Description and Major Findings of CoSMoS
Data Collection and Research Focus
The German twin study on Cognitive Ability, Self-Reported Motivation, and School Achievement (CoSMoS) is a longitudinally designed study aimed at identifying the predictors of and influences on school performance in a genetically sensitive design (Spinath & Wolf, Reference Spinath and Wolf2006). CoSMoS was initially conceptualized to be in consonant with the Twins' Early Development Study (TEDS; Oliver & Plomin, Reference Oliver and Plomin2007) in order to replicate results from TEDS in an independent German twin sample. To explore the underlying structure of the etiology of cognitive and especially non-cognitive predictors of school success, on the one hand, and to extend the knowledge about relevant family characteristics on the other, CoSMoS widened the palette of measures to different motivational constructs and specific environmental variables, as well as individual and familial characteristics (e.g. parental involvement and values; see Table 1 for an overview).
aAbility self-perceptions, intrinsic values, utility and importance, goals.
bTwo verbal (general knowledge including 18 items and 25 vocabulary items) and two non-verbal (figural classification and figural reasoning consisting of 25 items each) scales adapted from the German Cognitive Ability Test (KFT 4–12 + R) and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1991).
CoSMoS was initiated in 2005 and encompasses three waves of data collection with a 2-year period between each measurement, allowing genetic analyses of the causes of stability and change in the variables under study. For details of the recruitment procedure through individual inquiries at registrations offices in two German federal states, see Spinath and Wolf (Reference Spinath and Wolf2006). Zygosity was assessed by questionnaire measures (Oniszczenko et al., Reference Oniszczenko, Angleitner, Strelau and Angert1993) that typically yield accuracies in the magnitude of 95% (Price et al., Reference Price, Freeman, Craig, Petrill, Ebersole and Plomin2000). The data set finally consists of 407 twin pairs (M = 9.1 years, SD = 0.79) and their parents for the first wave of data collection. In total, 283 pairs of children participated in at least two of the three measurements.
Recent Research Findings
On the basis of the broad array of individual and familial predictors of school success, CoSMoS offers the opportunity to broaden our knowledge of the etiology and development of these variables. Intelligence as the single most important predictor of school success has been studied extensively and there is ample evidence that it is substantially heritable (e.g. Deary et al., Reference Deary, Spinath and Bates2006), and that the genetic influence on inter-individual differences in cognitive abilities increases with age (Plomin & Spinath, Reference Plomin and Spinath2004). At the same time, the etiology of non-cognitive predictors, such as motivational characteristics and the relationship between different predictors and school success, has been studied much less frequently. Consequently, this is the major research focus of CoSMoS.
Important predictors of school success over and beyond intelligence are children's self-perceived abilities (SPA). Contrary to theoretical assumptions (Eccles et al., Reference Eccles, Adler, Futterman, Goff, Kaczala, Meece, Midgley and Spence1983), studies based on TEDS data suggested that a substantial part of the inter-individual variance in SPAs is explained by genetic factors and that the main environmental influence is non-shared rather than shared (Spinath et al., Reference Spinath, Spinath and Plomin2008). This general pattern was confirmed in the German CoSMoS sample with twins at age 7–11 (Spinath et al., Reference Spinath, Spinath and Plomin2008) as well as at age 13 (Gottschling et al., Reference Gottschling, Spengler, Spinath and Spinath2012). Because CoSMoS gathered SPAs in all waves of the data collection, it was also possible to analyze the causes of stability and change over a period of 6 years, including the transition to secondary school. Here, genetic influences mainly accounted for stability, whereas non-shared environment accounted for change, especially just after the transition to secondary school (Gottschling, Reference Gottschling2012). Multivariate genetic analyses also revealed that in two school domains (German and Math), SPAs correlated with academic success for genetic rather than environmental reasons (Gottschling et al., Reference Gottschling, Spengler, Spinath and Spinath2012), which is in line with previous research based on TEDS data (Greven et al., Reference Greven, Harlaar, Kovas, Chamorro-Premuzic and Plomin2009).
With respect to prominent theories of the causal structure of SPA and school success, that is, whether former SPAs influence subsequent school success or rather vice versa (e.g., Marsh & Yeung, Reference Marsh and Yeung1997), we are currently analyzing the origins of these cross-lagged relationships through multivariate genetic analyses. As CoSMoS encompasses additional motivational variables (e.g., intrinsic values and achievement goals) further non-cognitive predictors of school achievement can be studied in a genetically sensitive design.
Description and Major Findings of the SOEP Twin Study
Data Collection and Research Focus
The SOEP twin study was established in 2009 at Saarland University in cooperation with the German SOcio-Economic Panel study (GSOEP), initiated by the German Institute for Economic Research (DIW) in Berlin. One of the major goals of the SOEP twin project was the creation of a genetically sensitive data set of twin and non-twin relatives. The SOEP is a longitudinal and representative survey, consulting around 12,000 individuals in nearly 6,000 private German households every year. The sample incorporates every person of a household aged 17 or older, collecting data across different areas, such as personality traits, health, childcare and education participation, employment, income, living situation, life-satisfaction, and several attitudes (Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Frick and Schupp2007). This family structure offers a unique and rich opportunity to study different family relationships. As the number of twin pairs in the original GSOEP was too low to realize an extended twin design, we used the GSOEP mainly as the basis for the non-twin part of the data set, identifying independent groups of non-twin sibling pairs (SIB) born less than 2 years apart, mother–child dyads (MoCh), and grandparent–child dyads (GpaCh; for more details see Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Spinath, Siedler, Wagner, Schupp and Kandler2011). We then added monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs who completed either an online or a paper–pencil version of our questionnaire, which included the same questions as used in the SOEP survey. In part, we reverted to contact information from twins who had participated in previous voluntary German twin studies (JeTSSA, BiLSAT, and TwinPaw), which have been described in detail elsewhere (BiLSAT: Spinath et al., Reference Spinath, Angleitner, Borkenau, Riemann and Wolf2002; JeTSSA: Stößel et al., Reference Stößel, Kämpfe and Riemann2006; TwinPaw: Spinath & Wolf, Reference Spinath and Wolf2006). Furthermore, new contacts were made by random telephone screenings, in which people were simply asked whether they happened to be members of a twin pair. If this was the case, names and contact information were registered. A total of 940 twin pairs from different parts of Germany were contacted via telephone and 562 complete twin pairs agreed to participate in the SOEP twin study. Finally, 349 twin pairs returned a complete set of questionnaires, including a self-report physical similarity questionnaire (Oniszczenko et al., Reference Oniszczenko, Angleitner, Strelau and Angert1993) to classify the zygosity of the twins. The resulting data set for the twin and non-twin groups contained 2,616 individuals (see Table 2). With regard to education, family background, relationship and employment status, the sample was heterogeneous. In addition to biographic and descriptive information (age, sex, weight, and size) as well as objective living conditions and personal development, the data set included a wide range of different topics (see Table 3).
MZ = monozygotic twins; DZ = dizygotic twins; SIB = siblings; MoCh = mother–child pairs; GpaCh = grandparent–child pairs.
BFI-S = GSOEP Big Five Inventory; NEO-PI-R = NEO-Personality Inventory revised; LOCI = Lark-Owl Chronotype Indicator; MEQ-DE = Morningness–Eveningness Questionaire; CISS = Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations.
The combination of independent twin and non-twin groups in the SOEP twin project allowed for the exploration of additional sources of variance in a Genetically Sensitive Multi-Group Design (GSMGD). In the GSMGD, analyses rely on five groups of relatives with differing degrees of genetic and environmental relatedness, which provided several methodological advantages: (1) additive and non-additive genetic influences as well as shared and non-shared environmental influences could be estimated simultaneously; (2) twin pairs could be compared to non-twin pairs to investigate twin specific (T) environmental influences; (3) also, influences of cultural transmission could be estimated through the inclusion of inter-generational pairs (MoCh and GpaCh dyads); (4) the GSMGD increased statistical power, leading to more fine-grained and less biased estimates and a greater generalizability of the results.
Major Findings
Different research questions could be addressed in the SOEP twin project with regard to the specific extended design and the wide range of topics included in the data set. Currently, the first studies based on the SOEP twin project are being published and a number of studies are in progress. For example, a study by Hahn et al. (Reference Hahn, Spinath, Siedler, Wagner, Schupp and Kandler2011) examined genetic and environmental influences on personality dimensions from the five-factor model with regard to additive genetic influences (A) in the presence of non-additive genetic influences (D), shared (C) and non-shared (E) environmental influences, as well as cultural transmission and twin specific (T) environmental influences (i.e., environmental influences that are unique to twin pairs). The analyses revealed contributions of C and D influences for the Big Five personality traits which were not evident in classical twin analyses conducted with the same data set. Evidence for T influences was found for extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Furthermore, the genetic and environmental influences on the relationship between personality and life satisfaction were investigated with a particular focus on twin-specific environmental influences (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Johnson and Spinath2012). The evaluation of life satisfaction involves a comparison process with other people which can be different for twin and non-twin family members. Using the GSMGD, twin-specific environmental influences and also shared environmental influences were found for life satisfaction. Concerning genetic influences, the analyses showed that the heritability of life satisfaction (additive and non-additive) was completely shared with the genetic influences on personality, indicating no specific genetic influences on life satisfaction independent from personality. Based on these findings, further investigations will focus on the identification of environmental influences of importance in twins versus non-twins. Further analyses in progress explore the nature and nurture of life attitudes, domain satisfaction (work, income, or health) and worries, as well as common genetic and environmental influences on these aspects and personality.
Description and Major Findings of the Chronotype Twin Study (ChronoS)
Data Collection and Research Focus
The chronotype twin project (ChronoS) started in 2010 with a special focus on genetic and environmental influences on inter-individual differences in morningness–eveningness types and the role of personality and coping strategies within this process. The chronotype encompasses a personal preference for being awake, active, and concentrated in the morning versus evening hours of the day. The ChronoS study also served as second wave for the SOEP twin project, incorporating a substantial number of participants from the previous sample and several issues of the SOEP questionnaire. A total of 172 complete data sets of twin pairs from the original SOEP sample were obtained. Additional twin pairs were recruited using contact information on persons with identical birth names, birthdays, and also birthplace provided by the registration office of Berlin, Germany. A request for 11 birth cohorts (1950, 1955, 1960, 1965, 1970, 1975, 1980, 1985, 1990, 1991, 1992) resulted in 1,804 addresses of potential twin pairs. Those twins were also contacted by mail and asked to collaborate in the ChronoS twin study. In total, complete data sets of 875 twins were obtained (see Table 2). Zygosity was diagnosed through self-reports assessing the frequency of twin confusion by different relatives, teachers, and peers across the life span as well as physical similarity (accuracies in the magnitude of 95%; for details, see Price et al., Reference Price, Freeman, Craig, Petrill, Ebersole and Plomin2000).
At present, only very few studies have investigated the heritability of morningness–eveningness types (Hur, Reference Hur2007; Hur et al., Reference Hur, Bouchard and Lykken1998; Koskenvuo et al., Reference Koskenvuo, Hublin, Partinen, Heikkilä and Kaprio2007). Furthermore, the underlying mechanisms on the relationship between personality, coping strategies, and chronotype are still unresolved. Depending on the measurement, some studies postulated a positive relationship between morningness and conscientiousness (Tsaousis, Reference Tsaousis2010), whereas others showed a negative correlation between extraversion and eveningness (Preckel et al., Reference Preckel, Lipnevich, Ross and Roberts2012). Therefore, the aim of the ChronoS twin study is to establish the chronotype with respect to different conceptualizations and its relationship to personality-related behavior from a behavior genetic point of view. Different questionnaires (Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire, MEQ: Horne & Östberg, Reference Horne and Östberg1976; Lark-Owl Chronotype Indicator, LOCI: Roberts, Reference Roberts1998) were used to address different concepts of the chronotype. Preliminary analyses showed that the estimates for additive and non-additive genetic effects on the chronotype collected by the two measurements were comparable. Multivariate genetic modeling can be used to investigate the underlying structure of different measurements of a construct testing for common and specific genetic and environmental influences. Future analyses will focus on common and specific mechanism of the chronotype and the role of personality and coping strategies in this process.