Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T23:52:17.324Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mental imagery in psychiatry: conceptual & clinical implications

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 January 2019

Julie L. Ji*
Affiliation:
Centre for the Advancement of Research on Emotion, School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Australia
David J. Kavanagh
Affiliation:
Institute of Health & Biomedical Innovation and School of Psychology & Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia
Emily A. Holmes
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology, Department for Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Colin MacLeod
Affiliation:
Centre for the Advancement of Research on Emotion, School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Australia
Martina Di Simplicio
Affiliation:
Centre for Psychiatry, Imperial College London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr. Julie L. Ji, School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Hwy, Crawley 6009 WA Australia. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Mental imagery refers to the experience of perception in the absence of external sensory input. Deficits in the ability to generate mental imagery or to distinguish it from actual sensory perception are linked to neurocognitive conditions such as dementia and schizophrenia, respectively. However, the importance of mental imagery to psychiatry extends beyond neurocognitive impairment. Mental imagery has a stronger link to emotion than verbal-linguistic cognition, serving to maintain and amplify emotional states, with downstream impacts on motivation and behavior. As a result, anomalies in the occurrence of emotion-laden mental imagery has transdiagnostic significance for emotion, motivation, and behavioral dysfunction across mental disorders. This review aims to demonstrate the conceptual and clinical significance of mental imagery in psychiatry through examples of mood and anxiety disorders, self-harm and suicidality, and addiction. We contend that focusing on mental imagery assessment in research and clinical practice can increase our understanding of the cognitive basis of psychopathology in mental disorders, with the potential to drive the development of algorithms to aid treatment decision-making and inform transdiagnostic treatment innovation.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2019

What is Mental Imagery?

Our conscious cognition can occur in mental imagery or verbal-linguistic formats. Mental imagery refers to perceptual experiences based on information stored in memory, in the absence of external sensory input,Reference Kosslyn, Ganis and Thompson 1 commonly referred to as seeing in the “mind’s eye,” hearing in the “mind’s ear,” etc.Reference Kosslyn, Ganis and Thompson 1 Mental imagery can be multi-sensory and occur in a variety of forms, from fleeting sensory impressions and static scenes (mental pictures) to detailed re-play or pre-play of events situated in a specific space, time, and context (mental movies). Mental imagery is involved in many everyday cognitive functionsReference Kosslyn, Seger, Pani and Hillger 2 such as spatial navigationReference Byrne, Becker and Burgess 3 and language comprehension.Reference Dils and Boroditsky 4 Mental imagery also plays a special role in representing our past and future experiencesReference Holmes and Mathews 5 , Reference Kosslyn 6 that is believed to facilitate adaptive decision-making, planning, and self-regulatory behavior.Reference Taylor, Pham and Rivkin 7 , Reference Seligman, Railton, Baumeister and Sripada 8 Importantly, the ability for mental imagery to emulate real-life experienceReference Moulton and Kosslyn 9 can powerfully impact emotionReference Holmes and Mathews 5 , Reference Di Simplicio, McInerney, Goodwin, Attenburrow and Holmes 10 and motivation.Reference May, Kavanagh and Andrade 11

While mental imagery dysfunction linked to neurocognitive impairment are implicated in mental disorders such as the dementiasReference Irish and Piolino 12 and schizophrenia,Reference Brébion, Ohlsen, Pilowsky and David 13 anomalies in the occurrence of mental imagery-based representations of salient stimuli is also prevalent across mental disorders and contribute to emotional, motivational, and behavioral dysregulation. Through examples of mood and anxiety disorders, self-harm and suicidality, and addiction, the present review aims to characterize the conceptual and clinical significance of mental imagery-based cognition as a tool for assessing psychopathology, and as a potential treatment target and a source of treatment innovation.

Approaches to Measuring Mental Imagery

When practitioners ask patients “what was running through your mind?” at times of reported distress, the assumption is that the content of cognition would assist in identifying factors that may be contributing to the distress. The present review contends that, in addition to the content of cognition, the representation format of such content (imagery or verbal) holds additional information about the impact of cognition. In other words, inviting patients to report on whether their cognition involved mental imagery may provide additional insight into the nature of psychopathology.Reference Di Simplicio, McInerney, Goodwin, Attenburrow and Holmes 14 Here, we move away from the standard clinical interview to possible research tools, too, some of which may also be useful in clinical practice.

Reporting naturally occurring mental imagery

Mental imagery can be consciously experienced and reported to another person, subject to the individual’s capacity to translate such experience into words or ratings and their willingness to communicate it. Clinically, people can report on their imagery in an assessment once given a definition of what is meant by mental imagery (eg, for intrusive memories in post-traumatic stress disorder [PTSD]). Retrospective reporting of mental imagery can introduce memory biases.Reference Kahneman 15 Thus, it may be desirable to minimize these effects in retrospective reports (eg, focusing on salient imagery or recent imagery rather than usual or average ratings), particularly for research purposes. Momentary assessments are now possible using mobile technologies, which can prompt the recording of naturally occurring cognition. If problematic emotion or behavior are more likely during certain times or occasions, prompts to record imagery can be scheduled accordingly. As such recording still relies on the person’s adherence, random sampling or assessment over selected periods may minimize the task burden. Initial tests of the feasibility of such methods are already underway in depression and anxiety research, with promising results.Reference Boukhechba, Gong and Kowsari 16 , Reference Chow, Fua and Huang 17

Questionnaire measures of mental imagery

Standardized measures of mental imagery ability tend to ask participants to generate imagery of everyday scenarios and, then, immediately report the phenomenology of such imagery, in either one sensory modality (Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire)Reference Marks 18 or multiple sensory modalities (The Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire).Reference Andrade, May, Deeprose, Baugh and Ganis 19 Alternatively, they may ask respondents to state how frequently they experience imagery during specific daily situations (Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale).Reference Reisberg, Pearson and Kosslyn 20 Such instruments attempt to assess generalized imagery ability, rather than assessing idiosyncratic imagery that may be rich in emotional content and personal relevance, the type that is of particular relevance to psychopathology. In some domains, psychometric measures have been developed to assess recent experiences of specific mental imagery, such as the Craving Experience QuestionnaireReference May, Andrade and Kavanagh 21 and the Motivation Thought Frequency and State Motivation Scales,Reference Kavanagh, Robinson, Connolly, Connor, Andrade and May 22 which assess the frequency and intensity of imagery associated with craving or motivation.

Laboratory assessments of mental imagery

Within the laboratory, mental imagery can be evoked and assessed in real time under controlled task conditions. Early experimental studies sought to understand the nature of mental imagery by devising problem-solving tasks that required the use of mental imagery, broken down into distinct components, from generation, maintenance, and inspection to transformation.Reference Kosslyn 6 Examples of such tasks include mentally visualizing clocks and reporting on the angle between clock hands for various times.Reference Paivio 23 Such tasks have the advantage of offering objective tests of reaction time performance that help to identify the underlying cognitive processes involved.

With the increasing availability of neuroimaging methods such as positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), researchers have been able to more directly compare brain activation during actual sensory perception and the mental imagery of the same information, such as visually presented faces and houses.Reference O’Craven and Kanwisher 24 However, these tasks are limited in their capacity to investigate mental imagery representations of real-world experiences that have emotional implications for a particular individual. To meet this need, laboratory imagery tasks were developed to assess mental imagery of emotion-provoking situations in everyday life, administered as paper-based instruments (eg, Prospective Imagery TaskReference Holmes, Lang, Moulds and Steele 25 ) or as computer tasks that can be completed in the laboratory or inside a brain scanner.Reference Szpunar and Schacter 26 , Reference Di Simplicio, Alfarano, Ji, Suri, Visser and Holmes 27 Importantly, spontaneous imagery can be assessed under standardized laboratory conditions using adapted mind-wandering paradigms,Reference Ji, Holmes, MacLeod and Murphy 28 which have potential to give further insight into the neurocognitive basis of associative imagery—a form of imagery that has not received sufficient attention to date. For a recent review of mental imagery measures, see Pearson et al., 2013Reference Pearson, Deeprose, Wallace-Hadrill, Burnett Heyes and Holmes 29 .

The Neurocognitive Basis of Mental Imagery

The nature of mental imagery has been the subject of a long-standing debate in psychological science,Reference Galton 30 and accounts of the main theoretical debates have been covered extensively.Reference Pearson and Kosslyn 31 Reference Pylyshyn 33 Neuroimaging studies have shown that both visual imagery generation and visual perception involves retinotopic activation of the primary visual cortex.Reference Dijkstra, Zeidman, Ondobaka, van Gerven and Friston 34 , Reference Slotnick, Thompson and Kosslyn 35 In addition to the visual modality, shared neural activation between actual sensory perception and mental imagery has been found for the auditory and somatosensory modalities.Reference Ehrsson, Geyer and Naito 36 , Reference Hubbard 37 Further, neuronal signals produced by imagined stimuli can integrate with signals generated by real stimuli of a different sensory modality,Reference Berger and Ehrsson 38 indicating common cross-modal sensory integration processes between perception and imagery. In consequence, mental imagery can function as a form of “weak perception.”Reference Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes and Kosslyn 39 Such neurocognitive findings help to explain why mental imagery can simulate past and future experiences in an as-if-real manner.Reference Ji, Heyes, MacLeod and Holmes 40 This key feature of mental imagery is central to its impact on emotion and motivation, which will be discussed later in this article.

While brain regions involved in actual sensory perception and mental imagery overlap substantially, perception is associated with bottom-up “feed-forward” projections from the primary sensory areas, and imagery generation and maintenance are associated with top-down “feedback” projections from the prefrontal cortex and superior parietal areas involved in cognitive control.Reference Stokes, Thompson, Cusack and Duncan 41 , Reference Mechelli, Price, Friston and Ishai 42 In addition, mental imagery also recruits neural regions involved in the retrieval of information from long-term memory stores and the maintenance or manipulation of such information in working memory.Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner 43 , Reference Baddeley and Andrade 44 A corollary is that neural damage to neural regions involved in cognitive control and memory will impair mental imagery functioning.

Impairments in General Mental Imagery Function

Congenital impairments in imagery generation

While most individuals can experience mental imagery, reports have emerged in recent years of healthy adults discovering for the first time that, unlike others, they cannot experience mental imagery. Such individuals exhibit severe deficiencies in the ability to remember the past as perceptual experiencesReference Palombo, Alain, Söderlund, Khuu and Levine 45 or to construct perceptual simulations of hypothetical future events.Reference Zeman, Dewar and Della Sala 46 Researchers coined new terms to describe such conditions, namely “aphantasia,”Reference Zeman, Dewar and Della Sala 46 and severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM).Reference Palombo, Alain, Söderlund, Khuu and Levine 45 Recent psychophysical evidence indicates that such individuals fail to show mental imagery-induced binocular rivalry, suggesting that the observed deficiencies in mental imagery reflect an absence of mental imagery, rather than alternative possibilities such as impaired metacognitive awareness of intact mental imagery.Reference Keogh and Pearson 47 Interestingly, such individuals report no major functional impairments in daily life and can perform highly on learning and memory tasks using compensatory strategies involving semantic memoryReference Palombo, Alain, Söderlund, Khuu and Levine 45 or spatial representation, which may be unimpaired despite the absence of visual images.Reference Keogh and Pearson 48 Neurocognitive research into aphantasia is at an early stage, and its structural or neuro-functional bases remains to be identified.

Acquired impairments in imagery generation due to neurological disease: the dementias

Unlike congenital aphantasia, acquired damage to the capacity to generate mental imagery has been observed in neurocognitive disorders. For example, patients with dementia can experience impaired ability to engage in mental imagery simulations of hypothetical future experiences. Dementia reflects a degeneration of neural regions that support the ability to remember past experiences as perceptually and experientially coherent events (episodic memory), including the medial prefrontal and parietal cortices, and temporal regions.Reference Schacter, Addis and Buckner 43 , Reference Buckner, Snyder and Shannon 49 Reference Szpunar, Watson and McDermott 53

The capacity to imagine perceptually and experientially coherent hypothetical future experiences (episodic prospection) relies on the ability to extract and recombine relevant contextual and sensory features from memory.Reference Addis and Schacter 54 In consequence, both episodic memory and episodic simulation are impaired in dementia.Reference Irish and Piolino 12 For example, when asked to recall or imagine an event in as much detail as possible in a three-minute time window, people with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) generated fewer event-related visuospatial and contextual perceptual details and experiential details (emotions and thoughts) relative to age-matched healthy controls.Reference Addis, Sacchetti, Ally, Budson and Schacter 55 Reference Irish, Hodges and Piguet 58

Impaired capacity to imagine future events appears to be ubiquitous across dementia subtypes.Reference Irish and Piolino 12 For example, patients with semantic dementia, a subtype of frontotemporal dementia that compromises memory of facts and knowledge rather than memory for events (episodic memory), also exhibit impaired ability to imagine hypothetical future events,Reference Irish and Piguet 59 , presumably because semantic information aids the recall of associated imagery elements and supports the construction of coherent episodic imagery. An impaired ability to imagine future events can cause significant functional impairments to daily life, including impaired decision-making because of insensitivity to future consequencesReference Bechara, Tranel and Damasio 60 and a loss of temporally coherent self-identity,Reference Addis, Sacchetti, Ally, Budson and Schacter 55 , Reference Irish, Addis, Hodges and Piguet 57 which has been linked to suicidality in patients with semantic dementia.Reference Hsiao, Kaiser, Fong and Mendez 61 However, the impact of impaired ability to imagine the future on emotional dysregulation and self-regulation in populations with dementia is still not well understood.Reference Irish and Piolino 12

Imagery anomalies associated with schizotypy

There has been significant research interest in the potential relationship between imagery and hallucinations in schizophrenia.Reference Seal, Aleman and McGuire 62 People who score higher on schizotypy questionnaires report greater levels of imagery use in daily lifeReference Aynsworth, Nemat, Collerton, Smailes and Dudley 63 and greater levels of reliving when asked to retrieve mental imagery of past events.Reference Winfield and Kamboj 64 Furthermore, people with higher schizotypy scores also report a greater tendency to experience imagery-based intrusions after a stressor.Reference Holmes and Steel 65 , Reference Steel, Fowler and Holmes 66 Additionally, people who experience hallucinationsReference Barrett 67 or are at high genetic risk for schizophrenia report greater ease of imagery generation and higher vividness of imagery generated in the laboratory.Reference Crespi, Leach, Dinsdale, Mokkonen and Hurd 68 However, other studies have either found no schizotypy-related differences in the vividness of imagery when assessed in the laboratoryReference Aynsworth, Nemat, Collerton, Smailes and Dudley 63 or have only observed such effects for imagery of specific sensory modalitiesReference Winfield and Kamboj 64 or emotional tone.Reference Aynsworth, Nemat, Collerton, Smailes and Dudley 63

In tasks in which pictures and written words are presented for encoding, followed by a surprise test of recognition memory, people who experience visual hallucinations were more likely to misremember verbally presented information as having been visually presented.Reference Aynsworth, Nemat, Collerton, Smailes and Dudley 63 This error has been observed in patients with schizophrenia who experience visual hallucinations, relative to patients with schizophrenia who do not experience visual hallucinationsReference Brébion, Ohlsen, Pilowsky and David 13 , Reference Brébion, Ohlsen, Bressan and David 69 or healthy individuals.Reference Brébion, Ohlsen, Pilowsky and David 13 Such results have been attributed to the presence of vivid visual imagery representations of the information presented, leading to impaired encoding or retrieval of the source information. An argument has been made that confusion between internal and external stimuli may also occur during perception, contributing to the experience of hallucinations in both schizophrenia Reference Bentall, Baker and Havers 70 and other conditions such as Parkinson’s disease (PD).Reference Barnes, Boubert, Harris, Lee and David 71 While this hypothesis is plausible, direct evidence for the confusion between mental imagery and actual sensory perception is still required.

Heightened imagery strength appears unlikely to be a contributor to sensory hallucinations. Consistent with the presence of deficits in executive function in schizophrenia,Reference Keefe and Harvey 72 the retrieval and generation of imagery are often impaired.Reference D’Argembeau, Raffard, Van and der Linden 73 , Reference Steel, Wykes, Ruddle, Smith, Shah and Holmes 74 Studies using objective measures of mental imagery ability (such as mental rotation) have found little relationship between auditory hallucinations and imagery vividness in schizophrenia.Reference Seal, Aleman and McGuire 62 Importantly, relative to healthy controls, the higher vividness of deliberately generated mental imagery appears to be unrelated to hallucination frequency in patients with schizophrenia,Reference Sack, van de Ven, Etschenberg, Schatz and Linden 75 , Reference Oertel, Rotarska-Jagiela and van de Ven 76 relatives of these patients, and people with high schizotypy.Reference Oertel, Rotarska-Jagiela and van de Ven 76 However, it may be premature to say that the issue is completely resolved. For example, the heightened vividness of deliberately generated imagery may not relate to hallucination frequency, but the heightened vividness of spontaneously generated imagery may contribute to reality-monitoring errors. Future studies could investigate schizotypy-linked individual differences in the vividness of spontaneously generated imagery that is assessed under standardized laboratory conditions (eg, using adapted mind-wandering paradigmsReference Ji, Holmes, MacLeod and Murphy 28 ).

Mood and anxiety disorders

Impaired general imagery generation ability in anxious individualsReference Morrison, Amir and Taylor 77 and reduced imagery generation and manipulation ability in depression have been demonstrated.Reference Zarrinpar, Deldin and Kosslyn 78 A more recent comprehensive assessment revealed no deficits in the ability to generate, manipulate, or recall images in people with bipolar disorder, major depression, or anxiety disorders relative to healthy controls matched on socio-demographic factors.Reference Di Simplicio, Renner and Blackwell 79 More isolated imagery generation deficits have been reported in anxiety disorders, concerning the ability to generate event-related details of future simulations.Reference Wu, Szpunar, Godovich, Schacter and Hoffman 80 However, mental imagery-linked dysfunction in mood and anxiety disorders appears to be primarily linked to its emotional amplification effects when the content of cognition is emotion laden, which is discussed in more detail in the following section.

The Impact of Mental Imagery on Emotion and Motivation

In addition to impairments in the general capacity to generate mental imagery or to discriminate it from actual sensory perception, anomalies in imagery-based emotion-laden cognition are of importance to mental disorders because of its impact on emotion and motivation.

Mental imagery can evoke strong emotional responses

Many experimental studies have demonstrated that mental imagery can evoke emotional responses at subjective and physiological levels.Reference Ji, Heyes, MacLeod and Holmes 40 For example, in healthy people, imagining fear and anger-related situations leads to heart rate accelerationReference Vrana and Lang 81 , Reference Witvliet and Vrana 82 ; increased skin conductanceReference Lang, Levin, Miller and Kozak 83 , Reference Weerts and Lang 84 and potentiation of the startle blink reflexReference Vrana and Lang 81 , Reference Cuthbert, Lang, Strauss, Drobes, Patrick and Bradley 85 relative to imagining emotionally neutral stimuli.

At the neural level, fMRI studies on the visual imagery of emotional stimuli have found patterns of activation akin to those found during visual perception of the same stimuli. For example, the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) are selectively activated during both visual perceptionReference Sabatinelli, Bradley, Lang, Costa and Versace 86 and visual imagery of emotionally positive scenes.Reference Costa, Lang, Sabatinelli, Versace and Bradley 87 The amygdala can also be activated during both visual perceptionReference Kim, Kim and Kim 88 and visual imageryReference Costa, Lang, Sabatinelli, Versace and Bradley 87 , Reference Kim, Kim and Kim 88 of emotionally positive and negative scenes or faces. Finally, fMRI studies using real-time neural activation feedback show that, over successive trials, participants were able to use idiosyncratic mental imagery of positive and negative scenarios to regulate activation of emotion processing regions such as the insula.Reference Lee, Ruiz, Caria, Veit, Birbaumer and Sitaram 89

The functional impact of imagery on emotion

To investigate whether imagery plays a functional role in eliciting emotional responses, researchers have interfered with imagery generation and tested whether such interference leads to reductions in imagery vividness and emotional impact. Typically, studies have employed a concurrent task to reduce the availability of the visuospatial working memory resources required for visual mental imagery generation. For example, Andrade and colleagues had participants generate mental imagery in response to emotional cues, and those who engaged in simultaneous finger tapping of a spatial pattern reported lower vividness of mental imagery than those who did not perform a concurrent task.Reference Andrade, Kavanagh and Baddeley 90 Importantly, the reduction in imagery vividness accompanied a corresponding decline in the intensity of the emotional response to the negative imagery. This result was replicated using concurrent eye movements, instead of finger tapping, during repeated emotional memory imagery generation.Reference Kavanagh, Freese, Andrade and May 91 Other researchers have likewise shown that similar types of concurrent task interference can reduce the emotional impact of imagery representing feared future events in healthy and clinical samples.Reference Engelhard, van den Hout, Janssen and van der Beek 92 Reference Lilley, Andrade, Turpin, Sabin-Farrell and Holmes 94

Researchers have also tested the impact of visuospatial interference on the frequency of negative intrusive mental imagery. There is growing evidence that playing the visuospatial computer game, Tetris,Reference Lau-Zhu, Holmes, Butterfield and Holmes 95 following exposure to analog trauma film clips depicting traumatic scenes reduces the frequency of subsequent involuntary memory imagery of the trauma film.Reference James, Lau-Zhu, Clark, Visser, Hagenaars and Holmes 96 For example, in an early study, healthy people either played Tetris or not after viewing the traumatic film and reported intrusive imagery of the film clip content in a diary over the following week.Reference Holmes, James, Coode-Bate and Deeprose 97 Those who played Tetris after the film reported experiencing fewer imagery intrusions and distress than those who had not.Reference Holmes, James, Coode-Bate and Deeprose 97 Recent proof of concept studies moving from the lab to the clinic show that a visuospatial interference task, relative to a control task, led to reductions in the frequency of negative intrusive mental imagery when administered soon after a road traffic accidentReference Iyadurai, Blackwell and Meiser-Stedman 98 or traumatic childbirth.Reference Horsch, Vial and Favrod 99

Mental imagery as an emotional amplifier: a comparison with verbal-linguistic cognition

The abovementioned findings showed that mental imagery can evoke emotional responses may be because of the emotional content of imagined stimuli, rather than because of the imagery representation of such stimuli. Several studies have sought to address this question by comparing the relative emotional impact of imagery-based and verbal-linguistic representations of the same information. In one study, healthy participants were instructed to either generate imagery or focus on the semantic meaning of initially ambiguous auditory scenarios that ended negatively (eg, “You are at work when you hear the fire alarms go off. You run to the exit to discover that it is . . . for real”).Reference Holmes and Mathews 5 Participants in the imagery condition reported greater increases in self-reported anxiety than the verbal condition.Reference Holmes and Mathews 5 This finding has since been replicated by subsequent studies that found imagery representations to have evoked greater emotional impact than did verbal-linguistic representations of negative informationReference Holmes, Lang and Shah 100 Reference Nelis, Holmes, Palmieri, Bellelli and Raes 102 and positive information.Reference Holmes, Lang and Shah 100 , Reference Holmes, Coughtrey and Connor 103 Researchers have, therefore, proposed that mental imagery can amplify the impact of cognition on emotion and exacerbate mood instability in both negative and positive directions, such as in bipolar disorder.Reference Holmes, Geddes, Colom and Goodwin 104

The impact of emotional, mental imagery on motivation

Functional theories of emotion postulate that emotions serve to motivate approach and avoidance behavior in response to appetitive and aversive cues, where cues can be internal cognition or stimuli in the environment.Reference Carver 105 , Reference Lang and Bradley 106 For appetitive or aversive stimuli, the moment-to-moment experience of being motivated to approach or avoid such stimuli involves emotionally charged cognition.Reference Kavanagh, Andrade and May 107 Mental imagery representations of appetitive stimuli can evoke intense desires or craving for, and motivation to acquire, such stimuli.

For example, higher levels of vividness of food imagery are associated with more intense craving for the imagined food item,Reference Tiggemann and Kemps 108 and such effects are amplified in dieters.Reference Harvey, Kemps and Tiggemann 109 The vividness of imagery concerning a wide range of other substances and activities are also positively related to the intensity of desires to consume the imagined substances or to engage in the imagined activities.Reference May, Andrade, Kavanagh and Penfound 110 Similarly, in cigarette smokers, instructions to generate smoking-related imagery is reliably associated with significant increases in self-reported craving,Reference Maude-Griffin and Tiffany 111 , Reference Tiffany and Drobes 112 comparable to those from in-vivo exposure to cigarettes.Reference Drobes and Tiffany 113 However, direct comparisons of mental imagery and smoking cues show stronger effects from lighting a cigarette than from smoking-related imagery,Reference Heishman, Lee, Taylor and Singleton 114 consistent with the idea that mental imagery functions as a weak form of actual sensory perception.Reference Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes and Kosslyn 39

Physiologically, evoking mental imagery of food has been shown to modulate the gustatory salivary reflex (salivary pH levels) in a similar way to having participants process the sensory qualities of real food.Reference Vanhaudenhuyse, Bruno, Brédart, Plenevaux and Laureys 115 Furthermore, repetitive mental imagery concerning food consumption has been shown to produce satiation.Reference Morewedge, Huh and Vosgerau 116

Neuroimaging evidence also corroborates the role of mental imagery in addiction-related processes. For example, in individuals with cocaine addiction, neural activation during mental imagery of personalized drug use, as compared with anger-related mental imagery, is associated with greater activation in the limbic, paralimbic, and striatal brain regions, which are involved in reward learning, wanting, and anticipation.Reference Kilts, Schweitzer and Quinn 117

In the natural environment, craving-related imagery is typically evoked by associated cues in the form of internal cognition or environmental stimuli. The anticipated pleasure or relief provided by imagery of the desired substance leads to the further elaboration of such imagery,Reference May, Kavanagh and Andrade 11 , Reference Kavanagh, Andrade and May 107 which may reduce the level of cognitive resources available for performing other cognitive tasks, particularly those requiring visuospatial working memory.Reference Tiggemann, Kemps and Parnell 118

The functional impact of imagery on motivation

A range of concurrent visuospatial tasks has been used to reduce craving by disrupting imagery of desired stimuli, including lateral eye movements,Reference Littel, van den Hout and Engelhard 119 the elicitation of competing mental imagery,Reference Schumacher, Kemps and Tiggemann 120 playing Tetris,Reference Skorka-Brown, Andrade, Whalley and May 121 and modeling clay.Reference Andrade, Pears, May and Kavanagh 122 Reductions in craving have also been observed from the use of more passive tasks such as viewing changing visual patterns on a screen such as “dynamic visual noise”Reference Kemps and Tiggemann 123 , Reference May, Andrade, Panabokke and Kavanagh 124 or scanning bodily sensations.Reference Hamilton, Fawson, May, Andrade and Kavanagh 125 Such results provide evidence that imagery representations of desired stimuli have a causal impact on approach motivation for such stimuli.

Contexts in which concurrent task interference do not reduce craving are also illuminating. For example, focusing on abdominal sensations,Reference May, Andrade, Batey, Berry and Kavanagh 126 inhaling a sweet scent during imagery of desired food items,Reference Firmin, Gillette, Hobbs and Wu 127 or completing a concurrent verbal working memory task Reference Andrade, Pears, May and Kavanagh 122 do not reduce craving. Successful disruption of cravings induced by imagining the desired stimuli appears to require tasks that tax the same sensory domains as those involved in cravings induced by actual encounters with the stimuli. In the case of coffeeReference Kemps and Tiggemann 128 and cigarettes,Reference Versland and Rosenberg 129 both concurrent visual and olfactory imagery are effective at reducing craving. However, concurrent auditory imagery does not interfere with cravings for foodReference Kemps and Tiggemann 130 or coffeeReference Kemps and Tiggemann 128 that is consistent with observations that sound is not a prominent feature in desires for these targets.Reference May, Andrade, Kavanagh and Penfound 110

In summary, evidence suggests that mental imagery-based cognitions are involved in emotional and motivational processing, and comparative data with verbal-linguistic cognition suggest that imagery has a greater impact on emotional and motivational outcomes. Interfering with imagery of desired stimuli weakens its emotional and motivational impact, providing persuasive evidence of a causal relationship between mental imagery and its emotional and motivational response.

Emotional, Mental imagery-linked Dysfunction in Psychopathology

Given the capacity of mental imagery to impact emotion and motivation, anomalies in the occurrence of emotional mental imagery may contribute to psychopathology in a transdiagnostic manner. This section reviews current evidence linking anomalies in the occurrence of mental imagery to emotional, motivational, and behavioral dysfunction through the example of several mental disorders.

Anxiety disorders

Elevated anxiety appears to be linked to elevated levels of vividness of negative future imageryReference Morina, Deeprose, Pusowski, Schmid and Holmes 131 , Reference Pile and Lau 132 and a greater ease of generating negative future imagery relative to non-negative future imagery.Reference Wu, Szpunar, Godovich, Schacter and Hoffman 80 In a sample of people with mood and anxiety disorders, higher anxiety levels corresponded with higher levels of negative future imagery vividness and higher sensations of “as-if-real” pre-experiencing of the imagined negative future event.Reference Di Simplicio, Renner and Blackwell 79

Unwanted mental imagery that comes to mind unbidden is prevalent in anxiety disorders.Reference Hirsch and Holmes 133 For example, intrusions of trauma-related scenes in PTSD,Reference Association 134 typically triggered by sensory stimuli associated with the trauma, are a core symptom that causes significant distress, anxiety, and avoidance behaviors that impair daily functioning.Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael 135 , Reference Grey and Holmes 136 Similarly, mental imagery of feared contamination scenes in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is highly prevalent and is associated with distress, anxiety, and compulsive washing.Reference Coughtrey, Shafran, Lee and Rachman 137 In social anxiety, individuals often experience mental imagery of themselves committing a social gaffe or appearing anxious to observers, with such imagery eliciting subjective anxiety and distress.Reference Hirsch, Clark, Mathews and Williams 138

Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is characterized by repetitive self-referential negative verbal styles of thinking, such as worry.Reference Borkovec, Alcaine and Behar 139 Researchers have proposed that people with GAD worry in verbal form to avoid the highly emotional and physiologically arousing negative imagery of feared outcomes.Reference Stöber 140 Experimental evidence provides support for this account, as inducing repeated verbal thinking of hypothetical future events leads increased abstractness of thought content and reduced imagery over time.Reference Behar, DiMarco, Hekler, Mohlman and Staples 141 However, while repetitive abstract verbal thinking about feared outcomes lowers fear and anxiety initially, it is followed by increases in anxiety that coincide with the occurrence of imagery.Reference Behar, DiMarco, Hekler, Mohlman and Staples 141

Another way in which mental imagery may contribute to dysfunctional avoidance behaviors that sustain anxiety, such as in phobias, is through aversive conditioning. Dadds and colleagues investigated individual differences in imagery ability and the tendency to report specific aversions such as phobias of certain foods.Reference Dadds, Hawes, Schaefer and Vaka 142 Results indicated that higher imagery ability was associated with higher levels of aversion, even after controlling for potential confounders such as proneness to disgust.Reference Dadds, Hawes, Schaefer and Vaka 142 Presumably, a greater ability to engage in the imagery of food consumption may strengthen the learned associations between the food stimuli and disgust,Reference Dadds, Bovbjerg, Redd and Cutmore 143 contributing to avoidance of the food item and, thereby, maintaining the phobia.

In summary, emotional, mental imagery could maintain anxiety either directly through vivid imagery of aversive scenarios or intrusive traumatic imagery or indirectly through avoidance of aversive images that sustains avoidance behaviors, maintaining anxiety.

Mood disorders: major depressive disorder

Depression has been associated with the elevated occurrence of emotionally distressing mental imagery and reduced occurrence of positive mental imagery.Reference Holmes, Blackwell, Burnett Heyes, Renner and Raes 144 For example, in community samples, depressive symptoms are related to higher frequencies of negative mental imagery and less frequent experiences of positive imagery.Reference Weßlau, Cloos, Höfling and Steil 145 When instructed to imagine negative and positive personal future events within different time periods, individuals with major depression are found to generate fewer positive, but not negative, future events relative to healthy controls.Reference MacLeod and Conway 146 Reference MacLeod, Tata, Tyrer, Schmidt, Davidson and Thompson 148

Investigators have also examined depression-linked biases in phenomenological aspects such as imagery vividness. When instructed to generate imagery in response to verbal sentences describing positive and negative future situations, people dysphoria,Reference Holmes, Lang, Moulds and Steele 25 , Reference Szőllősi, Pajkossy and Racsmány 149 major depression,Reference Kemps and Tiggemann 130 and major depression with acquired brain injury (ABI)Reference Murphy, Veers, Blackwell, Holmes and Manly 150 report lower vividness for imagery of positive relative to negative future events, as compared with healthy controls. In patients with ABI, a reduced capacity to anticipate positive relative to negative future events and reduced imagery vividness of positive relative to negative future events, as compared with healthy controls, was uniquely found in patients with ABI and depression, but not patients with ABI without major depression.Reference Murphy, Veers, Blackwell, Holmes and Manly 150 Further, reduced positive imagery vividness appears to be uniquely associated with depression, not anxiety, in adolescence.Reference Pile and Lau 132

In addition to anomalies in deliberately generated emotional imagery, patients with depression tend to report frequent experiences involving unwanted mental imagery in the form of spontaneously occurring emotionally aversive mental imagery of past and future experiences.Reference Patel, Brewin, Wheatley, Wells, Fisher and Myers 151 Recent laboratory research has shown that higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) are associated with lower tendencies to spontaneously imagine positive relative to negative aspects of the future during mind-wandering.Reference Ji, Holmes, MacLeod and Murphy 28 Such anomalies in mental imagery–based cognition are postulated to maintain the depression-linked elevation of negative emotional states and attenuation of positive emotional states.Reference Holmes, Blackwell, Burnett Heyes, Renner and Raes 144

At present, there is little research linking impoverished positive mental imagery to anhedonia (loss of interest and motivation in rewarding experiences), a core symptom of depression. However, results from a treatment study suggested that, for people with clinical levels of depression, four weeks of positive mental imagery training resulted in a faster and greater recovery of re-engagement in daily activities than in a non-imagery active control condition,Reference Renner, Ji, Pictet, Holmes and Blackwell 152 indicating that positive mental imagery–based manipulations may enhance behavioral activation in depression.

Mood disorders: bipolar disorder

Patients with bipolar disorder also have highly vivid negative future imagery, and vividness is higher in those with a more unstable course of the disorder.Reference Hales, Deeprose, Goodwin and Holmes 153 Unlike major depression, bipolar disorder is associated with the presence of vivid positive imagery at times of high positive affect.Reference Ivins, Di Simplicio, Close, Goodwin and Holmes 154 Hypomania is associated with intrusive positive imagery about future eventsReference Gregory, Brewin, Mansell and Donaldson 155 and a greater tendency to use imagery in daily life.Reference Meyer, Finucane and Jordan 156 In a laboratory study, hypomanic traits predicted greater emotional response to positive mental imagery in adolescents.Reference O’Donnell, Di Simplicio, Brown, Holmes and Burnett Heyes 157 The bipolar phenotype appears to be associated with greater vividness and emotional response to both negative and positive mental imagery, which is postulated to exacerbate mood oscillationsReference Holmes, Coughtrey and Connor 103 and maladaptive approach-related behaviors in bipolar disorder.Reference O’Donnell, Di Simplicio, Brown, Holmes and Burnett Heyes 157 While preliminary evidence suggests that mood lability is associated with greater vividness and self-involvement of mental imagery across mood and anxiety disorders,Reference Di Simplicio, Renner and Blackwell 79 further experimental research is required to investigate possible downstream impacts on behavior in bipolar disorder.

In summary, anomalies in the strength and occurrence of positive and negative mental imagery can contribute to mood dysregulation and mood instability in a transdiagnostic manner.

Non-suicidal self-injury and suicidal behavior

Mental imagery of self-harm has been linked to non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI). One study surveyed NSSI cognition and behavior in college students and found the occurrence of NSSI mental imagery of past NSSI acts frequently preceded NSSI behavior, particularly while the urge to self-harm is strong.Reference Hasking, Di Simplicio, McEvoy and Rees 158 Specific characteristics of self-harm imagery may relate to acting or not acting on the self-harm urges, depending on whether the imagery was appraised as aversive or comforting.Reference Hasking, Di Simplicio, McEvoy and Rees 158 These findings indicate that emotion-laden mental imagery of self-harm may play a role in motivating approach behaviors toward, or avoidance behaviors from, self-harm.Reference McEvoy, Hayes, Hasking and Rees 159

In addition to NSSI, compelling intrusions of imagined suicidal acts (suicide “flash-forwards”)Reference Holmes, Crane, Fennell and Williams 160 are often reported by people with major depressionReference Holmes, Crane, Fennell and Williams 160 , Reference Crane, Shah, Barnhofer and Holmes 161 and individuals with bipolar disorder.Reference Hales, Deeprose, Goodwin and Holmes 153 Consistent with the Interpersonal Theory of Suicide,Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner 162 graphic mental imagery of suicide may facilitate suicidal behavior by contributing to the process of habituating to the painful and fearful aspects of death by suicide and by facilitating planning, rehearsal, and action readiness.Reference Crane, Shah, Barnhofer and Holmes 161 Accordingly, cross-sectional research in college samples has reported associations between frequent and vivid suicide-related mental imagery and higher levels of suicidal intentReference Selby, Anestis, Joiner, Selby, Anestis and Joiner 163 or past suicidal behavior.Reference Holaday and Brausch 164 Importantly, in a recent longitudinal study in a Chinese in-patient sample, the occurrence of suicidal “flash-forwards” significantly predicted suicidal intent at both baseline and 7 weeks,Reference Ng, Di Simplicio, McManus, Kennerley and Holmes 165 and suicidal imagery was among the factors differentiating suicidal intent from suicidal attempts in a representative sample of young adults.Reference Wetherall, Cleare and Eschle 166

Substance-related and addictive disorders

As previously noted, more vivid imagery is associated with more intense craving—a diagnostic feature of addictions (DSM-V).Reference Association 134 During treatment for alcohol abuse or dependence, a study found that 81% of participants reported experiencing imagery associated with craving.Reference Kavanagh, May and Andrade 167 Importantly, this mental imagery was not just visual in modality, as taste imagery was also prominent, providing a unique contribution to the prediction of craving strength.Reference Kavanagh, May and Andrade 167

Mental imagery can also amplify cravings elicited by other factors such as physiological withdrawal. For example, smoking urges in both cigarette-deprived and non-deprived groups are accentuated by experimentally inducing mental imagery of cigarettes.Reference Drobes and Tiffany 113 However, if the person is attempting to abstain from acquisition or consumption of the desired stimuli, imagery-induced craving also elicits anxiety.Reference Fox, Bergquist, Hong and Sinha 168

The relationship between the imagery of appetitive stimuli and approach behavior toward such stimuli is complex. While simple actions such as lighting a cigarette, or more complex habitual chains of actions, may become automated and directly activated by environmental cues without conscious cognition,Reference Tiffany 169 most cases of substance use involve some degree of conscious planning or context-specific modification of behavior (eg, finding a tobacco outlet).Reference Kavanagh, Andrade and May 107 Conscious planning and behavioral modification are more likely to occur when craving intensity is high, that is, in turn, associated with more vivid and frequent imagery of the desired stimuli.Reference May, Kavanagh and Andrade 11

Of course, the effects of desires on behavior are constrained by additional factors such as stimuli availability, self-control, and the strength of countervailing goals desires.Reference Kavanagh, Andrade and May 107 Despite such constraints, the frequency of alcohol imagery accounted for 16% of the variance in monthly alcohol consumption in young adults.Reference Connor, Kavanagh and Andrade 170 The frequency of craving imagery also predicts relapse and treatment dropout in inpatients being treated for alcohol dependence.Reference Coates, Gullo and Feeney 171 Therefore, the occurrence of craving imagery holds important implications for high-risk approach behaviors in addiction.

Future Directions

Evidence of the link between mental imagery-based cognition and emotional, motivational, and behavioral dysfunction across several categories of mental disorders strongly suggests that clinical assessment should focus on the content and format of cognition.

One important reason for assessing both the content of cognition and the format of cognition is that frequent and vivid mental imagery may be a key factor behind the progression from ideation to action. The clinical implications of not assessing imagery have been highlighted in the field of suicidal behavior, in which missed identification of imagery of suicidal action means neglecting an index of progression to a potential attempt.Reference Wetherall, Cleare and Eschle 166 Future research should further investigate the link between mental imagery and emotionally charged risky behaviors such as NSSI, mania, aggression, and addiction.

A second reason to assess mental imagery is that we need to obtain a better understanding of the neurocognitive bases of imagery representations of salient stimuli that contribute to psychopathology. Initial advances have been made in exploring the basis of trauma flashbacksReference Clark, Holmes, Woolrich and Mackay 172 and also of elevated emotional imagery in bipolar disorder.Reference Di Simplicio, Alfarano, Ji, Suri, Visser and Holmes 27 Future research should continue to focus on delineating the precise neural substrates and cognitive processes involved in emotionally charged mental imagery and how such processes are disrupted in psychopathology.

While this review has focused on mental imagery representations that play a role in emotional and motivational dysfunction, mental imagery can also be a powerful lever toward wellbeing and adaptive behavior.Reference Holmes, Arntz and Smucker 173 There has been recent growing interest in developing imagery-based interventions that could train individuals to use positive imagery to improve mood,Reference Blackwell, Browning and Mathews 174 and the use of soothing imagery is already part of cognitive therapy approaches for depression, trauma, and personality disorders.Reference Wilson, Schwannauer, McLaughlin, Ashworth and Chan 175 , Reference Naismith, Mwale and Feigenbaum 176 Research on positive imagery has also demonstrated the importance of fostering motivation for functional goals such as weight reduction.Reference Solbrig, Whalley and Kavanagh 177 The emotional and motivational power of mental imagery suggests that these examples will be precursors of other, highly innovative applications of mental imagery to treatment.

Conclusions

Mental imagery–based cognition is of significant conceptual and clinical relevance to psychiatry and allied disciplines. Mental imagery is core to our capacity to re-experience past events and pre-experience future ones, affording it the power to impact emotion, motivation, and behavior. Through the examples of mood and anxiety disorders, self-injury, and addiction, this review aimed to characterize mental imagery–linked dysfunction as extending beyond the contexts of neurocognitive impairment. When problematic cognition occurs in mental imagery form, its impact on emotion, motivation, and behavior is amplified, which serves to maintain and exacerbate psychopathology and dysfunction. Therefore, mental imagery is an important target for clinical assessment across mental disorders, and a deeper understanding of mental imagery-linked dysfunction has the potential to drive the development of algorithms to aid treatment decision-making and inform transdiagnostic treatment innovation.

Disclosure

Julie Ji was supported by the Forrest Research Foundation. Emily Holmes was supported by the Karolinska Institutet and the Swedish Research Council (VR). Colin MacLeod, David Kavanagh, and Martina Di Simplicio have nothing to disclose.

References

1. Kosslyn, SM, Ganis, G, Thompson, WL. Neural foundations of imagery. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001; 2(9): 635642.Google Scholar
2. Kosslyn, SM, Seger, C, Pani, JR, Hillger, LA. When is imagery used in everyday life? A diary study. J Ment Imagery. 1990; 14(3–4):131152.Google Scholar
3. Byrne, P, Becker, S, Burgess, N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 2007; 114(2): 340375.Google Scholar
4. Dils, AT, Boroditsky, L. Visual motion aftereffect from understanding motion language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010; 107(37): 1639616400.Google Scholar
5. Holmes, EA, Mathews, A. Mental imagery and emotion: a special relationship? Emotion. 2005; 5(4): 489497.Google Scholar
6. Kosslyn, SM. Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1994.Google Scholar
7. Taylor, SE, Pham, LB, Rivkin, ID. Armor D a. Harnessing the imagination. Am Psychol. 1998; 53(4): 429439.Google Scholar
8. Seligman, MEP, Railton, P, Baumeister, RF, Sripada, C. Navigating Into the Future or Driven by the Past. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013; 8(2): 119141.Google Scholar
9. Moulton, ST, Kosslyn, SM. Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental emulation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009; 364(1521): 12731280.Google Scholar
10. Di Simplicio, M, McInerney, JE, Goodwin, GM, Attenburrow, M–J, Holmes, EA. Revealing the mind’s eye: bringing (mental) images into psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 2012; 169(12): 12451246.Google Scholar
11. May, J, Kavanagh, DJ, Andrade, J. The Elaborated Intrusion Theory of desire: a 10-year retrospective and implications for addiction treatments. Addict Behav. 2015; 44(suppl C): 2934.Google Scholar
12. Irish, M, Piolino, P. Impaired capacity for prospection in the dementias—theoretical and clinical implications. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016; 55(1): 4968.Google Scholar
13. Brébion, G, Ohlsen, RI, Pilowsky, LS, David, AS. Visual hallucinations in schizophrenia: confusion between imagination and perception. Neuropsychology. 2008; 22(3): 383389.Google Scholar
14. Di Simplicio, M, McInerney, JE, Goodwin, GM, Attenburrow, MJ, Holmes, EA. Revealing the mind’s eye: bringing (mental) images into psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 2012; 169(12): 12451246.Google Scholar
15. Kahneman, D. Experienced utility and objective happiness: A moment-based approach. In: Kahneman D, Tversky A, eds. Choices, Values and Frames. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2003.Google Scholar
16. Boukhechba, M, Gong, J, Kowsari, K, et al. Physiological changes over the course of cognitive bias modification for social anxiety. In: 2018 IEEE EMBS International Conference on Biomedical & Health Informatics (BHI). 2018: 422425 Google Scholar
17. Chow, PI, Fua, K, Huang, Y, et al. Using Mobile Sensing to Test Clinical Models of Depression, Social Anxiety, State Affect, and Social Isolation Among College Students. Calvo R, ed. J Med Internet Res. 2017; 19(3): e62.Google Scholar
18. Marks, DF. Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br J Psychol. 1973; 64(1): 1724.Google Scholar
19. Andrade, J, May, J, Deeprose, C, Baugh, S-J, Ganis, G. Assessing vividness of mental imagery: The Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire. Br J Psychol. 2014; 105(4): 547563.Google Scholar
20. Reisberg, D, Pearson, DG, Kosslyn, SM. Intuitions and introspections about imagery: the role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator’s theoretical views. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2003; 17(2): 147160.Google Scholar
21. May, J, Andrade, J, Kavanagh, DJ, et al. The craving experience questionnaire: a brief, theory-based measure of consummatory desire and craving. Addiction. 2014; 109(5): 728735.Google Scholar
22. Kavanagh, DJ, Robinson, N, Connolly, J, Connor, J, Andrade, J, May, J. The revised four-factor motivational thought frequency and state motivation scales for alcohol control. Addict Behav. 2018; 87: 6973.Google Scholar
23. Paivio, A. Comparisons of mental clocks. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1978; 4(1): 6171.Google Scholar
24. O’Craven, KM, Kanwisher, N. Mental imagery of faces and places activates corresponding stiimulus-specific brain regions. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000; 12(6): 10131023.Google Scholar
25. Holmes, EA, Lang, TJ, Moulds, ML, Steele, AM. Prospective and positive mental imagery deficits in dysphoria. Behav Res Ther. 2008; 46(8): 976981.Google Scholar
26. Szpunar, KK, Schacter, DL. Get real: effects of repeated simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future experiences. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2013; 142(2): 323327.Google Scholar
27. Di Simplicio, M, Alfarano, G, Ji, JL, Suri, S, Visser, RM, Holmes, EA. Neural activity during simulation of future negative scenarios in individuals with bipolar disorder phenotype. (In prep).Google Scholar
28. Ji, JL, Holmes, EA, MacLeod, C, Murphy, FC. Spontaneous cognition in dysphoria: reduced positive bias in imagining the future. Psychol Res. 2018Google Scholar
29. Pearson, DG, Deeprose, C, Wallace-Hadrill, SMA, Burnett Heyes, S, Holmes, EA. Assessing mental imagery in clinical psychology: a review of imagery measures and a guiding framework. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013; 33(1): 123.Google Scholar
30. Galton, F. Psychometric experiments. Brain. 1879; 2(2): 149162.Google Scholar
31. Pearson, J, Kosslyn, SM. The heterogeneity of mental representation: ending the imagery debate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015; 112(33): 1008910092.Google Scholar
32. Barsalou, LW. Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008; 59(1): 617645.Google Scholar
33. Pylyshyn, ZW. What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychol Bull. 1973; 80(1): 124.Google Scholar
34. Dijkstra, N, Zeidman, P, Ondobaka, S, van Gerven, MAJ, Friston, K. Distinct top-down and bottom-up brain connectivity during visual perception and imagery. Sci Rep. 2017; 7(1): 5677.Google Scholar
35. Slotnick, SD, Thompson, WL, Kosslyn, SM. Visual mental imagery induces retinotopically organized activation of early visual areas. Cereb Cortex. 2005; 15(10): 15701583.Google Scholar
36. Ehrsson, HH, Geyer, S, Naito, E. Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. J Neurophysiol. 2003; 90(5): 33043316.Google Scholar
37. Hubbard, TL. Auditory imagery: empirical findings. Psychol Bull. 2010; 136(2): 302329.Google Scholar
38. Berger, CC, Ehrsson, HH. Mental imagery induces cross-modal sensory plasticity and changes future auditory perception. Psychol Sci. 2018; 29(6): 926935.Google Scholar
39. Pearson, J, Naselaris, T, Holmes, EA, Kosslyn, SM. Mental imagery: functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015; 19(10): 590602.Google Scholar
40. Ji, JL, Heyes, SB, MacLeod, C, Holmes, EA. Emotional mental imagery as simulation of reality: fear and beyond-a tribute to Peter Lang. Behav Ther. 2016; 47(5): 702719.Google Scholar
41. Stokes, M, Thompson, R, Cusack, R, Duncan, J. Top-down activation of shape-specific population codes in visual cortex during mental imagery. J Neurosci. 2009; 29(5): 1565 LP1572.Google Scholar
42. Mechelli, A, Price, CJ, Friston, KJ, Ishai, A. Where bottom-up meets top-down: neuronal interactions during perception and imagery. Cereb Cortex. 2004; 14(11): 12561265.Google Scholar
43. Schacter, DL, Addis, DR, Buckner, RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007; 8(9): 657661.Google Scholar
44. Baddeley, AD, Andrade, J. Working memory and the vividness of imagery. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2000; 129(1): 126145.Google Scholar
45. Palombo, DJ, Alain, C, Söderlund, H, Khuu, W, Levine, B. Severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) in healthy adults: A new mnemonic syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2015; 72(suppl C): 105118.Google Scholar
46. Zeman, A, Dewar, M, Della Sala, S. Lives without imagery - congenital aphantasia. Cortex. 2015; 73(suppl C): 378380.Google Scholar
47. Keogh, R, Pearson, J. The perceptual and phenomenal capacity of mental imagery. Cognition. 2017; 162:124132.Google Scholar
48. Keogh, R, Pearson, J. The blind mind: no sensory visual imagery in aphantasia. Cortex. 2018; 105:5360.Google Scholar
49. Buckner, RL, Snyder, AZ, Shannon, BJ, et al. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci. 2005; 25(34): 7709 LP7717.Google Scholar
50. Addis, DR, Wong, AT, Schacter, DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2007; 45(7): 13631377.Google Scholar
51. Hassabis, D, Kumaran, D, Vann, SD, Maguire, EA. Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007; 104(5): 17261731.Google Scholar
52. Okuda, J, Fujii, T, Ohtake, H, et al. Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage. 2003; 19(4): 13691380.Google Scholar
53. Szpunar, KK, Watson, JM, McDermott, KB. Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007; 104(2):642647.Google Scholar
54. Addis, DR, Schacter, DL. Constructive episodic simulation: temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement. Hippocampus. 2008; 18(2):227237.Google Scholar
55. Addis, DR, Sacchetti, DC, Ally, BA, Budson, AE, Schacter, DL. Episodic simulation of future events is impaired in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2009; 47(12): 26602671.Google Scholar
56. Irish, M, Halena, S, Kamminga, J, Tu, S, Hornberger, M, Hodges, JR. Scene construction impairments in Alzheimer’s disease – A unique role for the posterior cingulate cortex. Cortex. 2015; 73(suppl C): 1023.Google Scholar
57. Irish, M, Addis, DR, Hodges, JR, Piguet, O. Exploring the content and quality of episodic future simulations in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2012; 50(14): 34883495.Google Scholar
58. Irish, M, Hodges, JR, Piguet, O. Episodic future thinking is impaired in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 2013; 49(9): 23772388.Google Scholar
59. Irish, M, Piguet, O. The pivotal role of semantic memory in remembering the past and imagining the future. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013; 7: 27.Google Scholar
60. Bechara, A, Tranel, D, Damasio, H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000; 123(Pt 11): 21892202.Google Scholar
61. Hsiao, JJ, Kaiser, N, Fong, SS, Mendez, MF. Suicidal behavior and loss of the future self in semantic dementia. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013; 26(2): 8592.Google Scholar
62. Seal, ML, Aleman, A, McGuire, PK. Compelling imagery, unanticipated speech and deceptive memory: neurocognitive models of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2004; 9(1–2): 4372.Google Scholar
63. Aynsworth, C, Nemat, N, Collerton, D, Smailes, D, Dudley, R. Reality monitoring performance and the role of visual imagery in visual hallucinations. Behav Res Ther. 2017; 97: 115122.Google Scholar
64. Winfield, H, Kamboj, SK. Schizotypy and mental time travel. Conscious Cogn. 2010; 19(1): 321327.Google Scholar
65. Holmes, EA, Steel, C. Schizotypy: a vulnerability factor for traumatic intrusions. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004; 192(1): 2834.Google Scholar
66. Steel, C, Fowler, D, Holmes, EA. Trauma-related intrusions and psychosis: an information processing account. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2005; 33(2): 139152.Google Scholar
67. Barrett, TR. Verbal hallucinations in normals—II: self-reported imagery vividness. Pers Individ Dif. 1993; 15(1): 6167.Google Scholar
68. Crespi, B, Leach, E, Dinsdale, N, Mokkonen, M, Hurd, P. Imagination in human social cognition, autism, and psychotic-affective conditions. Cognition. 2016; 150: 181199.Google Scholar
69. Brébion, G, Ohlsen, RI, Bressan, RA, David, AS. Source memory errors in schizophrenia, hallucinations and negative symptoms: a synthesis of research findings. Psychol Med. 2012; 42(12):25432554.Google Scholar
70. Bentall, RP, Baker, GA, Havers, S. Reality monitoring and psychotic hallucinations. Br J Clin Psychol. 1991; 30(Pt 3): 213222.Google Scholar
71. Barnes, J, Boubert, L, Harris, J, Lee, A, David, AS. Reality monitoring and visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2003; 41(5): 565574.Google Scholar
72. Keefe, RSE, Harvey, PD. Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia bt - novel antischizophrenia treatments. In: Geyer MA, Gross G, eds. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2012: 1137.Google Scholar
73. D’Argembeau, A, Raffard, S, Van, der Linden, M. Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008; 117(1): 247251.Google Scholar
74. Steel, C, Wykes, T, Ruddle, A, Smith, G, Shah, DM, Holmes, EA. Can we harness computerised cognitive bias modification to treat anxiety in schizophrenia? A first step highlighting the role of mental imagery. Psychiatry Res. 2010; 178(3): 451455.Google Scholar
75. Sack, AT, van de Ven, VG, Etschenberg, S, Schatz, D, Linden, DEJ. Enhanced vividness of mental imagery as a trait marker of schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull. 2005; 31(1): 97104.Google Scholar
76. Oertel, V, Rotarska-Jagiela, A, van de Ven, V, et al. Mental imagery vividness as a trait marker across the schizophrenia spectrum. Psychiatry Res. 2009; 167(1–2): 111.Google Scholar
77. Morrison, AS, Amir, N, Taylor, CT. A behavioral index of imagery ability in social anxiety. Cognit Ther Res. 2011; 35(4): 326332.Google Scholar
78. Zarrinpar, A, Deldin, P, Kosslyn, SM. Effects of depression on sensory/motor vs. central processing in visual mental imagery. Cogn Emotion. 2006; 20(6): 737758.Google Scholar
79. Di Simplicio, M, Renner, F, Blackwell, SE, et al. An investigation of mental imagery in bipolar disorder: Exploring “the mind’s eye”. Bipolar Disord. 2016; 18(8): 669683.Google Scholar
80. Wu, JQ, Szpunar, KK, Godovich, SA, Schacter, DL, Hoffman, SG. Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disorder. 2016; (617): 18.Google Scholar
81. Vrana, SR, Lang, PJ. Fear imagery and the startle-probe reflex. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990; 99(2): 189197.Google Scholar
82. Witvliet, CVO, Vrana, SR. Psychophysiological responses as indices of affective dimensions. Psychophysiology. 1995; 32(5): 436443.Google Scholar
83. Lang, PJ, Levin, DN, Miller, GA, Kozak, MJ. Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: the problem of affective response integration. J Abnorm Psychol. 1983; 92(3): 276306.Google Scholar
84. Weerts, TC, Lang, PJ. Psychophysiology of fear imagery: differences between focal phobia and social performance anxiety. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1978; 46(5): 11571159.Google Scholar
85. Cuthbert, BN, Lang, PJ, Strauss, C, Drobes, D, Patrick, CJ, Bradley, MM. The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology. 2003; 40(3): 407422.Google Scholar
86. Sabatinelli, D, Bradley, MM, Lang, PJ, Costa, VD, Versace, F. Pleasure rather than salience activates human nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2007; 98(3): 13741379.Google Scholar
87. Costa, VD, Lang, PJ, Sabatinelli, D, Versace, F, Bradley, MM. Emotional imagery: assessing pleasure and arousal in the brain’s reward circuitry. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010; 31(9): 14461457.Google Scholar
88. Kim, S-E, Kim, J-W, Kim, J-J, et al. The neural mechanism of imagining facial affective expression. Brain Res. 2007; 1145(suppl C): 128137.Google Scholar
89. Lee, S, Ruiz, S, Caria, A, Veit, R, Birbaumer, N, Sitaram, R. Detection of cerebral reorganization induced by real-time fMRI feedback training of insula activation: a multivariate investigation. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2011; 25(3): 259267.Google Scholar
90. Andrade, J, Kavanagh, D, Baddeley, A. Eye-movements and visual imagery: a working memory approach to the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Br J Clin Psychol. 1997; 36(Pt 2):209223.Google Scholar
91. Kavanagh, DJ, Freese, S, Andrade, J, May, J. Effects of visuospatial tasks on desensitization to emotive memories. Br J Clin Psychol. 2001; 40(Pt 3): 267280.Google Scholar
92. Engelhard, IM, van den Hout, MA, Janssen, WC, van der Beek, J. Eye movements reduce vividness and emotionality of “flashforwards”. Behav Res Ther. 2010; 48(5): 442447.Google Scholar
93. Engelhard, IM, van den Hout, MA, Dek, ECP, et al. Reducing vividness and emotional intensity of recurrent “flashforwards” by taxing working memory: an analogue study. J Anxiety Disord. 2011; 25(4): 599603.Google Scholar
94. Lilley, SA, Andrade, J, Turpin, G, Sabin-Farrell, R, Holmes, EA. Visuospatial working memory interference with recollections of trauma. Br J Clin Psychol. 2009; 48(Pt 3): 309321.Google Scholar
95. Lau-Zhu, A, Holmes, EA, Butterfield, S, Holmes, J. Selective association between tetris game play and visuospatial working memory: a preliminary investigation. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2017; 31(4): 438445.Google Scholar
96. James, EL, Lau-Zhu, A, Clark, IA, Visser, RM, Hagenaars, MA, Holmes, EA. The trauma film paradigm as an experimental psychopathology model of psychological trauma: intrusive memories and beyond. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016; 47: 106142.Google Scholar
97. Holmes, EA, James, EL, Coode-Bate, T, Deeprose, C. Can playing the computer game “Tetris” reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PLoS One. 2009; 4(1): e4153.Google Scholar
98. Iyadurai, L, Blackwell, SE, Meiser-Stedman, R, et al. Preventing intrusive memories after trauma via a brief intervention involving Tetris computer game play in the emergency department: a proof-of-concept randomized controlled trial. Mol Psychiatry. 2017; (March).Google Scholar
99. Horsch, A, Vial, Y, Favrod, C, et al. Reducing intrusive traumatic memories after emergency caesarean section: A proof-of-principle randomized controlled study. Behav Res Ther. 2017; 94: 3647.Google Scholar
100. Holmes, EA, Lang, TJ, Shah, DM. Developing interpretation bias modification as a “cognitive vaccine” for depressed mood: imagining positive events makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009; 118(1): 7688.Google Scholar
101. Holmes, EA, Mathews, A, Mackintosh, B, Dalgleish, T. The causal effect of mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues. Emotion. 2008; 8(3): 395409.Google Scholar
102. Nelis, S, Holmes, EA, Palmieri, R, Bellelli, G, Raes, F. Thinking back about a positive event: the impact of processing style on positive affect. Front Psychiatry. 2015; 6: 3.Google Scholar
103. Holmes, EA, Coughtrey, AE, Connor, A. Looking at or through rose-tinted glasses? Imagery perspective and positive mood. Emotion. 2008; 8(6): 875879.Google Scholar
104. Holmes, EA, Geddes, JR, Colom, F, Goodwin, GM. Mental imagery as an emotional amplifier: application to bipolar disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2008; 46(12): 12511258.Google Scholar
105. Carver, CS. Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. Motiv Emot. 2006; 30(2): 105110.Google Scholar
106. Lang, PJ, Bradley, MM. Emotion and the motivational brain. Biol Psychol. 2010; 84(3): 437450.Google Scholar
107. Kavanagh, DJ, Andrade, J, May, J. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: the elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychol Rev. 2005; 112(2): 446467.Google Scholar
108. Tiggemann, M, Kemps, E. The phenomenology of food cravings: the role of mental imagery. Appetite. 2005; 45(3): 305313.Google Scholar
109. Harvey, K, Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M. The nature of imagery processes underlying food cravings. Br J Health Psychol. 2005; 10(Pt 1): 4956.Google Scholar
110. May, J, Andrade, J, Kavanagh, DJ, Penfound, L. Imagery and strength of craving for eating, drinking, and playing sport. Cogn Emotion. 2008; 22(4): 633650.Google Scholar
111. Maude-Griffin, P, Tiffany, ST. Production of smoking urges through imagery: the impact of affect and smoking abstinence. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1996; 4(2): 198208.Google Scholar
112. Tiffany, ST, Drobes, DJ. Imagery and smoking urges: the manipulation of affective content. Addict Behav. 1990; 15(6):531539.Google Scholar
113. Drobes, DJ, Tiffany, ST. Induction of smoking urge through imaginal and in vivo procedures: physiological and self-report manifestations. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997; 106(1): 1525.Google Scholar
114. Heishman, SJ, Lee, DC, Taylor, RC, Singleton, EG. Prolonged duration of craving, mood, and autonomic responses elicited by cues and imagery in smokers: effects of tobacco deprivation and sex. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010; 18(3): 245256.Google Scholar
115. Vanhaudenhuyse, A, Bruno, M-A, Brédart, S, Plenevaux, A, Laureys, S. The challenge of disentangling reportability and phenomenal consciousness in post-comatose states. Behav Brain Sci. 2007; 30(5–6): 529530.Google Scholar
116. Morewedge, CK, Huh, YE, Vosgerau, J. Thought for food: imagined consumption reduces actual consumption. Science. 2010; 330(6010): 15301533.Google Scholar
117. Kilts, CD, Schweitzer, JB, Quinn, CK, et al. Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001; 58(4): 334341.Google Scholar
118. Tiggemann, M, Kemps, E, Parnell, J. The selective impact of chocolate craving on visuospatial working memory. Appetite. 2010; 55(1): 4448.Google Scholar
119. Littel, M, van den Hout, MA, Engelhard, IM. Desensitizing addiction: using eye movements to reduce the intensity of substance-related mental imagery and craving. Front Psychiatry. 2016; 7:14.Google Scholar
120. Schumacher, S, Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M. Acceptance- and imagery-based strategies can reduce chocolate cravings: A test of the elaborated-intrusion theory of desire. Appetite. 2017; 113: 6370.Google Scholar
121. Skorka-Brown, J, Andrade, J, Whalley, B, May, J. Playing Tetris decreases drug and other cravings in real world settings. Addict Behav. 2015; 51: 165170.Google Scholar
122. Andrade, J, Pears, S, May, J, Kavanagh, DJ. Use of a clay modeling task to reduce chocolate craving. Appetite. 2012; 58(3): 955963.Google Scholar
123. Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M. Hand-held dynamic visual noise reduces naturally occurring food cravings and craving-related consumption. Appetite. 2013; 68: 152157.Google Scholar
124. May, J, Andrade, J, Panabokke, N, Kavanagh, D. Visuospatial tasks suppress craving for cigarettes. Behav Res Ther. 2010; 48(6):476485.Google Scholar
125. Hamilton, J, Fawson, S, May, J, Andrade, J, Kavanagh, DJ. Brief guided imagery and body scanning interventions reduce food cravings. Appetite. 2013; 71: 158162.Google Scholar
126. May, J, Andrade, J, Batey, H, Berry, L-M, Kavanagh, DJ. Less food for thought. Impact of attentional instructions on intrusive thoughts about snack foods. Appetite . 2010; 55(2): 279287.Google Scholar
127. Firmin, MW, Gillette, AL, Hobbs, TE, Wu, D. Effects of olfactory sense on chocolate craving. Appetite. 2016; 105: 700704.Google Scholar
128. Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M. Competing visual and olfactory imagery tasks suppress craving for coffee. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009; 17(1): 4350.Google Scholar
129. Versland, A, Rosenberg, H. Effect of brief imagery interventions on craving in college student smokers. Addict Res Theory. 2007; 15(2): 177187.Google Scholar
130. Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M. Modality-specific imagery reduces cravings for food: an application of the elaborated intrusion theory of desire to food craving. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2007; 13(2): 95104.Google Scholar
131. Morina, N, Deeprose, C, Pusowski, C, Schmid, M, Holmes, EA. Prospective mental imagery in patients with major depressive disorder or anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2011; 25(8):10321037.Google Scholar
132. Pile, V, Lau, JYF. Looking forward to the future: impoverished vividness for positive prospective events characterises low mood in adolescence. J Affect Disord. 2018; 238(April): 269276.Google Scholar
133. Hirsch, CR, Holmes, EA. Mental imagery in anxiety disorders. Psychiatry. 2007; 6(4): 161165.Google Scholar
134. Association, AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.Google Scholar
135. Ehlers, A, Hackmann, A, Michael, T. Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory. 2004; 12(4): 403415.Google Scholar
136. Grey, N, Holmes, EA. “Hotspots” in trauma memories in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder: a replication. Memory. 2008; 16(7): 788796.Google Scholar
137. Coughtrey, AE, Shafran, R, Lee, M, Rachman, S. The treatment of mental contamination: a case series. Cognit Behav Pract. 2013; 20(2): 221231.Google Scholar
138. Hirsch, CR, Clark, DM, Mathews, A, Williams, R. Self-images play a causal role in social phobia. Behav Res Ther. 2003; 41(8): 909921.Google Scholar
139. Borkovec, TD, Alcaine, O, Behar, E. Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In: Heimberg RG, Turk CL, Mennin DS, eds. Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Advances in Research and Practice. New York: The Guildford Press; 2004.Google Scholar
140. Stöber, J. Worry, problem elaboration and suppression of imagery: the role of concreteness. Behav Res Ther. 1998; 36(7–8): 751756.Google Scholar
141. Behar, E, DiMarco, ID, Hekler, EB, Mohlman, J, Staples, AM. Current theoretical models of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD): conceptual review and treatment implications. J Anxiety Disord. 2009; 23(8): 10111023.Google Scholar
142. Dadds, MR, Hawes, D, Schaefer, B, Vaka, K. Individual differences in imagery and reports of aversions. Memory. 2004; 12(4): 462466.Google Scholar
143. Dadds, MR, Bovbjerg, DH, Redd, WH, Cutmore, TRH. Imagery in human classical conditioning. Psychol Bull. 1997; 122(1): 89103.Google Scholar
144. Holmes, EA, Blackwell, SE, Burnett Heyes, S, Renner, F, Raes, F. Mental imagery in depression: phenomenology, potential mechanisms, and treatment implications. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2016; 12(1): 249280.Google Scholar
145. Weßlau, C, Cloos, M, Höfling, V, Steil, R. Visual mental imagery and symptoms of depression - results from a large-scale web-based study. BMC Psychiatry. 2015; 15(1): 308.Google Scholar
146. MacLeod, AK, Conway, C. Well-being and positive future thinking for the self versus others. Cogn Emotion. 2007; 21(5): 11141124.Google Scholar
147. MacLeod, AK, Salaminiou, E. Reduced positive future-thinking in depression: cognitive and affective factors. Cogn Emotion. 2001; 15(1): 99107.Google Scholar
148. MacLeod, AK, Tata, P, Tyrer, P, Schmidt, U, Davidson, K, Thompson, S. Hopelessness and positive and negative future thinking in parasuicide. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005; 44(Pt 4): 495504.Google Scholar
149. Szőllősi, Á, Pajkossy, P, Racsmány, M. Depressive symptoms are associated with the phenomenal characteristics of imagined positive and negative future events. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2015; 29(5): 762767.Google Scholar
150. Murphy, F, Veers, P V, Blackwell, SE, Holmes, EA, Manly, T. Anticipated and imagined futures: prospective cognition and depressed mood following brain injury. 2017Google Scholar
151. Patel, T, Brewin, CR, Wheatley, J, Wells, A, Fisher, P, Myers, S. Intrusive images and memories in major depression. Behav Res Ther. 2007; 45(11): 25732580.Google Scholar
152. Renner, F, Ji, JL, Pictet, A, Holmes, EA, Blackwell, SE. Effects of engaging in repeated mental imagery of future positive events on behavioural activation in individuals with major depressive disorder. Cognit Ther Res. 2017; 41(3): 369380.Google Scholar
153. Hales, SA, Deeprose, C, Goodwin, GM, Holmes, EA. Cognitions in bipolar affective disorder and unipolar depression: imagining suicide. Bipolar Disord. 2011; 13(7–8): 651661.Google Scholar
154. Ivins, A, Di Simplicio, M, Close, H, Goodwin, GM, Holmes, E. Mental imagery in bipolar affective disorder versus unipolar depression: investigating cognitions at times of ‘positive’ mood. J Affect Disord. 2014; 166: 234242.Google Scholar
155. Gregory, JD, Brewin, CR, Mansell, W, Donaldson, C. Intrusive memories and images in bipolar disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2010; 48(7): 698703.Google Scholar
156. Meyer, TD, Finucane, L, Jordan, G. Is risk for mania associated with increased daydreaming as a form of mental imagery? J Affect Disord. 2011; 135(1–3): 380383.Google Scholar
157. O’Donnell, C, Di Simplicio, M, Brown, R, Holmes, EA, Burnett Heyes, S. The role of mental imagery in mood amplification: an investigation across subclinical features of bipolar disorders. Cortex. 2017; 105: 114.Google Scholar
158. Hasking, PA, Di Simplicio, M, McEvoy, PM, Rees, CS. Emotional cascade theory and non-suicidal self-injury: the importance of imagery and positive affect. Cogn Emot. August 2017: 112.Google Scholar
159. McEvoy, PM, Hayes, S, Hasking, PA, Rees, CS. Thoughts, images, and appraisals associated with acting and not acting on the urge to self-injure. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2017; 57(suppl C):163171.Google Scholar
160. Holmes, EA, Crane, C, Fennell, MJV, Williams, JMG. Imagery about suicide in depression—“Flash-forwards”? J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2007; 38(4): 423434.Google Scholar
161. Crane, C, Shah, D, Barnhofer, T, Holmes, EA. Suicidal imagery in a previously depressed community sample. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2012; 19(1): 5769.Google Scholar
162. Van Orden, KA, Witte, TK, Cukrowicz, KC, Braithwaite, SR, Selby, EA, Joiner, TE Jr. The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychol Rev. 2010; 117(2): 575600.Google Scholar
163. Selby, EA, Anestis, MD, Joiner, TE, Selby, EA, Anestis, MD, Joiner, TE. Behavior modification emotion dysregulation in suicidality. 2007: 867–879.Google Scholar
164. Holaday, TC, Brausch, AM. Suicidal imagery, history of suicidality, and acquired capability in young adults. J Aggress Conflict Peace Res. 2015; 7(3): 127138.Google Scholar
165. Ng, RMK, Di Simplicio, M, McManus, F, Kennerley, H, Holmes, EA. ‘Flash-forwards’ and suicidal ideation: A prospective investigation of mental imagery, entrapment and defeat in a cohort from the Hong Kong Mental Morbidity Survey. Psychiatry Res. 2016; 246(suppl C): 453460.Google Scholar
166. Wetherall, K, Cleare, S, Eschle, S, et al. From ideation to action: differentiating between those who think about suicide and those who attempt suicide in a national study of young adults. J Affect Disord. 2018; 241(June): 475483.Google Scholar
167. Kavanagh, DJ, May, J, Andrade, J. Tests of the elaborated intrusion theory of craving and desire: features of alcohol craving during treatment for an alcohol disorder. Br J Clin Psychol. 2009; 48(Pt 3): 241254.Google Scholar
168. Fox, HC, Bergquist, KL, Hong, K-I, Sinha, R. Stress-induced and alcohol cue-induced craving in recently abstinent alcohol-dependent individuals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007; 31(3): 395403.Google Scholar
169. Tiffany, ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev. 1990; 97(2): 147168.Google Scholar
170. Connor, JP, Kavanagh, DJ, Andrade, J, et al. Alcohol consumption in young adults: the role of multisensory imagery. Addict Behav. 2014; 39(3): 721724.Google Scholar
171. Coates, JM, Gullo, MJ, Feeney, GFX, et al. The mini alcohol craving experience questionnaire: development and clinical application. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2017; 41(1): 156164.Google Scholar
172. Clark, IA, Holmes, EA, Woolrich, MW, Mackay, CE. Intrusive memories to traumatic footage: the neural basis of their encoding and involuntary recall. Psychol Med. 2016; 46(3):505518.Google Scholar
173. Holmes, EA, Arntz, A, Smucker, MR. Imagery rescripting in cognitive behaviour therapy: images, treatment techniques and outcomes. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2007; 38(4):297305.Google Scholar
174. Blackwell, SE, Browning, M, Mathews, A, et al. Positive imagery-based cognitive bias modification as a web-based treatment tool for depressed adults: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015; 3(1): 91111.Google Scholar
175. Wilson, AC, Schwannauer, M, McLaughlin, A, Ashworth, F, Chan, SWY. Vividness of positive mental imagery predicts positive emotional response to visually presented Project Soothe pictures. Br J Psychol. 2017; 109(2): 259 Google Scholar
176. Naismith, I, Mwale, A, Feigenbaum, J. Inhibitors and facilitators of compassion-focused imagery in personality disorder. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2018; 25(2): 283291.Google Scholar
177. Solbrig, L, Whalley, B, Kavanagh, DJ, et al. Functional imagery training versus motivational interviewing for weight loss: a randomised controlled trial of brief individual interventions for overweight and obesity. Int J Obes. 2018.Google Scholar