Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:53:29.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Applied linguistics: A consumer's view

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 March 2018

Michael Swan*
Affiliation:
Freelance Writer [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Like many EFL teachers of my generation, I side-stepped into the profession. While doing post-graduate research at Oxford in the early 1960s, I took a job one summer vacation teaching English in a local language school. Though I had no idea how to do this, I enjoyed the work, and kept on a few hours’ teaching in the next academic year. As time went on, it became clear that I had somewhat more talent for this type of activity than for academic research, and I became a full-time teacher, subsequently opening my own school of English.

Type
First Person Singular
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

‘When you see reference to a new paradigm, you should always, under all circumstances, take cover.’ (Galbraith Reference Galbraith1998)

Introduction: learning my trade

Like many EFL teachers of my generation, I side-stepped into the profession. While doing post-graduate research at Oxford in the early 1960s, I took a job one summer vacation teaching English in a local language school. Though I had no idea how to do this, I enjoyed the work, and kept on a few hours’ teaching in the next academic year. As time went on, it became clear that I had somewhat more talent for this type of activity than for academic research, and I became a full-time teacher, subsequently opening my own school of English.

During this period I did my best to educate myself in my chosen profession. This initially involved spending much of my spare time in libraries studying the grammar and phonology of my mother tongue, about which I knew virtually nothing. At the same time, I learnt what I could about contemporary language-teaching methodology, which differed in many interesting ways from the approach of my secondary-school foreign language teachers. The journal English Language Teaching (later ELT Journal) offered valuable support in these areas, as did membership of ATEFL (later IATEFL) and BAAL. The new university Applied Linguistics departments were making an impact, and on a couple of occasions I had the pleasure of employing one of their graduates, Mike Long, a gifted, popular and thoughtful teacher whose views on language teaching, then as now, commanded respect. In general, our work in those days was strongly influenced by the prevailing audio-lingual and contrastive doctrines; whatever their shortcomings, it is a pity that what was good in them largely sank without trace when the behaviourist ship was torpedoed.

As I gained experience I began to undertake some theorizing and research of my own. I found it interesting to explore the middle ground between academic descriptions of English (which were of little use pedagogically), and the common pedagogic accounts of the language (which were often seriously inadequate). I was intrigued by the differences I saw in the English of students from different language backgrounds, and I started to build up my own notes and corpora of learner English, greatly helped by the archives of relevant material that were then contained in the British Council's English-teaching library.

In the early 1970s I moved to Paris, feeling that I had pretty much learnt my trade, only to discover that there was now something called the ‘Communicative Approach’, and that I had to learn my trade all over again. New books for teachers proliferated; they were not always easy for teachers to understand, and have largely merged in my memory into one daunting tome called ‘The communicative teaching of language as communication in the communicative classroom’. This was boom time for new methods, and fringe religions such as Silent Way, Suggestopaedia and Counselling Learning flourished. Some merged imperceptibly into DIY New Age psychotherapy, so that you could simultaneously learn a language, remodel your personality and find true happiness.

I had often felt contemptuous of much existing English-teaching material, believing naively that my experience and ability had equipped me to do better, and in the mid-1970s I began to mix teaching and writing. In due course I found myself writing for both Oxford and Cambridge University Presses (grammar reference for one and teaching material for the other). These books were successful enough to justify the risky decision to become a full-time writer, and in 1980 I taught my last lesson. I expected to miss teaching after 20 very good years in classrooms, but I didn't: enough was clearly enough. Having several smaller books under my belt by this time, I embarked on a four-level general English course for adult learners in collaboration with my partner Catherine Walter, a highly experienced teacher and trainer. This was published as the Cambridge English Course series.

Writing a complete general English course turned out to be an enormous job, taking at least twice as long as predicted, and it was a very formative professional experience. I finally learnt what is involved in selecting and packaging language material for learners, and wished I could go back and apologize to all the students who had been subjected to my haphazard scattershot approach, which was based on the arrogant belief that my home-made materials, and my selection strategy of ‘let's see what comes up in the lesson’, would work just as well as the courses I had been so contemptuous of. The experience left me with little patience for the view that good teachers can themselves produce all the materials and activities their classes need. I feel this is rather like believing that the first violinist can write the orchestra's repertoire on his or her evenings off.

In producing our materials, Catherine and I swam upstream against the prevailing fashion, which was for one-dimensional courses in which a single syllabus (typically structures, situations or functions) served as a framework on which everything else was hung, with lessons that all had the same structure, and content that was sometimes terribly bland. We set out to produce multi-syllabus materials which would give equal importance to the formal, semantic and pragmatic elements of the language without privileging any one. We believed that different aspects of language and language use are acquired in different ways, and that no single approach – traditional, ‘communicative’ or whatever – could do justice to this complexity. And we tried to respect the varied interests and strengths of learners and teachers, knowing that topics and communication styles that work for one may not work for another.

More books followed as the years went by, mostly to do with pedagogic grammar. Increasingly, I found myself giving talks at conferences – so much easier than actually teaching – and I developed great respect for the battle-hardened secondary-school teachers who often made up my audiences, and whose jobs I could not have done for five minutes. I went on reading and thinking about language and language teaching, and trying to keep track of the changing winds of theoretical fashion. I also published a fair number of articles, mostly with a practical orientation, but some dealing with worries I had about aspects of applied linguistic theory and its relationship with classroom practice.

1. How do we know things?

An ongoing concern for me, then as now, was to assess the various kinds of information and recommendations that are addressed to practitioners. Anyone working in language teaching – teacher, teacher trainer, materials writer, syllabus designer or administrator – is exposed to a bewildering range of what one can loosely call ‘theory’: principles of various kinds which purport to explain how languages are learnt and how they can best be taught. These principles originate from various sources. Some are based on the mass of experience that has been accumulated by language teaching practitioners over the years. Others derive from the immense body of work carried out by researchers studying instructed second language acquisition (SLA). And yet others have been enunciated by original thinkers in our field – scholars such as Prabhu, Krashen, Vygotsky, Lozanov, Stevick or Widdowson – and have taken hold in the profession. A major problem for the practitioner is to determine the status of such principles: to decide how relevant they are to his or her work, and how much faith can be put in them. Where are they situated on the cline from unfounded speculation to robust extrapolations from solid research?

Suppose, for instance, you are a classroom teacher with a few years’ experience. You like to think things out for yourself, and you want to improve your professional knowledge and skills within the limits set by time and resources. Among the many things you learnt during your training, you remember being told firmly that extensive reading is important for effective learning, that a high level of accuracy is desirable, that practice should be communicative, and that listening skills must be taught. At your teachers’ association's annual conference, speakers explain that training in metacognitive strategies is useful, that learners need to acquire large numbers of lexical ‘chunks’, and that structure drilling is discredited. The association's latest newsletter contains an article promoting learning strategies based on ‘brain research’, another article arguing that all vocabulary is best taught in context (and suggesting ways of doing this), and a third advocating the use of mineral crystals for promoting relaxation in the classroom. You subscribe to an online teachers’ website, and read a posting by an experienced teacher who says ‘I don't teach grammar. I use Krashen's communicative approach’. You dip into a book for language teachers about SLA, which tells you among other things that input processing promotes grammar learning, and that the conscious noticing of language items is necessary for their acquisition. It is common ground among your colleagues that the mother tongue should be avoided at all costs. Halfway through the academic year, the Ministry issues a directive saying that, in the light of recent research, all future language teaching is to follow a task-based approach.

What are you to make of all this? At one extreme you decide that the paper on crystals need not detain you, and a glance at the allegedly brain-based activities makes it clear that they owe little to any kind of research. You have learnt to be sceptical of Ministry directives, and to take all ‘Methods’ and ‘Approaches’ with a pinch of salt. At the other extreme, the advocacy of extensive reading and communicative practice seems solidly based, as far as you can tell, and corresponds to your own intuitions and experience. But common sense, intuitions and experience only take you so far, and as a practitioner you are not well qualified to judge the status of these various recommendations. If you had time, you tell yourself, you would look at the applied linguistics literature. Would this clarify matters, and help you to decide which of these prescriptions and proscriptions are well founded, which at least seem plausible, and which are no better than speculation, superstition, or survivals of the last theoretical doctrine but two? Or would you simply end up confused at a higher level?

Unlike my imaginary teacher, I have found time to engage with some of the applied linguistics literature. How well has the discipline answered for me the questions that I have encountered as a teacher, writer and occasional teacher trainer?

2. What applied linguistics has done for me

Practitioners like myself are, of course, much better able to do our jobs because of the work of thousands of academic applied linguists over more than half a century. How could things be otherwise? If I compare my view of language and language teaching when I left secondary school to my current understanding of these matters, I am conscious of enormous changes. Our knowledge of English grammar was already substantial thanks to the work of the great descriptive grammarians of the early 20th century and the more pedagogically oriented scholars who mediated their findings for teachers. But it has been greatly extended by subsequent work on the grammar of spoken and written discourse, and by corpus-based exploration of structural constraints at the grammar-lexis interface. Studies of vocabulary frequency, from West (Reference West1953) through to contemporary corpus-derived data, have fed directly into syllabus design, as has work on semantic syllabuses, which has given us valuable inventories of high-priority functional and notional categories and their linguistic exponents. We know far more than we did about phonology, and are perhaps better at teaching pronunciation as a result. We have a much clearer understanding than before of learner language and its development, facilitated by the important construct of ‘interlanguage’. Research on SLA has helped to remedy our older naive and over-confident ideas about course design and implementation. While this has confirmed our suspicion that teaching grammar does not mean that it is necessarily learnt, scholars such as Long (Reference Long1983), Norris & Ortega (Reference Norris and Ortega2000), and Spada & Tomita (Reference Spada and Tomita2010) have assembled reassuringly solid evidence that instruction can lead to acquisition. The communicative approach has transformed our methodology, and if this development has perhaps largely been practice- rather than theory-led, it has been underpinned by SLA researchers’ views on the value of bringing instructed language learning closer to naturalistic acquisition. All in all, if a full understanding of our business is still some distance off, at least – thanks largely to the research wing of our profession – we know much more than we did about how to teach languages and how not to.

3. Some problems

Naturally, however, there is not a seamless relationship between applied linguistics research and theory on the one hand, and practical language teaching on the other, and I have sometimes found myself concerned about questions of application and applicability.

3.1 Things taking over

Exciting developments inspired by new research interests can come to fill the language-teaching horizon. This happened with audio-lingual teaching, when views on learning as habit-formation, valuable in their place, sometimes gained a stranglehold on language teaching overall. In the early days of the communicative approach, some classes did little besides work on functions and communicative activities. Research on learning and communication strategies in the 1990s sparked off a virtual pedagogic cottage industry, with Oxford (Reference Oxford1990: 24–25), for example, recommending that teachers and students internalize and work from a complex and daunting categorization of strategies which would have left little time for anything else. The ‘lexical approach’, based on a precarious analogy with the emergence of first-language grammar, attracted more attention than it probably deserved in the 1990s, and its focus on formulaic sequences often led to an overemphasis on teaching ‘chunks’ (Swan Reference Swan2006). Corpus research has encouraged some teachers to bring corpus material into the classroom, with the risk of requiring their students inappropriately to become amateur language analysts. Pure linguists have not always respected Hatch's (Reference Hatch1978) principle that the only question a researcher should answer is the one that he or she has asked, and some have not been slow to step outside their specialities brandishing pedagogic recommendations. The eminent corpus linguist John Sinclair, for instance, told teachers, quite unrealistically, to present learners only with corpus-attested examples (Reference Sinclair and Wichmann1997: 30ff). Functional grammatical models such as Cognitive Grammar or Systemic Functional Grammar are often recommended as appropriate for pedagogic application, despite the unsuitability of any such systematic models for teaching purposes (Swan Reference Swan and Simpson2011: 565).

3.2 Misapplied linguistics: a disconnect

It is not enough for a teaching approach to conform to current SLA thinking, however persuasive the supporting arguments. It must also fit the relevant context, offering solutions that match the problems inherent in the situation. I feel, however, that there is sometimes a disconnect in this respect.

Task-based language teaching (TBLT) is often promoted as being preferable in general to ‘traditional’ approaches involving proactive language syllabuses (Willis Reference Willis1996: 133–137; Skehan Reference Skehan1998: 93–94; Ellis Reference Ellis2003: 319–320; Ellis & Shintani Reference Ellis and Shintani2014: Ch. 6; Long Reference Long2015b passim). However, there is a considerable mismatch between the problems central to many instructional contexts, and the solutions offered by teaching that is exclusively or primarily based on a syllabus of tasks. TBLT tends to focus on consolidating what has already been partially acquired – what we used to call, over-simply, ‘fluency practice’ – through communicative tasks in which learners develop their command of English while ‘[making] use of their own linguistic resources’ (Ellis Reference Ellis2003: 16). New linguistic material relevant to the tasks emerges or is (up to a point) supplied as needed, and may be acquired during task performance. However, many of the world's learners have little out-of-class exposure to the language they are studying and little time in class – 100 hours a year is not an uncommon pattern. In such situations, it is mathematically impossible for unstructured ‘naturalistic’ input and primarily meaning-based classroom communication to supply and adequately consolidate all the grammatical and lexical elements that are most needed (Swan Reference Swan2012: 63). These can only be presented and internalized through careful selection, prioritizing and (where necessary) focused practice, and consequently by means of appropriate formal and semantic syllabuses. Communicative tasks will naturally support the consolidation and activation of syllabus-based input, as they always have done, and task-related research has enriched our repertoire of such activities and our understanding of how to exploit them. But hard-core task-based teaching (unlike task-supported teaching) does not fit well with the main requirements of learners in this kind of situation, however appropriate it may be to certain other contexts. (For detailed discussion, see Swan Reference Swan2005.)

3.3 Straw men

Input-poor instructional situations are not then, in my view, well served by teaching approaches based on the theory that acquisition takes place primarily as a by-product of communication. Approaches that are in principle better adapted to such contexts have, however, frequently been condemned in the SLA literature on the grounds that they are ‘discredited’ and have ‘failed’ (e.g. Skehan Reference Skehan1998: 94; Ellis Reference Ellis2003: 207). ‘Discredited’, in this connection, seems to mean ‘incompatible with my views’; we are not generally told by whom or for whom these approaches have been discredited, or where, when or how seriously they have failed. Straw-man caricatures of ‘traditional’ instruction (often meaning any teaching other than TBLT) are frequent. Robinson (Reference Robinson and Robinson2001: 291) portrays such teaching as relying on ‘simplified classroom language and texts which are functionally and linguistically impoverished, prohibiting exposure to forms and functions learners may be ready to learn, or need to use’. Willis & Willis (Reference Willis, Willis, Carter and Nunan2001: 173) and Long (Reference Long2015a: 567) offer similar thumbnail sketches. Both Ellis (Reference Ellis2015: 244) and Long (Reference Long2015b: 349) use ‘PPP’ (the presentation–practice–production sequence often used in grammar teaching) as shorthand for ‘traditional approaches’. This is rather like equating the whole of driving instruction with, say, lessons in overtaking. While no doubt teaching of the kind pilloried does exist, these descriptions bear no relation whatever to well-designed syllabus-informed instruction, as a glance at the major published courses of the last 30-odd years will show. The impression is frequently given of a paradigm shift, in which unsatisfactory older methods with no theoretical foundation have been replaced by totally different approaches based on robust theory and research. This is quite misleading.

There is of course nothing new about this kind of thing. Grammar-translation and audio-lingual teaching, whatever their real defects, have been characterized for decades in over-simple dismissive terms. The contrastive analysts such as Lado and Fries (from whose work language teaching benefited greatly) were seriously misrepresented by their successors such as Dulay, Burt & Krashen (Reference Dulay, Burt and Krashen1982), who criticized them both for saying things that they did not say, and for not saying things that they did. The misrepresentation is routinely repeated in present-day references to their work; see Swan (Reference Swan2007a) for an attempt to set the record straight.

3.4 The invisible practitioner

In general, the theory-practice gap is narrowing. Lightbown, in her ‘Anniversary Article’ (Reference Lightbown2000), refers to the growth in classroom-based SLA research over the previous 15 years, and this growth has continued unabated, to our distinct benefit. Nonetheless, the invisible practitioner is still a familiar character – or non-character – in academic writing, and I am disturbed when researchers fail to acknowledge the work carried out on my side of the theory-practice divide. This can happen, for instance, in discussions of language course and syllabus design. Most of the world's millions of language learners follow syllabuses that are instantiated in textbooks. These are largely created by experienced professionals whose work calls on specific knowledge and skills, building on a developing tradition established by their predecessors over long periods. And yet it often seems as if these thousands of creative practitioners, perhaps because they do not often contribute to learned journals, are effectively invisible in the academic world – although names such as Alexander, O'Neill, Abbs, Freebairn or Soars have probably become known to more people worldwide, with good reason, than those of almost any contemporary novelist. An article on the role of materials in the language classroom (Crawford Reference Crawford, Richards and Renandya2002), for instance, makes no mention at all of the textbook as a selection and presentation vehicle. Several of the chapters on cognition and second language instruction in Robinson (Reference Robinson and Robinson2001) touch on language syllabus design. Of the 1,000-plus references at the end of the book, six are to work by language-teaching practitioners; none of these is a course designer. The authors of a recent book reporting on their progress in drawing up grammatical specifications for the different levels of the Common European Framework of Reference for English (Hawkins & Filipović Reference Hawkins and Filipović2012) make no reference at all to the vast body of relevant work carried out by pedagogic grammarians over half a century. It may be that, coming from another field (grammatical theory), they are simply unaware that such work exists. Alternatively, they may just have felt that the principles informing their research make it necessarily more valid than the approaches of their humble practitioner colleagues. This is not the case (Swan Reference Swan2014): their provisional findings amount to a small sample of what has long since been well charted by practitioners, and their methodology is of a kind that, to borrow a phrase from Meara (Reference Meara1993), ‘should not be recommended to numerate readers with high blood pressure’.

Sometimes the practitioner is not so much ignored as treated with what amounts to contempt. Ellis, in one of his various defences of TBLT (Ellis & Shintani Reference Ellis and Shintani2014: Ch. 6), divides its critics into two groups. One group, researchers, ‘[bases] its criticisms on evidence’, and the researchers are responded to with the normal professional courtesies. The other group comprises what Ellis derisively calls ‘pundits’: ill-informed practitioners who, in his perception, ‘rely on opinion derived from their own experience of teaching’, and who use his findings and the associated pedagogic recommendations as ‘aunt sallies’, easy targets for their unsophisticated critical missiles (Ellis & Shintani Reference Ellis and Shintani2014: 152–153). Two things need to be said here. First of all, one does not need to be a researcher to take issue with a researcher's logic, any more than one needs to be an animal to discuss animal rights. The key question is whether the argumentation is valid, not whether the person who questions it is a scholar, a teacher, a violinist or a garage mechanic. And secondly, this is not, in my view, an appropriate way in which to engage in academic debate.

3.5 Hypotheosis

It is not unusual in the social sciences for empirically ill-supported hypotheses or beliefs to harden into doctrine. In our own discipline, the direct method, the audio-lingual approach, the fringe methods of the 1970s and 1980s, and various brands of communicative methodology have all been promoted to the accompaniment of confident assertions that ‘now we know’ how languages are taught and learnt. Such assertions are sometimes little more than expressions of opinion, supported by citations from other scholars who share the same belief, like the strategy of the person in Wittgenstein's celebrated analogy (Reference Wittgenstein1953: §265) who buys several copies of the same newspaper and uses each to confirm the accuracy of the others. Where evidence is offered, the argument may involve questionable extrapolation from a limited empirical base, robust though the research findings may be in their own right.

In recent years, much of SLA discussion has revolved around a group of hypotheses to do with ‘noticing’ (Schmidt Reference Schmidt1990), ‘interaction’ (Long Reference Long and Winitz1981, Reference Long, Ritchie and Bahtia1996), incidental ‘focus on form’ (Long Reference Long, Lambert and Shohamy2000), ‘output’ (Swain Reference Swain and Lantolf2000) and ‘processability/teachability’ (Pienemann Reference Pienemann1998). Stated in their weak forms, these ideas are relatively uncontroversial. Few people would contest that:

  • noticing formal features may have some importance for acquisition

  • interaction may generate feedback, which may trigger acquisition

  • pushed output may lead to the consolidation of known material

  • acquisition may occur via incidental focus on form during communication

  • some second language morphemes and complex structures may be acquired (and therefore teachable) in predictable orders or stages.

The hypotheses have generated considerable research investigating how far, under what circumstances and for what language elements they apply, and this has added greatly to our knowledge.

However, I have found myself concerned when strong and over-generalized forms of these hypotheses have been enunciated and presented as justifying firm and wide-ranging pronouncements about instructed acquisition, with the implication – and even the direct assertion – that the pronouncements are validly grounded in research or theory. The reasonable observation that ‘things seem to happen like this under certain circumstances’ can transmute into a declaration that ‘this is the only (or principal or most effective) way in which things happen’. SLA may then be said to take place solely or primarily through communication as mediated by interaction, noticing, incidental focus on form and so on; confident references may be made to what is said to have been established by research, or to the cognitive processes through which, we are told, acquisition necessarily takes place (e.g. Willis Reference Willis1996: 13; Ellis Reference Ellis2003: 210). This cognitive-interactionist model of instructed SLA has figured largely in the theoretical support for TBLT (e.g. Long Reference Long2015b). However, empirical evidence for the strong forms of these hypotheses and the claims built on them necessarily has limited scope, and replication studies are rare. It is not clear, therefore, that the large generalizations put forward follow logically from the data cited in their support; nor has TBLT been shown generally to result in more successful learning.

To take one of these hypotheses as an example: the existence of natural developmental sequences in the acquisition of morphemes has been appealed to as justifying the rejection of traditional grammar syllabuses as fundamental components of a language course (Doughty Reference Doughty and Robinson2001: 227–228; Ellis Reference Ellis, Hinkel and Fotos2002: 19–20; Ellis & Shintani Reference Ellis and Shintani2014: 73). If there is a predetermined natural order constraining the acquisition of grammar, the argument goes, we cannot set up a teaching sequence for most grammatical items, since we do not know where they fit into the natural order, and cannot therefore determine when learners are developmentally ready for one or the other (Ellis & Shintani Reference Ellis and Shintani2014: 73). Now research by Pienemann and others has indeed confirmed some earlier beliefs about natural acquisition order, showing plausibly that structures which make greater demands than others on learners’ developing processing capacity tend to be acquired later (Pienemann Reference Pienemann1998). And it has been established that the acquisition of some complex structures typically goes through non-canonical stages leading up to native-like use (English negation is the standard example). But this is as far as the evidence goes. We have no reason at all to believe that the learning of most grammatical items is constrained in this way: that for yet-to-be uncovered developmental reasons, students might need to learn comparatives before relative pronouns, dativizing verbs before quantifiers or infinitives of purpose before possessive ’s. To claim that learnability findings preclude the operation of a grammatical syllabus is a large and unjustified leap across a wide logical gap.

The other hypotheses referred to also fail, in my view, to constitute a solid platform for a cognitive-interactionist model opposing the use of proactive syllabuses (Swan Reference Swan2005). Like instructors in other domains, language-teaching practitioners have always used inventories of high-priority learning targets and known difficulties to inform their work, and there appears to be no sound theoretical or research basis for the recurrent doctrinaire assertion that they are wrong to do so.

Large-scale pedagogic recommendations based on unwarranted extrapolation of this kind are effectively ideological in nature. They belong to a category which Richards (Reference Richards, Richards and Renandya2002: 21–23) characterizes as based on ‘theory-philosophy conceptions’: views of what stands to reason, is self-evident, or is manifestly desirable in psychological or sociological terms. The cognitive-interactionist construct, valuable in itself, can thus come to illustrate what Pullum (Reference Pullum1996) calls ‘an epistemological affliction: difficulty in telling the difference between (i) data that are well accounted for if construct C is posited and (ii) evidence that C actually exists’. In any scientific study, it is surely essential to remain aware of the logical gulf between the big things we believe and the small things we can prove.

4. Where have we got to, and where are we going?

As someone who works on the margins of applied linguistics, I do not feel well qualified to assess the current state of the discipline. Nor do I feel able to characterize all of present-day language teaching, ranging as it does from methods devised by the finest minds of the 15th century to approaches inspired by the latest ideas to emerge from the workshops where theory is forged. However, I would like to close with a few tentative general observations.

4.1 The simplicity bias

Perhaps one reason why we oscillate between conceptualizations of language teaching is for the sake of simplicity. Language and language learning are complex, with many different aspects, and the whole business can be difficult to conceptualize and deal with. Pedagogic messages which say ‘It's all . . . . . . . . .’ (habit-formation, lexis, communication or whatever – fill in the blank) have a seductive appeal. However, it is not all any one thing, and I cling to my conviction that no single approach or one-dimensional syllabus – of structures, functions, tasks or anything else – can serve as an effective organizing principle for instructed language teaching. Complexity is what we have to live with.

4.2 The success bias

Because existing methods are not very successful, we can easily feel that they are not working, throw them out and discredit their proponents. But we don't necessarily fail; we just don't usually do very well. Foreign languages are too hard for most people to learn well, especially in input-poor contexts, in the instructional time available. The sense of failure no doubt relates partly to the apparently intractable carry-over problem: the difficulty of achieving fluent and accurate spontaneous production of what is taught, and especially of grammar. Consolidation of the teaching and practice of structures by continuing attention to accurate use during more communicative work is difficult to contrive, and will usually have disappointing results. But switching to the opposite of what we were doing before is not necessarily a solution, even assuming that the problem in question – how to achieve a high level of grammatical accuracy in a second language – is one that can be, or needs to be, solved for most learners.

4.3 The English bias

A good deal of SLA theory grew up from studying the acquisition of English, and a certain bias continues. Now English happens to be a morphologically light language. Much of the basic structure can be picked up easily from exposure and practice, and beginning students can move quickly towards a modest working command. It is interesting to speculate what current language-teaching theory would look like if the main reference language were, say, Russian or Greek. These languages have a formidable array of inflectional grammar, and a beginning or elementary student has to pay a great deal of attention to these forms if he or she wants a moderate level of grammatical accuracy. They cannot easily be picked up from simple exposure to the limited input available to most students, supplemented by brief episodes of focus on form, and there is a powerful case for learning tables of inflections by heart to serve as ‘declarative crutches’ (DeKeyser Reference DeKeyser, Doughty and Williams1998: 49) on the road to procedural fluency. It is not at all certain that the act of faith whereby forms are taken to be learnable largely through classroom interaction would be possible if an inflecting language were the focus of language teaching theory to the extent that English is.

4.4 The ‘language in use’ bias

‘Something must be done. This is something. Therefore let us do it.’ (Antony Jay: Yes Minister)

Language is a formal code, used to convey meaningful messages, and as such it faces in two directions. Consequently, conceptualizations of language and language teaching tend to swing to and fro between the poles of code and message, form and meaning, knowledge and skill, imitation and expression, control and freedom, learning and using. It seems to me that much of applied linguistic thinking, along with theory-informed language teaching, is currently well towards the meaning-expression-freedom-communication end of the pendulum swing, and that this has been the case for several decades.

This pervasive bias has, I believe, skewed our thinking about language itself. Explorations of language in use, of the way different speech acts are expressed, and of the conventions governing verbal interaction and text construction have added greatly to our knowledge. They have also, however, encouraged a common confusion about the scope of pragmatics, leading to a widely repeated belief that language items in general inherently have two kinds of meaning, semantic and pragmatic, and that both kinds need to be taught. Hymes’ declaration (Reference Hymes, Pride and Holmes1971: 278) that there are rules of use without which the rules of grammar would be useless has resounded down the decades. Doughty & Williams suggest ‘that the degree of effectiveness . . . of focus on form ultimately depends on the level of integration of the learner's attention to all three aspects of form, meaning and function’ (Reference Doughty and Williams1998: 244–245). Similar recommendations are made by Celce-Murcia & Larsen-Freeman (Reference Celce-Murcia and Larsen-Freeman1999: 4) and Ellis (Reference Ellis2005). But the belief is, quite simply, a myth. To be sure, all utterances derive their communicative force from the interaction of their grammatical/lexical meanings with the context of utterance. ‘It's Wednesday’ can mean ‘Put the bins out’, ‘It's your turn to babysit’, ‘Remember Granny's coming to supper’, or any number of other things. But this is message meaning, not code meaning; there is no extra pragmatic information that we can give about declarative clause structure or the language items it, is, and Wednesday that will contribute to a better understanding of the utterance in question in context. Indeed, many structural elements in languages lack anything that can even reasonably be called semantic meaning, like the position of English relative clauses, gender agreement in Italian or the verb-second rule in German. For an optimistic attempt to strangle this confusion at birth, see Swan (Reference Swan1985); for a more extended treatment of the issue, see Swan (Reference Swan2007b).

In this context it is particularly irritating for a pedagogic grammarian to be told, as we often are, that the proper teaching of meaning, ‘language in use’ or ‘grammar as choice’, is a recent discovery.

Most grammars have focused on structure, describing the form and (sometimes) meaning of grammatical constructions out of context. They have not described how forms and meanings are actually used in spoken and written discourse. (Biber, Conrad & Leech Reference Biber, Conrad and Leech2002: 2)

Note the interesting implication that in the bad old days we did not even teach semantic meaning most of the time, concealing from our students the dangerous information that plural nouns refer to more than one entity, that past tenses are prototypically used to refer to past time, or that forms like older and more beautiful express comparison.

Perhaps the ‘two kinds of meaning’ myth is not really a cause for concern: few teachers are likely in practice to spend time trying to distinguish for their students the pragmatic from the semantic meanings of, say, microwave, waterproof or third-person -s. But the impact on course design and methodology of the ‘language in use’ bias is a different matter, and I believe it has seriously skewed our instructional principles and practice. ‘Communicative’ teaching has largely filled our horizons for 40-odd years, to the detriment of approaches and procedures which do not fit the paradigm. In our professional discussions, activity-related concepts are virtually guaranteed automatic approval. ‘Learner-centred’, ‘meaning-based’, ‘holistic’, ‘discourse’, ‘discovery’, ‘process’, ‘interaction’, ‘negotiation’ and ‘strategy’ are good things to say, on a par with ‘motherhood’ and ‘democracy’. ‘Teacher-dominated’, ‘form-based’, ‘discrete’, ‘sentence-level’, ‘transmission model’, ‘product’, ‘syllabus’ (unless preceded by ‘task’), ‘memorization’, ‘repetition’ and ‘drill’ are not so good: they relate to ‘bad’ pedagogic attitudes felt to be discredited and undesirable. At classroom level, our preference for learner-centred, naturalistic, activity-based learning can all too easily lead us to replace teaching language by doing things with it. The programme for the 2016 IATEFL conference listed around 650 presentations. Of these 25 or so were concerned with grammar teaching, 15 with vocabulary and 20 with pronunciation; these were vastly outnumbered by presentations related to communicative activities. In this conceptual climate, teachers can easily suppose that any kind of engagement with ‘real’ language – analysing a text, practising listening comprehension, launching a discussion or working through a communicative task – is automatically productive, without the need for critical scrutiny of the supposed language-learning payoff. For an unsympathetic examination of so-called ‘skills’ lessons from this point of view, see Swan & Walter (Reference Swan and Walter2017).

In the bad old days we were often better at teaching language than at teaching people to use it. We provided students, so to speak, with the necessary bricks and mortar and left them to build their own houses. Today I feel we often fall into the opposite trap: we get learners building houses right away, but assume that the various supplies needed will magically materialize – as if delivered by elves – at the right times and in the right quantities. This is a precarious assumption.

4.5 The need for common sense

Common sense is an unreliable faculty with no academic standing, and I was once roundly told off by a journal editor for attempting to invoke it. However, I think it can reasonably be appealed to in cases where theory seems to be losing contact with terra firma. One of my favourite odd ideas from an earlier generation is Candlin's ‘retrospective syllabus’ (Reference Candlin and Brumfit1984), which could only be constructed after the course had finished. More recently, I have found it hard to take seriously the dismissal of a concern with linguistic ‘products’ as typical of outmoded traditional approaches (Ellis Reference Ellis2003: 29). If I take foreign language lessons, products are exactly what I want, in the form of a reasonable command of the main aspects of the language, efficiently mediated. And I would not wish to come at this primarily by negotiating form and meaning with other beginners in the ways explored by Swain (Reference Swain2002). It has actually become possible, bizarrely, to criticize language teachers for focusing on language, as when Robinson (Reference Robinson and Robinson2001: 292) criticizes ‘language-based approaches’ because, unlike task-based approaches, they often involve simplified language and systematic practice. But what on earth is wrong with that? Would one condemn a piano teacher for giving ‘music-based’ lessons and setting work on scales and simple studies? One also finds teachers being told not to behave like teachers: Willis (Reference Willis2003: 167) takes exception to materials and activities which imply that students should learn from the teacher and follow his or her directives. Teaching can indeed go too far in the direction of form focus and teacher control; but if language teachers are not supposed either to teach or to concern themselves with language, perhaps we need to backtrack a little.

Some branches of 21st-century theory seem, indeed, to be in danger of losing touch with language teaching altogether. Allwright (2003: 114) tells us that according to the principles of ‘exploratory practice’ we should ‘above our concern for instructional efficiency, prioritize the quality of life in the language classroom’. Ellis & Shintani, in their characterization of TBLT at the level of method, say that ‘no attempt is made to specify the linguistic content to be learned’ (Reference Ellis and Shintani2014: 33). I would not wish to entrust my own quality of life to a heart surgeon whose training had paid no attention to the planned coverage of appropriate professional knowledge, no matter how happy a time he or she had had in medical school. Kumaravadivelu (Reference Kumaravadivelu2006: 201), speaking from some distance around the social turn, enunciates ten ‘macrostrategies’ which characterize ‘post-method’ language teaching. They include ‘maximize learning activities’, ‘activate intuitive heuristics’, ‘promote learner autonomy’, ‘ensure social relevance’ and ‘raise cultural consciousness’. These and his others are fine slogans, but I find it difficult to see what they have to do with language teaching. In the face of pronouncements like these (for which I would suggest that ‘punditry’ is a not inappropriate word), perhaps we need to remind ourselves that the main business of language teaching is, actually, teaching language.

In the belief that satire can be a more effective counter to this kind of thing than reasoned argument, I have published occasional papers poking fun at some of the more imperfectly earthed currents of thought that circulate in our field. Many of these appeared in various issues of the BAAL Newsletter; some are reprinted in Swan (Reference Swan2012). There is always a danger, unfortunately, that a satirical intention may pass the reader by. An article in the ELT Journal on teaching English by sensory deprivation (Swan & Walter Reference Swan and Walter1982), written to lampoon the fringe methods that mushroomed in the 1970s, elicited more than one serious request for further information about the approach, and for some time the title showed up occasionally in other writers’ reference lists.

4.6 The future

Applied linguistic research and theorizing will of course continue to provide essential support for the work of language-teaching practitioners. I feel, however, that these undertakings could sometimes benefit from a commitment to standards of argumentation and proof closer to those that are common in physical enquiry, to the extent that this is possible in the social sciences. Greater receptivity on the part of journals to replication studies would help in this respect (this journal is a notable exception to the general pattern). If this can be achieved, and if the problems and biases discussed above can be overcome, I believe it should be possible to make rapid progress. Given our knowledge of both formal and functional aspects of language, our growing understanding of acquisitional processes, and our impressive array of methods and materials, we probably already have most of the necessary ingredients for a balanced and effective model of instructed second-language learning. The pedagogic landscape is changing very quickly with the IT revolution, but it seems to me that the underlying principles governing language teaching are unaltered. Our job is still essentially to:

  • find out what knowledge and skills our learners need

  • subtract what they already know via their mother tongue or from earlier learning

  • subtract what they can get outside the classroom

  • of what is left, establish which elements matter most

  • of those, establish how many can be effectively taught and learnt under the instructional conditions in the time available

  • teach these by the methods that are most appropriate for each.

Defining the most appropriate methods is the difficult part, but that is what applied linguistics is for. It has my full confidence.

Michael Swan is a writer specializing in English Language teaching and reference materials. His many publications include Practical English Usage (Oxford University Press), the Cambridge English Course series (with Catherine Walter), and, also with Catherine Walter, the Oxford English Grammar Course. He has had extensive teaching experience with adult learners, and has worked with teachers in many countries. His academic interests include descriptive and pedagogic grammar, instructed and naturalistic SLA, cross-language influence in acquisition, and the relationship between applied linguistic theory and language-teaching practice. From 2002 to 2008 he was Visiting Professor of Applied Linguistics at St Mary's College, University of Surrey.

References

Allwright, D. (2003). Exploratory practice: Rethinking practitioner research in language teaching. Language Teaching Research 7.2, 113141.Google Scholar
Biber, D., Conrad, S. & Leech, D. (2002). Longman student grammar of spoken and written English. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Candlin, C. N. (1984). Syllabus design as a critical process. In Brumfit, C. J. (ed.), General English syllabus design. ELT Documents No. 118. London: Pergamon Press and The British Council, 29‒46.Google Scholar
Celce-Murcia, M. & Larsen-Freeman, D. (1999). The grammar book (2nd edn.). Boston, MA: Thomson Heinle.Google Scholar
Crawford, J. (2002). The role of materials in the language classroom: Finding the balance. In Richards, J. & Renandya, W. (eds.), 80–91.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (1998). Beyond focus on form: cognitive perspectives on learning and practicing second language grammar. In Doughty, C. & Williams, J. (eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 4263.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. (2001). Cognitive underpinnings of focus on form. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and second language instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 206257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C. & Williams, J. (eds.) (1998). Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dulay, H., Burt, H. & Krashen, S. (1982). Language two. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2002). The place of grammar instruction in the second/foreign language curriculum. In Hinkel, E. & Fotos, S. (eds.), New perspectives on grammar teaching in second language classrooms, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1734.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning and teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Principles of instructed language learning. System 33.2, 209224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2015). Understanding second language acquisition (2nd edn.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. & Shintani, N. (2014). Exploring language pedagogy through second language acquisition research. Abingdon/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Galbraith, J. K. (1998). Quoted in B. Laurance & W. Keegan, Crashes, Japan and walking sticks. The Observer 21 June 1998.Google Scholar
Hatch, E. (1978). Apply with caution. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 2, 123143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. & Filipović, L. (2012). English Profile Studies 1. Criterial features in L2 English: Specifying the reference levels of the Common European Framework. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hymes, D. H. (1971). On communicative competence. In Pride, J. & Holmes, J. (eds.), Sociolinguistics. London: Penguin, 269293.Google Scholar
Kumaravadivelu, B. (2006). Understanding language teaching: From method to postmethod. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lightbown, P. (2000). Anniversary article: Classroom SLA research and second language teaching. Applied Linguistics 21.4, 431462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H. (1981). Input, interaction and second language acquisition. In Winitz, H. (ed.), Native language and foreign language acquisition: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 379, 259‒278.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (1983). Does second language instruction make a difference? A review of the research. TESOL Quarterly 17: 359382.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. C. & Bahtia, T. K. (eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition. New York: Academic Press, 413‒468.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2000). Focus on form in task-based language teaching. In Lambert, R. H & Shohamy, E. (eds.), Language policy and pedagogy. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 179‒192.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2015a). First Person Singular: Building the road as we travel. Language Teaching 48.4, 561‒574.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2015b). Second language acquisition and task-based language teaching. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (1993). Review in Studies in Second Language Acquisition 15.1, 123.Google Scholar
Norris, J. M. & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning 50.3, 417‒528.Google Scholar
Oxford, R. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher should know. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language processing and second language development: Processability theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pullum, J. K. (1996). Nostalgic views from building 20. Journal of Linguistics 32.1, 137147.Google Scholar
Richards, J. (2002). Theories of teaching in language teaching. In Richards, J. & Renandya, W. (eds.), 19–25.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2001). Task complexity, cognitive resources, and syllabus design: A triadic framework for examining task influences on SLA. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and second language instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 287318.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. W. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics 11.2, 129‒158.Google Scholar
Sinclair, J. (1997). Corpus evidence in language description. In Wichmann, A. et al. (eds.), Teaching and language corpora. London: Longman, 2739.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spada, N. & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: A meta-analysis. Language Learning 60.2, 263308.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. In Lantolf, J. (ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 97114.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (2002). Peer-peer dialogue as a means of second language learning. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 22, 171185.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (1985). A critical look at the communicative approach (1). ELT Journal 39.1, 212.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2005). Legislating by hypothesis: The case of task-based instruction. Applied Linguistics 26.3, 376401.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2006). Chunks. The Teacher Trainer 20.3, 56.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2007a). History is not what happened: The case of contrastive analysis. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 17.3, 414419.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2007b). Grammar, meaning and pragmatics: Sorting out the muddle. TESL-EJ 11.2, 110.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2011). Grammar. In Simpson, J. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of applied linguistics, ch. 39. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2012). Thinking about language teaching: Selected articles 1982–2011. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Swan, M. (2014). Review of Hawkins & Filipović (2012). ELT Journal 68.1, 8996.Google Scholar
Swan, M. & Walter, H. C. (1982). The use of sensory deprivation in foreign language teaching. ELT Journal 36.3, 183185.Google Scholar
Swan, M. & Walter, H. C. (2017). Misunderstanding comprehension. ELT Journal 71.2, 228236.Google Scholar
West, M. (1953). A general service list of English words. London: Longman, Green & Co. Google Scholar
Willis, D. (2003). Rules, patterns and words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Willis, D. & Willis, J. (2001). Task-based language learning. In Carter, R. & Nunan, D. (eds.), The Cambridge guide to teaching English to speakers of other languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 173179.Google Scholar
Willis, J. (1996). A framework for task-based learning. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar