Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T12:51:56.400Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Polygenic risk and hostile environments: Links to stable and dynamic antisocial behaviors across adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2024

E. L. Acland*
Affiliation:
School of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada Research Centre, Sainte-Justine University Hospital, Montreal, QC, Canada Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
N. Pocuca
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
S. Paquin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
M. Boivin
Affiliation:
Ecole de Psychologie, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
I. Ouellet-Morin
Affiliation:
School of Criminology, Université de Montréal & Research Center of the Montreal Mental Health University Institute, Montreal, QC, Canada
T. F. M. Andlauer
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Klinikum rechts der Isar, School of Medicine, Technical University of Munich, Munchen, Germany
J. P. Gouin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada
S. M. Côté
Affiliation:
Research Centre, Sainte-Justine University Hospital, Montreal, QC, Canada School of Public Health, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
R. E. Tremblay
Affiliation:
Départements de Pédiatrie et de Psychologie, Université de Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
M. Geoffroy
Affiliation:
Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
N. Castellanos-Ryan
Affiliation:
School of Psychoeducation, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada Research Centre, Sainte-Justine University Hospital, Montreal, QC, Canada
*
Corresponding author: E. L. Acland; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Adverse environments are linked to elevated youth antisocial behavior. However, this relation is thought to depend, in part, on genetic susceptibility. The present study investigated whether polygenic risk for antisociality moderates relations between hostile environments and stable as well as dynamic antisocial behaviors across adolescence. We derived two antisocial-linked polygenic risk scores (PRS) (N = 721) based on previous genome-wide association studies. Forms of antisocial behavior (nonaggressive conduct problems, physical aggression, social aggression) and environmental hostility (harsh parenting and school violence) were assessed at age 13, 15, and 17 years. Relations to individual differences stable across adolescence (latent stability) vs. time-specific states (timepoint residual variance) of antisocial behavior were assessed via structural equation models. Higher antisocial PRS, harsh parenting, and school violence were linked to stable elevations in antisocial behaviors across adolescence. We identified a consistent polygenic-environment interaction suggestive of differential susceptibility in late adolescence. At age 17, harsher parenting was linked to higher social aggression in those with higher antisocial PRS, and lower social aggression in those with lower antisocial PRS. This suggests that genetics and environmental hostility relate to stable youth antisocial behaviors, and that genetic susceptibility moderates home environment-antisocial associations specifically in late adolescence.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Antisociality is a cluster of traits and behaviors centered around disregarding, harming, and violating the rights of others. The expression of antisocial behaviors is heterogenous; there is substantive variation in severity, form, time of onset, and persistence (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Dotterer and Hyde2019). Antisocial behaviors are theorized to fall into three etiologically and developmentally distinct, but related, types: physical aggression (acts or threats of bodily harm), social aggression (harming others via relationship or reputational damage), and nonaggressive conduct problems (rule-breaking, lying; Burt et al., Reference Burt, Donnellan and Tackett2012; Burt, Reference Burt2012; Reference Burt2013). Forms of antisocial behavior can be moderately to strongly linked to one another (Acland et al., Reference Acland, Jambon and Malti2021; Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Scott Huebner and Tian2023; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Sameroff, Lansford, Sexton, Davis-Kean, Bates, Pettit and Dodge2013). Yet, the expression of these behaviors can fluctuate asynchronously over time, which may be brought about by distinct environmental and genetic influences, especially upon entering adolescence (Karriker-Jaffe et al., Reference Karriker-Jaffe, Foshee, Ennett and Suchindran2013; Niv et al., Reference Niv, Tuvblad, Raine and Baker2013). The nuances of these interactive processes are not well-understood, and are thus, the focus of the present study.

The development of antisocial behaviors

Antisocial behaviors seldom begin in adulthood (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Patterson, Briley, Engelhardt, Kretsch, Mann, Tackett and Tucker-Drob2015; Moffitt, Reference Moffitt2018). Physical aggression peaks in early childhood, while nonaggressive conduct problems peak in adolescence (Côté et al., Reference Côté, Vaillancourt, LeBlanc, Nagin and Tremblay2006; Givens & Reid, Reference Givens and Reid2019; Niv et al., Reference Niv, Tuvblad, Raine and Baker2013; Tremblay et al., Reference Tremblay, Vitaro and Côté2018; Zheng & Cleveland, Reference Zheng and Cleveland2013). Both decline with age after their peak, emphasizing the importance of studying the development of antisocial behaviors pre-adulthood. Twin studies indicate that physical, stable aggression is particularly heritable (∼65%), more so than nonaggressive conduct problems (Burt, Reference Burt2012; Lacourse et al., Reference Lacourse, Boivin, Brendgen, Petitclerc, Girard, Vitaro, Paquin, Ouellet-Morin, Dionne and Tremblay2014). Nonaggressive antisocial behaviors (e.g., rule-breaking, stealing, truancy) are found to be more dynamic, more affected by shared environmental factors, and exhibit less genetic stability (Burt, Reference Burt2012; Eley et al., Reference Eley, Lichtenstein and Moffitt2003).

Distinctions between aggression and rule-breaking may be best understood in adolescence as the unique influencers of each behavior emerge more consistently post-puberty (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Patterson, Briley, Engelhardt, Kretsch, Mann, Tackett and Tucker-Drob2015; Niv et al., Reference Niv, Tuvblad, Raine and Baker2013). For instance, the effect of family-level environmental factors on aggression and rule-breaking becomes increasingly differentiated when children enter adolescence (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Patterson, Briley, Engelhardt, Kretsch, Mann, Tackett and Tucker-Drob2015). Additionally, some genetic influences on antisocial behavior are not observed until after puberty and the influence of genetics on nonaggressive conduct problems increases from childhood to adolescence (Burt & Neiderhiser, Reference Burt and Neiderhiser2009; Harden et al., Reference Harden, Patterson, Briley, Engelhardt, Kretsch, Mann, Tackett and Tucker-Drob2015; Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Prescott and Kendler2002; Niv et al., Reference Niv, Tuvblad, Raine and Baker2013). In sum, research suggests that differing forms of antisocial behavior have distinct etiological and developmental pathways, and that adolescence is a key period for understanding these differences.

Hostile environments and antisocial behavior

Exposure to hostile environments is an established risk factor for antisocial behavior (Bacchini et al., Reference Bacchini, Concetta Miranda and Affuso2011; Estrada & Baskin-Sommers, Reference Estrada and Baskin-Sommers2023). Namely, parent-child coercion and peer antisociality are identified as two critical microsystem factors for the maintenance and escalation of antisocial problems (Dishion & Patterson, Reference Dishion and Patterson2016). Harsh parenting styles are linked to increased aggression and antisocial behaviors more generally over childhood and adolescence (Côté et al., Reference Côté, Vaillancourt, LeBlanc, Nagin and Tremblay2006; Compton et al., Reference Compton, Snyder, Schrepferman, Bank and Shortt2003; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Simons and Simons2012; Girard et al., Reference Girard, Tremblay, Nagin and Côté2019; Kawabata et al., Reference Kawabata, Alink, Tseng, van IJzendoorn and Crick2011; Li et al., Reference Li, Putallaz and Su2011). Similarly, research suggests that being a victim of family violence and witnessing violence at school independently relate to increased antisocial behavior over adolescence (Bacchini et al., Reference Bacchini, Affuso and Aquilar2015; Calvete & Orue, Reference Calvete and Orue2011; O’Keefe, Reference O’Keefe1997). Direct contact with violence may not even be necessary, as fear evoked from living in violent neighborhoods is linked to poorer mental health in youth even when personal exposure is controlled (Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Silva, Jakaite, Krenzinger, Valiati, Gonçalves, Ribeiro, Heritage and Priebe2021). Social proximity to antisocial behaviors at school has been associated to their amplification and spread (Dimant, Reference Dimant2019; Kalvin & Bierman, Reference Kalvin and Bierman2017; Kornienko et al., Reference Kornienko, Dishion and Ha2018). Thus, sharing environments with hostile actors, whether that be parents or peers, can lead to distress in youth and the mirroring of those behaviors.

Together this suggests that both school and home environmental hostility are important etiological factors of antisocial behavior. However, adolescents who experience harsh parenting or report having violent peers at school do not necessarily go on to display elevated antisocial behavior. Such variability in the face of these criminogenic environments point to possible individual-level factors that may exacerbate or buffer the associations between environments and antisocial behavior. A key factor that may lead youth to be susceptible to environmental adversity is genetic risk for antisocial behavior.

Polygenic-environment interactions

Heritability of behavioral phenotypes is highly polygenic, and genome wide association studies (GWAS) are increasingly used to identify the polygenic contribution (via single nucleotide polymorphisms; SNPs) to antisocial behavior (Gard et al., Reference Gard, Dotterer and Hyde2019; Pappa et al., Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; Tielbeek et al., Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022). These genetic variation candidates can be used to create composite scores (i.e., polygenic risk scores; PRS) that represent individuals’ continuously distributed genetic risk for a particular trait. These derived PRS allow for the assessment of polygenic-environment interactions. For instance, a higher PRS for aggression was found to be related to greater lifetime incarceration risk, however, this effect was substantially reduced if at least one parent graduated high school (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Liu, Motz, Tanksley, Kail, Beckley, Belsky, Domingue, Moffitt, Pratt and Wertz2019). Similarly, children with low family instability combined with low polygenic risk for aggression had steeper declines in aggression from ages 7 to 14 (Womack et al., Reference Womack, Clifford, Wilson, Shaw and Lemery-Chalfant2021). A higher PRS for externalizing problems was also linked to increased adolescent externalizing symptoms, especially when parental monitoring was low (Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Aliev, Bucholz, Agrawal, Hesselbrock, Hesselbrock, Bauer, Kuperman, Schuckit, Kramer, Edenberg, Foroud and Dick2015). In somewhat contrast to these other studies that show more genetic and environment risk predicts greater antisociality, Musci et al. (Reference Musci, Bettencourt, Sisto, Maher, Masyn and Ialongo2019) found that middle schoolers with a lower PRS for conduct problems were actually more likely to engage in impulsive, aggressive behaviors if exposed to community violence. This research demonstrates that polygenic risk for antisociality can moderate links between environmental factors and the development of antisocial behaviors. Thus, how genetic liability moderates environmental-antisocial relations may depend on type and feature of the environment measured (e.g., home vs. outside home). To our knowledge, there has been no investigation that has attempted to disentangle these processes by assessing polygenic-environment interactions using multiple antisocial-linked PRS, environment contexts, forms of antisocial behavior, and whether relations affect stable vs. unstable behaviors with longitudinal data.

Present study

Using a sample followed longitudinally, this study examined whether genetic liability for antisociality moderated the relations between home and school environmental hostility and antisocial behaviors across adolescence. Specifically, we assessed (1) how genetic susceptibility (via two antisocial-linked PRS) moderated relations between harsh parenting and school violence and antisocial behaviors, (2) whether this moderation operated differently for aggressive versus nonaggressive and socially vs. physically aggressive forms of antisocial behavior, and (3) whether interactions differed between antisocial behaviors that are stable across adolescence vs. behaviors that fluctuate between ages.

Based on past research suggesting that physical aggression, nonaggressive conduct problems, and social aggression are distinct constructs with differing developmental trajectories and etiologies, we expected our model to have better fit when forms of antisocial behavior were modeled as separate, but covarying constructs, as opposed to modeling them as one general factor of antisocial behavior. Physical, stable aggression is estimated to be highly heritable, whereas nonaggressive conduct problems have lesser heritable components, which increase over development (Burt et al., Reference Burt, Rescorla, Achenbach, Ivanova, Almqvist, Begovac, Bilenberg, Bird, Chahed, Dobrean, Döpfner, Erol, Hannesdottir, Kanbayashi, Lambert, Leung, Minaei, Novik, Oh, Petot, Petot, Pomalima, Rudan, Sawyer, Simsek, Steinhausen, Valverde, van der Ende, Weintraub, Metzke, Wolanczyk, Zhang, Zukauskiene and Verhulst2015; Burt, Reference Burt2012). Nonaggressive problems are less stable across time and more influenced by environmental factors, however, environment still plays a role in physical aggression (Burt, Reference Burt2012).

Thus, we hypothesized that greater levels of home and school environment hostility would link to (1) higher stable physical aggression, but only in genetically susceptible youth, and (2) higher stable and dynamic nonaggressive problems. Social aggression was included to assess whether any relations found were unique to physical aggression, as opposed to all forms of aggression; we expected that the antisocial PRS would be specifically linked to stable physical aggression, as opposed to social aggression. Additionally, the strength of associations between PRS and antisociality may depend on sex, thus we explored whether relations differed between sexes (Tielbeek et al., Reference Tielbeek, Johansson, Polderman, Rautiainen, Jansen, Taylor, Tong, Lu, Burt, Tiemeier, Viding, Plomin, Martin, Heath, Madden, Montgomery, Beaver, Waldman, Gelernter, Kranzler, Farrer, Perry, Munafò, LoParo, Paunio, Tiihonen, Mous, Pappa, de Leeuw, Watanabe, Hammerschlag, Salvatore, Aliev, Bigdeli, Dick, Faraone, Popma, Medland and Posthuma2017). Sex and income were assessed as control variables given their established relations to antisocial behavior (Côté et al., Reference Côté, Vaillancourt, LeBlanc, Nagin and Tremblay2006).

Method

Participants

Data came from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (QLSCD): a well-established, population-based, longitudinal birth cohort in Canada (conducted by Institut de la Statistique du Québec) that began in 1997/98 (N = 2120). Participants were recruited from the Quebec Birth Registry via a stratified procedure based on location and birth rate. Data collection is ongoing and has been collected every one or two years from birth into adulthood. Further detail about the cohort can be found online (https://www.jesuisjeserai.stat.gouv.qc.ca/) and in the cohort profile (Orri et al., Reference Orri, Boivin, Chen, Ahun, Geoffroy, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay and Côté2021). In the present study, we used data from when participants were 13, 15, and 17 years of age. A subset of the QLSCD sample reporting European ancestry was genotyped (n = 951), resulting in a final sample size of n = 721 for the present study after genetic quality control (see missing data section for genetic subsample information).

Procedure

The QLSCD protocol was approved by the Institut de la statistique du Québec and the St-Justine Hospital Research Centre ethics committees, and informed assent and consent was acquired at each data collection. Interviewer Completed Computerized Questionnaires (ICCQ) were carried out mainly by phone by a researcher interviewing the person most knowledgeable about the child (the mother in more than 98% of cases) to collect sociodemographic and parenting information. At age 10, blood and saliva samples were collected (n = 951) and DNA extraction was performed as soon as the sample were received at the lab (∼ second day after sample draw).

Measures

Polygenic risk scores

Two GWAS on antisocial behavior (ASB) were used to derive PRS in the present study as participant characteristics (e.g., age), measures used (e.g., informant of antisocial behavior) and sample size of the original GWAS can affect the PRS’s predictive value. One ASB-PRS was derived from the Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016) GWAS, which included participants relatively close in age to the present sample (i.e., children and early adolescents; N = 18,988). Measures were caregiver-reported and included items that tapped into different facets of antisocial traits and behaviors (e.g., items used such as “Mean to others,” “Disobedient,” “Selfish,” “Attacks people,” “Often lies or cheats”). The second ASB-PRS was derived from the Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022) GWAS, which included largely adults, but had a larger sample (N = 85,359). It also included a range of antisocial measures, many of which were self-reported, as used in the present study. These scores are referred to ASB-PRSc (Pappa et al., Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; child sample GWAS) and ASB-PRSa (Tielbeek et al., Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022; adult sample GWAS).

DNA in the QLSCD sample was genotyped using the Illumina Infinium PsychArray-24 and the 1000 Genomes Phase 3 reference panel was used for quality control and imputation. After quality control and imputation, 721 individuals and several million genetic variants remained (see Appendix S1 for more details). The PRS was calculated using PRS-CS (see Ge et al., Reference Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng and Smoller2019). Here, the global shrinkage parameter phi was fixed to 0.01, as suggested for highly polygenic traits (Ge et al., Reference Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng and Smoller2019). Polygenic scores were then calculated using imputed dosage data in PLINK 1.9 using the PRS-CS adjusted summary statistics. PRS were transformed into standardized scores in main analyses.

To account for population stratification and outliers, 20 genetic ancestry principal components were included as covariates in the models. After controlling for the false discovery rate, none of the 20 components were significantly (p < .05) related to antisocial behaviors. Inclusion of ancestry components in the models did not substantially alter the estimates of the main predictor variables.

School violence

At age 13, 15, and 17 years, youth reported on violence that took place in their school environment using the Major Perceived Violence 3-item subscale (Janosz et al., Reference Janosz, Archambault, Pagani, Pascal, Morin and Bowen2008). Participants were asked to report how often they observed or have been informed of the following behaviors occurring at school during the present school year: “Students being physically assaulted by other students (beaten up, punched, kicked),” “Students who intimidate (threaten) adults at the school,” and “Students who physically attack adults at the school.” Participants rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = Never to 5 = Almost every day. Scale reliability was largely acceptable: average inter-item r = 0.35, 0.53, 0.49 for age 13, 15, 17, respectivelyFootnote 1 . In principal component factor analyses, all items loaded onto a single factor (loadings all > 0.7 at each age), suggesting all items tap into the same dimension. For main analyses, all items were standardized then averaged together.

Harsh parenting

At age 13, 15, and 17, the person most knowledgeable about the child was interviewed about their parenting behavior using a 4-item harsh parenting subscale assessing the frequency of their violent and harsh interactions with their child. Caregivers were asked (over the last 12 months): “How often did you hit (child’s name) when (child’s name) was difficult?” and “How often did you get angry when you punished (child’s name)?”; when (child’s name) broke the rules or did things that (child’s name) was not supposed to “how often did you raise your voice, scold or yell at (child’s name)?” and “how often did you use physical punishment?”. The first item was rated on a Likert scale from 1 = Never to 7 = Several times a day, and the latter three items were rated from 1 = Never to 5 = All the time/Always. Scale reliability was acceptable: average inter-item r = 0.30, 0.25, 0.20 for age 13, 15, 17, respectively. Principal component analyses conducted at each time point suggested items loaded onto two dimensions: (1) harsh parenting (all loadings > 0.4, except for the two violent items at age 17 which were > 0.3) and (2) violent parenting (> 0.5 loadings for physically violent items, < −0.2 loadings for the remaining items). This suggests that all items tap into a common harsh parenting factor, and that corporal punishment items also tap into a dimension specific to violent parenting. Since we are more interested in harsh parenting, these four items were standardized and then averaged together.

Antisocial behavior

At age 13, youth self-reported their nonaggressive conduct problems (stealing, lying, cheating, rule-breaking; 6 items, α = 0.70), physical aggression (physically harming or threatening to harm others; 8 items, α = 0.83), and social aggression (emotional harming others, damaging relationships/reputation; 4 items, mean inter-item r = 0.37) during the last six months via the Social Behavior Questionnaire (SBQ; Collet et al., Reference Collet, Orri, Tremblay, Boivin and Côté2023)Footnote 2 .

At age 15 and 17, youth completed the Mental Health and Social Inadaptation Assessment for Adolescents (MIA; Côté et al., Reference Côté, Orri, Brendgen, Vitaro, Boivin, Japel, Séguin, Geoffroy, Rouquette, Falissard and Tremblay2017). The MIA is a 113-item scale aimed at measuring DSM-5 symptoms of internalizing and externalizing disorders over the last 12 months. Items pertaining to nonaggressive conduct problems (14 items, α = 0.84, 0.79 for ages 15 and 17, respectively), physical aggression (12 items, α = 0.85, 0.87 for ages 15 and 17, respectively), and social aggression (5 items, mean inter-item r = 0.31, 0.26 for ages 15 and 17, respectively) were used to create subscales. For both the SBQ and MIA, youth were asked to rate each item on a 3-point Likert scale from 1 = Never/not true to 3 = Often/very true. For main analyses, all items were standardized then averaged together. To support the assumption that items from the SBQ tap into the same antisocial constructs assessed in the MIA, several steps were taken (see Appendix S2 and S3). Whether forms of antisocial behaviors were best modeled as distinct or as a general antisocial factor was also assessed (see Analytic Plan section).

Control variables

Sex and income were included as covariates to control for their potentially confounding influence on antisocial behavior. Sex was parent-reported at birth and was coded as female = 0, male = 1. Income was parent-reported at age 13, 15, and 17 via the ICCQ on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = less than $10,000 to 9 = $100,000 or more annually. Income items were standardized, and across timepoints had a mean inter-item r = .77 and loaded > 0.9 onto one common factor via a principal component factor analysis. Parental education was evaluated as a potential control variable; however, it was not significantly related to any antisocial outcome when income was controlled, and thus was not included in the analytical models. Both household income and the mother’s education at birth were used to evaluate potential biases in the selected genetic subsample and sample attrition.

Data analyses

Descriptive statistics and zero-order Spearman correlations were conducted on all study variables. Correlations between ASB-PRS and environmental factors were estimated to assess polygenic-environment associations (rGE), a known confounder for assessing genetic and environmental relations to phenotypes. The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were not significantly correlated. Model specification sensitivity testing was performed, whereby the ASB-PRSa was removed from models to assess its influence on ASB-PRSc (and vice versa). Removing each ASB-PRS did not substantially change the other’s p-values for main findings. In concert with their low correlation with one another, this supports that these PRS have independent contributions to outcomes and, therefore, were included in models together.

Path models were then created using Mplus (v. 1.8.8) to address main research questions. First, to assess whether antisocial behaviors were best conceptualized as distinct forms or one uniform indicator of antisocial behavior, a model with one general antisocial factor (all antisocial scales included) was compared to a three-factor model (nonaggressive conduct, physical aggression, social aggression modeled separately). Stable forms of antisocial behavior were assessed as latent variables across timepoints, e.g., age 13, 15, and 17 physical aggression were regressed on a latent variable representing stable physical aggression. Forms of antisocial behavior were allowed to covary within each timepoint, e.g., age 13 physical aggression, age 13 social aggression, and age 13 nonaggression conduct covaried. Models were compared using the Satorra-Bentler Scaled Chi-Square Difference Test (Satorra & Bentler, Reference Satorra and Bentler2010). To assess model fit, SRMR < .08, RSMEA < .06, CFI > .95 values were used to indicate models with acceptable fit (Hooper et al., Reference Hooper, Coughlan and Mullen2007). To adjust for the increased risk of a false positive due to multiple comparisons, within each model we adjusted all p-values representing links between independent and dependent variables using the Benjamini and Hochberg (Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) method via the R package stats (R Core Team, 2021).

Model 1: links to stable antisocial behaviors

The retained latent path model was then extended to examine how environmental, genetic, and control variables were associated with stable antisocial behaviors across adolescence (see Fig. 1 for illustration of model). Income, school violence, and harsh parenting were averaged across timepoints to assess level of exposure to these environmental factors across adolescence. To assess how the ASB-PRS moderated associations between home and school environment hostility and stable antisocial behaviors, four interaction terms were created by multiplying the relevant centered variables (e.g. ASB-PRSc*school violence). Exogenous (independent) variables were allowed to covary. Latent antisocial variables were regressed on all independent variables. Models 1 and 2 were run in two steps: first with all controls and main predictors, then again but with interaction terms added. Lastly, multigroup path analyses were performed to assess whether sex moderated associations.

Figure 1. Model 1: risk factors predicting stable antisocial behaviors. Illustrates model 1 which assessed how genetic and environmental factors relate to forms of antisocial behavior stable across adolescence. The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; child sample GWAS) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022; adult sample GWAS), respectively. ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score; HP = harsh parenting; SV = school violence; PA = physical aggression; SA = social aggression; NC = nonaggressive conduct problems; T1 = age 13; T2 = age 15; T3 = age 17.

Models 2a,b,c: links to dynamic antisocial behaviors

Next, latent path models were respecified to assess which factors were associated with time-specific antisocial behaviors across adolescence (see Fig. 2 for example model). To assess dynamic antisocial behaviors, the model design was the same as model 1 except that variance not explained by stable antisocial behaviors were the dependent variables (i.e., latent variable residuals). Three models were created, one for each timepoint (models 2a, b, c for age 13, 15, and 17, respectively). Predictors were matched for each timepoint, e.g., predictors assessed at age 15 were included in the residual model for antisocial behaviors specific to age 15 (model 2b; see Appendix S4 and S5 for Mplus syntax of models).

Figure 2. Model 2a: risk factors predicting antisocial behaviors specific (residual) to age 13. Illustrates model 2a which assessed how genetic and environmental factors relate forms of antisocial behavior that were specific to T1, i.e., antisocial variance at age 13 unexplained by stable behaviors. The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; child sample GWAS) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022; adult sample GWAS), respectively. ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score; HP = harsh parenting; SV = school violence; PA = physical aggression; SA = social aggression; NC = nonaggressive conduct problems; T1 = age 13; T2 = age 15; T3 = age 17.

Missing data

Participants that did not have genetic data available were significantly more likely to be male (r = .10, p < .001), have lower household income at birth (r = −.08, p < .001), age 13 (r = −.08, p = .004) and 15 (r = −.06, p = .025), and have mothers with less education at birth (r = −.07, p < .001). Within the subsample with genetic data, the attrition rate was 3% (n = 18) i.e., no data collected during adolescence. Each study variable had between 7% (n = 57; age 15 antisocial behavior) to 19% (149; age 17 school violence) missing data, with the exception of sex (complete data). At-birth household income and mother’s education were not significantly related to adolescent attrition within the genetic subsample. Observed values for sex and all adolescent variables were significantly associated with data missingness for at least one time point; thus, full information maximum likelihood was used to handle missing data. Specifically, the maximum likelihood with robust standard errors estimator was used in all models, as research indicates this estimator can accommodate severe departure from normality as long as models have good fit indices and a sample size over 250 (Lai, Reference Lai2018). All predictors/covariates except ASB-PRS (not related to data missingness) were specified to estimate missing data (Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). Models were specified to include all participants that had genetic data available (n = 721).

Results

Descriptive statistics

The means, SDs, ranges, and zero-order correlations for all study variables are provided in Tables 1 and 2 (see Table S1 for stability correlations of environmental predictors). Income and harsh parenting were strongly correlated between timepoints, while school violence was modestly correlated between timepoints. This indicates that school violence fluctuates over adolescence, while harsh parenting and income have greater stability. In the zero-order correlations, being reported as male at birth was associated with greater physical aggression at all timepoints and lower social aggression at age 13. Lower income, harsher parenting, and more reported school violence were significantly associated with higher antisocial behavior (all forms) across adolescence, with the exception of income and age 17 social aggression. Higher ASB-PRSc was associated with greater antisocial behaviors (all forms) at age 17, nonaggressive conduct problems and social aggression at age 15. ASB-PRSa was not significantly linked to any antisocial outcome. The strengths of relations between forms of antisocial behavior varied substantially across forms and timepoints (r = .07–.56). ASB-PRS were not significantly related to environmental factors, minimizing the confounding effect of underlying rGE.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics by timepoint for study variables

Note. Unstandardized study variables by timepoint. Sex, income, and harsh parenting were caregiver-reported, while school violence and all antisocial behaviors were self-reported by the participating youth. Genetic data was collected at age 10. The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022) GWAS, respectively. M = mean or median, SD = standard deviation, MAD = median absolute deviation, ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

a Antisocial behaviors were measured via the SBQ at T1 and the MIA at T2 and T3.

b Values were skewed (+/− 2): median and MAD were reported for these variables, respectively.

Table 2. Zero-order correlations for all study variables

Note. Sex, income, and harsh parenting were caregiver-reported, while school violence and all antisocial behaviors were self-reported by youth. Sex was coded as assigned at birth as male = 1 and female = 0. Spearman correlation coefficients are reported; cases were excluded using pairwise method (ns = 612 to 1537). The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the GWAS Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022), respectively. ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score, T1 = age 13, T2 = age 15, T3 = age 17, nonagg. = nonaggressive. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

a Income, harsh parenting, and school violence were averaged across T1, T2, and T3 for this table.

Latent structure of antisocial behavior across adolescence

The Likelihood Ratio Test was significant for the analysis comparing a one vs. three antisocial behavior factor model and was nonsignificant when comparing models that were allowed to vary between sexes vs. not. This suggests that models that differentiated between forms of antisocial behavior — i.e., a three-factor model: physical aggression, social aggression, nonaggressive conduct problems — and that were invariant between sexes had significantly better model fit (see Table 3). The final model’s fit indices were acceptable; thus, the three-factor model was retained for all subsequent analyses.

Table 3. Likelihood ratio tests for comparing stable antisocial behaviors models

Note. Model fit indices were compared in (1) a one (overall antisocial behavior) vs. three (social aggression, physical aggression, and nonaggressive conduct problems) latent factor model and (2) a model that constrained relations to be the same between sexes vs. a model that allowed relations to vary by sex. Model comparisons were performed using Satorra-Bentler Scaled Chi-Square Difference Tests, models with significant improvements in fit indices were bolded. The 3-factor model of antisociality and a model where latent factor and regression coefficients were not allowed to vary between sexes were supported. SRMR = standardized root mean squared residual; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CFI = comparative fit index; DF = degrees of freedom; AIC = akaike information criterion; BIC = sample-size adjusted bayesian information criterion. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Genetic and environmental links to stable antisocial behaviors

Model 1 estimates and fit indices are included in Table 4. After adjusting for false discovery rate, ASB-PRSc was significantly associated with stable nonaggressive conduct problems, while ASB-PRSa was linked to stable physical aggression. However, both scores had similar coefficient and p-values for physical aggression and nonaggressive conduct (β = .08–.11, ps = .02–.05) and each explained a similar amount of variance (∼1%) of these two antisocial behaviors. ASB-PRS did not significantly moderate any environment-stable behavior links.

Table 4. Model 1: latent path model for stable antisocial behaviors across adolescence

Note. Latent path model estimates for youths’ genetic, environmental, and stable antisociality. Genetic ancestry scores were included as covariates. Model fit indices: RMSEA = .02, CFI = .98, SRMR = .02, χ2(251, N = 721) = 295, p = .03. Bolded values indicate p < .05 after false discovery rate was adjusted. ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score, HP = harsh parenting, SV = school violence, β = standardized beta, SE = standardized standard error, CI = confidence interval. The ASB-PRSc,a were derived from the GWAS Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; child population) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022; adult population), respectively. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Several environmental and demographic associations with outcomes were found. Harsh parenting was significantly associated with higher stable levels of adolescent nonaggressive conduct problems and social aggression (ΔR 2 = .03, .02, respectively). School violence and income were positively and negatively (respectively) associated with stable physical aggression, social aggression, and nonaggressive conduct problems (school violence ΔR 2 = .11, .11, .14; income ΔR 2 = .03, .01, .02, respectively). Assigned male-at-birth was significantly positively associated with stable physical aggression (ΔR 2 = .04).

Genetic and environmental links to time-specific (residual) variance in antisocial behaviors

Models 2a-c estimates and fit indices are included in Table 5. After adjusting for multiple comparisons, ASB-PRS were not significantly associated with any form of time-specific antisocial behavior. However, interestingly both ASB-PRS showed a significant consistent polygenic-environment interaction prior to p-value adjustment (model 2c). At age 17 and in youth with lower ASB-PRS (> 0.5 SD below mean), harsher parenting was significantly associated with lower social aggression (see Figure S1 for Johnson Neyman graphs showing regions of significance). Conversely, in those with higher ASB-PRS (> 2 SDs above mean), harsher parenting was significantly associated with higher social aggression (Fig. 3; ΔR2 = .04, .04).

Figure 3. Polygenic-environment interaction in late adolescence. Plots illustrate the relations between harsh parenting (standardized) and time-specific social aggression at age 17 (i.e., residual variance) with high or low (+/− 2 SD) antisocial behavior polygenic risk scores (ASB-PRS). The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; child sample GWAS) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022; adult sample GWAS), respectively. Johnson Neyman plots (see Figure S1) indicate the negative (blue) and positive (red) relations between harsh parenting and social aggression are significant when PRS are less than approximately 0.5 SDs below and 2 SDs above the mean, respectively. Each plot was computed using a trimmed model which includes only significant predictors from model 2c; ABS-PRSc and ABS-PRSa were not included in the same model for computing these plots. SD = standard deviation. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Table 5. Models 2a,b,c: latent path model for time-specific (i.e., residual) antisocial behaviors across adolescence

Note. Latent path model estimates for youths’ genetic, environmental, and time−specific antisocial behaviors. Model fit indices: RMSEAs = .02, CFI = .97.98, SRMRs = .02, χ2(167, N = 721) = 213, 234, 229 (respectively), ps < .05. Bolded values indicate < .05 after false discovery rate was adjusted. Interactions terms were added in a second step in the model. The ASB − PRSc,a were derived from the GWAS Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016; child population) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022; adult population), respectively. Agg = aggression, ASB − PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score, HP = harsh parenting, SV = school violence, β = standardized beta, SE = standardized standard error, CI = confidence interval. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

There were also two environmental and demographic links to time-specific outcomes. Being male was significantly positively associated with higher physical aggression specifically at age 13 (model 2a; ΔR 2 = .04) and school violence was positively associated with nonaggressive conduct problems at specifically age 17 (model 2c; ΔR 2 = .05).

Discussion

The present study employed longitudinal latent analyses to assess how genetics and environmental hostility interact to affect stable and dynamic antisocial behaviors across adolescence. We assessed two antisocial-linked PRS, two forms of environmental hostility (harsh parenting and school violence), and three forms of antisocial behavior (physical aggression, social aggression, and nonaggressive conduct problems) across three timepoints (age 13, 15, 17 years) in a population-based sample.

Our findings support that forms of antisocial behavior should be treated as distinct, but related, constructs. We identified associations between genetics and environment and stable antisocial behavior. Polygenic risk for antisociality also moderated the association between environmental hostility and late adolescent changes in antisocial behavior. These findings contribute several novel insights into the extent to which genes and key environmental adversity factors independently and jointly contribute to distinct forms of antisocial behavior in youth.

Polygenic-environment interactions and antisocial behavior

Consistent with our expectations, one of the antisocial-linked PRS (ASB-PRSa) was significantly associated with only stable physical aggression. The other antisocial PRS (ASB-PRSc) was significantly associated with only stable nonaggressive conduct problems. However, both antisocial PRS had similar relation estimates and explained similar modest amounts of variance (∼1% each) of both physical aggression and nonaggressive conduct. This suggests that nonaggressive conduct problems and physical aggression share some genetic overlap that is distinct from social aggression.

Contrary to our expectations, we did not find support that the selected polygenic scores moderated relations between environment and stable antisocial behavior. Although the two GWASs used to derive the present PRS were the largest to-date, sample sizes between 19,000 and 85,000 are still relatively small; a larger GWAS sample increases statistical power, especially for lower-frequency variants (Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Thomas and Daly2009). Thus, larger GWAS on antisocial behavior would likely result in a PRS with greater predictive power than used here. Additionally, genome-wide complex trait analysis heritability estimates in the published GWAS ranged from 10% and 54% depending on the cohort, suggesting substantial phenotypic heterogeneity; the performance of these PRS likely vary substantially based on sample characteristics. The present study provided meaningful insights into the effectiveness of these antisocial PRS, which is critical for moving the field forward.

Unexpectedly, we also identified a novel time-specific link. Polygenic risk for antisocial behavior may result in differential susceptibility to home environment in late adolescence. Specifically, those with relatively high genetic risk for antisociality were more likely to exhibit social aggression when exposed to harsher parenting, but also showed relatively lower social aggression when exposed to less harsh parenting. Interestingly, although a harsher parenting style related to greater stable social aggression in youth, harsher parenting resulted in a dip in social aggression in older adolescents who had lower genetic antisocial risk. This finding was consistent between the two tested antisocial PRS; each explained a notable amount of social aggression (∼4%) specific to age 17. Perhaps, the genetic variants included in the PRS also influence other traits that then affect the expression of antisocial behavior in unexpected ways. For example, youth with a lower genetic risk may become more inhibited and withdrawn when harshly punished, reducing both prosocial and antisocial interactions. This may be specific to late adolescence due to normative increases in their abilities to inhibit base impulses (Hammond et al., Reference Hammond, Potenza, Mayes, Grant and Potenza2012). Alternately, they may know they are nearing independence from their parents and are thus more willing to temporarily modify their behavior to reduce conflict in the home. However, these are speculative explanations for this unanticipated finding; further research will need to replicate this relation, understand the mechanisms involved, and determine whether this change in social aggression is temporary or sustained into adulthood.

This polygenic-environment link is complimentary to much of the previous literature reporting that a combination of high polygenic and environmental risk associates to greater antisocial problems (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Liu, Motz, Tanksley, Kail, Beckley, Belsky, Domingue, Moffitt, Pratt and Wertz2019; Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Aliev, Bucholz, Agrawal, Hesselbrock, Hesselbrock, Bauer, Kuperman, Schuckit, Kramer, Edenberg, Foroud and Dick2015; Womack et al., Reference Womack, Clifford, Wilson, Shaw and Lemery-Chalfant2021). Similar to our finding that low genetic risk combined with an adverse environment may increase antisocial behavior, Musci et al. (Reference Musci, Bettencourt, Sisto, Maher, Masyn and Ialongo2019) reported that middle schoolers with a lower PRS for conduct problems were more impulsive-aggressive when exposed to community violence. Perhaps this means that youth with low antisocial genetic risk respond to their environmental dynamics in ways that do not follow our established understanding of these processes. These type of complex, temporally sensitive interactions could help explain why some youth respond in opposite ways to the same stimuli. This research highlights the need for further studies on how environment and genetics interact to explain the substantive heterogeneity and instability of antisocial behavior across development.

Interestingly, we found that the PRS used in the present study were not significantly correlated and each explained unique portions of antisocial behavior. Each PRS was derived from a GWAS with very different methodologies (e.g., parent- vs. self-reports/records and childhood vs. adulthood). This could have implications for the PRS literature more broadly, in that different methodologies may capture different facets of antisocial genetics. Antisocial behaviors fluctuate across contexts and developmental periods. Thus, selecting for genes that are predictive of antisociality across discrepant methodologies (e.g. meta-GWAS) may limit potential for identifying genetic contributions to antisociality. This could explain why antisocial PRS are most predictive when studies use methodologies that are more closely in-line with the original GWAS (e.g., Kretschmer et al., Reference Kretschmer, Ouellet-Morin, Vrijen, Nolte and Hartman2021).

Environment and antisocial behavior across adolescence

We expected based on past twin studies that environmental factors would be especially influential on youths’ nonaggressive conduct problems (Burt, Reference Burt2012), which was partially supported by our findings. Harsh parenting was significantly associated with both nonaggressive conduct problems and social aggression, but not physical aggression. This is suggestive of social aggression and nonaggressive problems having greater overlap in adolescent environmental etiologies. Although generally in-line with our expectations, this result is in contrast to one of the few studies that have explored a similar question; Becht et al. (Reference Becht, Prinzie, Deković, van den Akker and Shiner2016) reported that harsh parenting (yelling at, insulting, or hitting child) predicted a high aggression trajectory over childhood and adolescence (age 9 to 15), but not rule-breaking. These contrasting results could be seen as support for other research showing that shared environmental factors have less influence on physical aggression after childhood (Burt, Reference Burt2009). Albeit, other researchers have reported the converse (Eley et al., Reference Eley, Lichtenstein and Moffitt2003).

Further complicating matters, parenting can also be a part of children’s nonshared environment; parents may treat siblings differently and each child may have a unique perspective on similar parenting practices (Turkheimer & Waldron, Reference Turkheimer and Waldron2000). If the influence of parenting on aggression decreases after childhood, perhaps early hostile parenting increases early-onset physical aggression making whether one exhibits chronically high physical aggression in adolescence a result of childhood factors. Thus, parenting may no longer influence physical aggression in adolescence, but would still affect other more malleable antisocial behaviors.

Although more violent school environments were associated with all forms of stable antisocial behaviors, school violence explained 3% more variance in stable nonaggressive conduct problems than other forms antisocial behavior and was related to time-specific increased nonaggressive problems in late adolescence. This provides some support for environmental hostility being especially related to stable and dynamic nonaggressive problems, which partially supports our initial hypothesis. We also expected that environment would only be related to stable physical aggression among genetically susceptible youth, which was not supported by our results. Interestingly, school violence explained a substantial amount of variance for all antisocial behaviors relative to harsh parenting (e.g., R 2 = .14 vs. .03 for stable nonaggressive conduct problems). Family-level factors decrease in influence on rule-breaking and aggression as children age, which may partially explain this discrepancy (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Patterson, Briley, Engelhardt, Kretsch, Mann, Tackett and Tucker-Drob2015). It is also possible that more violent youth are more aware of violent behavior at the school as they enact some of this violence and/or associate with more antisocial peers. The magnitude of this relation may also have been affected by school violence and antisocial behavior both being self-reported measures (i.e., shared informant bias). To our knowledge, we are the first to investigate how school violence may relate to distinct forms of antisocial behavior across adolescence. Thus, more research encompassing teacher- and peer-reports is needed to help elucidate why school violence explained such a large proportion of antisocial behavior.

Limitations and strengths

Some of the strengths and limitations of our study have already been discussed above. In our model, we conducted analyses that allowed us to differentiate between behaviors that were stable across adolescence, as opposed to time-specific behavior. Understanding what factors (using two informants and biological data) are associated with changes in antisocial behavior is an understudied area, thus we were able to provide novel insights into the dynamics of antisocial behavior over a critical developmental period. Additionally, multi-informant, multi-timepoint study designs are considered best practice for developmental research, which is a strength of this study. However, we used two different antisocial scales for age 13 vs. 15 and 17 years. They had similar items, Likert scales, and physical aggression scales were similarly correlated with one another — e.g., correlations between ages 13 and 15 vs. 15 and 17 r = .32 vs. .37 — however, ages 15 and 17 were ∼ 0.2 more correlated than ages 13 and 15 in nonaggressive conduct problems and social aggression scales. It is unclear whether this is due to scale differences or developmental dynamics. Thus, the sex relation to antisocial behavior specific to age 13 should be treated with caution. Further research is needed to replicate our work to determine whether developmental dynamics had an impact on findings.

Although we used multiple informants for this study, the choice of informants could have impacted the results. For example, we asked informants to report on their own behavior, i.e., parents self-reported their harsh parenting behavior. Self-reports may lead to under-reporting of problem behaviors due to social desirability biases. To tease out how these informant choices may have affected our findings, research incorporating multiple informants for each measure is needed.

As is best practice, we adjusted our alpha significance threshold to reduce the likelihood of reporting a false positive. However, such adjustments can increase the likelihood of a false negative. More research is needed to discern whether our adjustments were overly conservative. As the original Pappa et al. (Reference Pappa, St Pourcain, Benke, Cavadino, Hakulinen, Nivard, Nolte, Tiesler, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Davies, Evans, Geoffroy, Grallert, Groen‐Blokhuis, Hudziak, Kemp, Keltikangas‐Järvinen, McMahon, Mileva‐Seitz, Motazedi, Power, Raitakari, Ring, Rivadeneira, Rodriguez, Scheet, Seppälä, Snieder, Standl, Thiering, Timpson, Veenstra, Velders, Whitehouse, Smith, Heinrich, Hypponen, Lehtimäki, Middeldorp, Oldehinkel, Pennell, Boomsma and Tiemeier2016) and Tielbeek et al. (Reference Tielbeek, Uffelmann, Williams, Colodro-Conde, Gagnon, Mallard, Levitt, Jansen, Johansson, Sallis, Pistis, Saunders, Allegrini, Rimfeld, Konte, Klein, Hartmann, Salvatore, Nolte, Demontis, Malmberg, Burt, Savage, Sugden, Poulton, Harris, Vrieze, McGue, Iacono, Mota, Mill, Viana, Mitchell, Morosoli, Andlauer, Ouellet-Morin, Tremblay, Côté, Gouin, Brendgen, Dionne, Vitaro, Lupton, Martin, Hesselbrock, Foroud, Agrawal, Edenberg, Liu, Plawecki, Kuperman, Kramer, Meyers, Kamarajan, Pandey, Bierut, Rice, Bucholz, Schuckit, Tischfield, Hart, Almasy, Goate, Slesinger, Scott, Räikkönen, Eriksson, Lahti, Hartman, Oldehinkel, Snieder, Liu, Preisig, Whipp, Vuoksimaa, Lu, Jern, Rujescu, Giegling, Palviainen, Kaprio, Harden, Munafò, Morneau-Vaillancourt, Plomin, Viding, Boutwell, Aliev, Dick, Popma, Faraone, Børglum, Medland, Franke, Boivin, Pingault, Glennon, Barnes, Fisher, Moffitt, Caspi, Polderman and Posthuma2022) GWAS were conducted on a European sample, we restricted our sample to include only those of European decent. This has the benefit of maximizing the predictive power of the PRS and avoiding interpretation problems that can occur if the PRS is not generalizable to differing ethnicities (Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Thomas and Daly2009; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Gignoux, Walters, Wojcik, Neale, Gravel, Daly, Bustamante and Kenny2017). However, this means that our findings likely will not generalize to individuals of differing ethnic backgrounds. Lastly, our findings are correlational, and although our models are built on directional hypotheses, we are ultimately unable to determine whether identified associations are causal. For example, Haan et al. (Reference Haan, Prinzie and Deković2012) reported that the relation between overreactive parenting and antisocial behavior in adolescence is reciprocal, thus engaging in antisocial behavior more frequently may also lead parents to use more harsh parenting strategies. Similarly, coercion theory states that problem behavior and harsh child-parent interactions feed into each other, and this dynamic begins early in life (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Bell and Gilliom2000; Sitnick et al., Reference Sitnick, Shaw, Gill, Dishion, Winter, Waller, Gardner and Wilson2015). Thus, investigations starting in early childhood may be needed to tease apart the long-term interactions between harsh parenting and antisociality.

Conclusion

Our findings indicate independent genetic and environmental contributions to stable adolescent antisocial behavior and the presence of a polygenic-environment interaction in late adolescence. Specifically, amongst late adolescents with higher genetic risk for antisociality, harsher parenting was associated with higher social aggression in late adolescence. Alternately among late adolescents with lower genetic risk for antisociality, harsher parenting was linked to lower social aggression. Harsher parenting was also associated with greater stable social aggression and nonaggressive conduct problems across adolescence. School violence was not moderated by antisocial PRS but was independently associated with all forms of stable antisocial behavior, especially nonaggressive problems at age 17. Together, this suggests that genetics and environmental hostility link to stable youth antisocial behaviors, whereas genetic-environment interactions may be more influential on late adolescent changes in antisocial behavior. This research highlights the interplay between biological and environmental factors and how they may be critical for understanding stable and fluctuating antisocial behaviors over development.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942400004X.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study was obtained from the Institut de la statistique du Québec and, as stipulated in the clauses 10 and 11 of the Institut de la statistique’s Québec Act (Canada), the access to the data is restricted to the parties identified in the partnership agreement signed to ensure the conduct of the study and which describes the author’s right. In the QLSCD cohort, the participants only consented to share their data with the study’s financial partners and affiliated researchers and their collaborators. Those partners and researchers only have access to the data after signing a data sharing agreement. Requests to access these data can be directed to the Institut de la statistique du Québec’s Research Data Access Services - Home (www.quebec.ca). For more information, contact Marc-Antoine Côté-Marcil ().

Acknowledgments

Thank you to all the children and families who participated in the study and the research assistants who aided in collecting the data.

Funding statement

The Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development was supported by funding from the ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux, le ministère de la Famille, le ministère de l’Éducation et de l’Enseignement supérieur, the Lucie and André Chagnon Foundation, the Institut de recherche Robert-Sauvé en santé et en sécurité du travail, the Research Centre of the Sainte-Justine University Hospital, the ministère du Travail, de l’Emploi et de la Solidarité sociale and the Institut de la statistique du Québec. Additional funding was received by the Fonds de Recherche du Québec - Santé, the Fonds de Recherche du Québec - Société et Culture, the Social Science and Humanities Research Council of Canada, the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), Réseau québécois sur le suicide, les troubles de l’humeur et les troubles associés. This work was also supported by CIHR (FRN-489925).

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

1 Cronbach’s alpha penalizes for number of scale items. For scales with fewer than 10 items, inter-item correlations are recommended to assess scale reliability (Pallant, Reference Pallant2020, p. 102). Average inter-item correlations are recommended to be between 0.15–0.50 (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995), the lower range is expected for scales capturing more heterogenous constructs.

2 SBQ includes items adapted from the Canadian National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth (Statistics Canada, 1996), Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock, Reference Achenbach and Edelbrock1991), the Ontario Child Health Study Scales (Byles et al., Reference Byles, Byrne, Boyle and Offord1988), the Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire (Behar, Reference Behar1977), and the Preschool Behaviour Questionnaire (Tremblay et al., Reference Tremblay, Vitaro, Gagnon, Piché and Royer1992).

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1991). Child behavior checklist. Burlington (Vt), 7, 371392.Google Scholar
Acland, E. L., Jambon, M., & Malti, T. (2021). Children’s emotion recognition and aggression: A multi-cohort longitudinal study. Aggressive Behavior, 47(6), 646658. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21989 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacchini, D., Affuso, G., & Aquilar, S. (2015). Multiple forms and settings of exposure to violence and values: Unique and interactive relationships with antisocial behavior in adolescence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30(17), 30653088. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260514554421 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacchini, D., Concetta Miranda, M., & Affuso, G. (2011). Effects of parental monitoring and exposure to community violence on antisocial behavior and anxiety/depression among adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26(2), 269292. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260510362879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, J. C., Liu, H., Motz, R. T., Tanksley, P. T., Kail, R., Beckley, A. L., Belsky, D. W., Domingue, B. W., Moffitt, T. E., Pratt, T. C., & Wertz, J. (2019). The propensity for aggressive behavior and lifetime incarceration risk: A test for gene-environment interaction (G × E) using whole-genome data. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 49, 101307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2019.07.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becht, A. I., Prinzie, P., Deković, M., van den Akker, A. L., & Shiner, R. L. (2016). Child personality facets and overreactive parenting as predictors of aggression and rule-breaking trajectories from childhood to adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 28(2), 399413. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000577 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behar, L. B. (1977). The preschool behavior questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 5(3), 265275. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00913697 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.Google Scholar
Burt, S. A. (2009). Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(2), 163178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.12.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A. (2012). How do we optimally conceptualize the heterogeneity within antisocial behavior? An argument for aggressive versus non-aggressive behavioral dimensions. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(4), 263279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A. (2013). Do etiological influences on aggression overlap with those on rule breaking? A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 43(9), 18011812. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712001894 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Tackett, J. L. (2012). Should social aggression be considered “antisocial”? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34(2), 153163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-011-9267-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, S. A., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2009). Aggressive versus nonaggressive antisocial behavior: Distinctive etiological moderation by age. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 11641176. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016130 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A., Rescorla, L. A., Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Almqvist, F., Begovac, I., Bilenberg, N., Bird, H., Chahed, M., Dobrean, A., Döpfner, M., Erol, N., Hannesdottir, H., Kanbayashi, Y., Lambert, M. C., Leung, P. W. L., Minaei, A., Novik, T. S., Oh, K.-J., Petot, D., Petot, J.-M., Pomalima, R., Rudan, V., Sawyer, M., Simsek, Z., Steinhausen, H.-C., Valverde, , van der Ende, J., Weintraub, S., Metzke, C. W., Wolanczyk, T., Zhang, E. Y., Zukauskiene, R., &…Verhulst, F. C. (2015). The association between aggressive and non-aggressive antisocial problems as measured with the achenbach system of empirically based assessment: A study of 27,861 parent-adolescent dyads from 25 societies. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 8692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.04.036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byles, J., Byrne, C., Boyle, M. H., & Offord, D. R. (1988). Ontario Child Health Study: Reliability and validity of the general functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device. Family Process, 27(1), 97104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1988.00097.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvete, E., & Orue, I. (2011). The impact of violence exposure on aggressive behavior through social information processing in adolescents. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 81(1), 3850. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01070.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, J., Scott Huebner, E., & Tian, L. (2023). Co-developmental trajectories of aggression and rule-breaking from middle childhood to early adolescence: Individual and environmental predictors. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-023-02171-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development (p. 203). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14805-012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collet, O. A., Orri, M., Tremblay, R. E., Boivin, M., & Côté, S. M. (2023). Psychometric properties of the social behavior questionnaire (SBQ) in a longitudinal population-based sample. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 47(2), 180189. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254221113472 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Compton, K., Snyder, J., Schrepferman, L., Bank, L., & Shortt, J. W. (2003). The contribution of parents and siblings to antisocial and depressive behavior in adolescents: A double jeopardy coercion model. Development and Psychopathology, 15(1), 163182. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579403000099 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Côté, S., Orri, M., Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Japel, C., Séguin, J. R., Geoffroy, M.-C., Rouquette, A., Falissard, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (2017). Psychometric properties of the mental health and social inadaptation assessment for adolescents (MIA) in a population-based sample. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 26(4), e1566. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1566 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Côté, S., Vaillancourt, T., LeBlanc, J. C., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2006). The development of physical aggression from toddlerhood to pre-adolescence: A nation wide longitudinal study of Canadian children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(1), 6882. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-9001-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cruz, M. S., Silva, E. S., Jakaite, Z., Krenzinger, M., Valiati, L., Gonçalves, D., Ribeiro, E., Heritage, P., & Priebe, S. (2021). Experience of neighbourhood violence and mental distress in Brazilian favelas: A cross-sectional household survey. The Lancet Regional Health-Americas, 4, 100067. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lana.2021.100067 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimant, E. (2019). Contagion of pro- and anti-social behavior among peers and the role of social proximity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 73, 6688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.04.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (2016). The development and ecology of antisocial behavior: Linking etiology, prevention, and treatment. In Developmental psychopathology (pp. 132). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy315 Google Scholar
Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 15(2), 383402. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940300021X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8(3), 430457. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estrada, S., & Baskin-Sommers, A. (2023). Trajectories of parental harshness and exposure to community violence differentially predict externalizing and internalizing mental health problems in legal system-involved youth. Development and Psychopathology, 112. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422001420 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, S. Z., Simons, L. G., & Simons, R. L. (2012). The effect of corporal punishment and verbal abuse on delinquency: Mediating mechanisms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(8), 10951110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-012-9755-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, A. M., Dotterer, H. L., & Hyde, L. W. (2019). Genetic influences on antisocial behavior: Recent advances and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 27, 4655. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.07.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, T., Chen, C.-Y., Ni, Y., Feng, Y.-C. A., & Smoller, J. W. (2019). Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09718-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girard, L.-C., Tremblay, R. E., Nagin, D., & Côté, S. M. (2019). Development of aggression subtypes from childhood to adolescence: A group-based multi-trajectory modelling perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(5), 825838. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0488-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Givens, E. M., & Reid, J. A. (2019). Developmental trajectories of physical aggression and nonaggressive rule-breaking during late childhood and early adolescence. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 46(3), 395414. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854818803652 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haan, A. D.de, Prinzie, P., & Deković, M. (2012). Change and reciprocity in adolescent aggressive and rule-breaking behaviors and parental support and dysfunctional discipline. Development and Psychopathology, 24(1), 301315. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000848 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, C. J., Potenza, M. N., & Mayes, L. C. (2012). Development of impulse control, inhibition, and self-regulatory behaviors in normative populations across the lifespan. In Grant, J. E., & Potenza, M. N. (Eds.), The oxford handbook of impulse control disorders (pp. 232244). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harden, K. P., Patterson, M. W., Briley, D. A., Engelhardt, L. E., Kretsch, N., Mann, F. D., Tackett, J. L., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2015). Developmental changes in genetic and environmental influences on rule-breaking and aggression: Age and pubertal development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(12), 13701379. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12419 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2007). Structural equation modeling: Guidelines for determining model fit. The Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 6(1), 155.Google Scholar
Ioannidis, J. P. A., Thomas, G., & Daly, M. J. (2009). Validating, augmenting and refining genome-wide association signals. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 10(5), 318329. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2544 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, K. C., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2002). Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 395416. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579402002110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Pagani, L. S., Pascal, S., Morin, A. J. S., & Bowen, F. (2008). Are there detrimental effects of witnessing school violence in early adolescence? The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 43(6), 600608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.04.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalvin, C. B., & Bierman, K. L. (2017). Child and adolescent risk factors that differentially predict violent versus nonviolent crime. Aggressive Behavior, 43(6), 568577. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21715 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T., & Suchindran, C. (2013). Associations of neighborhood and family factors with trajectories of physical and social aggression during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(6), 861877. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-012-9832-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawabata, Y., Alink, L. R. A., Tseng, W.-L., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Crick, N. R. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles associated with relational aggression in children and adolescents: A conceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 31(4), 240278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.08.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornienko, O., Dishion, T. J., & Ha, T. (2018). Peer network dynamics and the amplification of antisocial to violent behavior among young adolescents in public middle schools. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 26(1), 2130. https://doi.org/10.1177/1063426617742345 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kretschmer, T., Ouellet-Morin, I., Vrijen, C., Nolte, I. M., & Hartman, C. (2021). Polygenic risk for aggressive behavior from late childhood through early adulthood. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(4), 651660. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01906-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacourse, E., Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Petitclerc, A., Girard, A., Vitaro, F., Paquin, S., Ouellet-Morin, I., Dionne, G., & Tremblay, R. E. (2014). A longitudinal twin study of physical aggression during early childhood: Evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome. Psychological Medicine, 44(12), 26172627. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713003218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, K. (2018). Estimating standardized SEM parameters given nonnormal data and incorrect model: Methods and comparison. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(4), 600620. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1392248 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y., Putallaz, M., & Su, Y. (2011). Interparental conflict styles and parenting behaviors: Associations with overt and relational aggression among chinese children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 57(4), 402428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A. R., Gignoux, C. R., Walters, R. K., Wojcik, G. L., Neale, B. M., Gravel, S., Daly, M. J., Bustamante, C. D., & Kenny, E. E. (2017). Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 100(4), 635649. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.03.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E. (2018). Male antisocial behaviour in adolescence and beyond. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(3), 177186. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0309-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musci, R. J., Bettencourt, A. F., Sisto, D., Maher, B., Masyn, K., & Ialongo, N. S. (2019). Violence exposure in an urban city: A GxE interaction with aggressive and impulsive behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 60(1), 7281. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12966 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niv, S., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2013). Aggression and rule-breaking: Heritability and stability of antisocial behavior problems in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(5), 285291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.06.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Keefe, M. (1997). Adolescents’ exposure to community and school violence: Prevalence and behavioral correlates. Journal of Adolescent Health, 20(5), 368376. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(97)80131-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Lansford, J. E., Sexton, H., Davis-Kean, P., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2013). Deconstructing the externalizing spectrum: Growth patterns of overt aggression, covert aggression, oppositional behavior, impulsivity/inattention, and emotion dysregulation between school entry and early adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 25(3), 817842. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000199 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orri, M., Boivin, M., Chen, C., Ahun, M. N., Geoffroy, M.-C., Ouellet-Morin, I., Tremblay, R. E., & Côté, S. M. (2021). Cohort profile: Quebec longitudinal study of child development (QLSCD). Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 56(5), 883894. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-020-01972-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pallant, J. (2020). SPSS Survival Manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS (7th ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003117452 Google Scholar
Pappa, I., St Pourcain, B., Benke, K., Cavadino, A., Hakulinen, C., Nivard, M. G., Nolte, I. M., Tiesler, C. M. T., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., Davies, G. E., Evans, D. M., Geoffroy, M‐Claude, Grallert, H., Groen‐Blokhuis, M. M., Hudziak, J. J., Kemp, J. P., Keltikangas‐Järvinen, L., McMahon, G., Mileva‐Seitz, V. R., Motazedi, E., Power, C., Raitakari, O. T., Ring, S. M., Rivadeneira, F., Rodriguez, A., Scheet, P. A., Seppälä, I., Snieder, H., Standl, M., Thiering, E., Timpson, N. J., Veenstra, , Velders, F. P., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Smith, G. D., Heinrich, J., Hypponen, E., Lehtimäki, T., Middeldorp, C. M., Oldehinkel, A. J., Pennell, C. E., Boomsma, D. I., &… Tiemeier, H. (2016). A genome-wide approach to children’s aggressive behavior: The EAGLE consortium. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(5), 562572. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32333 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Bucholz, K., Agrawal, A., Hesselbrock, V., Hesselbrock, M., Bauer, L., Kuperman, S., Schuckit, M. A., Kramer, J., Edenberg, H. J., Foroud, T. M., & Dick, D. M. (2015). Polygenic risk for externalizing disorders: Gene-by-development and gene-by-environment effects in adolescents and young adults. Clinical Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 3(2), 189201. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614534211 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika, 75(2), 243248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Bell, R. Q., & Gilliom, M. (2000). A truly early starter model of antisocial behavior revisited. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3(3), 155172. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009599208790 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sitnick, S. L., Shaw, D. S., Gill, A., Dishion, T., Winter, C., Waller, R., Gardner, F., & Wilson, M. (2015). Parenting and the family check-up: Changes in observed parent-child interaction following early childhood intervention. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44(6), 970984. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2014.940623 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (1996). National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth, user’s handbook and microdata guide. Special Surveys Division, Statistics Canada.Google Scholar
Tielbeek, J. J., Johansson, A., Polderman, T. J. C., Rautiainen, M.-R., Jansen, P., Taylor, M., Tong, X., Lu, Q., Burt, A. S., Tiemeier, H., Viding, E., Plomin, R., Martin, N. G., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A. F., Montgomery, G., Beaver, K. M., Waldman, I., Gelernter, J., Kranzler, H. R., Farrer, L. A., Perry, J. R. B., Munafò, M., LoParo, D., Paunio, T., Tiihonen, J., Mous, S. E., Pappa, I., de Leeuw, C., Watanabe, K., Hammerschlag, A. R., Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Bigdeli, T. B., Dick, D., Faraone, S. V., Popma, A., Medland, S. E., Posthuma, D., &… for the Broad Antisocial Behavior Consortium collaborators (2017). Genome-wide association studies of a broad spectrum of antisocial behavior. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(12), 12421250. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3069 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tielbeek, J. J., Uffelmann, E., Williams, B. S., Colodro-Conde, L. D., Gagnon, Éloi, Mallard, T. T., Levitt, B. E., Jansen, P. R., Johansson, A., Sallis, H. M., Pistis, G., Saunders, G. R. B., Allegrini, A. G., Rimfeld, K., Konte, B., Klein, M., Hartmann, A. M., Salvatore, J. E., Nolte, I. M., Demontis, D., Malmberg, A. L. K., Burt, S. A., Savage, J. E., Sugden, K., Poulton, R., Harris, K. M., Vrieze, S., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., Mota, N. R., Mill, J., Viana, J. F., Mitchell, B. L., Morosoli, J. J., Andlauer, T. F. M., Ouellet-Morin, I., Tremblay, R. E., Côté, S. M., Gouin, J.-P., Brendgen, M. R., Dionne, G., Vitaro, F., Lupton, M. K., Martin, N. G., Hesselbrock, V., Foroud, T., Agrawal, A., Edenberg, H. J., Liu, Y., Plawecki, M. H., Kuperman, S., Kramer, J. R., Meyers, J. M., Kamarajan, C., Pandey, A., Bierut, L., Rice, J., Bucholz, K. K., Schuckit, M. A., Tischfield, J., Hart, R., Almasy, L., Goate, A., Slesinger, P., Scott, D., Räikkönen, K., Eriksson, J. G., Lahti, J., Hartman, C. A., Oldehinkel, A. J., Snieder, H., Liu, H., Preisig, M., Whipp, A., Vuoksimaa, E., Lu, Y., Jern, P., Rujescu, D., Giegling, I., Palviainen, T., Kaprio, J., Harden, K. P., Munafò, M. R., Morneau-Vaillancourt, Gève, Plomin, R., Viding, E., Boutwell, B. B., Aliev, F., Dick, D. M., Popma, A., Faraone, S. V., Børglum, A. D., Medland, S. E., Franke, B., Boivin, M., Pingault, J.-B., Glennon, J. C., Barnes, J. C., Fisher, S. E., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Polderman, T. J. C., &… Posthuma, D. (2022). Uncovering the genetic architecture of broad antisocial behavior through a genome-wide association study meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(11), 44534463. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01793-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., & Côté, S. M. (2018). Developmental origins of chronic physical aggression: A bio-psycho-social model for the next generation of preventive interventions. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(1), 383407. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044030 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Gagnon, C., Piché, C., & Royer, N. (1992). A prosocial scale for the Preschool Behaviour Questionnaire: Concurrent and predictive correlates. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15(2), 227245. https://doi.org/10.1177/016502549201500204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. (2000). Nonshared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 78108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.78 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Womack, S. R., Clifford, S., Wilson, M. N., Shaw, D. S., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2021). Genetic moderation of the association between early family instability and trajectories of aggressive behaviors from middle childhood to adolescence. Behavior Genetics, 51(5), 476491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10069-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, Y., & Cleveland, H. H. (2013). Identifying gender-specific developmental trajectories of nonviolent and violent delinquency from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Adolescence, 36(2), 371–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.12.007.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Model 1: risk factors predicting stable antisocial behaviors. Illustrates model 1 which assessed how genetic and environmental factors relate to forms of antisocial behavior stable across adolescence. The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (2016; child sample GWAS) and Tielbeek et al. (2022; adult sample GWAS), respectively. ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score; HP = harsh parenting; SV = school violence; PA = physical aggression; SA = social aggression; NC = nonaggressive conduct problems; T1 = age 13; T2 = age 15; T3 = age 17.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Model 2a: risk factors predicting antisocial behaviors specific (residual) to age 13. Illustrates model 2a which assessed how genetic and environmental factors relate forms of antisocial behavior that were specific to T1, i.e., antisocial variance at age 13 unexplained by stable behaviors. The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (2016; child sample GWAS) and Tielbeek et al. (2022; adult sample GWAS), respectively. ASB-PRS = antisocial behavior polygenic risk score; HP = harsh parenting; SV = school violence; PA = physical aggression; SA = social aggression; NC = nonaggressive conduct problems; T1 = age 13; T2 = age 15; T3 = age 17.

Figure 2

Table 1. Descriptive statistics by timepoint for study variables

Figure 3

Table 2. Zero-order correlations for all study variables

Figure 4

Table 3. Likelihood ratio tests for comparing stable antisocial behaviors models

Figure 5

Table 4. Model 1: latent path model for stable antisocial behaviors across adolescence

Figure 6

Figure 3. Polygenic-environment interaction in late adolescence. Plots illustrate the relations between harsh parenting (standardized) and time-specific social aggression at age 17 (i.e., residual variance) with high or low (+/− 2 SD) antisocial behavior polygenic risk scores (ASB-PRS). The ASB-PRSc and ASB-PRSa were derived from the Pappa et al. (2016; child sample GWAS) and Tielbeek et al. (2022; adult sample GWAS), respectively. Johnson Neyman plots (see Figure S1) indicate the negative (blue) and positive (red) relations between harsh parenting and social aggression are significant when PRS are less than approximately 0.5 SDs below and 2 SDs above the mean, respectively. Each plot was computed using a trimmed model which includes only significant predictors from model 2c; ABS-PRSc and ABS-PRSa were not included in the same model for computing these plots. SD = standard deviation. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (1998–2018), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Figure 7

Table 5. Models 2a,b,c: latent path model for time-specific (i.e., residual) antisocial behaviors across adolescence

Supplementary material: File

Acland et al. supplementary material

Acland et al. supplementary material
Download Acland et al. supplementary material(File)
File 81.7 KB