Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T13:54:16.529Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2020

Daniel Allington*
Affiliation:
Department of Digital Humanities, King's College London, Strand, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
Bobby Duffy
Affiliation:
Policy Institute, King's College London, Strand, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
Simon Wessely
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, King's College London, Strand, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
Nayana Dhavan
Affiliation:
Department of Digital Humanities, King's College London, Strand, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
James Rubin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, King's College London, Strand, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Daniel Allington, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Social media platforms have long been recognised as major disseminators of health misinformation. Many previous studies have found a negative association between health-protective behaviours and belief in the specific form of misinformation popularly known as ‘conspiracy theory’. Concerns have arisen regarding the spread of COVID-19 conspiracy theories on social media.

Methods

Three questionnaire surveys of social media use, conspiracy beliefs and health-protective behaviours with regard to COVID-19 among UK residents were carried out online, one using a self-selecting sample (N = 949) and two using stratified random samples from a recruited panel (N = 2250, N = 2254).

Results

All three studies found a negative relationship between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and COVID-19 health-protective behaviours, and a positive relationship between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and use of social media as a source of information about COVID-19. Studies 2 and 3 also found a negative relationship between COVID-19 health-protective behaviours and use of social media as a source of information, and Study 3 found a positive relationship between health-protective behaviours and use of broadcast media as a source of information.

Conclusions

When used as an information source, unregulated social media may present a health risk that is partly but not wholly reducible to their role as disseminators of health-related conspiracy beliefs.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Conspiracism is the tendency to assume that major public events are secretly orchestrated by powerful and malevolent entities acting in concert (Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Uscinski, Sutton, Cichocka, Nefes, Siang Ang and Deravi2019). The idea that such plotting explains social reality was influentially termed ‘the conspiracy theory of society’ by Popper (Reference Popper1969), and what Hofstadter termed ‘conspiratorial fantasies’ (Reference Hofstadter1964) are now popularly referred to as ‘conspiracy theories’. Here, the more neutral ‘conspiracy beliefs’ is preferred. Online, such beliefs are now frequently offered as explanations of Coronavirus Disease 2019, or COVID-19. This outbreak of conspiracism is only the latest wave in an ongoing ‘deluge of conflicting information, misinformation and manipulated information on social media’ which some researchers have long argued ‘should be recognised as a global public-health threat’ (Larson, Reference Larson2018, p. 309).

Multiple studies have found a link between medical conspiracy beliefs and reluctance to engage in health-protective behaviours with regard to vaccination or safer sex (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Surian, Leask, Dey, Mandl and Coiera2017; Goertzel, Reference Goertzel2010; Grebe & Nattrass, Reference Grebe and Nattrass2011; Jolley & Douglas, Reference Jolley and Douglas2014; Thorburn & Bogart, Reference Thorburn and Bogart2005; Zimmerman et al., Reference Zimmerman, Wolfe, Fox, Fox, Nowalk, Troy and Sharp2005). This raises the possibility that the circulation of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs might be associated with similar risks. Indeed, two recent studies have found a negative relationship between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and health-protective behaviours intended to help control the COVID-19 pandemic (Allington & Dhavan, Reference Allington and Dhavan2020; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Waite, Rosebrock, Petit, Causier, East and Lambe2020).

YouTube and Facebook have been identified as major vectors for dissemination of conspiracy beliefs and misinformation, on medical and other topics (AVAAZ, 2020; Bora, Das, Barman, & Borah, Reference Bora, Das, Barman and Borah2018; Buchanan & Beckett, Reference Buchanan and Beckett2014; Byford, Reference Byford2011, p. 11; Chaslot, Reference Chaslot2017; Oi-Yee Li, Bailey, Huynh, & Chan, Reference Oi-Yee Li, Bailey, Huynh and Chan2020; Pandey, Patni, Sing, Sood, & Singh, Reference Pandey, Patni, Sing, Sood and Singh2010; Pathak et al., Reference Pathak, Poudel, Karmacharya, Pathak, Aryal, Mahmood and Donata2015; Seymour, Getman, Saraf, Zhang, & Kalenderian, Reference Seymour, Getman, Saraf, Zhang and Kalenderian2014; Sharma, Yadav, Yadav, & Ferdinand, Reference Sharma, Yadav, Yadav and Ferdinand2017). Most studies of Twitter suggest that it plays a similar role (Broniatowski et al., Reference Broniatowski, Jamison, Qi, AlKulaib, Chen, Benton and Dredze2018; Kouzy et al., Reference Kouzy, Jaoude, Kraitem, El Alam, Karam, Adib and Baddour2020; Ortiz-Martínez & Jiménez-Arcia, Reference Ortiz-Martínez and Jiménez-Arcia2017; Oyeyemi, Gabarron, & Wynn, Reference Oyeyemi, Gabarron and Wynn2014). But while social media misinformation is both pervasive and popular, its effects are hard to quantify, and it is unclear which groups are most susceptible to its influence (Wang, McKee, Torbica, & Stuckler, Reference Wang, McKee, Torbica and Stuckler2019).

In the UK, broadcast and print media are regulated (albeit by different mechanisms), while social media are unregulated. For example, when COVID-19 misinformation was propagated by David Icke and Brian Rose on the London Live television station, the owner of the station was sanctioned by the UK broadcasting regulator for disseminating content which ‘had the potential to cause significant harm to viewers’ (Ofcom, 2020, p. 16). However, similar content continues to circulate freely on social media platforms (Brennen, Simon, Howard, & Nielsen, Reference Brennen, Simon, Howard and Nielsen2020). While social media platforms can and do exercise editorial control over the content they disseminate, they appear to do so inconsistently (AVAAZ, 2020). Purveyors of conspiracy beliefs and other misinformation successfully exploit this situation for economic gain (CCDH, 2020; Scott, Reference Scott2020).

We report on three online questionnaire surveys of engagement in COVID-19-specific health-protective behaviours, use of social media as a source of information about COVID-19, and COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, defined as beliefs which entail that the COVID-19 public health crisis was produced through intentional agency (whether through manufacture of the coronavirus or through deliberate exaggeration or incorrect attribution of negative health outcomes). The first and third surveys measured adherence to multiple conspiracy beliefs, while the second measured adherence to just one. The first and second measured media usage in very general terms, while the third separately measured informational reliance on legacy media as well as on specific social media platforms.

Methods: data collection

Data for Study 1 were collected in partnership with CitizenMe. Invitations were sent to all members of a panel of UK residents aged 18 or more who had expressed an interest in answering surveys about COVID-19. Data for Studies 2 and 3 were collected in partnership with Ipsos-MORI, a member of the British Polling Council. The sampling frame was a recruited panel of UK adults aged 16–75. Stratified random samples were selected, with quotas employed to achieve national representativeness with regard to age within gender, region, working status, social grade and education, using census and mid-year estimates from the Office of National Statistics. Questionnaires were completed online. The data collection followed ethical and data protection procedures at King's College London and at the partner organisations. Fieldwork dates were 3–7 April 2020 for Study 1, 1–3 April 2020 for Study 2 and 20–22 May 2020 for Study 3.

Table 1 contains descriptive statistics for all samples. Where gender percentages do not sum to 100, this is because very small numbers of respondents did not self-identify as ‘Female’ or ‘Male’. Due to missing data, total N may vary throughout each study.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, all three samples

Methods: data analysis

Hypotheses regarding relationships between usage of or reliance on sources of information and either conspiracy beliefs or health-protective behaviours were tested using Mann–Whitney–Wilcoxon U tests, with effect sizes estimated using Vargha and Delaney's A with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) calculated through bootstrapping with 1000 replications, on the assumption that each sample can be treated as equivalent to a random sample. Hypotheses concerning conspiracy beliefs and health-protective behaviours were tested using Fisher's exact test both to calculate significance and to estimate odds ratios with a 95% CI (on the same assumption).

In all three studies, hypotheses were tested by treating conspiracy beliefs and health-protective behaviours both individually and in combination, with an aggregate variable to indicate whether a respondent held at least one conspiracy belief and another to indicate whether a respondent engaged in all health-protective behaviours. In Study 3, measurements of media use were both treated individually and aggregated by recoding ordinal variables as numeric variables and taking the mean, creating one aggregate variable for the legacy media and one aggregate variable for social media. Tests covering combinations of raw and aggregated variables are reported in tables in the online Supplementary materials, which are prefixed ‘S’.

Welch unequal variance t tests were used to test for effects of age and Fisher tests were used for effects of gender with regard to aggregate variables, again with a 95% CI. In Study 3, linear probability models were used to control for the effects of multiple variables.

‘Don't know’ responses were treated as missing data. All tests were carried out using base R v. 3.6.1 (R Core Team, 2019), with the exception of Vargha and Delaney's A, which was calculated using the R library, rcompanion v. 2.3.21 (Mangiafico, Reference Mangiafico2020).

Study 1

Introduction

Respondents identified true statements from a list of six statements which included these three conspiracy beliefs: ‘The virus that causes COVID-19 was probably created in a laboratory’, ‘The symptoms of COVID-19 seem to be connected to 5G mobile network radiation’ and ‘The COVID-19 pandemic was planned by certain pharmaceutical corporations and government agencies’. Respondents answered the question ‘How do you find out what's going on in the world?’ using a five-point scale from ‘Always from major newspapers and/or TV channels (including online)’ to ‘Always from social media’, and were also asked to identify behaviours in which they were engaging from a list of six which included three health-protective behaviours (Table 2).

Table 2. Key variables, Study 1

IS.1.1 Answer options:

  1. 1. Always from major newspapers and/or TV channels (including online)

  2. 2. More from major newspapers and/or TV channels (including online) than from social media

  3. 3. Equally from major newspapers and/or TV channels (including online) and from social media

  4. 4. More from social media than from major newspapers and/or TV channels (including online)

  5. 5. Always from social media

  6. 6. Don't know

Hypotheses

H.1.1 A positive relationship between conspiracy belief and preference for social media over mainstream media

H.1.2 A negative relationship between health-protective behaviour and preference for social media over mainstream media

H.1.3 A negative relationship between conspiracy belief and health-protective behaviour

Findings

The most commonly held conspiracy belief was CB.1.1 (a laboratory origin for the coronavirus). Those holding one or more conspiracy beliefs were very slightly younger, t(604.71) = −2.69, p = 0.007, 95% CI (−3.22 to −0.50), while those who engaged in health-protective behaviours were very slightly older, t(781.54) = 3.49, p < 0.001, 95% CI (1.06–3.78). Women were more likely to engage in all health-protective behaviours than men, p = 0.003, 95% CI (1.15–2.05), and there was no relationship between gender and conspiracy belief, p = 0.591, 95% CI (0.80–1.50). See online Supplementary Tables S.1.1–4.

There was a positive relationship between holding one or more conspiracy beliefs and preference for social media over legacy media as a general source of information, U(N1 = 266, N2 = 665) = 99 987.0, p = 0.001, 95% CI (0.52–0.60). Hypothesis H.1.1 is thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.1.5).

There was no relationship between engagement in all health-protective behaviours and preference for social media over legacy media as a general source of information, U(N1 = 580, N2 = 351) = 100 207.0, p = 0.680, 95% CI (0.46–0.53). There was also no relationship for individual health-protective behaviours. H.1.2 is thus unsupported (online Supplementary Table S.1.6).

There was a very strong negative relationship between holding one or more conspiracy beliefs and following all health-protective behaviours, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.34–0.61). The strongest effects were observed for CB.1.2 (a connection between COVID-19 symptoms and 5G). H.1.3 is thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.1.7).

Study 2

Introduction

Respondents were asked whether the statement that ‘Coronavirus was probably made in a laboratory’ was true or false. They were also asked how frequently they were checking social media for information or updates about COVID-19 and whether they were engaging in each of several health-protective behaviours (Table 3).

Table 3. Key variables, Study 2

a Reverse-coded.

IS.2.1 Answer options:

  1. 1. Once an hour or more

  2. 2. Several times a day

  3. 3. Daily

  4. 4. Less often

  5. 5. Never

  6. 6. I don't use social media

  7. 7. Don't know

Answer options 5 and 6 were treated as equivalent, producing an ordinal variable with five levels (as in Study 1).

Hypotheses

H.2.1 A positive relationship between conspiracy belief and frequency of checking social media for information or updates about coronavirus

H.2.2 A negative relationship between health-protective behaviour and frequency of checking social media for information or updates about coronavirus

H.2.3 A negative relationship between conspiracy belief and health-protective behaviour

Findings

Those holding the conspiracy belief were several years younger, t(950.90) = −6.44, p < 0.001, 95% CI (−7.54 to −4.02), while those who followed all health-protective behaviours were several years older, t(589.10) = 9.28, p < 0.001, 95% CI (6.99–10.74). Women were significantly more likely to engage in all health-protective behaviours than men, p < 0.001, 95% CI (1.65–2.62), and slightly less likely to hold the conspiracy belief, although this was not statistically significant, p = 0.214, 95% CI (0.70–1.09). See online Supplementary Tables S.2.1–4.

There was a significant positive relationship between holding the conspiracy belief and frequency of checking social media for news about COVID-19, U(N1 = 468, N2 = 1221) = 343 152, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.57–0.63). H.2.1 is thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.2.5).

There was no relationship between engaging in all health-protective behaviours and frequency of checking social media for information or updates about COVID-19, U(N1 = 1785, N2 = 391) = 343 412.0, p = 0.611, 95% CI (0.46–0.52). However, there were significant negative relationships between frequency of checking social media for information or updates about COVID-19 and two individual health-protective behaviours, i.e. HPB.2.3 (avoiding social encounters outside the home) and HPB.2.4 (not going to work or outside with possible COVID-19 symptoms), as well as negative relationships for two further health-protective behaviours that fell just short of significance. H.2.2 thus receives qualified support (online Supplementary Table S.2.6).

There was a significant negative relationship between holding the conspiracy belief and engagement in all health-protective behaviours, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.39–0.66). There was also a significant negative relationship between holding the conspiracy belief and engagement in each individual health-protective behaviour. H.2.3 is thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.2.7).

Study 3

Introduction

Respondents were asked about four of the same health-protective beliefs as in Study 2 and about a wider range of conspiracy beliefs than in either of the two previous studies. The media usage question was more detailed, inviting respondents to assess how much of their knowledge about COVID-19 was drawn from seven different sources, including four named social media platforms (Table 4).

Table 4. Key variables, study 3

a Reverse-coded.

b Not aggregated.

IS.3.1-7 Answer options:

  1. 1. Nothing at all

  2. 2. Not very much

  3. 3. A fair amount

  4. 4. A great deal

  5. 5. Don't know

Hypotheses

H.3.1 A negative relationship between conspiracy belief and reliance on legacy media (TV and radio broadcasters, newspapers and magazines) for knowledge about COVID-19

H.3.2 A positive relationship between conspiracy belief and reliance on the social media platforms (YouTube, Facebook, WhatsApp, Twitter) for knowledge about COVID-19

H.3.3 A positive relationship between health-protective behaviour and reliance on legacy media for knowledge about COVID-19

H.3.4 A negative relationship between health-protective behaviour and reliance on social media platforms for knowledge about COVID-19

H.3.5 A negative relationship between conspiracy belief and health-protective behaviour

No explicit hypothesis was formulated for information source IS.3.7 (‘Friends and family’), but the implicit hypothesis of an effect was tested for comparative purposes.

Findings

Those holding one or more conspiracy beliefs were slightly younger, t(1597.01) = −4.33, p < 0.001, 95% CI (−5.02 to −1.89). Those who followed all health-protective behaviours were several years older, t(943.24) = 6.98, p < 0.001, 95% CI (3.99–7.12). Women in the sample were significantly more likely to follow all health-protective behaviours than men, p < 0.001, 95% CI (1.65–2.62), and slightly more likely to hold one or more conspiracy beliefs, although this was not statistically significant, p = 0.164, 95% CI (0.94–1.40). See online Supplementary Tables S.3.1–4.

There was a small but significant negative relationship between use of legacy media as a source of knowledge about COVID-19 and belief in one or more conspiracy theories, U(N1 = 884, N2 = 748) = 296 848.5, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.42–0.48). However, the effect of television and radio considered alone was in most cases statistically significant, while the effect of newspapers and magazines considered alone was not. Hypothesis H.3.1 is thus supported, with the caveat that this is primarily due to the contribution made by broadcast media (online Supplementary Table S.3.5).

There was a strong positive relationship between use of social media platforms as sources of knowledge about COVID-19 and holding one or more conspiracy beliefs, U(N1 = 882, N2 = 748) = 424 640.0, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.62–0.67). In almost all cases, there was also a significant positive relationship between each individual conspiracy belief and use of each platform. YouTube had the strongest association with conspiracy beliefs, followed by Facebook. The conspiracy beliefs most strongly associated with social media usage were CB.3.2 (a link between COVID-19 symptoms and 5G) and CB.3.3 (which cast doubt on the existence of the coronavirus). Hypothesis H.3.2 is thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.3.6).

There was also a smaller but significant positive relationship between holding one or more conspiracy beliefs and use of friends and family as a source of information about COVID-19, U(N1 = 878, N2 = 749) = 397 473.5, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.57–0.63). The implicit hypothesis of an effect for reliance on this information source was thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.3.7).

There was a positive relationship between use of legacy media as a source of knowledge about COVID-19 and following all health-protective behaviours; however, this effect was small and of borderline significance U(N1 = 1610, N2 = 563) = 478 174.0, p < 0.046, 95% CI (0.50–0.56). The effect associated with TV and radio was more significant, U(N1 = 1601, N2 = 561) = 481 068.5, p = 0.006, 95% CI (0.51–0.56), while there was no individual effect for newspapers and magazines, U(N1 = 1594, N2 = 553) = 447 648.5, p = 0.564, 95% CI (0.48–0.54). Hypothesis H.3.3 is thus supported, with the caveat that this is largely due to the contribution made by broadcast media (online Supplementary Table S.3.8).

There was a much stronger and more significant negative relationship between use of social media as a source of knowledge about COVID-19 and engagement in health-protective behaviours, U(N1 = 1603, N2 = 563) = 342 191.5, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.35–0.41). Except with regard to HPB.3.1 (hand-washing), where there was a weak negative effect that was only significant with regard to YouTube, WhatsApp and the aggregate social media variable, there was in every case a very highly significant negative relationship between use of each social media platform and each of the health-protective behaviours considered individually (p < 0.001). As with conspiracy beliefs, YouTube was the platform with the strongest association with this undesirable outcome. H.3.4 was thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.3.9).

There was a weaker but still significant negative relationship between use of friends and family as a source of knowledge about COVID-19 and engagement in all health-protective behaviours, U(N1 = 1601, N2 = 560) = 240 145.0, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.40–0.47). The implicit hypothesis of an effect for this source of knowledge was thus supported (online Supplementary Table S.3.10).

There was a strong negative relationship between holding one or more conspiracy beliefs and engagement in all health-protective behaviours, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.29–0.47). Except with regard to belief CB.3.1 (laboratory origin), this relationship was very highly significant with regard to every combination of behaviours and beliefs (p < 0.001). In relation to the aggregate variable, HPB.3.All, the strongest effect was associated with CB.3.3 (which cast doubt on the existence of the coronavirus), followed by CB.3.4 (which holds that deaths from the coronavirus have been exaggerated) and CB.3.2 (a link between COVID-19 symptoms and 5G). H.3.5 was thus supported, with the caveat that some conspiracy beliefs may have stronger effects on behaviour than others (online Supplementary Table S3.11).

Finally, a series of binomial linear probability models were constructed to control for multiple variables as predictors of health-protective behaviour (Table 5). Conspiracy belief emerges as a more powerful predictor of health-protective behaviour non-engagement than gender, and as a more powerful predictor than age once controls for media usage are applied. Media usage – and especially social media usage – appears to be the most powerful predictor of all (at least as represented in these models), with age losing much of its predictive power once it is controlled for. (This contrasts with the finding of qualified or absent effects on behaviour in Studies 1 and 2. However, those studies did not collect such detailed data on media usage.)

Table 5. Linear probability models, probability of following all health-protective behaviours (HPB.3.All)

95% confidence intervals; predictor variables standardised to the range 0–1.

Discussion

The studies reported here find a positive association between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and use of social media as a source of information about COVID-19, and a negative association between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and COVID-19-specific health-protective behaviours, with the strongest negative effects being associated with beliefs that imply that the coronavirus may not exist, that its lethality has been exaggerated, or that its symptoms may have a non-viral cause. In addition, Study 3 and, to a lesser extent, Study 2 find a negative association between use of social media as a source of information about COVID-19 and health-protective behaviours. Study 3 also finds a weaker negative association for use of friends and family as a source of information, and a positive association for use of legacy media, especially broadcast media. Findings are suggestive of a hierarchy of effects associated with different social media platforms, with YouTube appearing to be the most problematic, but all being associated with significant negative effects.

A consistent finding across the studies is that COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs are more likely to be held by younger respondents, and that health-protective behaviour was associated with both age and female gender. However, the regression analysis in Study 3 suggests that effects on health-protective behaviour associated with conspiracy belief and social media use are not accounted for by age or gender, and that some of the apparent effects of age may indeed be accounted for by differences in media usage. That is, it may be that older people are more likely to reject conspiracy beliefs and engage in health-protective behaviours in part because they make more use of broadcast media and less use of social media.

Conclusions

All three studies suggest that conspiracy beliefs act to inhibit health-protective behaviours and that social media act as a vector for such beliefs. Studies 2 and 3 find further evidence of a link between social media and non-engagement in health-protective behaviours, and Study 3 finds evidence of an opposite relationship for legacy media, especially broadcast media. In the UK, broadcast media are subject to official regulation, and many print media platforms are subject to voluntary regulation, but social media are largely unregulated. One wonders how long this state of affairs can be allowed to persist while social media platforms continue to provide a worldwide distribution mechanism for medical misinformation.

Limitations

Study 1 relies on a self-selecting sample, while Studies 2 and 3 rely on stratified random samples from a recruited panel. Moreover, all three studies rely on self-reports for measurement both of media usage and of compliance with health-protective behaviours.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172000224X.

Acknowledgements

Data collection for Study 1 was provided free of charge by CitizenMe. Data collection for Studies 2 and 3 was part-funded by King's Together, an internal programme of funding to enable initial exploratory research and analysis at King's College London, and part-funded by the National Institute for Health Research Health Protection Research Unit (NIHR HPRU) in Emergency Preparedness and Response: a partnership between Public Health England, King's College London, and the University of East Anglia. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of King's College London, CitizenMe, the NIHR, Public Health England or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Footnotes

The online version of this article has been updated since original publication. A notice detailing the changes has also been published at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000593

References

Allington, D., & Dhavan, N. (2020). The relationship between conspiracy beliefs and compliance with public health guidance with regard to COVID-19. Working paper published by the Center for Countering Digital Hate, London. Retrieved from https://kclpure.kcl.ac.uk/portal/files/127 048 253/Allington_and_Dhavan_2020.pdf.Google Scholar
AVAAZ. (2020). How Facebook can flatten the curve of the coronavirus infodemic: study indicates Facebook is rife with bogus cures and conspiracy theories that remain on the platform long enough to put millions of people at risk. Report published by AVAAZ, London. Retrieved from https://avaazimages.avaaz.org/facebook_coronavirus_misinformation.pdf.Google Scholar
Bora, K., Das, D., Barman, B., & Borah, P. (2018). Are Internet videos useful sources of information during global public health emergencies? A case study of YouTube videos during the 2015–16 Zika virus pandemic. Pathogens and Global Health, 112(6), 320328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennen, J. S., Simon, F., Howard, P. N., & Nielsen, R. K. (2020). Types, sources, and claims of COVID-19 misinformation. Report published by the Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism, Oxford. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/types-sources-and-claims-covid-19-misinformation.Google Scholar
Broniatowski, D. A., Jamison, A. M., Qi, S., AlKulaib, L., Chen, T., Benton, A., … Dredze, M. (2018). Weaponized health communication: Twitter bots and Russian trolls amplify the vaccine debate. American Journal of Public Health, 108(10), 13781384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, R., & Beckett, R. D. (2014). Assessment of vaccination-related information for consumers available on Facebook. Health Information and Libraries Journal, 31(3), 227234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byford, J. (2011). Conspiracy theories: A critical introduction. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CCDH. (2020). #DeplatformIcke: How Big Tech powers and profits from David Icke's lies and hate, and why it must stop. Report published by the Center for Countering Digital Hate, London. Retrieved from https://252f2edd-1c8b-49f5-9bb2-cb57bb47e4ba.filesusr.com/ugd/f4d9b9_db8ff469f6914534ac02309bb488f948.pdf.Google Scholar
Chaslot, G. (2017). How YouTube's A.I. boosts alternative facts: YouTube's recommendation A.I. is designed to maximize the time users spend online. Fiction often outperforms reality. Retrieved from https://medium.com/@guillaumechaslot/how-youtubes-a-i-boosts-alternative-facts-3cc276f47cf7.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Siang Ang, C., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40, 335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, A. G., Surian, D., Leask, J., Dey, A., Mandl, K. D., & Coiera, E. (2017). Mapping information exposure on social media to explain differences in HPV vaccine coverage in the United States. Vaccine, 35, 30333040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Waite, F., Rosebrock, L., Petit, A., Causier, C., East, A., … Lambe, S. (2020). Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England. Psychological Medicine, 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. European Molecular Biology Organization Reports, 11, 493499.Google Scholar
Grebe, E., & Nattrass, N. (2011). AIDS conspiracy beliefs and unsafe sex in Cape Town. AIDS and Behavior, 16(3), 761773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, R. (1964). The paranoid style in American politics. Harper's Magazine(November), 7786.Google Scholar
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kouzy, R., Jaoude, J. A., Kraitem, A., El Alam, M. B., Karam, B., Adib, E., … Baddour, K. (2020). Coronavirus goes viral: Quantifying the COVID-19 misinformation epidemic on Twitter. Cureus, 12(3), e7255.Google ScholarPubMed
Larson, H. J. (2018). The biggest pandemic risk? Viral misinformation. Nature, 562(7727), 309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangiafico, S. (2020). rcompanion: Functions to support extension education program evaluation. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rcompanion.Google Scholar
Ofcom (2020). London Real: Covid-19. Ofcom Broadcast and On Demand Bulletin. Retrieved from https://www.ofcom.org.uk/__data/assets/pdf_file/0020/194402/sanction-decision-estv.pdf.Google Scholar
Oi-Yee Li, H., Bailey, A., Huynh, D., & Chan, J. (2020). Youtube as a source of information on COVID-19: A pandemic of misinformation? BMJ Global Health, 5, e002604.Google Scholar
Ortiz-Martínez, Y., & Jiménez-Arcia, L. F. (2017). Yellow fever outbreaks and Twitter: Rumours and misinformation. American Journal of Infection Control, 45, 815816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyeyemi, S. O., Gabarron, E., & Wynn, R. (2014). Ebola, Twitter, and misinformation: A dangerous combination? British Medical Journal, 349, g6178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pandey, A., Patni, N., Sing, M., Sood, A., & Singh, G. (2010). Youtube as a source of information on the H1N1 influenza pandemic. American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 38(3), e1e3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pathak, R., Poudel, D. R., Karmacharya, P., Pathak, A., Aryal, M. R., Mahmood, M., & Donata, A. (2015). Youtube as a source of information on Ebola virus disease. North American Journal of Medical Sciences, 7(7), 306309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popper, K. (1969). Conjectures and refutations: The growth of scientific knowledge. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Scott, B. (2020). The YouTuber accused of using coronavirus to scam his followers: Brian Rose has raised nearly £1 million to fund a ‘free speech’ platform where he'll host video interviews with conspiracy theorists and other guests. But some followers have questioned how the money will really be used. VICE, 21 May. Retrieved from https://www.vice.com/en_uk/article/bv8x5a/london-real-brian-rose-digital-freedom-coronavirus.Google Scholar
Seymour, B., Getman, R., Saraf, A., Zhang, L. H., & Kalenderian, E. (2014). When advocacy obscures accuracy online: Digital pandemics of public health misinformation through an antiflouride case study. American Journal of Public Health, 105, 517523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharma, M., Yadav, K., Yadav, N., & Ferdinand, K. C. (2017). Zika virus pandemic – analysis of Facebook as a social media health information platform. American Journal of Infection Control, 45(3), 301302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorburn, S., & Bogart, L. M. (2005). Conspiracy beliefs about birth control: Barriers to pregnancy prevention among African Americans of reproductive age. Health Education & Behavior, 32, 474487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., McKee, M., Torbica, A., & Stuckler, D. (2019). Systematic literature review on the spread of health-related misinformation on social media. Social Science & Medicine, 240, 112552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, R. K., Wolfe, R. M., Fox, D. E., Fox, J. R., Nowalk, M. P., Troy, J. A., & Sharp, L. K. (2005). Vaccine criticism on the World Wide Web. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 7(2), e17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, all three samples

Figure 1

Table 2. Key variables, Study 1

Figure 2

Table 3. Key variables, Study 2

Figure 3

Table 4. Key variables, study 3

Figure 4

Table 5. Linear probability models, probability of following all health-protective behaviours (HPB.3.All)

Supplementary material: PDF

Allington et al. supplementary material

Supplementary tables

Download Allington et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 208.5 KB