Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-vmclg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-15T00:53:02.766Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The longitudinal relationship between psychological symptoms and social functioning in displaced refugees

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2025

Angela Nickerson*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Gulsah Kurt
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Belinda Liddell
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia School of Psychological Sciences, University of Newcastle, Newcastle, Australia
David Keegan
Affiliation:
HOST International, Parramatta, NSW, Australia School of Social Work, Excelsia University College, Macquarie Park, NSW, Australia
Randy Nandyatama
Affiliation:
Department of International Relations, Gadjah Mada University Yogyakarta, Yogyakarta, Indonesia
Atika Yuanita
Affiliation:
SUAKA, Indonesian Civil Society Network for Refugee Rights Protection, Jakarta Pusat, Indonesia
Rizka Argadianti Rachmah
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Joel Hoffman
Affiliation:
Sydney School of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney
Shraddha Kashyap
Affiliation:
Bilya Marlee School of Indigenous Studies, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Natalie Mastrogiovanni
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Vivian Mai
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Anna Camilleri
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Dessy Susanty
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Diah Tricesaria
Affiliation:
School of Social Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Hasti Rostami
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Jenny Im
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Marta Gurzeda
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Mitra Khakbaz
Affiliation:
HOST International, Parramatta, NSW, Australia
Sarah Funnell
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Zico Pestalozzi
Affiliation:
SUAKA, Indonesian Civil Society Network for Refugee Rights Protection, Jakarta Pusat, Indonesia
Philippa Specker
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Angela Nickerson, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Refugee experiences of trauma and displacement can significantly disrupt established social networks. While social functioning has been routinely associated with mental health, to our knowledge, no study has tested the direction of influence between social and psychological functioning within displaced refugee communities. This study investigated the temporal association between psychological symptoms (PTSD, depression, anger) and multiple facets of social functioning (including community connectedness, perceived social responsibility, positive social support and negative social support).

Method

A culturally diverse sample of refugees (N = 1,235) displaced in Indonesia completed an online survey at four time-points, six months apart. Longitudinal structural equation modelling was used to investigate the temporal ordering between psychological symptoms and social functioning.

Results

Findings revealed that greater psychological symptoms were associated with a subsequent deterioration in social functioning (decreased positive social support and community connectedness and increased negative social support and perceived social responsibility). Greater perceived social responsibility was also associated with subsequent increases in psychological symptoms, while positive social support and community connectedness were bi-directionally associated over-time.

Conclusions

These findings highlight the potential utility of mental health interventions for displaced refugees as a means to improve social functioning and inclusion with host communities. Findings have important implications in guiding the development of interventions and allocation of resources to support refugee engagement and wellbeing in displacement contexts.

Type
Invited Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Refugees and asylum-seekers are forced to flee their homes due to war, conflict, and persecution. The mental health impact of these experiences is often compounded by significant adversity encountered during and following displacement. Accordingly, approximately one in three refugees and asylum-seekers have probable diagnoses of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression (Henkelmann et al., Reference Henkelmann, de Best, Deckers, Jensen, Shahab, Elzinga and Molendijk2020; Patanè et al., Reference Patanè, Ghane, Karyotaki, Cuijpers, Schoonmade, Tarsitani and Sijbrandij2022; Steel et al., Reference Steel, Chey, Silove, Marnane, Bryant and van Ommeren2009). In addition to psychological sequelae, forced displacement disrupts social networks, with refugees being compelled to leave behind family and friends when seeking safety in a new country (Liddell, Byrow et al., Reference Liddell, Byrow, O’Donnell, Mau, Batch, McMahon, Bryant and Nickerson2021; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Bryant, Steel, Silove and Brooks2010). Indeed, experiences of loneliness, disconnection, isolation have all been documented amongst refugees (Kurt et al., Reference Kurt, Ekhtiari, Ventevogel, Ersahin, Ilkkursun, Akbiyik and Acarturk2023; Liddell, O’Donnell et al., Reference Liddell, O’Donnell, Bryant, Murphy, Byrow, Mau, McMahon, Benson and Nickerson2021; Liddell et al., Reference Liddell, Batch, Hellyer, Bulnes-Diez, Kamte, Klassen, Wong, Byrow and Nickerson2022; Nguyen, Slewa-Younan, & Rioseco, Reference Nguyen, Slewa-Younan and Rioseco2023; Strang & Quinn, Reference Strang and Quinn2021). Notably, isolation has been linked to poorer mental health outcomes in refugees (Chen, Hall, Ling, & Renzaho, Reference Chen, Hall, Ling and Renzaho2017). Further, there is substantial cross-sectional evidence that poorer social functioning (i.e., the capacity to form and maintain good-quality relationships and participate in social activities (Lahiri, van Ommeren, & Roberts, Reference Lahiri, van Ommeren and Roberts2017) is associated with worse mental health outcomes amongst refugees (Al-Adhami, Berglund, Wångdahl, & Salari, Reference Al-Adhami, Berglund, Wångdahl and Salari2022; Berthold et al., Reference Berthold, Loomis, Kuoch, Scully, Hin-McCormick, Casavant and Buckley2019; Çankaya, Alan Dikmen, & Dereli Yılmaz, Reference Çankaya, Alan Dikmen and Dereli Yılmaz2020; Haer, Scharpf, & Hecker, Reference Haer, Scharpf and Hecker2021; Kurt et al., Reference Kurt, Ekhtiari, Ventevogel, Ersahin, Ilkkursun, Akbiyik and Acarturk2023; Pak, Yurtbakan, & Acarturk, Reference Pak, Yurtbakan and Acarturk2023; Schick et al., Reference Schick, Zumwald, Knöpfli, Nickerson, Bryant, Schnyder, Müller and Morina2016; Yildirim, Isik, Firat, & Aylaz, Reference Yildirim, Isik, Firat and Aylaz2020). This is problematic as good social functioning is critical to successfully navigating the post-displacement environment (Wachter et al., Reference Wachter, Bunn, Schuster, Boateng, Cameli and Johnson-Agbakwu2021). This may be especially important for refugees who are displaced in low-resource transit settings (such as low-and-middle income countries), where individuals may rely on social networks in the absence of formal supports to meet basic needs (Posselt et al., Reference Posselt, Eaton, Ferguson, Keegan and Procter2019).

To date, however, the temporal ordering of psychological symptoms and social functioning in refugees is unclear. Understanding the direction of influence between psychological symptoms and social functioning is important for identifying the most powerful point of intervention to facilitate overall wellbeing, particularly in low-resource settings. One hypothesized direction of influence is that greater social functioning predicts improvements in mental health over time. This has been elaborated in social causation models, such as the stress buffering hypothesis (Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985) and the social support deterioration model (Kaniasty & Norris, Reference Kaniasty and Norris1993). These models propose that social support – the practical or emotional support obtained through interpersonal connections (Wachter et al., Reference Wachter, Bunn, Schuster, Boateng, Cameli and Johnson-Agbakwu2021) – is an important antecedent to mental health in the aftermath of trauma exposure, such that varying levels of social support may protect against or exacerbate psychological symptoms following a traumatic event. An alternative possibility is that psychological symptoms temporally precede changes in social support. This would be consistent with the social selection and social erosion models, which posit that PTSD symptoms may lead to reduced availability and quality of social support over time due to the individual withdrawing from and/or pushing away important potential sources of support (King et al., Reference King, Taft, King, Hammond and Stone2006). It is also possible that the social selection and social causation models can co-exist. Consistent with this, longitudinal evidence from studies with non-refugee trauma survivors have found support for both the social causation and social erosion models, with a recent meta-analysis finding a bidirectional relationship between social support and PTSD symptoms (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Chung, Wang, Yu and Kenardy2021).

Psychological models considering the temporal ordering between social functioning and psychological symptoms tend to focus on the relationship between perceived positive social support and mental health. There are, however, other important facets of social functioning that are especially relevant to the refugee experience and may influence or be influenced by mental health. For example, connectedness with the host community may be important for refugees to foster a sense of belonging in their new home (Ager & Strang, Reference Ager and Strang2008; Kurt et al., Reference Kurt, Ekhtiari, Ventevogel, Ersahin, Ilkkursun, Akbiyik and Acarturk2023). Further, refugees may feel a strong sense of social responsibility for family or friends who are living with them in the transit context or in another country and requiring financial, logistical or emotional assistance (Liddell et al., Reference Liddell, Batch, Bulnes-Diez, Hellyer, Wong, Byrow and Nickerson2020). In addition, perceived social support may not always be positive in nature, with perceived negative social support (e.g. the experience of conflict, tensions, and demands from others) being linked to psychological symptoms in the aftermath of exposure to trauma (Andrews, Brewin, & Rose, Reference Andrews, Brewin and Rose2003; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Creamer, Forbes, McFarlane, O’Donnell, Silove, Steel, Felmingham, Hadzi-Pavlovic and Bryant2017; Zoellner, Foa, & Brigidi, Reference Zoellner, Foa and Brigidi1999). In summary, social functioning is multi-faceted; it is thus plausible that the association with psychological symptoms may differ across specific aspects of social functioning.

To date, however, understanding of the longitudinal association between psychological symptoms and social functioning in refugees is limited. One study undertaken with resettled refugees found that social integration stressors predicted the worsening of psychological distress over three years, although the inverse relationship was not tested (Nguyen, Slewa-Younan, & Rioseco, Reference Nguyen, Slewa-Younan and Rioseco2023). A second study with resettled refugees found no association between social engagement and mental health outcomes over time, however the measure used to index social engagement (a count of the number of types of individuals/groups participants were engaged with), may have lacked sensitivity to detect these relationships (Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Byrow, O’Donnell, Bryant, Mau, McMahon, Benson and Liddell2022). This relative scarcity of evidence uncovers an important gap in our knowledge – namely understanding the direction of influence between psychological symptoms and social functioning amongst refugees. The current study aimed to address this research gap by investigating the temporal ordering between key indicators of social functioning and psychological symptoms amongst a sample of refugees living in protracted displacement in Indonesia. Indonesia does not permanently resettle refugees, thus all refugees in Indonesia are living in this country temporarily, awaiting resettlement to other countries, although in practicality, this waiting period is often indefinite due to limited resettlement places (Curby, Reference Curby2020). During this protracted displacement, refugees in Indonesia do not have rights to work or to reunite with family and have limited access to government-sponsored services. This might also increase reliance on social networks to navigate their needs.

Based on theoretical models of social support and mental health following trauma, and research undertaken with trauma-affected populations to date, we hypothesized a bidirectional relationship between social functioning and psychological symptoms over time, consistent with both social selection and social erosion models. Specifically, we predicted that greater positive social support and connectedness with the host community, as well as lower negative social support and perceived social responsibility, would be associated with decreases in psychological symptoms over time. We also predicted that greater psychological symptoms would be associated with subsequent decreases in positive social support and connectedness with the host community, and subsequent increases in negative social support and perceived social responsibility.

Method

Participants

Participants were refugees from Arabic, Farsi, Dari, Somali or English-speaking backgrounds living in Indonesia. Refugees from these language groups represented over 80% of refugees living in Indonesia in 2018 at the outset of the study (UNHCR Indonesia, 2018). Participants were recruited via advertisements in refugee services and community-based organizations, referrals, social media and snowball sampling (Sadler, Lee, Lim, & Fullerton, Reference Sadler, Lee, Lim and Fullerton2010). Participants were required to (1) have a refugee or asylum-seeking background, (2) be literate in written Arabic, Farsi, Dari, Somali or English, (3) be over 18 years of age, and (4) have arrived in Indonesia in January 2013 or later.

Demographic information of participants is presented in Table 1. In this study, 1,235 participants completed the survey at Time-point 1 (T1), 968 at T2, 779 at T3 and 753 at T4, with data being collected six months apart (61.0% retention rate from T1 to T4). Compared to participants who completed T1 only, those who completed all time-points had been in Indonesia longer (T1 only mean = 4.31 years, SD = 1.44 vs all time-points mean = 4.80 years, SD = 1.81, t(171.385) = 5.20, p < .001), were more likely to have completed the survey in Dari (T1 only = 16.2% vs all time-points = 24.2%) than English (T1 only = 22.6% vs all time-points = 14.2%, χ2(4) = 34.53, p < .001), reported greater positive social support (T1 only mean = 3.25, SD = 0.82 vs all time-points mean = 3.41, SD = 0.84, t(1114) = 0.31, p < .001) and reported greater perceived social responsibility (T1 only mean = 3.01, SD = 0.95 vs all time-points mean = 3.22, SD = 0.91, t(1154) = 0.37, p < .001). There were no T1 differences in these groups in gender, age, exposure to potentially traumatic events (PTEs), negative social support, or symptoms of PTSD, depression or anger.

Table 1. Participant characteristics

Note: N = 1,235.

Measures

All measures were translated and blind-back translated by accredited translators. Measures were pilot-tested with refugees from each language group with varying levels of education.

Demographics. Demographic information collected included age, gender, language, country of birth, education, marital status and years lived in Indonesia. Exposure to PTEs was indexed using the 16-item Harvard Trauma Questionnaire (Mollica et al., Reference Mollica, Caspi-Yavin, Bollini, Truong, Tor and Lavelle1992) and 3 additional items relating to natural disaster, physical assault or exposure to serious accident, fire, or explosion (Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Hoffman, Keegan, Kashyap, Argadianti, Tricesaria, Pestalozzi, Nandyatama, Khakbaz, Nilasari and Liddell2023). A total count of the number of different PTEs that each participant had experienced and/or witnessed was derived for the present study.

Psychological symptoms. PTSD symptoms were indexed using an adapted 20-item version of the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale for DSM-IV (Foa, Riggs, Dancu, & Rothbaum, Reference Foa, Riggs, Dancu and Rothbaum1993), with four items added (persistent negative beliefs, distorted blame, persistent negative emotional state, and risk-taking behaviour) and one item removed (sense of a foreshortened future) to reflect the updated DSM-5 criteria. Items were scored on a four-point Likert scale (1 = Not at all/only one time to 4 = 5 or more times a week/almost always). Depression symptoms were measured using the 8-item Patient Health Questionnaire (Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Strine, Spitzer, Williams, Berry and Mokdad2009). Items were scored on a four-point Likert scale (1 = Not at all to 4 = nearly every day). Anger symptoms were measured using the 5-item Dimensions of Anger Reactions scale (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Alkemade, Hopcraft, Hawthorne, O’Halloran, Elhai, McHugh, Bates, Novaco, Bryant and Lewis2014). Items were scored on a five-point Likert scale (1 = None or almost none of the time to 5 = all or almost all of the time). All scales showed strong internal consistency at all time-points (PTSD: α = 0.95–0.97; Depression: α = 0.89–0.93; Anger: α = 0.89 to 0.92). In this study, means of PTSD, depression and anger symptoms formed the three indicators for the psychological symptoms latent variable at each time-point.

Positive social support. Positive social support was measured using an 8-item scale adapted from social support questions used by Araya, Chotai, Komproe, and de Jong (Reference Araya, Chotai, Komproe and de Jong2007). Items (presented in Table 2) measured the extent to which an individual perceived that they had others they could rely on and talk to. Items were scored on a five-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree). In this study, individual items were used as indicators for the positive social support latent variable at each time-point.

Table 2. Factor loadings in metric invariance model for confirmatory factor analysis at time 1

Negative social support. Negative social support was measured using three items that were adapted from the Schuster Social Support Questions (Schuster, Kessler, & Aseltine, Reference Schuster, Kessler and Aseltine1990). These items indexed how often the participants had negative interactions with family and friends. One item (“how often did family or friends make too many demands on you?”) was removed from the scale due to a low factor loading and conceptual overlap with the Perceived Social Responsibility Scale (see Results). The remaining two items are presented in Table 2. Items were scored on a four-point Likert scale (1 = Never, 4 = Extremely often). In this study, individual items were used as indicators for the negative social support latent variable at each time-point.

Perceived social responsibility. Perceived social responsibility was indexed using three items developed for this study (see Table 2). These items were scored on a five-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree). In this study, individual items were used as indicators for the perceived social responsibility latent variable at each time-point.

Connectedness with the Indonesian community. Connectedness with the Indonesian community was indexed using a single item developed for this study (see Table 2). This was measured on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Not at all connected, 5 = Extremely connected).

Procedure

T1 data was collected between February and October 2020. Data for T2 to T4 was collected at six-month intervals following completion of T1. Participants initially registered their interest in participating on the study website where they completed eligibility screening questions. Eligible participants completed online informed consent procedures and were sent a personalized study link. Measures were administered via the KeySurvey platform in the participant’s preferred language (English, Arabic, Dari, Somali, Farsi). Participants received a IDR100,000 ($USD7) shopping voucher for taking part in each time-point of the study. This study was approved by UNSW Human Research Ethics Committee (HC190494) and Atma Jaya University, Jakarta (0792/III/LPPM-PM.10.05/07/2019).

Data analysis

We used longitudinal structural equation modelling in MPlus version 8 (Muthen & Muthen, Reference Muthen and Muthen1998) to investigate the association between psychological symptoms and different facets of social functioning over time. Full information maximum likelihood estimation was used to account for missing data on endogenous variables by adjusting model parameters based on the available information. Model fit was evaluated using the following indices: comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.95, Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) > 0.95, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) < 0.08, and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.06 (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). Where relevant, we compared nested models using a chi-square difference test.

First, we used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to evaluate the measurement model. For this model, we investigated the fit of latent variables and their various indicator variables at each time-point in a single model. The latent variable of psychological symptoms was indexed by three indicator variables, namely mean PTSD, depression and anger symptom scores; positive social support was indexed by eight indicator items; negative social support was indexed by two indicator items, and perceived social responsibility was indexed by three indicator items. Connection to the Indonesian community was indexed by a single item, and thus was represented by an observed variable and not included in the measurement model. We elected to include multiple latent variables for social functioning (compared to a single latent variable for mental health) as we were most interested in investigating the association between overall mental health and specific types of social functioning over time. After estimating an unconstrained CFA model that included all latent and indicator variables at all time-points, we evaluated measurement invariance across time by estimating increasingly constrained models (Little, Reference Little2013). Model fit was progressively tested and compared to obtain the most parsimonious measurement model. First, we estimated a baseline or unconstrained model. Next, we tested metric invariance, where factor loadings were constrained to equality at each time-point, followed by scalar invariance where intercepts were also constrained to equality over time-points. Next, we tested for residual invariance where residual errors were set to equality over time-points, and finally we tested factor invariance where the variance of factors were set to 1. Each model was compared to the previous model, and model fit determined by the following indices: CFI > 0.95, TLI > 0.95, SRMR<0.08, RMSEA<0.06 (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999).

Next, we tested the structural model, retaining the measurement model structure from the previous step that balanced parsimony and model fit. The structural model investigated the association between psychological symptoms, positive social support, negative social support, perceived social responsibility and connection with the Indonesian community across time-points. Pathways that were modelled between each time-point included autoregressive and cross-lagged paths. When evaluating the structural model, we again sought to optimise parsimony. Accordingly, we first tested an unconstrained model where all structural pathways (e.g., pathways between latent variables) were allowed to vary freely over time. Next, we tested a model where autoregressive pathways between variables were constrained to equality between time-points. We then tested a model where cross-lagged pathways between variables were constrained to equality between time-points. Finally, we tested a model where residual covariances between latent variables were constrained to equality between time-points.

After establishing the optimal model, we included covariates in the model to control for the effects of variables known to predict mental health and social functioning outcomes, including age, gender, language group (which was considered a proxy for cultural group), time in Indonesia, and exposure to potentially traumatic events. All T1 variables were regressed on covariates. We used multiple imputation (20 datasets) to account for missing data on covariates.

Results

Measurement model

Testing of the baseline (configural invariance) model revealed adequate model fit: CFI = 0.88, TLI = 0.87, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.05. Inspection of factor loadings revealed that one of the items indexing negative social support (“How often did family or friends make too many demands on you?”) yielded standardized factor loadings of <0.50 at two time-points (T1 = 0.44, T2 = 0.48, T3 = 0.52, T4 = 0.57), which indicates that this item did not load adequately onto the negative social support factor (Taherdoost, Sahibuddin, & Jalaliyoon, Reference Taherdoost, Sahibuddin and Jalaliyoon2022). Further, this item overlapped with the Perceived Responsibility Scale. We thus removed this item and re-tested the baseline configural invariance model. This model showed slightly improved model fit: CFI = 0.89, TLI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.04. Subsequent models omitted this item. Next, we tested metric invariance where factor loadings were constrained to equality over time within each latent variable. Model fit was not significantly different to the configural invariance model: χ2diff (36) = 38.51, p = .657, CFI = 0.89, TLI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.04. Thus, we retained the metric invariance model. We then tested scalar variance, where intercepts were constrained to equality over time within each latent variable. This model showed significantly poorer fit to the metric invariance model: χ2diff (48) = 81.71, p = .002, CFI = 0.89, TLI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.04. Accordingly, the metric invariance model was retained for subsequent analyses. Unstandardized and standardized factor loadings for this model are presented in Table 2.

Structural model

The unconstrained structural model revealed adequate fit: CFI = 0.875, TLI = 0.871, RMSEA = 0.040, SRMR = 0.072. Constraint of autoregressive paths to equality between time-points revealed a small but significant change in model fit according to the chi-square difference statistic: χ2diff (10) = 18.73, p = .044, however other indices revealed slight improvements in fit: CFI = 0.879, TLI = 0.871, RMSEA = 0.040, SRMR = 0.066. Similarly, constraint of cross-lagged paths to equality between time-points revealed a small but significant change in model fit compared to the autoregressive constrained model according to the chi-square difference statistic: χ2diff (40) = 56.16, p = .046. Again, other indices revealed little to no change in model fit: CFI = 0.878, TLI = 0.873, RMSEA = 0.039, SRMR = 0.067. Constraint of residual error variances to equality between time-points revealed a significant change in model fit according to the chi-square difference statistic: χ2diff (26) = 226.62, p < .001, and small decreases in model fit according to other indices: CFI = 0.875, TLI = 0.871, RMSEA = 0.040, SRMR = 0.072. Theorists have noted that the chi-square can be sensitive to trivial influences in the case of large sample sizes (Little, Reference Little2013), and thus yield assessments of model fit that are too conservative. Given that (1) other model indices revealed no change in fit, (2) the change in chi-square was close to the 0.05 p-value in the instance of the constrained autoregressive and cross-lagged paths, and (3) the goal was to optimise model parsimony, we elected to retain the model with autoregressive and cross-lagged pathways constrained between time-points. We considered retaining the model with residual error variances constrained to equality between time-points. However, while this model yielded a highly significant chi-square difference statistic, (small) decreases in model fit in other indices were observed. We thus elected to allow error variances to vary between time-points and retained the model that only constrained autoregressive and cross-lagged paths.

Model parameters are presented in Table 3, and residual covariances between latent variables are presented in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 3. Model parameters in structural model

Note: Gender was coded where 0, male and 1, female.

Abbreviations: PSS , Positive Social Support; NSS, Negative Social Support; PSR, Perceived Social Responsibility; PTE, Potentially Traumatic Events.

Associations between psychological symptoms and subsequent social functioning. Findings revealed that psychological symptoms were associated with subsequent changes in all social functioning variables over time including decreases in positive social support and connection with the Indonesian community and increases in negative social support and perceived social responsibility.

Associations between social functioning and subsequent psychological symptoms. Findings revealed that greater perceived social responsibility was associated with subsequent increases in psychological symptoms. No other facet of social functioning (i.e., community connectedness, positive social support and negative social support) was associated with subsequent changes in psychological symptoms.

Associations between different types of social functioning over time. Greater connection with the Indonesian community was associated with subsequent increases in positive social support and vice versa.

Discussion

This study was the first, to our knowledge, to investigate the temporal association between psychological symptoms and multiple facets of social functioning in refugees. A key finding of this study (consistent with our hypotheses) was that greater psychological symptoms were associated with subsequent changes in social functioning, including decreased positive social support and connectedness with the host community and increased negative social support and perceived social responsibility. Contrary to our hypotheses, this temporal relationship was unidirectional with the exception of perceived social responsibility, which was bi-directionally associated with psychological symptoms.

These findings highlight the pivotal role of mental health in contributing to social functioning amongst refugees living in a transit setting. The results are consistent with social selection and social erosion models that posit an association between mental health and social support in the aftermath of traumatic events (King et al., Reference King, Taft, King, Hammond and Stone2006). These models suggest that social resources, such as the availability and quality of social support, reduces over time as psychological symptoms cause an individual to withdraw from and/or push away others (King et al., Reference King, Taft, King, Hammond and Stone2006; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Chung, Wang, Yu and Kenardy2021). Our findings build on these models to suggest that psychological symptoms are associated with not only subsequent reductions in positive social supports and community connectedness, but also with increased negative social support.

In contrast, these findings are not in line with social causation models of mental health, which argue that reduced social support leads to exacerbated psychological symptoms over time (Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985; Kaniasty & Norris, Reference Kaniasty and Norris1993). One possible explanation for this finding is that differential associations between mental health and social functioning may be observed across contexts. This is consistent with contemporary models of wellbeing which focus on the interaction between the individual’s environmental circumstances, psychological skills and social context (Bonanno, Chen, & Galatzer-Levy, Reference Bonanno, Chen and Galatzer-Levy2023; Craske, Herzallah, Nusslock, & Patel, Reference Craske, Herzallah, Nusslock and Patel2023; Kashyap et al., Reference Kashyap, Keegan, Liddell, Thomson and Nickerson2021). Specifically, a unidirectional association between mental health and social functioning (with the exception of perceived social responsibility) may be more likely in contexts of impermanence, such as refugee transit settings like Indonesia. In transit contexts, a refugee’s ability to leverage social resources to buffer against mental ill-health is often undermined by notable external factors. For example, in Indonesia, many refugees have travelled alone, or without critical social attachment figures, and thus rebuilding important social connections can be challenging (Liddell et al., Reference Liddell, Batch, Bulnes-Diez, Hellyer, Wong, Byrow and Nickerson2020; Liddell, Byrow et al., Reference Liddell, Byrow, O’Donnell, Mau, Batch, McMahon, Bryant and Nickerson2021; Liddell et al., Reference Liddell, Batch, Hellyer, Bulnes-Diez, Kamte, Klassen, Wong, Byrow and Nickerson2022). Additionally, government policies that disallow work rights and mainstream schooling serves to discourage integration. At the same time, refugees in transit settings may be reluctant to develop deep social relationships as they do not intend to stay in the transit country permanently (Mixed Migration Center, 2021). This social context differs markedly to that of non-refugee trauma survivors (who represent the samples in most research studies to date) where there may be greater opportunity to establish, and leverage, social networks to support one’s psychological recovery (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Chung, Wang, Yu and Kenardy2021). Further research investigating differences between the number and types of social connections, and their associations with mental health across displacement contexts (e.g. transit vs permanent resettlement settings) would shed light on this issue.

It is notable that greater perceived social responsibility was bidirectionally associated with increased psychological symptoms over time in this study. Perceived social responsibility in this study was operationalized as feeling responsible for other people, including concern that others would suffer in the absence of the participant. This finding highlights the substantial social burden experienced by many refugees living in transit settings (Liddell et al., Reference Liddell, Batch, Bulnes-Diez, Hellyer, Wong, Byrow and Nickerson2020). Anecdotal reports from participants in this study revealed that many participants were financially responsible for family members who remained in their country of origin. This is extremely challenging in a context like Indonesia where refugees are denied work rights and access to social security and where resettlement options are very limited. It may be the case that being responsible for others, in a context where an individual’s capacity to fulfill these social responsibilities is very limited, may lead to increased distress. Further, experiencing greater psychological symptoms may impair individuals’ perceived capacity to meet these responsibilities, thus increasing their sense of responsibility and associated distress. This finding points to the importance of structural changes, for example, facilitating permanent resettlement for refugees living in transit settings or, if not possible, increasing the financial and social security of refugees by providing access to essential services, the right to work, and community-building initiatives both within refugee communities and between refugee and host communities. These changes have important implications for alleviating the negative mental health consequences of social burdens often held by refugees in transit settings.

The findings of this study should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. It is notable that our measures of social support in this study focused on perceived social support and were thus not objective indicators of social engagement. Perceived social support may be subject to reporting biases, which may have strengthened the association between this variable and psychological symptoms. Future research could index social networks to gain a more nuanced understanding of the type, quantity, and quality of social interactions and their associations with psychological symptoms. Similarly, a more in-depth scale indexing one’s connection to the host community would be preferable instead of a single item. Finally, the sample in this study was a convenience sample. While our sample demonstrated characteristics that were consistent with the United Nations High Commissioner records of refugees in Indonesia (UNHCR Indonesia, 2018), it is unclear how generalizable these results are to other refugees in Indonesia or those displaced to other transit settings.

Findings from this study have important implications for government and non-government organizations supporting refugees living in transit countries. The finding that greater psychological symptoms were associated with subsequent worsening across multiple facets of social functioning highlights the critical importance of providing mental health-focused programs for refugees living in these settings. In low-resource contexts where formal supports are not available, the ability to successfully navigate the social environment is likely to be critical to survival. These findings suggest that supporting psychological recovery among refugees may also confer social benefits. This is consistent with findings from research suggesting that improving psychological symptoms via the implementation of psychological interventions results in improved social functioning and reduced interpersonal-related stressors (De Silva et al., Reference De Silva, Cooper, Li, Lund and Patel2013; Renner, Cuijpers, & Huibers, Reference Renner, Cuijpers and Huibers2014; Spaaij et al., Reference Spaaij, de Graaff, Akhtar, Kiselev, McDaid, Moergeli, Pfaltz, Schick, Schnyder, Bryant, Cuijpers, Sijbrandij and Morina2023). In many transit settings, there exist very limited mental health services for refugees due to lack of resources to support these programs. These findings suggest that the establishment of these services represents a critical step to support refugees to adapt to their new environments, and to foster social cohesion within societies hosting refugees.

Further, these findings point to the challenges associated with the establishment of social networks for refugees living in transit countries. In low-resource contexts where there may be policies and circumstances that actively discourage the integration of refugees and local communities, the task of developing a social network is very challenging. This is further highlighted by our findings that psychological symptoms make this process even more difficult. It may be the case that difficulty forming social networks reduces the potential down-stream benefits of social engagement on psychological symptoms. If this is the case, greater efforts need to be made to facilitate the development of social networks for refugees living in transit countries.

In conclusion, this study represented the first longitudinal investigation of social functioning and psychological symptoms amongst refugees living in a transit setting. A key finding of this study was that psychological symptoms led to significantly reduced social functioning over time. Findings highlight the importance of the provision of mental health services for refugees living in a state of uncertainty to facilitate positive social outcomes.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724003519.

Data availability statement

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the refugees and asylum-seekers living in Indonesia who took part in this study. The authors acknowledge the contributions of HOST International, SUAKA and Universitas Gadjah Mada to this study. We wish to thank Yunizar Adiputera and Shaila Tieken, for their contribution to study design and implementation, refugee organizations and services in Indonesia, for their assistance with recruitment, and finally, community interpreters, for the translation of study measures.

Funding statement

The current study was supported by an Australian Research Council Linkage Grant (LP170100852). AN was supported by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Investigator Leadership Grant. PS was supported by an MQ: Transforming Mental Health Postdoctoral Scholarship (MPSIP\15).

Competing interest

The authors declare that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

References

Ager, A., & Strang, A. (2008). Understanding integration: A conceptual framework. Journal of Refugee Studies, 21(2), 166191. https://doi.org/10.1093/jrs/fen016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Adhami, M., Berglund, E., Wångdahl, J., & Salari, R. (2022). A cross-sectional study of health and well-being among newly settled refugee migrants in Sweden-The role of health literacy, social support and self-efficacy. PloS One, 17(12), e0279397. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0279397CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, B., Brewin, C. R., & Rose, S. (2003). Gender, social support, and PTSD in victims of violent crime. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(4), 421427. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024478305142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Araya, M., Chotai, J., Komproe, I. H., & de Jong, J. T. V. M. (2007). Gender differences in traumatic life events, coping strategies, perceived social support and sociodemographics among postconflict displaced persons in Ethiopia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42(4), 307315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-007-0166-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthold, S. M., Loomis, A. M., Kuoch, T., Scully, M., Hin-McCormick, M. M., Casavant, B., & Buckley, T. (2019). Social disconnection as a risk factor for health among Cambodian refugees and their offspring in the United States. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 21(2), 290298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10903-018-0760-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonanno, G. A., Chen, S., & Galatzer-Levy, I. R. (2023). Resilience to potential trauma and adversity through regulatory flexibility. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(11), 663675. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00233-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Çankaya, S., Alan Dikmen, H., & Dereli Yılmaz, S. (2020). Investigation of social support perceptions and hopelessness levels of refugee women in Turkey. International Social Work, 63(4), 459472. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020872818798002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, W., Hall, B. J., Ling, L., & Renzaho, A. M. (2017). Pre-migration and post-migration factors associated with mental health in humanitarian migrants in Australia and the moderation effect of post-migration stressors: Findings from the first wave data of the BNLA cohort study. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 4(3), 218229. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30032-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 310357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craske, M. G., Herzallah, M. M., Nusslock, R., & Patel, V. (2023). From neural circuits to communities: An integrative multidisciplinary roadmap for global mental health. Nature Mental Health, 1(1), 1224. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44220-022-00012-wCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curby, N. (2020). The wait: Indonesia’s refugees describe life stuck in an interminable limbo. The Guardian.Google Scholar
De Silva, M. J., Cooper, S., Li, H. L., Lund, C., & Patel, V. (2013). Effect of psychosocial interventions on social functioning in depression and schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 202(4), 253260. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.118018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1993). Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6(4), 459473. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00974317Google Scholar
Forbes, D., Alkemade, N., Hopcraft, D., Hawthorne, G., O’Halloran, P., Elhai, J. D., McHugh, T., Bates, G., Novaco, R. W., Bryant, R., & Lewis, V. (2014). Evaluation of the Dimensions of Anger Reactions-5 (DAR-5) Scale in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(8), 830835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.09.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haer, R., Scharpf, F., & Hecker, T. (2021). The social legacies of conflict: The mediating role of mental health with regard to the association between war exposure and social capital of Burundian refugees. Psychology of Violence, 11(1), 4049. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henkelmann, J.-R., de Best, S., Deckers, C., Jensen, K., Shahab, M., Elzinga, B., & Molendijk, M. (2020). Anxiety, depression and post-traumatic stress disorder in refugees resettling in high-income countries: Systematic review and meta-analysis. BJPsych Open, 6(4), e68. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.54CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaniasty, K., & Norris, F. H. (1993). A test of the social support deterioration model in the context of natural disaster. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(3), 395408. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.64.3.395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashyap, S., Keegan, D., Liddell, B. J., Thomson, T., & Nickerson, A. (2021). An interaction model of environmental and psychological factors influencing refugee mental health. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 34(1), 257266. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.22636CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, D. W., Taft, C., King, L. A., Hammond, C., & Stone, E. R. (2006). Directionality of the association between social support and posttraumatic stress disorder: A longitudinal investigation1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36(12), 29802992. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00138.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroenke, K., Strine, T. W., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., Berry, J. T., & Mokdad, A. H. (2009). The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. Journal of Affective Disorders, 114(1), 163173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurt, G., Ekhtiari, M., Ventevogel, P., Ersahin, M., Ilkkursun, Z., Akbiyik, N., & Acarturk, C. (2023). Socio-cultural integration of Afghan refugees in Türkiye: The role of traumatic events, post-displacement stressors and mental health. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 32, e51. https://doi.org/10.1017/S204579602300063XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahiri, S., van Ommeren, M., & Roberts, B. (2017). The influence of humanitarian crises on social functioning among civilians in low- and middle-income countries: A systematic review. Global Public Health, 12(12), 14611478. https://doi.org/10.1080/17441692.2016.1154585CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, B., Batch, N., Bulnes-Diez, M., Hellyer, S., Wong, J., Byrow, Y., & Nickerson, A. (2020). The effects of family separation on forcibly dispaced people in Australia: Findings from a pilot research project. Australian Red Cross.Google Scholar
Liddell, B. J., Batch, N., Hellyer, S., Bulnes-Diez, M., Kamte, A., Klassen, C., Wong, J., Byrow, Y., & Nickerson, A. (2022). Understanding the effects of being separated from family on refugees in Australia: A qualitative study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 46(5), 647653. https://doi.org/10.1111/1753-6405.13232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, B. J., Byrow, Y., O’Donnell, M., Mau, V., Batch, N., McMahon, T., Bryant, R., & Nickerson, A. (2021). Mechanisms underlying the mental health impact of family separation on resettled refugees. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 55(7), 699710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867420967427CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, B. J., O’Donnell, M., Bryant, R. A., Murphy, S., Byrow, Y., Mau, V., McMahon, T., Benson, G., & Nickerson, A. (2021). The association between COVID-19 related stressors and mental health in refugees living in Australia. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 12(1), 1947564. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2021.1947564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mixed Migration Center. (2021). A transit country no more: Refugees and asylum seekers in Indonesia. https://mixedmigration.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/05/170_Indonesia_Transit_Country_No_More_Research_Report.pdfGoogle Scholar
Mollica, R. F., Caspi-Yavin, Y., Bollini, P., Truong, T., Tor, S., & Lavelle, J. (1992). The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire. Validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180(2), 111116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998). Mplus User’s Guide, Version 8. Muthen & Muthen.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. P., Slewa-Younan, S., & Rioseco, P. (2023). Trajectories of psychological distress and social integration in newly resettled refugees: Findings from the Building a New Life in Australia longitudinal study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-023-02528-7Google ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, A., Bryant, R. A., Steel, Z., Silove, D., & Brooks, R. (2010). The impact of fear for family on mental health in a resettled Iraqi refugee community. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44(4), 229235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, A., Byrow, Y., O’Donnell, M., Bryant, R. A., Mau, V., McMahon, T., Benson, G., & Liddell, B. J. (2022). Cognitive mechanisms underlying the association between trauma exposure, mental health and social engagement in refugees: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 307, 2028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.03.057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, A., Creamer, M., Forbes, D., McFarlane, A. C., O’Donnell, M. L., Silove, D., Steel, Z., Felmingham, K., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., & Bryant, R. A. (2017). The longitudinal relationship between post-traumatic stress disorder and perceived social support in survivors of traumatic injury. Psychological Medicine, 47(1), 115126. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716002361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, A., Hoffman, J., Keegan, D., Kashyap, S., Argadianti, R., Tricesaria, D., Pestalozzi, Z., Nandyatama, R., Khakbaz, M., Nilasari, N., & Liddell, B. (2023). Intolerance of uncertainty, posttraumatic stress, depression, and fears for the future among displaced refugees. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 94, 102672. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2023.102672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pak, Ş., Yurtbakan, T., & Acarturk, C. (2023). Social Support and Resilience among Syrian Refugees: The Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 32(3), 382398. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2022.2061882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patanè, M., Ghane, S., Karyotaki, E., Cuijpers, P., Schoonmade, L., Tarsitani, L., & Sijbrandij, M. (2022). Prevalence of mental disorders in refugees and asylum seekers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Global Mental Health, 9, 250263. https://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2022.29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posselt, M., Eaton, H., Ferguson, M., Keegan, D., & Procter, N. (2019). Enablers of psychological well-being for refugees and asylum seekers living in transitional countries: A systematic review. Health & Social Care in the Community, 27(4), 808823. https://doi.org/10.1111/hsc.12680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renner, F., Cuijpers, P., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2014). The effect of psychotherapy for depression on improvements in social functioning: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(14), 29132926. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713003152CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadler, G. R., Lee, H.-C., Lim, R. S.-H., & Fullerton, J. (2010). Recruitment of hard-to-reach population subgroups via adaptations of the snowball sampling strategy. Nursing & Health Sciences, 12(3), 369374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-2018.2010.00541.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schick, M., Zumwald, A., Knöpfli, B., Nickerson, A., Bryant, R. A., Schnyder, U., Müller, J., & Morina, N. (2016). Challenging future, challenging past: The relationship of social integration and psychological impairment in traumatized refugees. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 7(1), 28057. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v7.28057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuster, T. L., Kessler, R. C., & Aseltine, R. H. (1990). Supportive interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(3), 423438. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00938116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spaaij, J., de Graaff, A. M., Akhtar, A., Kiselev, N., McDaid, D., Moergeli, H., Pfaltz, M. C., Schick, M., Schnyder, U., Bryant, R. A., Cuijpers, P., Sijbrandij, M., Morina, N., & STRENGTHS consortium. (2023). The effect of a low-level psychological intervention (PM+) on post-migration living difficulties—Results from two studies in Switzerland and in the Netherlands. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 127, 152421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2023.152421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steel, Z., Chey, T., Silove, D., Marnane, C., Bryant, R. A., & van Ommeren, M. (2009). Association of torture and other potentially traumatic events with mental health outcomes among populations exposed to mass conflict and displacement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA, 302(5), 537549. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strang, A. B., & Quinn, N. (2021). Integration or isolation? Refugees’ social connections and wellbeing. Journal of Refugee Studies, 34(1), 328353. https://doi.org/10.1093/jrs/fez040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taherdoost, H., Sahibuddin, S., & Jalaliyoon, N. (2022). Exploratory factor analysis; concepts and theory (SSRN Scholarly Paper 4178683). https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=4178683Google Scholar
UNHCR Indonesia. (2018). UNHCR Indonesia Monthly Statistical Report, November 2018. UNHCR Indonesia.Google Scholar
Wachter, K., Bunn, M., Schuster, R. C., Boateng, G. O., Cameli, K., & Johnson-Agbakwu, C. E. (2021). A scoping review of social support research among refugees in resettlement: Implications for conceptual and empirical research. Journal of Refugee Studies, 35(1), 368395. https://doi.org/10.1093/jrs/feab040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Chung, M. C., Wang, N., Yu, X., & Kenardy, J. (2021). Social support and posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 85, 101998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101998CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yildirim, H., Isik, K., Firat, T. Y., & Aylaz, R. (2020). Determining the correlation between social support and hopelessness of syrian refugees living in Turkey. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 58(7), 2733. https://doi.org/10.3928/02793695-20200506-04CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoellner, L. A., Foa, E. B., & Brigidi, B. D. (1999). Interpersonal friction and PTSD in female victims of sexual and nonsexual assault. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12(4), 689700. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024777303848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Factor loadings in metric invariance model for confirmatory factor analysis at time 1

Figure 2

Table 3. Model parameters in structural model

Supplementary material: File

Nickerson et al. supplementary material

Nickerson et al. supplementary material
Download Nickerson et al. supplementary material(File)
File 18.9 KB