Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T20:30:01.895Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Polygenic Risk Scores for Psychiatric Disorders Reveal Novel Clues About the Genetics of Disordered Gambling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2019

Thomas M. Piasecki*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
Ian R. Gizer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
Wendy S. Slutske
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Thomas M. Piasecki, Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Disordered gambling (DG) is a rare but serious condition that results in considerable financial and interpersonal harms. Twin studies indicate that DG is heritable but are silent with respect to specific genes or pathways involved. Existing genomewide association studies (GWAS) of DG have been substantially underpowered. Larger GWAS of other psychiatric disorders now permit calculation of polygenic risk scores (PRSs) that reflect the aggregated effects of common genetic variants contributing risk for the target condition. The current study investigated whether gambling and DG are associated with PRSs for four psychiatric conditions found to be comorbid with DG in epidemiologic surveys: major depressive disorder (MDD), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), bipolar disorder (BD) and schizophrenia (SCZ). Genotype data and survey responses were analyzed from the Wave IV assessment (conducted in 2008) of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health, a representative sample of adolescents recruited in 1994–1995 and followed into adulthood. Among participants classified as having European ancestry based on genetic analysis (N = 5215), 78.4% reported ever having gambled, and 1.3% reported lifetime DG. Polygenic risk for BD was associated with decreased odds of lifetime gambling, OR = 0.93 [0.87, 0.99], p = .045, pseudo-R2(%) = .12. The SCZ PRS was associated with increased odds of DG, OR = 1.54 [1.07, 2.21], p = .02, pseudo-R2(%) = .85. Polygenic risk scores for MDD and ADHD were not related to either gambling outcome. Investigating features common to both SCZ and DG might generate valuable clues about the genetically influenced liabilities to DG.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019 

For many, gambling is a form of entertainment, but a subset of individuals progresses to develop a pattern of disordered gambling (DG) characterized by difficulty controlling gambling behavior, chasing losses, and serious financial and interpersonal hardships (Hodgins et al., Reference Hodgins, Stea and Grant2011; Li et al., Reference Li, Browne, Rawat, Langham and Rockloff2017). Surveys of nationally representative US samples estimate that approximately 2.3% of adults experience at least one lifetime symptom of gambling disorder (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008) and that the lifetime prevalence of diagnosable DG among adults is between 0.4% to 0.6% (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008; Petry et al., Reference Petry, Stinson and Grant2005).

Twin studies have established that DG is heritable, with additive genetic influences accounting for between 40% and 80% of the phenotypic variance in samples of adults and 5% and 20% of the variance in adolescents and young adults (Slutske, Reference Slutske, Heinz, Romanczuk-Seiferth and Potenza2019). Classical twin studies provide an estimate of the aggregated effect of genes, but are silent with respect to the specific genes or biological pathways that may be involved in the disorder.

Starting from theory and empirical clues about possible neural mechanisms implicated in dysregulated gambling and related traits, researchers have tested associations between DG and a variety of functional candidate genes (Nautiyal et al., Reference Nautiyal, Okuda, Hen and Blanco2017; Slutske, Reference Slutske, Heinz, Romanczuk-Seiferth and Potenza2019). These efforts have not yet identified strong and consistent associations between selected variants and DG. Accumulating experience with large-scale, hypothesis-free genomewide association studies (GWAS) casts doubt on the utility of the candidate gene approach. The top GWAS hits for psychiatric traits frequently arise in unexpected genomic regions and implicate biological pathways unanticipated by prevailing etiologic theory, suggesting our intuitions about candidate genes may not be adequate for steering inquiry into the genetic determinants of disorders (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Agrawal, Bulik, Andreassen, Børglum and Breen2018).

To date, there have been 2 GWAS of DG, including a total of only 2742 participants, with no genomewide significant single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) or genes detected in either study (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Leménager, Streit, Fauth-Bühler, Frank, Juraeva and Kiefer2016; Lind et al., Reference Lind, Zhu, Montgomery, Madden, Heath, Martin and Slutske2012). Available evidence from GWAS involving other psychiatric conditions indicate that these disorders most likely arise from the combined influence of a large number of common variants, each of which has a small effect (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Agrawal, Bulik, Andreassen, Børglum and Breen2018). Practically, this requires very large sample sizes to identify associated variants at a genomewide level of statistical significance. GWAS may yet make important contributions to elucidating the genetic underpinnings of DG, but this will require concerted efforts to amass much larger samples.

Epidemiologic studies indicate that DG is comorbid with numerous other psychiatric conditions (Edens & Rosenheck, Reference Edens and Rosenheck2012; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008; Petry et al., Reference Petry, Stinson and Grant2005). Investigating the extent to which these comorbidity patterns are attributable to overlapping genetic influences could provide valuable clues concerning the biological bases of DG. Multivariate twin studies have produced evidence for genetic correlations between DG and substance use disorders (Slutske et al., Reference Slutske, Eisen, True, Lyons, Goldberg and Tsuang2000, Reference Slutske, Ellingson, Richmond-Rakerd, Zhu and Martin2013; Xian et al., Reference Xian, Giddens, Scherrer, Eisen and Potenza2014), antisocial behaviors (Slutske et al., Reference Slutske, Eisen, Xian, True, Lyons, Goldberg and Tsuang2001) and obsessive-compulsive disorder features (Scherrer et al., Reference Scherrer, Xian, Slutske, Eisen and Potenza2015). Existing studies of DG and major depression have yielded mixed findings (Blanco et al., Reference Blanco, Myers and Kendler2012; Potenza et al., Reference Potenza, Xian, Shah, Scherrer and Eisen2005). Fitting joint twin models is tractable when studying overlap between DG and common mental disorders. However, because DG is itself fairly rare, sparse data often prevent meaningful twin analyses of genetic overlap between DG and less prevalent conditions. This may effectively limit what can be learned about genetic causes of comorbidity from biometric models using twin samples.

Although gambling research lags behind, a number of large-scale collaborative efforts have now been organized to conduct powerful GWAS investigations of several psychiatric disorders (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Agrawal, Bulik, Andreassen, Børglum and Breen2018). This has generated new tools — polygenic risk scores (PRSs) — that can be leveraged to investigate the genetic bases of psychiatric disorders (Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018; Dudbridge, Reference Dudbridge2016; Maier et al., Reference Maier, Visscher, Robinson and Wray2018). A PRS is constructed by applying regression weights derived from a target GWAS to genotype data collected from an independent sample, yielding a single summary score for each individual estimating their level of risk for the target disorder based on the aggregated effects of their common genetic variants. PRSs provide a method for examining how the phenotypic expression of an understudied disorder, such as DG, relates to common variant risk for other psychiatric disorders, including comparatively rare conditions that have been examined in large case-control GWASs.

The current study examines associations between DG and PRSs for four psychiatric conditions — attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), major depressive disorder (MDD), bipolar disorder (BD) and schizophrenia (SCZ) — in a nationally representative sample of young adults from the USA. Each of these disorders overlaps with DG at the phenotypic level in epidemiological samples (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Nower and Walker2013; Edens & Rosenheck, Reference Edens and Rosenheck2012; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008; McIntyre et al., Reference McIntyre, McElroy, Konarski, Soczynska, Wilkins and Kennedy2007; Park et al., Reference Park, Cho, Chang, Jeon, Cho, Kim and Hong2011, Reference Petry, Stinson and Grant2005).

Genetic risks for MDD and ADHD are of interest because prominent theoretical accounts posit that mood disturbance and impulsivity are important risk factors (Blaszczynski & Nower, Reference Blaszczynski and Nower2002; Sharpe, Reference Sharpe2002). BD is the condition that shows the strongest association with DG in epidemiologic analyses of comorbidity (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008; Petry et al., Reference Petry, Stinson and Grant2005). SCZ has substantial genetic overlap with BD (Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Yip, Björk, Pawitan, Cannon, Sullivan and Hultman2009; Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O’Donovan and Sullivan2009). Polygenic risk scores for BD and SCZ are each related to a variety of substance use phenotypes (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Agrawal, Bucholz, Hartz, Lynskey, Nelson and Bogdan2016; Hartz et al., Reference Hartz, Horton, Oehlert, Carey, Agrawal, Bogdan and Bierut2017; Reginsson et al., Reference Reginsson, Ingason, Eusden, Bjornsdottir, Olafsson, Sigurdsson and Stefansson2017), suggesting they may influence multiple forms of addictive behaviors — possibly including DG.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were drawn from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; Harris et al., Reference Harris, Halpern, Whitsel, Hussey, Tabor, Entzel and Udry2009). The Add Health cohort is a nationally representative US sample that has been followed into young adulthood with four in-home interviews. Variables used were obtained in Wave IV (N = 15,701; response rate = 80%), which was conducted in 2008 when the participants were 24–34 years of age.

Procedure

Saliva samples were collected from consenting participants (96%) during the Wave IV assessment. Consent for long-term archiving was obtained from approximately 12,200 (80%) of those participants, making them eligible for genomewide genotyping. Approximately 80% of the sample genotyping was performed with the Illumina Omni1-Quad BeadChip and 20% was performed using the Illumina Omni2.5-Quad BeadChip. After quality control procedures, genotyped data were available for 9974 individuals (7917 from the Omni1 chip and 2057 from the Omni2 chip) on 609,130 SNPs common across the 2 platforms (Braudt & Harris, Reference Braudt and Harris2018; Highland et al., Reference Highland, Avery, Duan, Li and Harris2018).

To account for population stratification, the genotyped sample was limited to the 9129 individuals who could be assigned to one of four genetic ancestry groups based upon principal components analysis: European, African, Hispanic and East Asian. Genetic ancestry was strongly correlated (r = .89) with self-identified race/ethnicity. The self-identified race/ethnicity of the 9129 individuals was 5754 (63%) non-Hispanic White, 1940 (21%) non-Hispanic Black, 961 (11%) Hispanic, 449 (5%) Asian and 23 (<1%) Native American (Braudt & Harris, Reference Braudt and Harris2018).

Measures

Gambling and disordered gambling

The gambling assessment from the Add Health Wave IV interview included two questions: ‘Have you ever bought lottery tickets, played video games or slot machines for money, bet on horses or sporting events, or taken part in any other kinds of gambling for money?’ and (if yes to the previous question) ‘Has your gambling ever caused serious financial problems or problems in your relationships with any of your family members or friends?’ A dichotomous gambling phenotype was based on endorsing the first question, and a dichotomous disordered gambling phenotype was based on endorsing the second.

Polygenic risk scores

Details regarding the construction of PRSs can be found in an Add Health technical report (Braudt & Harris, Reference Braudt and Harris2018). Briefly, for a given PRS, each SNP was weighted by the regression coefficient for the corresponding SNP estimated in the discovery GWAS. These weighted effects were summed across all available variants (i.e., p-value threshold = 1.0) to yield a single quantitative estimate of risk for the target condition attributable to common variants for each participant in the independent target sample. These calculations were performed using the PRSice wrapper for R within the PLINK package (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Chow, Tellier, Vattikuti, Purcell and Lee2015). The PRSs were then standardized within each ancestry group, yielding within-group PRSs with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1.

The MDD PRS was based upon summary statistics from a genomewide association meta-analysis involving 135,458 cases and 344,901 controls (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Ripke, Mattheisen, Trzaskowski, Byrne and Abdellaoui2018). Owing to data-sharing restrictions, the PRSs for MDD were based on summary statistics that excluded 75,607 cases and 231,747 controls from the 23andMe cohort. The ADHD score was calculated using statistics from a study of 20,183 cases and 35,191 controls (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo and Cerrato2019). The BD PRS was based upon results from a study of 7481 cases and 9250 controls (Sklar et al., Reference Sklar, Ripke, Scott, Andreassen, Cichon, Craddock and Corvin2011). The SCZ score used summary statistics from a GWAS investigation of 36,989 cases and 113,075 controls (Ripke et al., Reference Ripke, Neale, Corvin, Walters, Farh, Holmans and Pers2014). No Add Health data were included in any of these discovery GWASs.

Data Analysis

Data from close relatives were eliminated by retaining data from only a single member of each family. Because the discovery samples used to derive the PRSs were predominantly of European ancestry, analyses were restricted to individuals classified as having European ancestry according to the principal components analyses of genotyped SNPs. Together, these constraints yielded an analytic sample of 5215 individuals.

Logistic regressions were conducted predicting gambling and DG. Analyses of DG were limited to individuals who endorsed lifetime gambling. Covariates were age, sex and the first 10 ancestry principal components. For each model, the amount of variance in the phenotype accounted for by each PRS was estimated by computing the difference in Nagelkerke’s pseudo-R 2 from a model that included both the covariate set and a particular PRS relative to a baseline model that included the covariates alone. This difference was multiplied by 100 to express it as a percentage of variance.

Results

Lifetime experience with gambling was reported by more than three-quarters of the analytic sample (78.4%, n = 4091). Table 1 summarizes results from four logistic regression analyses using individual PRSs to predict gambling endorsement. Surprisingly, higher polygenic risk for BD was associated with a significant reduction in the odds of gambling, OR = 0.93, 95% CI [0.87, 0.99], p = .045, accounting for 0.12% of the variance. No other PRS was associated with gambling behavior.

Table 1. Results from logistic regression models predicting gambling and disordered gambling

DG was comparatively rare (1.3%, n = 70). Table 1 also summarizes results from models predicting endorsement of DG. Polygenic risk for SCZ was associated with significantly increased risk of DG, OR = 1.54, 95% CI [1.07, 2.21], p = .020, accounting for 0.85% of variance. Figure 1 illustrates this effect by plotting the OR for DG as a function of SCZ PRS deciles, with the lowest decile as the reference category. This plot reveals that the effect was driven primarily by an increased risk of DG among those with the highest level of genetic risk for SCZ. Although not statistically significant, there was suggestive evidence of a possible association between BD risk and DG, OR = 1.24, 95% CI [0.97, 1.58], p = .082, accounting for 0.47% of the variance. MDD and ADHD were not associated with DG.

Fig. 1. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for disordered gambling by deciles of schizophrenia polygenic risk score, with the lowest decile serving as the reference category.

In light of the similar effects for SCZ and BPD and the known genetic overlap between these conditions, we conducted an additional analysis predicting DG including both polygenic scores as predictors. The PRSs for SCZ and BPD were significantly correlated in the current sample, r = .28, p < .001. In the simultaneous logistic regression, the pair of PRSs accounted for 0.98% of the variance. Neither PRS was nominally significant, but SCZ was associated with a comparatively stronger effect. The SCZ PRS (OR = 1.44, 95% CI [0.97, 2.12], p = .07) accounted for an additional .51% of variance relative to the model including BD alone. The BD score (OR = 1.13, 95% CI [0.87, 1.48], p = .37) accounted for an additional 0.13% of variance in DG relative to a model including SCZ alone.

Discussion

The primary finding was that polygenic risk for SCZ was associated with DG in a nationally representative sample of young adults. This effect was small, accounting for <1% of the variance in DG. However, this is comparable to the average effect size for the SCZ PRS in predicting other psychiatric disorders and traits and is actually stronger than its prediction of some SCZ-related features and outcomes (Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018). This cross-disorder association suggests that common genetic factors have pleiotropic effects on DG and SCZ. Theoretically, this might indicate that one disorder is an intermediate phenotype that provides a crucial link in the causal chain, setting conditions that may facilitate the acquisition of the second disorder. Under this scenario, SCZ could facilitate dysregulated gambling, or DG might increase risk for onset of SCZ. Although one could generate hypotheses about how such effects could occur, it seems unlikely that casual transactions involving the fully diagnosable clinical conditions explain the overlap. Instead, it seems more plausible that subtle, subclinical manifestations of genetic risk for SCZ increase risk for DG.

The current findings suggest that investigating features common to both SCZ and DG might generate valuable clues about the genetically influenced liabilities to DG. Below, we speculate about some research domains that may merit closer scrutiny based on available evidence. Overlapping features that have been empirically associated with the SCZ PRS may represent especially interesting targets in light of the present findings.

Reduced cortical thickness has been observed in individuals with DG relative to healthy controls, a finding thought to be consistent with a diminished top-down control of impulsive behavior (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Odlaug and Chamberlain2015). Reduced cortical thickness is also characteristic of individuals with BD and SCZ (Knöchel et al., Reference Knöchel, Reuter, Reinke, Stäblein, Marbach, Feddern and Oertel-Knöchel2016; Rimol et al., Reference Rimol, Hartberg, Nesvåg, Fennema-Notestine, Halger, Pung and Agartz2010), and the extent of this cortical thinning is correlated with the SCZ PRS (Neilson et al., Reference Neilson, Bois, Gibson, Duff, Watson, Roberts and Lawrie2017). Interestingly, investigation of the genomic region most strongly associated with SCZ in the discovery GWAS (hence, producing the most heavily weighted effects in the SCZ PRS) implicates variations in the immune-related complement system that might result in excessive synaptic pruning and cortical thinning during adolescence and early adulthood (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and Genovese2016). This pattern of evidence suggests that additional work examining a potential role of cortical thinning in DG may be warranted.

Aberrant dopamine functioning has long been hypothesized to play a major role in the pathophysiology of SCZ (Howes et al., Reference Howes, McCutcheon, Owen and Murray2016). Several lines of evidence also point to the involvement of dopaminergic mechanisms in DG (Nautiyal et al., Reference Nautiyal, Okuda, Hen and Blanco2017; Zack & Poulos, Reference Zack and Poulos2009). Compared to healthy controls, persons with DG show enhanced striatal dopamine release in response to an amphetamine challenge, and the magnitude of this effect is correlated with the severity of gambling problems (Boileau et al., Reference Boileau, Payer, Chugani, Lobo, Houle, Wilson and Zack2014). Pharmacologic manipulations of dopamine modulate motivation to gamble and the rewarding effects of gambling in problem gamblers (Zack & Poulos, Reference Zack and Poulos2004, Reference Zack and Poulos2007), and treatment with dopamine agonists is associated with an increased risk of DG onset in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Dagher & Robbins, Reference Dagher and Robbins2009; Dodd et al., Reference Dodd, Klos, Bower, Geda, Josephs and Ahlskog2005). Some studies have found associations between dopamine-related candidate genes and gambling behaviors (e.g., Comings et al., Reference Comings, Rosenthal, Lesieur, Rugle, Muhleman, Chiu and Gade1996; Gray & MacKillop, Reference Gray and MacKillop2014; Lobo et al., Reference Lobo, Aleksandrova, Knight, Casey, El-Guebaly, Nobrega and Kennedy2015). A SNP associated with the DRD2 gene was among the 108 genomewide significant loci in the SCZ GWAS (Ripke et al., Reference Ripke, Neale, Corvin, Walters, Farh, Holmans and Pers2014). However, there is limited evidence that the SCZ PRS is related to SCZ features thought to be dopamine-related, such as positive symptoms or antispsychotic dosage (Hettige et al., Reference Hettige, Cole, Khalid and De Luca2016; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron, Cannon and Zammit2016; Sengupta et al., Reference Sengupta, MacDonald, Fathalli, Yim, Lepage, Iyer and Joober2017; Stepniak et al., Reference Stepniak, Papiol, Hammer, Ramin, Everts, Hennig and Ehrenreich2014). Furthermore, common genetic variants implicated in dopamine functioning do not collectively discriminate SCZ cases from controls (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Bacanu, Bigdelli, Mosczti and Kendler2016). Thus, the polygenic liability shared by DG and SCZ might not prove to be strongly related to dopaminergic mechanisms.

Relative to healthy controls, individuals with DG (Kovács et al., Reference Kovács, Richman, Janka, Maraz and Andó2017) and SCZ (Betz et al., Reference Betz, Brambilla, Ilankovic, Prekumar, Kim, Raffard and Kambeitz2019; Woodrow et al., Reference Woodrow, Sparks, Bobrovskaia, Paterson, Murphy and Hutton2018) exhibit impaired decision-making abilities on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara et al., Reference Bechara, Damasio, Samasio and Anderson1994). The IGT is a complex task that taps a multifaceted construct and can reflect effects originating in a number of neural systems related to bottom-up impulsive processes and top-down reflective impulse control (Bechara, Reference Bechara2005; Buelow & Suhr, Reference Buelow and Suhr2009). Moreover, impaired performance on this task is seen in a wide variety of psychiatric and neurological patient populations (Mukherjee & Kable, Reference Mukherjee and Kable2014). Thus, it is unclear whether the deficits observed in persons with DG and SCZ arise from a shared mechanism. There is some evidence that the common variant risk for SCZ is associated with risk-taking (Linnér et al., Reference Linnér, Biroli, Kong, Meddens, Wedow, Fontana and Nivard2019; Maxwell et al., Reference Maxwell, Socrates, Glanville, Di Forti, Murray, Vassos and O’Reilly2019), and that much of the association between the SCZ PRS and SCZ diagnosis is mediated by cognitive deficits (Toulopoulou et al., Reference Toulopoulou, Zhang, Cherny, Dickinson, Berman, Straub and Weinberger2019). However, no studies have specifically examined whether SCZ polygenic risk is related to performance on neuropsychological gambling tasks. Future research examining how the SCZ PRS is related to parameters of IGT behavioral responding and measures of the neural underpinnings of task performance could help to determine whether and how common genetic risk for SCZ might contribute to the impaired decision-making, thereby potentially increasing risk for gambling problems.

Schizotypy is a multidimensional personality construct that is hypothesized to reflect subtle effects of genetic risk for SCZ (Lenzenweger, Reference Lenzenweger2006; Meehl, Reference Meehl1990; Raine, Reference Raine2006). Magical ideation, defined as belief in forms of causation that are invalid by conventional standards, is a trait associated with schizotypy (Eckblad & Chapman, Reference Eckblad and Chapman1983). Magical ideation is similar to many of the superstitious beliefs and cognitive distortions characteristic of DG (Goodie & Fortune, Reference Goodie and Fortune2013; Leonard & Williams, Reference Leonard and Williams2018; Toneatto, Reference Toneatto1999; Zack & Poulos, Reference Zack and Poulos2009). Magical ideation is elevated among gamblers, particularly those who prefer games of chance, such as lotteries and electronic gaming machines (Savage et al., Reference Savage, Slutske and Martin2014). There is currently mixed evidence concerning an association between the SCZ PRS and measures of positive schizotypy or magical ideation (Hatzimanolis et al., Reference Hatzimanolis, Avramopoulos, Arking, Moes, Bhatangar, Lencz and Stefanis2017; Isvoranu et al., Reference Isvoranu, Guloksuz, Epskamp, van Os and Borsboom2019; van Os et al., Reference van Os, van der Steen, Islam, Gülöksüz, Rutten and Simons2017).

The SCZ PRS has been shown to be associated with various forms of substance use and dependence (Carey et al., Reference Carey, Agrawal, Bucholz, Hartz, Lynskey, Nelson and Bogdan2016; Hartz et al., Reference Hartz, Horton, Oehlert, Carey, Agrawal, Bogdan and Bierut2017; Reginsson et al., Reference Reginsson, Ingason, Eusden, Bjornsdottir, Olafsson, Sigurdsson and Stefansson2017). The current findings extend this work, demonstrating that polygenic risk for SCZ associates with a behavioral addiction, DG, that is highly comorbid with substance use disorders in the general population (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008; Petry et al., Reference Petry, Stinson and Grant2005) and among individuals with psychosis (Fortgang et al., Reference Fortgang, Hoff and Potenza2018). Together, findings to date might indicate that common genetic risk for SCZ confers a general liability to impaired control over rewarding behaviors rather than modulating response to a particular drug or drug class. Alternatively, the findings are consistent with a process in which polygenic SCZ risk increases substance use, which may then serve as a mediator that causally influences downstream acquisition of DG.

Prominent theoretical accounts of DG etiology point to impulsivity and dysphoric mood as important liabilities (Blaszczynski & Nower, Reference Blaszczynski and Nower2002; Sharpe, Reference Sharpe2002). The SCZ PRS has been shown to be associated with negative symptoms, anxiety, depression, and externalizing traits and symptoms (Jansen et al., Reference Jansen, Polderman, Bolhuis, van der Ende, Jaddoe, Verhulst and Tiemeier2017; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron, Cannon and Zammit2016; Nivard et al., Reference Nivard, Gage, Hottenga, van Beijsterveldt, Abdellaoui, Bartels and Middeldorp2017; Riglin et al., Reference Riglin, Collishaw, Richards, Thapar, Maughan, O’Donovan and Thapar2017; Sengupta et al., Reference Sengupta, MacDonald, Fathalli, Yim, Lepage, Iyer and Joober2017; van Os et al., Reference van Os, van der Steen, Islam, Gülöksüz, Rutten and Simons2017). Thus, some of the theorized DG risk factors could conceivably arise from pleiotropic effects of genetic liability to SCZ.

Polygenic risk for BD was associated with decreased odds of ever having gambled. This finding was surprising because (a) diagnostic criteria for mania and hypomania include excessive involvement in pleasurable activities with a high potential for painful consequences and (b) higher scores on the BD PRS were (nonsignificantly) associated with increased odds of DG. If this apparent protective effect of BD genetic risk on lifetime gambling proves replicable, more research will be needed to account for this puzzling finding.Footnote 1

Several limitations should be considered. A simple two-item assessment was used to define lifetime gambling and DG status. Using formal diagnostic criteria or more nuanced multiitem screening instruments would have allowed us to probe whether particular clinical features of DG were associated with polygenic risk for various psychiatric disorders. Corrections for multiple testing were not used and the nominally significant effects in Table 1 would not remain if adjusted p-value thresholds were applied. This approach was adopted because (a) the current study was exploratory given the lack of available information concerning the molecular genetics of DG and (b) the analyses also generated effect size estimates that can be directly compared to other studies. Nonetheless, there is clearly a need for replication of the current findings in independent samples. As expected in a general population survey, the prevalence of DG was low, limiting statistical power. Further investigation using clinical or case-control samples featuring more affected individuals would be informative. The PRSs were computed based on the ∼600,000 markers shared across 2 genotyping arrays. Imputation to a reference panel prior to PRS calculation might have improved the precision of the computed scores. Add Health did not include assessments of psychosis, so we were not able to determine the prevalence of SCZ and related conditions. Thus, although we speculate that the findings are not attributable to DG being a direct cause or consequence of diagnosable SCZ spectrum disorders, these possibilities could not be ruled out empirically. The sample size of the discovery GWAS data set has implications for the predictive power of the PRS, with larger samples permitting more precise estimates of allelic effect sizes (Chatterjee et al., Reference Chatterjee, Wheeler, Sampson, Hartge, Chanock and Park2013). The SCZ PRS has the highest estimated predictive power relative to polygenic scores for other psychiatric traits, in part owing to the massive sample size of the SCZ GWAS (So & Sham, Reference So and Sham2016). This may partly explain why a nominally significant association was only observed between DG and SCZ.

In conclusion, our study indicates that genetic predisposition to SCZ is associated with increased risk of problematic gambling in young adulthood. Although replication studies are needed, the current finding provides new clues about the biological bases of DG. The SCZ PRS is being widely used in studies of various neurobehavioral domains. As evidence accumulates concerning the most robust correlates of polygenic risk for SCZ, the nature of the specific mechanism(s) conferring risk for DG may become more evident. Multitrait GWAS approaches that allow joint modeling of DG and SCZ might improve detection of DG-related loci and generate more informative PRSs for use in future research (Andreassen et al., Reference Andreassen, Thompson, Schork, Ripke, Mattingsdal, Kelsoe and Sklar2013; Turley et al., Reference Turley, Walters, Maghzian, Okbay, Lee, Fontana and Magnusson2018).

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported in part by a Center of Excellence award from the National Center for Responsible Gaming. This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due to Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.

Footnotes

1 We considered the possibility that failure to control for educational attainment in the analysis could produce this effect. Lower educational attainment has been associated with increased odds of ever gambling (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008), and higher scores on the BD PRS have been associated with completing more years of schooling and attainment of a university degree (Power et al., Reference Power, Steinberg, Bjornsdottir, Rietveld, Abdellaoui, Nivard and Cesarini2015). However, the association between BD PRS and lifetime gambling was essentially unchanged when controlling for either measured educational attainment or an educational attainment polygenic score (computed based upon Lee et al., Reference Lee, Wedow, Okbay, Kong, Maghzian, Zacher and Fontana2018).

References

Andreassen, O. A., Thompson, W. K., Schork, A. J., Ripke, S., Mattingsdal, M., Kelsoe, J. R., … Sklar, P. (2013). Improved detection of common variants associated with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder using pleiotropy-informed conditional false discovery rate. PLoS Genetics, 9, e1003455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 14581463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Samasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betz, L. T., Brambilla, P., Ilankovic, A., Prekumar, P., Kim, M.-S., Raffard, S., … Kambeitz, J. (2019). Deciphering reward-based decision-making in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis and behavioral modeling of the Iowa Gambling Task. Schizophrenia Research, 204, 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanco, C., Myers, J., & Kendler, K. S. (2012). Gambling, disordered gambling and their association with major depression and substance use: A web-based cohort and twin-sibling study. Psychological Medicine, 42, 497508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97, 487499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogdan, R., Baranger, D. A. A., & Agrawal, A. (2018). Polygenic risk scores in clinical psychology: Bridging genomic risk to individual differences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14, 119157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boileau, I., Payer, D., Chugani, B., Lobo, D. S. S., Houle, S., Wilson, A. A., … Zack, M. (2014). In vivo evidence for greater amphetamine-induced dopamine release in pathological gambling: A positron emission tomography study with [11 C]-(+)-PHNO. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, 13051313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braudt, D. B., & Harris, K. M. (2018) Polygenic scores (PGSs) in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health) — Release 1. Retrieved from https://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/documentation/guides/PGS_AH1_UserGuide.pdf Google Scholar
Buelow, M. T., & Suhr, J. A. (2009). Construct validity of the Iowa Gambling Task. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 102114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, C. E., Agrawal, A., Bucholz, K. K., Hartz, S. M., Lynskey, M. T., Nelson, E. C., … Bogdan, R. (2016). Associations between polygenic risk for psychiatric disorders and substance involvement. Frontiers in Genetics, 7, 149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C. C., Chow, C. C., Tellier, L. C., Vattikuti, S., Purcell, S. M., & Lee, J. J. (2015). Second-generation PLINK: Rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. GigaScience, 4, 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chatterjee, N., Wheeler, B., Sampson, J., Hartge, P., Chanock, S. J., & Park, J.-H. (2013). Projecting the performance of risk prediction based on polygenic analyses of genome-wide association studies. Nature Genetics, 45, 400405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C., Nower, L., & Walker, D. M. (2013). The relationship of ADHD symptoms to gambling behaviour in the USA: Results from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. International Gambling Studies, 13, 3751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comings, D. E., Rosenthal, R. J., Lesieur, H. R., Rugle, L. J., Muhleman, D., Chiu, C., … Gade, R. (1996). A study of the dopamine D2 receptor gene in pathological gambling. Pharmacogenetics, 6, 223234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dagher, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2009). Personality, addiction, dopamine: Insights from Parkinson’s disease. Neuron, 61, 502510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., … Cerrato, F. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51, 6375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, M. L., Klos, K. J., Bower, J. H., Geda, Y. E., Josephs, K. A., & Ahlskog, J. E. (2005). Pathological gambling caused by drugs used to treat Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology, 62, 13771381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudbridge, F. (2016). Polygenic epidemiology. Genetic Epidemiology, 40, 268272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckblad, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 215225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edens, E. L., & Rosenheck, R. A. (2012). Rates and correlates of pathological gambling among VA mental health service users. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, A. C., Bacanu, S.-A., Bigdelli, T. N., Mosczti, A., & Kendler, K. S. (2016). Evaluating the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia in a large-scale genome-wide association study. Schizophrenia Research, 176, 136140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fortgang, R. G., Hoff, R. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2018). Problem and pathological gambling in schizophrenia: Exploring links with substance use and impulsivity. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34, 673688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodie, A. S., & Fortune, E. E. (2013). Measuring cognitive distortions in pathological gambling: Review and meta-analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 730743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, J. E., Odlaug, B. L., & Chamberlain, S. R. (2015). Reduced cortical thickness in gambling disorder: A morphometric MRI study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265, 655661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. C., & MacKillop, J. (2014). Genetic basis of delay discounting in frequent gamblers: Examination of a priori candidates and exploration of a panel of dopamine-related loci. Brain and Behavior, 4, 812821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, K. M., Halpern, C. T., Whitsel, E., Hussey, J., Tabor, J., Entzel, P., & Udry, J. R. (2009). The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health: Research Design. Retrieved from http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design Google Scholar
Hartz, S. M., Horton, A. C., Oehlert, M., Carey, C. E., Agrawal, A., Bogdan, R., … Bierut, L. J. (2017). Association between substance use disorder and polygenic liability to schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 82, 709715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatzimanolis, A., Avramopoulos, D. D., Arking, D. E., Moes, A., Bhatangar, P., Lencz, T., … Stefanis, N. C. (2017). Stress-dependent association between polygenic risk for schizophrenia and schizoptypal traits in young army recruits. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44, 223347.Google Scholar
Hettige, N. C., Cole, C. B., Khalid, S., & De Luca, V. (2016). Polygenic risk score prediction of antipsychotic dosage in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 170, 265270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Highland, H. M., Avery, C. L., Duan, Q., Li, Y., & Harris, K. M. (2018). Quality control analysis of Add Health GWAS Data. Retrieved from https://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/documentation/guides/AH_GWAS_QC.pdf Google Scholar
Hodgins, D. C., Stea, J. N., & Grant, J. E. (2011). Gambling disorders. Lancet, 378, 18741884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, O. D., McCutcheon, R., Owen, M. J., & Murray, R. M. (2016). The role of genes, stress, and dopamine in the development of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 81, 920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isvoranu, A. M., Guloksuz, S., Epskamp, S., van Os, J., Borsboom, D., & GROUP Investigators. (2019). Toward incorporating genetic risk scores into symptom networks of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171900045X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jansen, P. R., Polderman, T. J. C., Bolhuis, K., van der Ende, J., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Verhulst, F. C., … Tiemeier, H. (2017). Polygenic scores for schizophrenia and educational attainment are associated with behavioural problems in early childhood in the general population. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59, 3947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, H. J., Stergiakouli, E., Tansey, K. E., Hubbard, L., Heron, J., Cannon, M., … Zammit, S. (2016). Phenotypic manifestations of genetic risk for schizophrenia during adolescence in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry, 73, 221228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Hwang, I., LaBrie, R., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Winters, K. C., & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine, 38, 13511360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knöchel, C., Reuter, J., Reinke, B., Stäblein, M., Marbach, K., Feddern, R., … Oertel-Knöchel, V. (2016). Cortical thinning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 172, 7885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, I., Richman, M. J., Janka, Z., Maraz, A., & Andó, B. (2017). Decision making measured by the Iowa Gambling Task in alcohol use disorder and gambling disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 181, 152161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, M., Leménager, T., Streit, F., Fauth-Bühler, M., Frank, J., Juraeva, D., … Kiefer, F. (2016). Genome-wide association study of pathological gambling. European Psychiatry, 36, 3846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J. J., Wedow, R., Okbay, A., Kong, E., Maghzian, O., Zacher, M., … Fontana, M. A. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a 1.1-million-person GWAS of educational attainment. Nature Genetics, 50, 11121121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenzenweger, M. F. (2006). Schizotypy: An organizing framework for schizophrenia research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 162166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, C. A., & Williams, R. J. (2018). Fallacious beliefs: Gambling specific and beliefs in the paranormal. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 51, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, E., Browne, M., Rawat, V., Langham, E., & Rockloff, M. (2017). Breaking bad: Comparing gambling harms among gamblers and affected others. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33, 223248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Yip, B. H., Björk, C., Pawitan, Y., Cannon, T. D., Sullivan, P. F., & Hultman, C. M. (2009). Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: A population-based study. Lancet, 373, 234239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lind, P. A., Zhu, G., Montgomery, G. W., Madden, P. A. F., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., & Slutske, W. S. (2012). Genome-wide association study of a quantitative disordered gambling trait. Addiction Biology, 18, 511522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linnér, R. K., Biroli, P., Kong, E., Meddens, S. F. W., Wedow, R., Fontana, M. A., … Nivard, M. G. (2019). Genome-wide association analyses of risk tolerance and risky behaviors in over 1 million individuals identify hundreds of loci and shared genetic influences. Nature Genetics, 51, 245257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lobo, D. S., Aleksandrova, L., Knight, J., Casey, D. M., El-Guebaly, N., Nobrega, J. N., & Kennedy, J. L. (2015). Addiction-related genes in gambling disorders: New insights from parallel human and pre-clinical models. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 10021010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, R. M., Visscher, P. M., Robinson, M. R., & Wray, N. R. (2018). Embracing polygenicity: A review of methods and tools for psychiatric genetics research. Psychological Medicine, 48, 10551067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maxwell, J., Socrates, A., Glanville, K. P., Di Forti, M. D., Murray, R. M., Vassos, E., & O’Reilly, P. F. (2019). Investigating the role of behaviour in the genetic risk for schizophrenia. BioRxiv, 611079. https://doi.org/10.1101/611079 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McIntyre, R. S., McElroy, S. L., Konarski, J. Z., Soczynska, J. K., Wilkins, K., & Kennedy, S. H. (2007). Problem gambling in bipolar disorder: Results from the Canadian Community Health Survey. Journal of Affective Disorders, 102, 2734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meehl, P. E. (1990). Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukherjee, D., & Kable, J. W. (2014). Value-based decision making in mental illness: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 767782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nautiyal, K. M., Okuda, M., Hen, R., & Blanco, C. (2017). Gambling disorder: An integrative review of animal and human studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1394, 106127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neilson, E., Bois, C., Gibson, J., Duff, B., Watson, A., Roberts, N., … Lawrie, S. M. (2017). Effects of environmental risks and polygenic loading for schizophrenia on cortical thickness. Schizophrenia Research, 184, 128136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nivard, M. G., Gage, S. H., Hottenga, J. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Abdellaoui, A., Bartels, M., … Middeldorp, C. M. (2017). Genetic overlap between schizophrenia and developmental psychopathology: Longitudinal and multivariate polygenic risk prediction of common psychiatric traits during development. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43, 11971207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, S., Cho, M. J., Chang, S. M., Jeon, H. J., Cho, S. J., Kim, B. S., … Hong, J. P. (2011). Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidities of adult ADHD symptoms in Korea: Results of the Korean epidemiologic catchment area study. Psychiatry Research, 186, 378383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petry, N. M., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 564574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potenza, M. N., Xian, H., Shah, K., Scherrer, J. F., & Eisen, S. A. (2005). Shared genetic contributions to pathological gambling and major depression in men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 10151021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, R. A., Steinberg, S., Bjornsdottir, G., Rietveld, C. A., Abdellaoui, A., Nivard, M. M., … Cesarini, D. (2015). Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder predict creativity. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 953955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S. M., Wray, N. R., Stone, J. L., Visscher, P. M., O’Donovan, N. C., Sullivan, P. F., … International Schizophrenia Consortium. (2009). Common polygenic variation contributes to risk for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature, 460, 748752.Google ScholarPubMed
Raine, A. (2006). Schizotypal personality: Neurodevelopmental and psychosocial trajectories. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 291326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reginsson, G. W., Ingason, A., Eusden, J., Bjornsdottir, G., Olafsson, S., Sigurdsson, E., … Stefansson, K. (2017). Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder associate with addiction. Addiction Biology, 23, 485492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riglin, L., Collishaw, S., Richards, A., Thapar, A., Maughan, B., O’Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2017). Schizophrenia risk alleles and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood: A population-based cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 5762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimol, L. M., Hartberg, C. B., Nesvåg, R., Fennema-Notestine, C., Halger, D. J. Jr., Pung, C. J., … Agartz, I. (2010). Cortical thickness and subcortical volumes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 4150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ripke, S., Neale, B. M., Corvin, A., Walters, J. T., Farh, K. H., Holmans, P. A., … Pers, T. H. (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature, 511, 421427.Google Scholar
Savage, J. E., Slutske, W. S., & Martin, N. G. (2014). Personality and gambling involvement: A person-centered approach. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 11981211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherrer, J. F., Xian, H., Slutske, W. S., Eisen, S. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2015). Associations between obsessive-compulsive classes and pathological gambling in a national cohort of male twins. JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 342349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekar, A., Bialas, A. R., de Rivera, H., Davis, A., Hammond, T. R., Kamitaki, N., … Genovese, G. (2016). Schizophrenia risk from complex variation of complement component 4. Nature, 530, 177183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sengupta, S. M., MacDonald, K., Fathalli, F., Yim, A., Lepage, M., Iyer, S., … Joober, R. (2017). Polygenic risk score associated with specific symptom dimensions in first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 184, 116121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, L. (2002). A reformulated cognitive-behavioral model of problem gambling: A biopsychosocial perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sklar, P., Ripke, S., Scott, L. J., Andreassen, O. A., Cichon, S., Craddock, N., … Corvin, A. (2011). Large-scale genome-wide association analysis of bipolar disorder identifies a new susceptibility locus near ODZ4. Nature Genetics, 43, 977983.Google Scholar
Slutske, W. S. (2019). Genetic and environmental contributions to risk for disordered gambling. In Heinz, A., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., & Potenza, M., (Eds.), Gambling disorder (pp. 73100). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slutske, W. S., Eisen, S. A., True, W. R., Lyons, M. J., Goldberg, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (2000). Common genetic vulnerability for pathological gambling and alcohol dependence in men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 666673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S., Eisen, S. A., Xian, H., True, W. R., Lyons, M. J., Goldberg, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (2001). A twin study of the association between pathological gambling and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 297308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S., Ellingson, J. M., Richmond-Rakerd, L. S., Zhu, G., & Martin, N. G. (2013). Shared genetic vulnerability for disordered gambling and alcohol use disorder in men and women: Evidence from a national community-based Australian twin study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 525534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
So, H.-C., & Sham, P. C. (2016). Exploring the predictive power of polygenic scores derived from genome-wide association studies: A study of 10 complex traits. Bioinformatics, 33, 886892.Google Scholar
Stepniak, B., Papiol, S., Hammer, C., Ramin, A., Everts, S., Hennig, L., … Ehrenreich, H. (2014). Accumulated environmental risk determining age at schizophrenia onset: A deep phenotyping-based study. Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 444453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, P. F., Agrawal, A., Bulik, C. M., Andreassen, O. A., Børglum, A. D., Breen, G., … for the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2018). Psychiatric genomics: An update and an agenda. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 1527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toneatto, T. (1999). Cognitive psychopathology of problem gambling. Substance Use & Misuse, 34, 15931604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulopoulou, T., Zhang, X., Cherny, S., Dickinson, D., Berman, K. F., Straub, R. E., … Weinberger, D. R. (2019). Polygenic risk score increases schizophrenia liability through cognition-relevant pathways. Brain, 142, 471485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turley, P., Walters, R. K., Maghzian, O., Okbay, A., Lee, J. J., Fontana, M. A., … Magnusson, P. (2018). Multi-trait analysis of genome-wide association summary statistics using MTAG. Nature Genetics, 50, 229237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J., van der Steen, Y., Islam, Md. A., Gülöksüz, S., Rutten, B. P., Simons, C.J., & GROUP Investigators. (2017). Evidence that polygenic risk for psychotic disorder is expressed in the domain of neurodevelopment, emotion regulation, and attribution of salience. Psychological Medicine, 47, 24212437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodrow, A., Sparks, S., Bobrovskaia, V., Paterson, C., Murphy, P., & Hutton, P. (2018). Decision-making ability in psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the magnitude, specificity, and correlates of impaired performance on the Iowa and Cambridge Gambling Tasks. Psychological Medicine, 49, 3248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Ripke, S., Mattheisen, M., Trzaskowski, M., Byrne, E. M., Abdellaoui, A., … Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2018). Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nature Genetics, 50, 668681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xian, H., Giddens, J. L., Scherrer, J. F., Eisen, S. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Environmental factors selectively impact co-occurrence of problem/pathological gambling with specific drug-use disorders in male twins. Addiction, 109, 635644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zack, M., & Poulos, C. X. (2004). Amphetamine primes motivation to gamble and gambling-related semantic networks in problem gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 195207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zack, M., & Poulos, C. X. (2007). A D2 antagonist enhances the rewarding and priming effects of a gambling episode in pathological gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, 16781686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zack, M., & Poulos, C. X. (2009). Parallel roles for dopamine in pathological gambling and psychostimulant addiction. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 2, 1125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Results from logistic regression models predicting gambling and disordered gambling

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for disordered gambling by deciles of schizophrenia polygenic risk score, with the lowest decile serving as the reference category.