and gestures of Maya dancers could provide a glimpse into their innermost emotional states. We have attempted to deliver on their suggestion by analyzing Classic period scenes of dance through the lenses of both LMA and Peircean semiotic theory. We believe this particular amalgam of approaches offers a unique but methodologically robust platform that can be effectively used to draw meanings from Classic period iconography, which have previously gone unnoticed. In effect, we consider these methods to be translation tools that have the potential to increase our fluency in ancient Maya body language.

The application of Peircean semiotic analysis has allowed us to postulate that these movement processes are indexical markers of cultural training. In understanding the movements of the Maize God dancers as indices of specific cultural training, we approach understanding the symbolic mode of Classic Maya dance. Why? Because cultural training is rule driven and therefore symbolic of the technical language-like aspects of the dance. It is tempting to consider the depictions of dance as “true signs” that depict social status (Ancona-Ha et al. Reference Ancona-Ha, De Lara, Van Stone, Kerr and Kerr2000). However, it is the layered nature of dance—its ability to “work” iconically, symbolically, and indexically—that makes it such a rich repository of cultural information.

Acknowledgments

We thank Christina Halperin, Kerry Hull, Lucia Henderson, Stephen Houston, Matthew Looper, Victoria Lyall, Karl Taube, Rhonda Taube, and Marc Zender for their helpful responses to our iconographic, epigraphic, and ethnographic questions. We are indebted to Justin Kerr, Jorge Pérez de Lara, Timothy Turner, and Mark Van Stone for granting permission to use their images. We also express gratitude to the four anonymous reviewers whose feedback dramatically improved the quality and clarity of this paper.

Data Availability Statement

Photographs of all polychrome ceramics analyzed or cited in this study are available through the Maya Vase Database (www.mayavase.com) or the sources listed in the references.

Footnotes

1. We will follow the standard practice of capitalizing key terms from Laban movement analysis to distinguish them from common English usage throughout this paper.

2. The states are Awake (space + time), Dream (weight + flow), Stable (weight + space), Mobile (time + flow), Remote (flow + space), and Rhythm (weight + time).

3. The drives are Action (space + weight + time), Vision (time + space + flow), Spell (weight + space + flow), and Passion (time + weight + flow).

4. The orthography of way vs. wahy is unsettled. Our usage follows Stone and Zender (Reference Stone and Zender2011:233, note 7).

References

References Cited

Adams, Richard E.W. 1971 The Ceramics of Altar de Sacrificios. Papers of the Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology, Harvard University, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Ancona-Ha, Patricia, De Lara, Jorge Pérez, and Van Stone, Mark 2000 Some Observations on Hand Gestures in Maya Art. In The Maya Vase Book, Vol. 6, edited by Kerr, Barbara and Kerr, Justin, pp. 10721089. Kerr Associates, New York.Google Scholar
Baron, Joanne 2016 Patron Gods and Patron Lords: The Semiotics of Classic Maya Community Cults. University of Colorado Press, Boulder.Google Scholar
Barrera Vásquez, Alfredo, Manzano, Juan Ramón Bastarrachea, Sansores, William Brito, Salas, Refugio Vermont, Góngora, David Dzul, and Dzul Pot, Domingo 1980 Diccionario Maya Cordemex, Maya-Español, Español-Maya. Ediciones Cordemex, Merida.Google Scholar
Bartenieff, Irmgard, and Lewis, Dori 1980 Body Movement: Coping with the Environment. Gordon and Breach Science Publishers, New York.Google Scholar
Boot, Erik 2003 An Annotated Overview of “Tikal Dancer Plates.” Mesoweb: www.mesoweb.com/features/boot/TikalDancerPlates.pdf, accessed May 25, 2015.Google Scholar
Bricker, Victoria Reifler 1981 The Indian Christ, the Indian King. University of Texas Press, Austin.Google Scholar
Burdick, Catherine E. 2010 Text and Image in Classic Maya Scultpure. PhD dissertation, Department of Art History, University of Illinois, Chicago.Google Scholar
Burton, Sarah Jane, Samadini, Ali-akbar, Gorbet, Rob, and Kulic, Dana 2016 Laban Movement Analysis and Affective Movement Generation for Robots and Other Near-Living Creatures. In Dance Notations and Robot Motion, edited by Laumond, Jean-Paul and Abe, Naoko, pp. 2548. Springer, New York.Google Scholar
Calvin, Inga 1997 Where the Wayob Live: A Further Examination of Classic Maya Supernaturals. In The Maya Vase Book, Vol. 5, edited by Kerr, Barbara and Kerr, Justin, pp. 868883. Kerr Associates, New York.Google Scholar
Carballo, David M. 2014 Obsidian Symbolism in a Temple Offering from La Laguna, Tlaxcala. In Obsidian Reflections: Symbolic Dimensions of Obsidian in Mesoamerica, edited by Carballo, David M. and Levine, Marc, pp. 195221. University of Colorado Press, Boulder.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, James M., and Russell, James A. 1996 Do Facial Expressions Signal Specific Emotions? Judging Emotion from the Face in Context. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology 70 (2):205.Google Scholar
Chandler, Daniel 2002 Semiotics: The Basics. Routledge, London.Google Scholar
Chi, Diane, Costa, Monica, Zhao, Liwei, and Badler, Norman I. 2000 The EMOTE Model for Effort and Shape. Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques, pp. 173182. ACM Press/Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., New York.Google Scholar
Chippindale, Christopher 1993 Ambition, Deference, Discrepancy, Consumption: The Intellectual Background to a Post-processual Archaeology. In Archaeological Theory: Who Sets the Agenda?, edited by Yoffee, Norman and Sherratt, Andrew, pp. 2736. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Christenson, Allen J. 2006 Sacred Bundle Cults in Highland Guatemala. In Sacred Bundles: Ritual Acts of Wrapping and Binding in Mesoamerica, edited by Guernsey, Julia and Kent Reilly, F., pp. 226246. Boundary End Archaeology Research Center, Barnardsville, North Carolina.Google Scholar
Coulson, Mark 2004 Attributing Emotion to Static Body Postures: Recognition Accuracy, Confusions, and Viewpoint Dependence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 28 (2): 117139.Google Scholar
Danielsson, Ing-Marie Back 2002 (Un)masking Gender—Gold Foil (Dis)embodiments in Late Iron Age Scandinavia. In Thinking through the Body: Archaeologies of Corporeality, edited by Hamilakis, Yannis, Pluciennik, Mark, and Tarlow, Sarah, pp. 179199. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York.Google Scholar
de Gelder, Beatriz 2009 Why Bodies? Twelve Reasons for Including Bodily Expressions in Affective Neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 364 (1535):34753484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Downey, Greg 2005 Learning Capoeira: Lessons in Cunning from an Afro-Brazilian Art. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Eisner, Will 1990 Comics and Sequential Art. Revised edition. Poorhous Press, New York.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, Hillary A., and Ambady, Nalini 2003 Universals and Cultural Differences in Recognizing Emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 12:159164.Google Scholar
Freidel, David, Schele, Linda, and Parker, Joy 1993 Maya Cosmos: Three-Thousand Years on the Shaman's Path. William Morrow and Company, New York.Google Scholar
Gombrich, Ernst H. 1982 The Image and the Eye: Further Studies in the Psychology of Pictorial Representation. Phaidon, Oxford.Google Scholar
Gombrich, Ernst H. 1999 Aby Warburg: His Aims and Methods: An Anniversary Lecture. Journal of the Warburg and Courtauld Institutes 62:268282.Google Scholar
Grube, Nikolai 1992 Classic Maya Dance: Evidence from Hieroglyphs and Iconography. Ancient Mesoamerica 3:201218.Google Scholar
Grube, Nikolai 2004 Akan—The God of Drinking, Disease and Death. In Continuity and Change: Maya Religious Practices in Temporal Perspective. Fifth European Maya Conference, University of Bonn, December 2000, edited by Graña-Behrens, Daniel, Grube, Nikolai, Prager, Christian M., Sachse, Frauke, Teufel, Stefanie, and Wagner, Elisabeth, pp. 5976. Acta Mesoamericana 14. Verlag Anton Sauerwein, Markt Schwaben, Germany.Google Scholar
Halperin, Christina T., and Foias, Antonia E. 2010 Pottery Politics: Late Classic Maya Palace Production at Motul de San José, Petén, Guatemala. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 29 (3):392411.Google Scholar
Helmke, Christophe, and Nielsen, Jesper 2009 Hidden Identity and Power in Ancient Mesoamerica: Supernatural Alter Egos as Personified Diseases. Acta Americana 17 (2):4998.Google Scholar
Hendon, Julia A. 2009 Houses in a Landscape: Memory and Everyday Life in Mesoamerica. Duke University Press, Durham.Google Scholar
Hendon, Julia A., Joyce, Rosemary, and Lopiparo, Jeanne 2014 Material Relations: The Marriage Figurines of Prehispanic Honduras. University of Colorado Press, Boulder.Google Scholar
Herring, Adam 2005 Art and Writing in the Maya Cities A.D. 600–800: A Poetics of Line. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Hofllng, Charles Andrew, and Tesucun, Felix Fernando 1997 Itzaj Maya-Spanish-English Dictionary. University of Utah, Salt Lake City.Google Scholar
Houston, Stephen D. 2001 Decorous Bodies and Disordered Passions: Representations of Emotion Among the Classic Maya. World Archaeology 33 (2):206219.Google Scholar
Houston, Stephen D. 2006 Impersonation, Dance, and the Problem of Spectacle Among the Classic Maya. In Archaeology of Performance: Theaters of Power, Community, and Politics, edited by Inomata, Takeshi and Coben, Lawrence S., pp. 135155 AltaMira, Lanham, MD.Google Scholar
Houston, Stephen D., and Inomata, Takeshi. 2009 The Classic Maya. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Houston, Stephen D., Stuart, David, and Taube, Karl 2006 The Memory of Bones: Body, Being, and Experience among the Classic Maya. University of Texas Press, Austin.Google Scholar
Hutson, Scott R., Magnoni, Aline, and Stanton, Travis W. 2012All That is Solid. . .”: Sacbes, Settlement, And Semiotics At Tzacauil, Yucatan. Ancient Mesoamerica 23:297311.Google Scholar
Inomata, Takeshi 2007 Knowledge and Belief in Artistic Production by Classic Maya Elites. In Rethinking Craft Specialization in Complex Societies: Archaeological Analyses of the Social Meaning of Production, edited by Fiad, Rowan and Hruby, Zachary, pp. 129141. Archaeological Papers No. 17. American Anthropological Association, Arlington, Virginia.Google Scholar
Jackson, Sarah E. 2009 Imagining Courtly Communities: An Exploration of Classic Maya Experiences of Status and Identity Through Painted Ceramic Vessels. Ancient Mesoamerica 20:71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, Rosemary 2005 Archaeology of the Body. Annual Review of Anthropology 34:139158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, Rosemary 2014 Ties That Bind. In Wearing Culture: Dress and Regalia in Early Mesoamerica and Central America, edited by Orr, Heather and Looper, Matthew, pp. 6178. University of Colorado Press, Boulder.Google Scholar
Kendon, Adam 1988 How Gestures Can Become Like Words. In Cross-Cultural Perspectives in Nonverbal Communication, edited by Poyatos, Fernando, pp. 131141. C.J. Hogrefe, Lewiston, New York.Google Scholar
Klein, Cecelia F. 1976 The Face of the Earth: Frontality in Two-dimensional Mesoamerican Art. Garland Publishing, New York.Google Scholar
Kleinsmith, Andrea, Ravindra De Silva, P., and Bianchi-Berthouze, Nadia 2006 Cross-Cultural Differences in Recognizing Affect from Body Posture. Interacting with Computers 18 (6):13711389.Google Scholar
Kleinsmith, Andrea, Bianchi-Berthouze, Nadia, and Steed, Anthony 2011 Automatic Recognition of Non-Acted Affective Postures. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, Part B (Cybernetics) 41:10271038.Google Scholar
Kurath, Gertrude, and Marti, Samuel 1964 Dances of Anahuac: The Choreography and Music of Precortesian Dances. Vol. 38. Wenner-Gren Foundation, New York.Google Scholar
Laban, Rudolf 1972 The Mastery of Movement, 3rd ed. Revised and enlarged by Lisa Ullman. PLAYS, Inc. Boston.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George, and Johnson, Mark 1980 Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Lemos, Justine 2012 Radical Recreation; Non-Iconic Movements of Tradition in Keralite Classical Dance. Recherches Anthropologiques: Semiotic Inquiry 32:4782.Google Scholar
Levy, Robert 1984 Emotion, Knowing, and Culture. In Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion, edited by Shweder, Richard A., and LeVine, Robert A., pp. 214237. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Lock, Margaret, and Farquhar, Judith (editors) 2007 Beyond the Body Proper: Reading the Anthropology of Material Life. Duke University Press, Durham.Google Scholar
Looper, Matthew G. 1991 The Dances of the Classic Maya Deities: Chak and Hun Nal Ye. Master's thesis, Department of Art and Art History, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Looper, Matthew G. 2001 Dance Performances at Quirigua. In Landscape and Power in Ancient Mesoamerica, edited by Koontz, Rex, pp. 113135. Westview Press, Boulder, Colorado.Google Scholar
Looper, Matthew G. 2008 Stylistic Analysis of Dancing Maize Gods on Polychrome Ceramics. Glyph Dwellers No. 25, https://nas.ucdavis.edu/sites/nas.ucdavis.edu/files/attachments/R25.pdf, accessed May 18, 2015.Google Scholar
Looper, Matthew G. 2009 To Be Like Gods: Dance in Ancient Maya Civilization. University of Texas Press, Austin.Google Scholar
Lourens, Tino, Van Berkel, Roos, and Barakova, Emilia 2010 Communicating Emotions and Mental States to Robots in a Real Time Parallel Framework Using Laban Movement Analysis. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 58:12561265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McColl, Derek, Hong, Alexander, Hatakeyama, Naoaki, Nejat, Goldie, and Benhabib, Beno 2016 A Survey of Autonomous Human Affect Detection Methods for Social Robots Engaged in Natural HRI. Journal of Intelligent & Robotic Systems 82:101133.Google Scholar
McNeill, David 1992 Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Macri, Martha, and Looper, Matthew 2003 Nahua in Ancient Mesoamerica: Evidence from Maya Inscriptions. Ancient Mesoamerica 14:285297.Google Scholar
Martin, Simon 2002 The Baby Jaguar: An Exploration of its Identity and Origins in Maya Art and Writing. In La organización social entre los mayas prehispánicos, coloniales y modernos. Memoria de la Tercera Mesa Redonda de Palenque, edited by Tiesler Blos, Vera, Cobos, Rafael, and Green Robertson, Merle, pp. 4978. Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia, Mexico.Google Scholar
Martin, Simon 2006 Cacao in Ancient Maya Religion: First Fruit from the Maize Tree and other Tales from the Underworld. In Chocolate in Mesoamerica: A Cultural History of Cacao, edited by McNeil, Cameron L., pp. 154183. University Press of Florida, Gainsville.Google Scholar
Miettinen, Jukka O. 1992 Classical Dance and Theatre in South-East Asia. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Miller, Virginia Elizabeth 1981 Pose and Gesture in Classic Maya Monumental Sculpture. PhD dissertation, Department of Art History, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Naerebout, F.G. 1997 Attractive Performances: Ancient Greek Dance: Three Preliminary Studies. J.C. Gieben, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Ness, Sally Ann 1992 Body, Movement, and Culture: Kinesthetic and Visual Symbolism in a Philippine Community. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Ness, Sally Ann 2004 Being a Body in a Cultural Way: Understanding the Cultural in the Embodiment of Dance. In Cultural Bodies: Ethnography and Theory, edited by Thomas, Helen and Ahmed, Jamilah, pp. 121144. Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Oxford.Google Scholar
Ness, Sally Ann 2008 The Inscriptions of Gesture: Inward Migrations in Dance. In Migrations of Gesture, edited by Noland, Carrie and Ann Ness, Sally, pp. 130. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis.Google Scholar
Palka, Joel W. 2002 Left/Right Symbolism and the Body in Ancient Maya Iconography and Culture. Latin American Antiquity 13:419443.Google Scholar
Peirce, Charles Sanders 1992 [1894]The Essential Peirce: Selected Philosophical Writings. 2 vols. Indiana University Press, Bloomington.Google Scholar
Peirce, Charles Sanders 1998 The Essential Peirce: Selected Philosophical Writings: Vol. 2 (1893–1913). Indiana University Press. Bloomington.Google Scholar
Preucel, Robert W. 2006 Archaeological Semiotics. Blackwell Publishing, Malden, Massachusetts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preucel, Robert W., and Bauer, Alexander A. 2001 Archaeological Pragmatics. Norwegian Archaeological Review 34:8596.Google Scholar
Proskouriakoff, Tatiana 1950 A Study of Classic Maya Sculpture, Publication No. 593. Carnegie Institution of Washington, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Reents-Budet, Dorie 1994 Painting the Maya Universe: Royal Ceramics of the Classic Period. Duke University Press, Durham, North Carolina.Google Scholar
Reents-Budet, Dorie 1998 Elite Maya Pottery and Artisans as Social Indicators. In Craft and Social Identity, edited by Costin, Cathy and Wright, Rita, pp. 7189. Archaeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association 8, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Reents-Budet, Dorie, Foias, Antonia, Bishop, Ronald L., Blackman, James, and Guenter, Stanley 2007 Interacciones políticas y el sitio Ik’ (Motul de San José): Datos de la cerámica. In XX Simposios de Investigaciones Arqueológicas en Guatemala, edited by Pedro Laporte, Juan, Arroyo, Bárbara, Mejía, Héctor E., pp. 11411160. Instituto de Antropología e Historia de Guatemala, Asociación Tikal, Guatemala City.Google Scholar
Rice, Prudence 2009 Late Classic Maya Pottery Production: Review and Synthesis. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 16:117156.Google Scholar
Robicsek, Francis, and Hales, Donald M. 1981 The Maya Book of the Dead: The Ceramic Codex. University of Virginia Art Museum, Charlottesville.Google Scholar
Sahagún, Fray Bernardino de 1950–1982 [1575–1578] Florentine Codex: General History of the Things of New Spain. 11 vols. Translated by Anderson, Arthur J.O. and Dibble, Charles E.. School of American Research and the University of Utah, Santa Fe, New Mexico.Google Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de 1966 [1916]Course in General Linguistics, edited by Bally, Charles and Sechehaye, Albert. Translated by Baskin, Wade. McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
Schapiro, Meyer 1973 Words and Pictures: On the Literal and the Symbolic in the Illustration of a Text. Mouton, The Hague.Google Scholar
Schele, Linda, and Miller, Mary Ellen 1986 The Blood of Kings: Dynasty and Ritual in Maya Art. Kimbell Art Museum, Fort Worth, Texas.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, Priya 2007 The Bodies Beneath the Smoke or What's Behind the Cigarette Poster: Unearthing Kinesthetic Connections in American Dance History. Discourses in Dance 4:748.Google Scholar
Stone, Andrea J. 1995 Images from the Underworld. Naj Tunich and the Tradition of Maya Cave Painting. University of Texas Press, Austin.Google Scholar
Stone, Andrea, and Zender, Marc 2011 Reading Maya Art: A Hieroglyphic Guide to Ancient Maya Painting and Sculpture. Thames & Hudson, New York.Google Scholar
Stross, Brian 1992 Maize and Blood: Mesoamerican Symbolism on an Olmec Vase and a Maya Plate. RES: Anthropology and Aesthetics 22:82107.Google Scholar
Stuart, David 2012 Maya Spooks. Maya Decipherment: Ideas on Ancient Maya Writing and Iconography. https://decipherment.wordpress.com/2012/10/26/maya-spooks/, accessed April 17, 2016.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 1985 The Classic Maya Maize God: A Reappraisal. In Fifth Palenque Round Table, 1983, edited by Fields, Virginia M., pp. 171181. Pre-Columbian Art Research Institute, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 1989 Ritual Humor in Classic Maya Religion. In Word And Image in Maya Culture: Explorations in Language, Writing, And Culture, edited by Hanks, William F. and Rice, Don S., pp. 351382. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 1994 The Birth Vase: Natal Imagery in Ancient Maya Myth. In The Maya Vase Book, Vol. 4, edited by Kerr, Justin, pp. 652685. Kerr Associates, New York.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 1996 The Olmec Maize God: The Face of Corn in Formative Mesamerica. RES 29 (30):3981.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 1998 The Jade Hearth: Centrality, Rulership, and the Classic Maya Temple. In Function and Meaning in Classic Maya Architecture, edited by Houston, Stephen D., pp. 427478. Dumbarton Oaks, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 2001 Dance. In The Oxford Encyclopedia of Mesoamerican Cultures, edited by Carrasco, David, pp. 305308. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 2003 Ancient and Contemporary Maya Conceptions about Field and Forest. In The Lowland Maya Area: Three Millennia at the Human-Wildland Interface, edited by Gomez-Pompa, Arturo, Fedick, Scott L., and Jimenez-Osornio, Juan J.. Food Products Press, New York.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl 2009 The Maya Maize God and the Mythic Origins of Dance. In: The Maya and their Sacred Narratives: Text and Context in Maya Mythologies, Proceedings of the 12th European Maya Conference, edited by Le Fort, Geneviève, Gardiol, Raphaël, Matteo, Sebastian, and Helmke, Christophe, pp. 4152. Acta Mesoamericana 20, Anton Saurwein Publishing, Markt Schwaben, Germany.Google Scholar
Taube, Karl A., Saturno, William A., Stuart, David, and Hurst, Heather 2010 The Murals of San Bartolo, El Petén, Guatemala, Part 2: The West Wall. Ancient America 10. Boundary End Archaeology Research Center, Barnardsville, North Carolina.Google Scholar
Taube, Rhonda 2014 The History of the Dance in Mesoamerica. In The Maestro and the Dance, edited by Markman, Peter, pp. 104129. Xipe Projects: Latin American Masks and Popular Art. Huntington Beach, California.Google Scholar
Tiesler, Vera 2014 The Bioarchaeology of Artificial Cranial Modifications. New Approaches to Head Shaping and its Meanings in Pre-Columbian Mesoamerica and Beyond. Springer, New York.Google Scholar
Tokovinine, Alexandre 2003 A Classic Maya Term for Public Performance. Mesoweb: www.mesoweb.com/features/tokovinine/Performance.pdf, accessed August 13, 2015.Google Scholar
Werness-Rude, Maline D., and Spencer, Kaylee R. 2015 Imagery, Architecture, and Activity in the Maya World: An Introduction. In Maya Imagery, Architecture, and Activity: Space and Spatial Analysis in Art History, edited by Werness-Rude, Maline D., and Spencer, Kaylee R., pp. 1105. University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque.Google Scholar
Wichmann, Soren and Nielsen, Jesper 2016 Sequential Text-Image Pairing among the Classic Maya. In The Visual Narrative Reader, edited by Cohn, Neil, pp. 282313. Bloomsbury, London.Google Scholar
Zacharatos, Haris, Gatzoulis, Christos, and Chrysanthou, Yiorgos L. 2014 Automatic Emotion Recognition Based on Body Movement Analysis: A Survey. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications 34 (6):3545.Google Scholar
Zender, Marc 2004 A Study of Classic Maya Priesthood. PhD dissertation, Department of Archaeology, University of Calgary, Canada.Google Scholar
Zhang, Jiaming, and Sharkey, Amanda 2011 Contextual Recognition of Robot Emotions. Towards Autonomous Robotic Systems, edited by Groß, Roderich, Alboul, Lyuba, Melhuish, Chris, Witkowski, Mark, Prescott, Tony J., and Penders, Jacques, pp. 7889. Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Example of Tikal Dancer Plate. Excavated from Uaxactún Burial a3 (painting by M. Louise Baker, courtesy of the University of Pennsylvania Museum [Image 165116]).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Octahedral Movement Constellation (illustration by Timothy Turner).

Figure 2

Figure 3. Maize God sculpture from Copan (note the peripheral spatial tension and controlled gestures). Drawing by Linda Schele, reproduced courtesy of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art [SD-3518].

Figure 3

Figure 4. Supernatural scribe forming “Gesture 14” (drawing by Jorge Pérez de Lara and Mark Van Stone).

Figure 4

Figure 5. Scene of drunkenness from K1092 (drawing by Emily Davis-Hale).

Figure 5

Figure 6. Itzamnaaj using “Gesture 1” as an apparent greeting in a supernatural palace scene. Such controlled gestures serve as indexical markers of proprioceptive refinement among both gods and elites (drawing by Jorge Pérez de Lara and Mark Van Stone, in Ancona-Ha et al. 2000:1075).

Figure 6

Figure 7. Dancing wahy being. Note the downward focus, Light Weight, and Free Flow efforts. Detail from the Altar de Sacrificios vase. Museo Nacional de Arqueología y Etnología, Guatemala City. Excavated from Burial 96 of Structure A-III (Adams 1971. Drawing by Linda Schele, courtesy of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art [SD-5504].