Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:52:15.015Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal clinical and functional outcome in distinct cognitive subgroups of first-episode psychosis: a cluster analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2021

Priscilla P. Oomen*
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Marieke J. H. Begemann
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Bodyl A. Brand
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Lieuwe de Haan
Affiliation:
Department of Early Psychosis, Amsterdam UMC, Academic Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Wim Veling
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Sanne Koops
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Jim van Os
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MheNS), Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands King's College London, King's Health Partners Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London, UK
Filip Smit
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, location VUmc, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Clinical, Neuro and Developmental Psychology, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Centre of Economic Evaluation & Machine Learning, Trimbos Institute (Netherlands Institute of Mental Health), Utrecht, The Netherlands
P. Roberto Bakker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MheNS), Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Department of Research, Arkin Mental Health Care, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Nico van Beveren
Affiliation:
Antes Center for Mental Health Care, Rotterdam, The Netherlands Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Nynke Boonstra
Affiliation:
NHL/Stenden, University of Applied Sciences, Leeuwarden, The Netherlands KieN VIP Mental Health Care Services, Leeuwarden, The Netherlands
Sinan Gülöksüz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MheNS), Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut, USA
Martijn Kikkert
Affiliation:
Department of Research, Arkin Mental Health Care, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Joran Lokkerbol
Affiliation:
Centre of Economic Evaluation & Machine Learning, Trimbos Institute (Netherlands Institute of Mental Health), Utrecht, The Netherlands
Machteld Marcelis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht, The Netherlands Institute for Mental Health Care Eindhoven (GGzE), Eindhoven, The Netherlands
Bram-Sieben Rosema
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Franciska de Beer
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Shiral S. Gangadin
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Chris N. W. Geraets
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Erna van ‘t Hag
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Yudith Haveman
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Inge van der Heijden
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands Janssen-Cilag B.V., Breda, the Netherlands
Alban E. Voppel
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Elske Willemse
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Therese van Amelsvoort
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MheNS), Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Mondriaan Mental Health Care, Heerlen, The Netherlands
Maarten Bak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MheNS), Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands Mondriaan Mental Health Care, Heerlen, The Netherlands
Albert Batalla
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Agaath Been
Affiliation:
Dimence Institute for Mental Health, Deventer, Zwolle, The Netherlands
Marinte van den Bosch
Affiliation:
Dimence Institute for Mental Health, Deventer, Zwolle, The Netherlands
Truus van den Brink
Affiliation:
Early Intervention Team, GGZ Centraal, Amersfoort, The Netherlands
Gunnar Faber
Affiliation:
Yulius, Mental Health Institute, Dordrecht, The Netherlands
Koen P. Grootens
Affiliation:
Reinier van Arkel Institute for Mental Health Care, ‘s Hertogenbosch, The Netherlands
Martin de Jonge
Affiliation:
Program for Psychosis & Severe Mental Illness, Pro Persona Mental Health, Wolfheze, The Netherlands
Rikus Knegtering
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands Lentis Research, Lentis Psychiatric Institute, Groningen, The Netherlands
Jörg Kurkamp
Affiliation:
Center for Youth with Psychosis, Mediant ABC Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
Amrita Mahabir
Affiliation:
Early Psychosis Team, GGNet, Apeldoorn, The Netherlands
Gerdina H. M. Pijnenborg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychotic Disorders, GGZ-Drenthe, Assen, The Netherlands Department of Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, Faculty BSS, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Tonnie Staring
Affiliation:
Department ABC Early Psychosis, Altrecht Psychiatric Institute, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Natalie Veen
Affiliation:
GGZ Delfland, Delfland Institute for Mental Health Care, Delft, The Netherlands
Selene Veerman
Affiliation:
Community Mental Health, Mental Health Service Noord-Holland Noord, Alkmaar, The Netherlands
Sybren Wiersma
Affiliation:
Early Intervention Psychosis Team, GGZ inGeest Specialized Mental Health Care, Hoofddorp, The Netherlands
Ellen Graveland
Affiliation:
Yulius, Mental Health Institute, Dordrecht, The Netherlands
Joelle Hoornaar
Affiliation:
Antes Center for Mental Health Care, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Iris E. C. Sommer
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: P.P. Oomen, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Cognitive deficits may be characteristic for only a subgroup of first-episode psychosis (FEP) and the link with clinical and functional outcomes is less profound than previously thought. This study aimed to identify cognitive subgroups in a large sample of FEP using a clustering approach with healthy controls as a reference group, subsequently linking cognitive subgroups to clinical and functional outcomes.

Methods

204 FEP patients were included. Hierarchical cluster analysis was performed using baseline brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS). Cognitive subgroups were compared to 40 controls and linked to longitudinal clinical and functional outcomes (PANSS, GAF, self-reported WHODAS 2.0) up to 12-month follow-up.

Results

Three distinct cognitive clusters emerged: relative to controls, we found one cluster with preserved cognition (n = 76), one moderately impaired cluster (n = 74) and one severely impaired cluster (n = 54). Patients with severely impaired cognition had more severe clinical symptoms at baseline, 6- and 12-month follow-up as compared to patients with preserved cognition. General functioning (GAF) in the severely impaired cluster was significantly lower than in those with preserved cognition at baseline and showed trend-level effects at 6- and 12-month follow-up. No significant differences in self-reported functional outcome (WHODAS 2.0) were present.

Conclusions

Current results demonstrate the existence of three distinct cognitive subgroups, corresponding with clinical outcome at baseline, 6- and 12-month follow-up. Importantly, the cognitively preserved subgroup was larger than the severely impaired group. Early identification of discrete cognitive profiles can offer valuable information about the clinical outcome but may not be relevant in predicting self-reported functional outcomes.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Despite relatively successful treatment of clinical symptoms after first-episode psychosis (FEP) (Kahn et al. Reference Kahn, Winter van Rossum, Leucht, McGuire, Lewis, Leboyer and Wilson2018), many patients continue to experience ongoing functional impairment in day-to-day life (Henry et al. Reference Henry, Amminger, Harris, Yuen, Harrigan, Prosser and McGorry2010; Lally et al. Reference Lally, Ajnakina, Stubbs, Cullinane, Murphy, Gaughran and Murray2017). Large variability exists in the outcome of FEP with recovery rates ranging from 13.5% to 38% (Jääskeläinen et al. Reference Jääskeläinen, Juola, Hirvonen, McGrath, Saha, Isohanni and Miettunen2013; Lally et al. Reference Lally, Ajnakina, Stubbs, Cullinane, Murphy, Gaughran and Murray2017). A significant minority of patients shows the excellent recovery, but a large proportion of patients continues to exhibit moderate or severe functional impairment (Jääskeläinen et al. Reference Jääskeläinen, Juola, Hirvonen, McGrath, Saha, Isohanni and Miettunen2013; Lally et al. Reference Lally, Ajnakina, Stubbs, Cullinane, Murphy, Gaughran and Murray2017). Recovery rates appear to be stable 2 years after illness onset as demonstrated in a large meta-analysis (Lally et al. Reference Lally, Ajnakina, Stubbs, Cullinane, Murphy, Gaughran and Murray2017), underscoring the importance of identifying factors that can predict outcome overtime in the early stages of disease onset. However, most longitudinal studies have examined predictors at the diagnostic group level and do not take the high heterogeneity between individual patients with the same diagnosis into account (Santesteban-Echarri et al. Reference Santesteban-Echarri, Paino, Rice, González-Blanch, McGorry, Gleeson and Alvarez-Jimenez2017; Suvisaari et al. Reference Suvisaari, Mantere, Keinänen, Mäntylä, Rikandi, Lindgren and Raij2018). This hampers a more personalized treatment approach in clinical care, as individuals require treatment tailored to their illness profile.

During past years, cognitive impairment received substantial attention because of its presence prior to illness onset and associations with both clinical and functional outcomes over time (Helldin, Mohn, Olsson, & Hjärthag, Reference Helldin, Mohn, Olsson and Hjärthag2020; Johansson, Hjärthag, & Helldin, Reference Johansson, Hjärthag and Helldin2020; Lindgren, Holm, Kieseppä, & Suvisaari, Reference Lindgren, Holm, Kieseppä and Suvisaari2020; Santesteban-Echarri et al. Reference Santesteban-Echarri, Paino, Rice, González-Blanch, McGorry, Gleeson and Alvarez-Jimenez2017). Some authors have considered cognitive dysfunction to be the core feature of schizophrenia (Heinrichs, Reference Heinrichs2005). However, recent literature shows that global cognitive deficits are not a general finding, as it is becoming increasingly apparent that several cognitive subgroups may exist within the FEP population, including a substantial subset of patients that remains cognitively intact (Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner, & Rossell, Reference Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner and Rossell2019; Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Klein, Desler, Lill, Gallinat and Schneider2017; Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling, & Allott, Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017). Also, the predictive value of cognitive deficits in terms of functional impairment may be less pronounced as previously thought. Notably, a recent meta-analysis showed only small to medium effect sizes for the association between cognition and functional outcome, leaving a significant proportion of the variance unexplained (Halverson et al. Reference Halverson, Orleans-Pobee, Merritt, Sheeran, Fett and Penn2019). It is plausible that variance in both functional and clinical outcomes may be related to differences in severity of cognitive dysfunction. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that cognitive performance in a “neuropsychologically normal” range does not correlate well with aspects of everyday functioning whereas more severe levels of cognitive impairment do seem to be associated with functional outcomes (Strassnig et al. Reference Strassnig, Bowie, Pinkham, Penn, Twamley, Patterson and Harvey2018). This underscores the value of grouping FEP patients into subtypes along the cognitive continuum, demonstrating possible subgroups with distinct illness profiles.

An essential and relatively novel solution for determining homogeneous subgroups is a data-driven clustering approach. Defining subgroups based on baseline cognitive profile may provide crucial information regarding functional outcome and prognosis. Such information is urgently needed, as the high heterogeneity and lack of good predictors hamper clinicians in providing optimal care for individual patients. Early identification of risk factors associated with poor outcomes is highly valuable as this would aid individually tailored interventions that may positively impact the long-term outcome.

The current study includes a large sample of FEP patients who were 3–6 months in remission of their psychotic symptoms at baseline, to identify homogeneous subgroups of cognition based on a data-driven clustering approach. Factors that may influence cognitive function, such as the distraction by unusual ideas and/or hallucinations, long-term antipsychotic medication use or the duration of illness, are limited in the current sample as all patients were in a similar early stage of their illness. Emergent cognitive subgroups were subsequently compared to healthy controls to assess the level of cognitive (under)performance. Cognitive subgroups were then evaluated regarding clinical [Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF)] and functional [WHO Disability Assessment Scale 2.0 (WHODAS2.0)] outcome at baseline and longitudinally at 6- and 12-month follow-up. The clinician-rated GAF has been widely used in clinical and research settings and has been adopted as meaningful, however, the DSM-5 recommends a new tool for the assessment of global functioning and impairment, the WHODAS 2.0, a patient self-report assessment tool that evaluates the patient's ability to perform activities in six domains of functioning (Gold, Reference Gold2014). Based on a recent systematic review regarding cognitive subgrouping studies in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, we expected to find three distinct cognitive subtypes; a relatively intact cognitive subgroup, an intermediate cognitive subgroup and a globally impaired subgroup (Carruthers et al. Reference Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner and Rossell2019). We further hypothesized that emergent cognitive subtypes are characterized by differences in both clinical and functional outcomes at baseline and follow-up.

Method

Participants

Data were used from the ongoing Handling Antipsychotic Medication: Long-term Evaluation of Targeted Treatment (HAMLETT) study (Begemann et al. Reference Begemann, Thompson, Veling, Gangadin, Geraets, Van ‘T Hag and Sommer2020). Patients were recruited from outpatient settings in 24 healthcare centers throughout the Netherlands. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and study procedures were performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki (64th WMA general assembly; October 2013). Ethics approval was obtained from the research and ethics committee of the University Medical Center Groningen, the Netherlands (protocol number: NL 62202.042.17, trial registration EudraCT number: 2017-002406-12). Recruitment and study procedures are described in detail by Begemann et al. (Reference Begemann, Thompson, Veling, Gangadin, Geraets, Van ‘T Hag and Sommer2020).

In short, the current study included data from 204 patients aged between 16 and 60 years old with the first episode of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, schizophreniform disorder, brief psychotic disorder, delusional disorder, substance/medication-induced psychotic disorder, or those classified as Unspecified Schizophrenia Spectrum and Other Psychotic Disorders (DSM-5, or as described in the International Classification of Diseases-10). Diagnosis and duration of illness were established by their treating psychiatrist and confirmed by the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH) (Andreasen, Flaum, & Arndt, Reference Andreasen, Flaum and Arndt1992). At baseline, all patients were 3–6 months in remission of their first psychotic episode and used antipsychotic medication. Symptomatic remission is defined as “sustained improvement of psychotic symptoms to the level that any remaining psychotic symptoms (such as hallucinatory experiences, unusual thought content, conceptual disorganization) are mild, which means (consistent with international remission criteria) that they do not interfere with behavior and daily functioning.”

Self-reports of current antipsychotic medication use (mg/day) were converted into a chlorpromazine equivalent (CPZE, mg/day) for each patient (Gardner, Murphy, O'Donnell, Centorrino, & Baldessarini, Reference Gardner, Murphy, O'Donnell, Centorrino and Baldessarini2010). The highest educational level achieved (CASH) (Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Flaum and Arndt1992), was converted into the number of years of education (YOE; see Online Supplementary Table S1).

Moreover, 40 healthy controls were included as a reference group for cognitive functioning. Healthy controls did not have any history of psychiatric illness and were aged between 19 and 45 years (Trial registration: ABR NL50657.041.14).

Procedures

Cognitive testing

Cognitive performance was assessed at baseline using the Dutch version of the brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia (BACS) (Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004). The test consists of six subtests that assess different cognitive domains, including:

  1. (1) List Learning – Verbal memory

  2. (2) Digit Sequencing Task – Working memory

  3. (3) Token Motor Task – Motor speed

  4. (4) Category Instances and Controlled Oral Word Association Test – Verbal fluency

  5. (5) Symbol Coding – Attention and information processing speed

  6. (6) Tower of London – Executive function

Performances of all participants on the subtests of the BACS were standardized by creating z-scores adjusted for gender and age using the norms of Keefe et al. (Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004). A composite z-score was calculated by averaging all of the six standardized primary measures from the BACS. Participants missing more than 2 cognitive sub-scores were excluded from analysis (n = 2). For participants with ⩽2 missing sub-scores, scores were replaced by the corresponding population mean for that specific domain (n = 8).

Clinical outcome

Clinical symptomatology was assessed by trained central study personnel using the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS) at baseline, 6 months and 12-month follow-up (Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1987).

In addition, clinical global functioning was evaluated by trained central study personnel at baseline, 6 months and 12-month follow-up using the GAF (Jones, Thornicroft, Coffey, & Dunn, Reference Jones, Thornicroft, Coffey and Dunn1995).

To ensure data quality, assessors are comprehensively trained and the central team of assessors have biannual meetings during which inter-rater reliability is assessed and protocol adherence is checked.

Self-reported functional outcome

Self-reported global functioning and disability were evaluated at baseline, 6 months and 12-month follow-up using the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0 (WHODAS 2.0). This questionnaire consists of 36 items covering six domains of functioning in everyday life: cognition (understanding and communicating), mobility (moving and getting around), self-care (hygiene, eating, and staying alone), getting along (interacting with other people), life activities (domestic responsibilities, leisure, work and school) and participation (joining in community activities). Participants respond to each item on a 5-point scale from 0 (No Difficulty) to 4 (Extreme Difficulty/Cannot Do). Overall scores range from 0 to 100 with higher scores indicating a greater level of self-reported disability (Üstün, Reference Üstün2010).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25 (IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.). Healthy controls and FEP patients were compared on demographic variables such as gender, age, years of education and cognitive performance using Pearson's chi-square (categorical variables) and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA, continuous variables).

Patients were clustered based on their composite BACS Z-score, as all domains of cognitive functioning that are assessed by the BACS are found to be consistently impaired in schizophrenia (Keefe et al. Reference Keefe, Goldberg, Harvey, Gold, Poe and Coughenour2004). A hierarchical clustering approach (HCA) was performed for the total sample of patients. Case similarity was computed using squared Euclidean distance and Ward's linkage was used as agglomeration procedure specification. By using Ward's method, the difference or distance between two clusters is defined by the increase of the sum of squares when merging them (Ward, Reference Ward1963). After careful inspection of the dendrogram and meaningful jumps in the agglomeration schedule coefficients, the optimal number of clusters was defined. For the dendrogram and agglomeration schedule, see Supplementary Figure S1 and S2. Next, a k-means clustering technique was applied to optimize the retained clusters. The number of k clusters and initial partitions in the k-means solution was defined by results obtained from the hierarchical clustering procedure.

Emergent cognitive patient clusters were then compared to a group of healthy controls to verify the level of cognitive (under)performance. Furthermore, differences in demographic variables and clinical characteristics were assessed using Pearson's χ2 (categorical variables) and One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA, continuous variables). Subsequently, cognitive patient clusters were compared on both clinical (PANSS, GAF) and functional (WHODAS 2.0) outcomes using ANOVA for baseline comparisons and ANCOVA for comparisons at 6- and 12-month follow-up. Post hoc comparisons were conducted for all significant ANOVA and ANCOVA effects, using Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Results

Demographics

A total of 204 patients and 40 healthy controls were included at baseline. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Online Supplementary Table S2. The group of patients consisted of 148 males (72.5%), the healthy controls included 32 males (80.0%). Patients were significantly older (M = 27.93, s.d. = 8.90) than healthy individuals (M = 24.48, s.d. = 4.98), (p = 0.018). Patients attained fewer years of education compared to healthy controls (p = 0.006) and patients scored significantly worse on both the BACS composite and all subtests (all p < 0.001, executive functioning p = 0.047). At 6- and 12-month follow-up, the sample consisted of 145 and 132 patients respectively.

Cluster solution

Hierarchical clustering (Ward's method) and K-means optimization using BACS composite scores for the total sample of patients resulted in three distinct cognitive clusters (Table 1). Subgroups were subsequently compared to a group of healthy controls to assess the level of cognitive (under)performance.

Table 1. Mean (s.d.) baseline demographic and cognitive characteristics for FEP cognitive clusters and healthy controls

FEP, first-episode psychosis; BACS, brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia; df, degrees of freedom.

* a HC significantly different from the relatively preserved cluster; b HC significantly different from the moderately impaired cluster; c HC significantly different from the severely impaired cluster; d relatively preserved cluster significantly different from moderately impaired cluster; e relatively preserved cluster significantly different from severely impaired cluster; f moderately impaired cluster significantly different from a severely impaired cluster.

One cluster could be described as a relatively preserved group (n = 76). The BACS composite score was not significantly different compared to healthy controls, yet these patients scored significantly lower on attention and processing speed compared to healthy controls (p = 0.008). An intermediate or moderately impaired cognitive cluster (n = 74) displaying reduced functioning on all cognitive domains compared to healthy controls (all p < 0.001), except for executive function (p = 0.730) was observed. Lastly, the severely impaired cognitive cluster (n = 54) showed significant impairments across all domains assessed relative to the controls, with working memory and motor speed showing the most severe deficits (all p < 0.001). Results are demonstrated in Figs 1 and 2.

Fig. 1. BACS composite means Z-scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters and healthy controls Error bars represent standard deviations. All groups showed significant differences (p < 0.05) except for healthy controls compared to the preserved cognitive cluster. BACS, brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

Fig. 2. BACS subdomain means Z-scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters and healthy controls Pentagons represent mean BACS Z-scores. For detailed statistics, see Table 1. BACS, brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

The relatively preserved cluster was significantly older than the healthy controls (p = 0.011), but no age differences were demonstrated between the three cognitive patient clusters. The moderately impaired cluster and severely impaired cluster had received significantly fewer years of education compared to both the healthy controls (p = 0.007 and p < 0.001, respectively) and the relatively preserved cluster (p = 0.004 and p < 0.001, respectively). Parental years of education attained showed an overall effect (F(3) = 3.11, p = 0.027) but no significant differences between clusters. Furthermore, chlorpromazine equivalents were not significantly different between clusters (p = 0.107).

Clinical outcome

Although all patients were in clinical remission at baseline, the subgroup of patients with severely impaired cognition had significantly higher symptom severity compared to the cognitively preserved subgroup, with higher scores on the PANSS total subscale (p < 0.001), as well as the positive (p = 0.014), negative (p < 0.001) and general subscales (p = 0.014). Results are demonstrated in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3. PANSS mean scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters at baseline, 6-month follow-up and 12-month follow-up comparisons at 6- and 12-month follow-up were corrected for clinical symptoms at baseline. * illustrates p < 0.05; Error bars represent standard deviations. PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

After correcting for clinical symptoms at baseline, the patient groups with severely impaired and preserved cognitive performance showed significant differences on PANSS negative symptomatology at 6- and 12-month follow-up (n = 145, p = 0.017; n = 132, p = 0.018, respectively). Those with severely impaired and moderately impaired cognitive performance differed on the PANSS negative subscale (6 months: p = 0.010; 12 months: p = 0.010). Thus, consistently across time points, the group with severely impaired cognition was characterized by more severe negative symptoms compared to the other clusters at baseline, 6- and 12-month follow-up.

Furthermore, the patient subgroup with severely impaired cognition had lower clinical global functioning (total GAF score, Fig. 4) compared to patients with relatively preserved cognition, at baseline (p = 0.001), and trend-level effects were shown for 6-month follow-up (n = 144; p = 0.094) and 12-month follow-up (n = 132; p = 0.052), corrected for global functioning at baseline. In addition, lower clinical global functioning was shown in the subgroup with severely impaired cognition compared to the moderately impaired cluster at baseline (p = 0.045) and 6-month follow-up (p = 0.047).

Fig. 4. GAF mean scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters at baseline, 6-month follow-up and 12-month follow-up GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

Functional outcome

Self-reported global functioning and disability were evaluated by the WHODAS 2.0. Although the clusters did not significantly differ across all time points, corrected for global functioning and disability at baseline (all p > 0.05), there was a gradual and stepwise increase in disability, with the relatively preserved cluster having lower disability scores compared to the moderately impaired and severely impaired cluster.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the largest study investigating cognitive subgroups of FEP patients who all reached symptomatic remission after treatment in relation to longitudinal clinical and functional outcomes. We found three distinct cognitive subgroups in a sample of FEP, including one relatively large subgroup with preserved cognition (37.2%), one moderately impaired group (36.3%) and one severely impaired group (26.5%) as compared to healthy controls. Of note, the severely impaired group included only one-fourth of the sample. The cognitive subgroups were characterized by significant differences in clinical symptoms, with more severe clinical symptoms in the severely impaired cognitive cluster compared to the relatively preserved cluster, at baseline (PANSS total and all subscales) and 6- and 12-month follow-up (PANSS negative subscale). In addition, evaluation of global functioning (GAF) was significantly higher in the relatively preserved cluster compared to the severely impaired cluster at baseline and showed trend-level effects at 6- and 12-month follow-up. No significant differences in self-reported measures of functional outcome (WHODAS 2.0) were found between the patient subgroups at baseline and follow-up, yet the same trend could be observed.

The current results provide support for cognitive heterogeneity in FEP, delineated by three cognitive subtypes. This is consistent with previous clustering studies reporting on three subgroups of cognition in both first episode and chronic samples of schizophrenia (Carruthers et al. Reference Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner and Rossell2019; Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Mérette, Jomphe, Émond, Rouleau, Bouchard and Maziade2014; Menkes, Armstrong, Blackford, Heckers, & Woodward, Reference Menkes, Armstrong, Blackford, Heckers and Woodward2019; Sauvé, Malla, Joober, Brodeur, & Lepage, Reference Sauvé, Malla, Joober, Brodeur and Lepage2018; Uren et al. Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017; Wells et al. Reference Wells, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Bruggemann, Jacomb and Weickert2015). The relatively preserved subgroup did not perform worse on overall cognition compared to the healthy controls, confirming the existence of a subset of patients with relatively intact cognitive performance (Ammari et al. Reference Ammari, Walter Heinrichs, Pinnock, Miles, Muharib and Vaz2014; Carruthers et al. Reference Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner and Rossell2019; Menkes et al. Reference Menkes, Armstrong, Blackford, Heckers and Woodward2019; Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Klein, Desler, Lill, Gallinat and Schneider2017; Uren et al. Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017). Although cognitive impairment has long been recognized as a core symptom of psychotic disorders, our results show that a significant proportion of patients (37.8%) perform in the same range as healthy controls. This underscores the importance of taking individual variability into account in both research and clinical practice. It should be noted that cognitive performance similar to that of healthy controls is not necessarily synonymous with cognitively unaffected. However, no differences in years of education were observed between the relatively intact subgroup and healthy controls, and no decline relative to parents' years of education was observed, suggesting that cognitive functioning did not decline relative to a higher premorbid level (Keefe, Eesley, & Poe, Reference Keefe, Eesley and Poe2005). We further showed that both the moderately and severely impaired subgroups had attained significantly fewer years of education compared to the relatively preserved subgroup. The moderately impaired subgroup showed global cognitive impairment compared to the healthy controls, including all subdomains except for executive function. Findings regarding the intermediate cluster show global impairments of cognitive performance rather than domain-specific deficits. This is in line with previous studies performed in both first episode and chronic schizophrenia samples, which identified an intermediate cluster with overall moderate cognitive impairment (Lewandowski, Sperry, Cohen, & Öngür, Reference Lewandowski, Sperry, Cohen and Öngür2014; Uren et al. Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017; Van Rheenen et al. Reference Van Rheenen, Bryce, Tan, Neill, Gurvich, Louise and Rossell2016). The severely impaired subgroup (25.5%) showed pronounced cognitive impairments that were not restricted to specific domains, with more severe performance deficits compared to the other cognitive subgroups. The existence of a severely impaired cognitive subgroup has been previously demonstrated (Lewandowski et al. Reference Lewandowski, Sperry, Cohen and Öngür2014; Uren et al. Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017; Van Rheenen et al. Reference Van Rheenen, Bryce, Tan, Neill, Gurvich, Louise and Rossell2016). However, the percentage of individuals showing severely impaired cognition in this study is lower than the 44% reported in a large recent systematic review (Carruthers et al. Reference Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner and Rossell2019). Remarkably, executive function was relatively spared across all subgroups of cognition, although previous FEP studies demonstrated reduced executive function compared to healthy controls (Kravariti et al. Reference Kravariti, Morgan, Fearon, Zanelli, Lappin, Dazzan and Reichenberg2009). Attention and speed of processing showed most severe impairments across all subgroups, which is in line with previous studies performed in FEP (Kravariti et al. Reference Kravariti, Morgan, Fearon, Zanelli, Lappin, Dazzan and Reichenberg2009; Leeson et al. Reference Leeson, Barnes, Harrison, Matheson, Harrison, Mutsatsa and Joyce2010; Weinberg et al. Reference Weinberg, Lenroot, Jacomb, Allen, Bruggemann, Wells and Weickert2016)

Our finding of more severe clinical symptoms, specifically negative symptoms in the group with severely impaired cognition is in line with previous research demonstrating an association between cognitive function and negative symptoms in both FEP (Engen et al. Reference Engen, Simonsen, Melle, Færden, Lyngstad, Haatveit and Ueland2019; Reser, Allott, Killackey, Farhall, & Cotton, Reference Reser, Allott, Killackey, Farhall and Cotton2015; Uren et al. Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017) and chronic schizophrenia (Lewandowski et al. Reference Lewandowski, Sperry, Cohen and Öngür2014; Weinberg et al. Reference Weinberg, Lenroot, Jacomb, Allen, Bruggemann, Wells and Weickert2016; Wells et al. Reference Wells, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Bruggemann, Jacomb and Weickert2015). However, the relationship between cognitive function and negative symptoms seems complex. Severe negative symptoms such as lack of motivation or decreased effort may impact cognitive performance but similarly, cognitive impairment could affect the manifestation of negative symptoms as more preserved cognitive function may be essential for the ability to plan, initiate, motivate and carry out daily activities (Beck, Himelstein, Bredemeier, Silverstein, & Grant, Reference Beck, Himelstein, Bredemeier, Silverstein and Grant2018; Fervaha et al. Reference Fervaha, Zakzanis, Foussias, Graff-Guerrero, Agid and Remington2014; Fortgang, Srihari, & Cannon, Reference Fortgang, Srihari and Cannon2020; Jurado & Rosselli, Reference Jurado and Rosselli2007; Lindgren et al. Reference Lindgren, Holm, Kieseppä and Suvisaari2020). More longitudinal studies are required to gain more insight into the relationship between cognitive function and negative symptoms in FEP.

In the subgroup of individuals with severely impaired cognition, we found lower objectively evaluated global functioning (GAF) when compared to the relatively preserved subgroup. These findings are substantiated by other studies suggesting that global functioning is related to cognitive cluster membership (Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Mérette, Jomphe, Émond, Rouleau, Bouchard and Maziade2014; Lewandowski et al. Reference Lewandowski, Sperry, Cohen and Öngür2014; Uren et al. Reference Uren, Cotton, Killackey, Saling and Allott2017; Wells et al. Reference Wells, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Bruggemann, Jacomb and Weickert2015). Moreover, studies investigating cognitive subtypes in both psychotic patients and unaffected siblings showed that patients with cognitively impaired siblings reflect a poorer course of the disease. This suggests that cognitive impairment may indeed be predictive for the course of illness (Burger et al. Reference Burger, Schirmbeck, Vermeulen, Quee, De Koning, Bruggeman and Van Os2021; Quee et al. Reference Quee, Alizadeh, Aleman, Van Den Heuvel, Kahn, Linszen and Bruggeman2014). However, no significant differences between cognitive subgroups could be demonstrated on self-reported measures of functional outcome (WHODAS 2.0). This is remarkable, as both the GAF and the WHODAS 2.0 assess measures of outcome. It is plausible that not all types of cognition are associated with the evaluation of functional outcomes. It has been suggested that not global cognition but specifically social cognition plays a critical role in outcome regarding everyday functioning. A recent study by Kim et al. (Reference Kim, Jung, Moon, Jeon, Seo, Jung and Kim2021) demonstrated significant correlations between the WHODAS 2.0 and social cognition, such as communication and learning abilities (Kim et al. Reference Kim, Jung, Moon, Jeon, Seo, Jung and Kim2021). Similarly, Tan, Rossell, and Lee (Reference Tan, Rossell and Lee2020) demonstrated that mostly verbal-linguistic cognitive skills such as semantics and language are associated with subjective measures of functioning and wellbeing, as those have a direct effect on community functioning (Tan et al. Reference Tan, Rossell and Lee2020). Indeed, medium to large associations between social cognition and community functioning have been reported in a meta-analysis (Fett et al. Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, de Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011), whereas only small to moderate associations have been reported between nonsocial cognition and functional outcome (Halverson et al. Reference Halverson, Orleans-Pobee, Merritt, Sheeran, Fett and Penn2019). This suggests that interventions targeting social cognition may improve functional outcomes more than neurocognitive interventions. Another explanation for the lack of differences in WHODAS 2.0 between the cognitive subgroups may be the lack of awareness of functioning and disability in patients as the accuracy of assessing daily functioning in patients with schizophrenia is under debate (Jongs et al. Reference Jongs, Penninx, Arango, Ayuso-Mateos, van der Wee, van Rossum and Kas2020). An overestimation of functioning by the patient may be affected by disease-related factors such as negative symptomatology and lack of insight (Jongs et al. Reference Jongs, Penninx, Arango, Ayuso-Mateos, van der Wee, van Rossum and Kas2020; Sabbag et al. Reference Sabbag, Twamley, Vella, Heaton, Patterson and Harvey2012). This indicates that despite symptoms or restrictions in clinician observed functioning, patients may be satisfied with their lives and consider their level of functioning high. Thus, our findings suggest that daily functioning from a patient's perspective is not necessarily synonymous with the clinician's interpretation of recovery and may be related to a different set of predictors. Finally, the WHODAS 2.0 includes domains of daily functioning that are hardly affected in the current FEP sample and only minimally associated with cognitive function, such as mobility (getting around, standing up, walking a long-distance) and self-care (getting dressed, washing, eating) and hence do not differentiate between the groups (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Liou, Chang, Yen, Liao, Chi and Chou2018).

Strengths, limitations and future directions

A strength of the current study is its large sample size and longitudinal design, evaluating both clinical and functional outcomes over a 12-month follow-up period. The participants were included shortly after diagnosis and had all achieved symptomatic remission before the baseline measurement. Therefore, factors that may influence cognitive function, such as long-term antipsychotic medication use or duration of illness, are being limited. In addition to the assessment of clinical outcome (PANSS and GAF) by trained central raters, we extensively measured functioning and disability with the self-reported WHODAS 2.0 questionnaire, which is recommended for the assessment of functioning in the DSM-5 (Gold, Reference Gold2014). We also note that our study comes with some limitations. First, antipsychotic medication use was not stable for all participants throughout follow-up as some participants may have tapered off their antipsychotic medication gradually. However, the process of medication discontinuation also occurs in the general population of first-episode patients. Furthermore, although we did not include a cumulative dose of antipsychotic medication as a factor, all participants were in a similar early stage of the illness (3 to 6 months in remission of their first psychotic episode) at the time of inclusion and we found that current chlorpromazine equivalents were not significantly different between clusters. Moreover, although cognitive performance at baseline was not affected by psychotic symptoms as solely patients in symptomatic remission were included, generalizability to wider FEP populations may be limited within this study. Finally, cluster analyses come with the limitation that the determination of the number of clusters may be arbitrary as it depends on the methods used. However, we followed the recommended guidelines for reporting on cluster analysis (Carruthers et al. Reference Carruthers, Van Rheenen, Gurvich, Sumner and Rossell2019).

Conclusion

The results of the present study provide strong support for high heterogeneity in cognition among FEP patients who reach symptomatic remission. Besides finding a moderately impaired and severely impaired subgroup, we also show that a significant subset of patients have relatively preserved cognitive function. This underscores the importance of taking individual variability into account. In addition, we found that FEP patients with severe cognitive impairment have poor clinical outcomes compared to those with relatively preserved cognitive function. These findings suggest that grouping patients in subtypes along the cognitive continuum may offer crucial information about illness profiles and clinical prognosis. In conclusion, early identification of distinct cognitive profiles in FEP and corresponding longitudinal differences in clinical profile has clear implications for prognosis and personalized treatment of psychotic disorders. However, self-reported measures of functional outcome seem to have different sets of predictors in FEP and more longitudinal studies are required to further assess determinants of functional outcome.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004153.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all of our participants, collaborators and dedicated includers who assist in recruitment, and our students for their help with data collection and preparation.

Financial support

The HAMLETT study is funded by ZonMW in the Netherlands (grant number 80-84800-98-41015). The funders have no role in the study design, collection, management, analysis and interpretation of data, writing the report, or the decision to submit the report for publication.

Conflicts of interest

The authors report no potential conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Ammari, N., Walter Heinrichs, R., Pinnock, F., Miles, A. A., Muharib, E., & Vaz, S. M. D. (2014). Preserved, deteriorated, and premorbidly impaired patterns of intellectual ability in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 28(3), 353358. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., & Arndt, S. (1992). The comprehensive assessment of symptoms and history (CASH): An instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49(8), 615623. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080023004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Himelstein, R., Bredemeier, K., Silverstein, S. M., & Grant, P. (2018). What accounts for poor functioning in people with schizophrenia: A re-evaluation of the contributions of neurocognitive v. Attitudinal and motivational factors. Psychological Medicine, 48(16), 27762785. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begemann, M. J. H., Thompson, I. A., Veling, W., Gangadin, S. S., Geraets, C. N. W., Van ‘T Hag, E., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2020). To continue or not to continue? Antipsychotic medication maintenance versus dose-reduction/discontinuation in first episode psychosis: HAMLETT, a pragmatic multicenter single-blind randomized controlled trial. Trials, 21(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-019-3822-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burger, T. J., Schirmbeck, F., Vermeulen, J. M., Quee, P. J., De Koning, M. B., Bruggeman, R., & Van Os, J. (2021). Association between cognitive phenotype in unaffected siblings and prospective 3- and 6-year clinical outcome in their proband affected by psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 51(11), 19161926. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720000719CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, S. P., Van Rheenen, T. E., Gurvich, C., Sumner, P. J., & Rossell, S. L. (2019). Characterising the structure of cognitive hetereogenity in schizophrenia spectrum disorder: A systematic review and narrative synthesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 107, 252278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, R., Liou, T. H., Chang, K. H., Yen, C. F., Liao, H. F., Chi, W. C., & Chou, K. R. (2018). Assessment of functioning and disability in patients with schizophrenia using the WHO disability assessment schedule 2.0 in a large-scale database. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 268(1), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-017-0834-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engen, M. J., Simonsen, C., Melle, I., Færden, A., Lyngstad, S. H., Haatveit, B., & Ueland, T. (2019). Cognitive functioning in patients with first-episode psychosis stratified by level of negative symptoms: A 1-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Research, 281, 112554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112554CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fervaha, G., Zakzanis, K. K., Foussias, G., Graff-Guerrero, A., Agid, O., & Remington, G. (2014). Motivational deficits and cognitive test performance in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(9), 10581065. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A. K. J., Viechtbauer, W., de Dominguez, M. G., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortgang, R. G., Srihari, V., & Cannon, T. D. (2020). Cognitive effort and amotivation in first-episode psychosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(4), 422431. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000509CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, D. M., Murphy, A. L., O'Donnell, H., Centorrino, F., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2010). International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(6), 686693. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, E., Mérette, C., Jomphe, V., Émond, C., Rouleau, N., Bouchard, R. H., & Maziade, M. (2014). Cluster analysis of cognitive deficits may mark heterogeneity in schizophrenia in terms of outcome and response to treatment. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264(4), 333343. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0463-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, L. H. (2014). DSM-5 and the assessment of functioning: The world health organization disability assessment schedule 2.0 (WHODAS 2.0). Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 42(2), 173181.Google ScholarPubMed
Halverson, T. F., Orleans-Pobee, M., Merritt, C., Sheeran, P., Fett, A. K., & Penn, D. L. (2019). Pathways to functional outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Meta-analysis of social cognitive and neurocognitive predictors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 105, 212219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, R. W. (2005). The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. American Psychologist, 60(3), 229242. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.3.229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helldin, L., Mohn, C., Olsson, A. K., & Hjärthag, F. (2020). Neurocognitive variability in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Relationship to real-world functioning. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 20, 100172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2020.100172Google ScholarPubMed
Henry, L. P., Amminger, G. P., Harris, M. G., Yuen, H. P., Harrigan, S. M., Prosser, A. L., & McGorry, P. D. (2010). The EPPIC follow-up study of first-episode psychosis: Longer-term clinical and functional outcome 7 years after index admission. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(6), 716728. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.08m04846yelCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jääskeläinen, E., Juola, P., Hirvonen, N., McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Isohanni, M., & Miettunen, J. (2013). A systematic review and meta-analysis of recovery in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(6), 12961306. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs130CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, M., Hjärthag, F., & Helldin, L. (2020). Cognitive markers related to long-term remission status in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Research, 289, 113035. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. H., Thornicroft, G., Coffey, M., & Dunn, G. (1995). A brief mental health outcome scale. Reliability and validity of the global assessment of functioning (GAF). British Journal of Psychiatry, 166(5), 654659. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.166.5.654CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jongs, N., Penninx, B., Arango, C., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L., van der Wee, N., van Rossum, I. W., & Kas, M. J. (2020). Effect of disease-related biases on the subjective assessment of social functioning in Alzheimer's disease and schizophrenia patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, S0022–3956(20), 3107231074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.11.013.Google Scholar
Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3):213233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9040-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahn, R. S., Winter van Rossum, I., Leucht, S., McGuire, P., Lewis, S. W., Leboyer, M., & Wilson, D. (2018). Amisulpride and olanzapine followed by open-label treatment with clozapine in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder (OPTiMiSE): A three-phase switching study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(10), 797807. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30252-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2), 261276. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/13.2.261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Eesley, C. E., & Poe, M. P. (2005). Defining a cognitive function decrement in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 57(6), 688691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Gold, J. M., Poe, M. P., & Coughenour, L. (2004). The brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2–3), 283297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S. J., Jung, D. U., Moon, J. J., Jeon, D. W., Seo, Y. S., Jung, S. S., & Kim, Y. S. (2021). Relationship between disability self-awareness and cognitive and daily living function in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 19(23), 100192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2020.100192Google Scholar
Kravariti, E., Morgan, K., Fearon, P., Zanelli, J. W., Lappin, J. M., Dazzan, P., & Reichenberg, A. (2009). Neuropsychological functioning in first-episode schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(4), 336345. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.108.055590CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lally, J., Ajnakina, O., Stubbs, B., Cullinane, M., Murphy, K. C., Gaughran, F., & Murray, R. M. (2017). Remission and recovery from first-episode psychosis in adults: Systematic review and meta-analysis of long-term outcome studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 211(6):350358. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.117.201475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeson, V. C., Barnes, T. R. E., Harrison, M., Matheson, E., Harrison, I., Mutsatsa, S. H., & Joyce, E. M. (2010). The relationship between IQ, memory, executive function, and processing speed in recent-onset psychosis: 1-year stability and clinical outcome. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(2), 400409. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewandowski, K. E., Sperry, S. H., Cohen, B. M., & Öngür, D. (2014). Cognitive variability in psychotic disorders: A cross-diagnostic cluster analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(15), 32393248. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714000774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindgren, M., Holm, M., Kieseppä, T., & Suvisaari, J. (2020). Neurocognition and social cognition predicting 1-year outcomes in first-episode psychosis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.603933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menkes, M. W., Armstrong, K., Blackford, J. U., Heckers, S., & Woodward, N. D. (2019). Neuropsychological functioning in early and chronic stages of schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 206, 413419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Klein, J. P., Desler, T., Lill, H., Gallinat, J., & Schneider, B. C. (2017). Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Are we making mountains out of molehills? Psychological Medicine, 47(15), 26022612. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000939CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quee, P. J., Alizadeh, B. Z., Aleman, A., Van Den Heuvel, E. R., Kahn, R. S., Linszen, D. H., & Bruggeman, R. (2014). Cognitive subtypes in non-affected siblings of schizophrenia patients: Characteristics and profile congruency with affected family members. Psychological Medicine, 44(2), 395405. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713000809CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reser, M. P., Allott, K. A., Killackey, E., Farhall, J., & Cotton, S. M. (2015). Exploring cognitive heterogeneity in first-episode psychosis: What cluster analysis can reveal. Psychiatry Research, 229(3), 819827. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbag, S., Twamley, E. W., Vella, L., Heaton, R. K., Patterson, T. L., & Harvey, P. D. (2012). Predictors of the accuracy of self-assessment of everyday functioning in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 137(1–3), 190195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santesteban-Echarri, O., Paino, M., Rice, S., González-Blanch, C., McGorry, P., Gleeson, J., & Alvarez-Jimenez, M. (2017). Predictors of functional recovery in first-episode psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 58:5975. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauvé, G., Malla, A., Joober, R., Brodeur, M. B., & Lepage, M. (2018). Comparing cognitive clusters across first- and multiple-episode of psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 269, 707718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassnig, M., Bowie, C., Pinkham, A. E., Penn, D., Twamley, E. W., Patterson, T. L., & Harvey, P. D. (2018). Which levels of cognitive impairments and negative symptoms are related to functional deficits in schizophrenia? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 104, 124129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.06.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suvisaari, J., Mantere, O., Keinänen, J., Mäntylä, T., Rikandi, E., Lindgren, M., & Raij, T. T. (2018). Is It possible to predict the future in first-episode psychosis? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 580. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, E. J., Rossell, S. L., & Lee, S. J. (2020). Impaired meaning-based cognitive skills are specifically associated with poorer subjective quality of life in schizophrenia. Personalized Medicine in Psychiatry, 23, 100062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmip.2020.100062CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uren, J., Cotton, S. M., Killackey, E., Saling, M. M., & Allott, K. (2017). Cognitive clusters in first-episode psychosis: Overlap with healthy controls and relationship to concurrent and prospective symptoms and functioning. Neuropsychology, 31(7), 787797. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000367CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Üstün, T. B. (2010). Measuring Health and Disability: Manual for WHO Disability Assessment Schedule WHODAS 2.0. World Health Organization. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/43974CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Rheenen, T. E., Bryce, S., Tan, E. J., Neill, E., Gurvich, C., Louise, S., & Rossell, S. L. (2016). Does cognitive performance map to categorical diagnoses of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and bipolar disorder? A discriminant functions analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 192, 109115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, J. H. Jr (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 58(301), 236244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinberg, D., Lenroot, R., Jacomb, I., Allen, K., Bruggemann, J., Wells, R., & Weickert, T. W. (2016). Cognitive subtypes of schizophrenia characterized by differential brain volumetric reductions and cognitive decline. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(12), 12511259. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, R., Swaminathan, V., Sundram, S., Weinberg, D., Bruggemann, J., Jacomb, I., & Weickert, T. W. (2015). The impact of premorbid and current intellect in schizophrenia: Cognitive, symptom, and functional outcomes. Npj Schizophrenia, 4(1), 15043. https://doi.org/10.1038/npjschz.2015.43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean (s.d.) baseline demographic and cognitive characteristics for FEP cognitive clusters and healthy controls

Figure 1

Fig. 1. BACS composite means Z-scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters and healthy controls Error bars represent standard deviations. All groups showed significant differences (p < 0.05) except for healthy controls compared to the preserved cognitive cluster. BACS, brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. BACS subdomain means Z-scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters and healthy controls Pentagons represent mean BACS Z-scores. For detailed statistics, see Table 1. BACS, brief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. PANSS mean scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters at baseline, 6-month follow-up and 12-month follow-up comparisons at 6- and 12-month follow-up were corrected for clinical symptoms at baseline. * illustrates p < 0.05; Error bars represent standard deviations. PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. GAF mean scores illustrated for FEP cognitive clusters at baseline, 6-month follow-up and 12-month follow-up GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning; FEP, first-episode psychosis.

Supplementary material: File

Oomen et al. supplementary material

Oomen et al. supplementary material

Download Oomen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 170.5 KB