Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T08:33:17.320Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A cross-cultural exploration of compassion, and facilitators and inhibitors of compassion in UK and Sri Lankan people

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 February 2022

Lasara Kariyawasam*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Margarita Ononaiye
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Chris Irons
Affiliation:
Balanced Mind, London, UK
Sarah E. Kirby
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Lasara Kariyawasam, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Practising compassion has shown to increase well-being and reduce distress in people across cultures. However, very little research has explored cultural differences in different facets of compassion with a dearth of research evident especially in the Asian context. Several inhibitors and facilitators of compassion have been identified although the nuances of cultural differences of these remain unexploited. This study aimed to discover cross-cultural similarities and differences of the levels of compassion, facilitators and inhibitors of compassion between Sri Lankan and UK people.

Methods

A cross-sectional, questionnaire-based quantitative research was conducted among 149 Sri Lankan and 300 UK participants. Individual predictors (such as fears of compassion, self-reassurance, external shame, social safeness and pleasure, depression and anxiety) were also explored in relation to compassion, compassion to others, and compassion from others in each group.

Results

The results indicated that Sri Lankan participants were more self-reassured and self-compassionate and self-identifying as a Buddhist predicted higher self-compassion, when compared to UK participants. However, Sri Lankan participants reported higher levels of external shame and fear of compassion not just towards themselves, but also towards and from others, indicating difficulty in engaging compassionately with others. In contrast, UK participants reported higher social safeness, indicating that they were more likely to feel safe and soothed by the society than the Sri Lankan participants.

Conclusions

Society plays a pivotal role in shaping one's experiences of compassion. This study suggests that specific cultural and social factors should be considered when implementing Western compassionate approaches to non-Western settings.

Type
Original Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Compassion is ‘a sensitivity to the suffering of self and others, with a deep wish and commitment to relieve the suffering’ (Gilbert, Reference Gilbertn.d., p. 22). The underlying motive is to collate our emotions into a helpful balance that enhances our sense of well-being (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2014a). Practising compassion towards others and oneself has shown promising potential for increasing well-being (Lutz et al., Reference Lutz, Greischar, Rawlings, Ricard and Davidson2004). Developing compassionate thoughts towards the self, others and even strangers, has increased positive emotions, social support and mindfulness (Fredrickson et al., Reference Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek and Finkel2008), and decreased depression and anxiety (MacBeth and Gumley, Reference MacBeth and Gumley2012). Despite the perceived benefits and increased interest in compassion practice, there is a dearth of research investigating the cross-cultural differences of compassion and the multifaceted factors affecting it (Montero-Marin et al., Reference Montero-Marin, Kuyken, Crane, Gu, Baer, Al-Awamleh, Akutsu, Araya-Véliz, Ghorbani, Chen and Kim2018). Therefore, this study attempted to explore compassion, and facilitators and inhibitors of compassion among people in Sri Lanka, a collectivistic Asian culture, in comparison with a group of UK people.

Theoretical perspective of compassion

Gilbert (Reference Gilbert2014b, Reference Gilbert2016) developed social mentality theory (SMT), with the notion that compassion is an evolved care-based motivational system, known as a social mentality, which originally evolved to regulate distress in parent–infant relationships. The SMT is underpinned by evolutionary psychology, neurophysiology (Porges, Reference Porges2007), attachment theory (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1982) and Buddhism (Wong, Reference Wong2021). It emphasises that compassion activates the motivation to pay attention to suffering to make sense of it, and the ability to relieve and prevent that suffering (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert, Gilbert and Bailey2000). The social mentality of compassion comprises six essential competencies that are related to sensitivity, sympathy, distress tolerance, empathy, non-judgement and care for well-being (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2009). These competencies flow across three directional paths, known as the three flows of compassion, which are compassion to others, compassion from others and self-compassion (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2014a). Based on the aforementioned theory, Gilbert (Reference Gilbert2009) introduced compassion focused therapy (CFT), to treat people experiencing psychological issues that involve high levels of shame and criticism, by cultivating compassion across the three flows.

Gilbert (Reference Gilbert and Gilbert2005b) used a tripartite model to conceptualise psychopathology. According to this model of affect regulation, three systems interact to regulate signals of threat, resources/incentives and affiliation/soothing, which trigger the negative affect, high arousal positive affect and social safeness, respectively. This model explains how various psychosocial vulnerabilities can be understood using the interplay between these three regulatory systems (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2005a, Reference Gilbert2015). Whilst an overactive threat system is found to inhibit compassion cultivation, the soothing system holds the capacity to suppress the threat system and facilitate the manifestation of compassion (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Mitra, Franks, Richter and Rockliff2008).

Inhibitors and facilitators of compassion

According to Gilbert (Reference Gilbert2007, Reference Gilbertn.d., Reference Gilbert2014b), attachment insecurities, neglect, abuse or emotional conflicts with significant others generate fear reactions, such as avoidances and resistances that inhibit compassion. Such experiences pose a vulnerability to self-criticism, which hinders compassion cultivation (Rector et al., Reference Rector, Bagby, Segal, Joffe and Levitt2000), and acts as a pervasive element of shame and psychopathology (Gilbert and Irons, Reference Gilbert and Irons2005). Studies have found that the conceptualisation of shame may differ between cultures (Mesquita, Reference Mesquita2001), with shame being an internal, self-directed construct in individualistic cultures and an external construct, which relates to how a person exists in the minds of others and their judgements (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert, Gilbert and Andrews1998), in collectivistic cultures such as the Asian countries. Self-criticism, fears of compassion and experiencing shame are found to positively correlate with depression (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Matos and Rivis2011, Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Catarino and Baião2014) and anxiety (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Catarino and Baião2014; Hermanto et al., Reference Hermanto, Zuroff, Kopala-Sibley, Kelly, Matos, Gilbert and Koestner2016). Thus, psychopathology, including depression and anxiety is believed to stem from an over-activation of the threat system and an under-activation of the soothing system making it difficult for one to experience compassion (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Catarino and Baião2014).

On the contrary, the soothing system seeks signals of care, warmth and affiliation, and arouses calmness and reassurance (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Mitra, Franks, Richter and Rockliff2008). Therefore, in the presence of social safeness, the warm, calming experience of feeling cared about, reassured by, and connected to others in the society, people are more likely to generate warm affiliative feelings such as compassion (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Mitra, Ritcher, Franks, Mills, Bellew and Gale2009). Self-reassurance is another factor that activates the soothing system and facilitates compassion. In fact, the ability to self-reassure and recognise one's strengths during suffering has reduced depression in clinical and non-clinical groups (Castilho et al., Reference Castilho, Pinto-Gouveia and Duarte2013). Studies have found that whilst self-criticism inhibits compassion and correlates with depressive symptomatology, higher ability to self-reassure could weaken this relationship between self-criticism and depression (Petrocchi et al., Reference Petrocchi, Dentale and Gilbert2019). This indicates that although it has been discovered that external shame and attachment insecurities can suppress one's compassion (Gilbert and Irons, Reference Gilbert and Irons2005), a soothing-affiliation system with others can increase compassion across all three flows (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2005a).

Compassion across cultures

Despite the increased interest in compassion research and the evidence supporting the benefits of compassion practice, most studies are limited to Western countries (Neff et al., Reference Neff, Pisitsungkagarn and Hsieh2008; Sinclair et al., Reference Sinclair, Torres, Raffin-Bouchal, Hack, McClement, Hagen and Chochinov2016). Application of Western models to non-Western societies is challenging, as compassion is a context-dependent construct influenced by group norms, cultural practices and values (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Matos and Rivis2011). Whilst compassion is seen as universal, cross-cultural differences have been identified in various facets of compassion (Birkett, Reference Birkett2013), such as the six compassion competencies (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2009), and inhibitors and facilitators of compassion (Steindl et al., Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020). Eastern collectivistic societies such as Sri Lanka (Pathirana, Reference Pathirana2016) are appreciative of devotion and concerns for others (Triandis, Reference Triandis1993), and may show more compassion to others than the Western societies (Steindl et al., Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020). Thus, it seems fair to propose that people's underlying motivations and views of compassion may vary cross-culturally (Cheon et al., Reference Cheon, Im, Harada, Kim, Mathur, Scimeca, Parrish, Park and Chiao2011).

Neff (Reference Neff2011) viewed self-compassion as an Asian construct due to compassion being broadly discussed in Buddhism, a religion that is primarily followed by Asian people (Prebish and Baumann, Reference Prebish and Baumann2002). From a Buddhist standpoint, compassion is the desire to free all people from suffering (Davidson and Harrington, Reference Davidson and Harrington2002), and Buddhist practices such as loving-kindness and mindful meditation (Leighton, Reference Leighton2012) promote compassion cultivation (Lama and Vreeland, Reference Lama and Vreeland2008). Thus, one would expect Buddhist followers to be affluent in compassion. In support, a study conducted in the USA where the majority of the participants self-identified as Caucasian found that participants practising Buddhist meditation were more self-compassionate than college undergraduates and older adults recruited from the wider community (Neff and Pommier, Reference Neff and Pommier2013).

In contrast, a study that explored practising Buddhists in a Japanese collectivistic country, where people's lifestyle is influenced by high levels of social interconnectedness (Neff et al., Reference Neff, Pisitsungkagarn and Hsieh2008), discovered that Japanese participants had low self-compassion and high self-criticism when compared to the USA participants (Kitayama and Markus, Reference Kitayama and Markus2000). The social pressure to abide by cultural norms in Japan may explain their low self-compassion (Neff et al., Reference Neff, Pisitsungkagarn and Hsieh2008). Thus, despite the strong Buddhist influence of compassion, cultural differences may tentatively explain why Asian Buddhist people living in Western countries indicated higher self-compassion (Neff and Pommier, Reference Neff and Pommier2013). In support, Wong (Reference Wong2021) emphasised that the lives of several Asian people living in Asian countries are controlled by external forces, pain and tragedy that are beyond their control, which may explain their general lack of self-compassion. Furthermore, Asian Confucian cultures, such as Taiwan, where self-improvement is determined by shame, judgement and threatened isolation indicated higher self-criticism rather than self-compassion. In the same study, however, Thai participants (a Buddhist-influenced culture) were more self-compassionate than the American and Taiwanese participants. The collectivistic social dynamic in cultures such as Sri Lanka is found to inhibit people from receiving compassion from themselves and others (Montero-Marin et al., Reference Montero-Marin, Kuyken, Crane, Gu, Baer, Al-Awamleh, Akutsu, Araya-Véliz, Ghorbani, Chen and Kim2018; Steindl et al., Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020; Kariyawasam et al., Reference Kariyawasam, Ononaiye, Irons, Stopa and Kirby2021), due to eastern cultural norms discouraging help-seeking behaviour, as seeking help is considered as a failure that brings shame to one and those around oneself (Kee, Reference Kee2004). Thus, it seems fair to propose that whilst the Buddhist religion encourages compassion, the collectivistic cultural dynamic may be a factor that inhibits people's compassionate experiences. However, only a few studies have looked at self-compassion in a cross-cultural Asian context (Neff et al., Reference Neff, Pisitsungkagarn and Hsieh2008; Birkett, Reference Birkett2013), implying the need for further research.

So far, studies exploring the three flows of compassion in the Asian context remain to be very limited (Asano et al., Reference Asano, Kotera, Tsuchiya, Ishimura, Lin, Matsumoto, Matos, Basran and Gilbert2020). It is also noteworthy that several Asian people feel that Western theories are only applicable to people living in the West, as they believe that compared to Westerners, they have been through, and continue to face, more tragedy and pain in their daily living (Wong, Reference Wong2021). For example, Sri Lanka is a multi-ethnic, multi-cultural collectivistic South Asian island, where almost 70% of the population practice Buddhism (De Zoysa, Reference De Zoysa2021). Sri Lankans have, however, experienced several catastrophes such as a civil war and tsunami over the past few decades, and report high rates of grief, domestic violence, learned helplessness, alcohol abuse, self-harming and attempted suicides (World Health Organisation, 2018), depression, anxiety and post-traumatic stress disorder (Gunaratnam et al., Reference Gunaratnam, Gunaratnam and Somasundaram2003). In view of this, it is presumed that Sri Lankan people may benefit from compassion cultivation.

Rationale for the current study

In consideration of the aforementioned, including the proposal that compassion is at least partially determined by culture, cross-cultural explorations remain at an infancy stage (Montero-Marin et al., Reference Montero-Marin, Kuyken, Crane, Gu, Baer, Al-Awamleh, Akutsu, Araya-Véliz, Ghorbani, Chen and Kim2018). Furthermore, as one's level of compassion is determined by specific cultural practices that are more nuanced than a simple East-West contrast (Neff et al., Reference Neff, Pisitsungkagarn and Hsieh2008), there is an apparent research gap on cross-cultural compassion.

This study aimed to compare the three flows of compassion (self-compassion, compassion to and from others) between Sri Lankan and UK participants, to explore cross-cultural similarities and differences in the compassion constructs. Additionally, this research investigated which of the inhibitors of compassion (of fears of compassion, self-criticism and external shame), and facilitators of compassion (of self-reassurance and social safeness) and psychopathology (depression and anxiety) have the biggest impact on predicting each of the three flow of compassion within a cross-cultural perspective. Due to the scarcity of cross-cultural studies and ambiguity of the theoretical associations of the concepts discussed above (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2005a; Neff et al., Reference Neff, Pisitsungkagarn and Hsieh2008; López et al., Reference López, Sanderman, Ranchor and Schroevers2018), no firm directional hypotheses were constructed.

Method

Research design

This study used a cross-sectional, between-participants, questionnaire-based exploratory quantitative research design.

Participants

Participants were either UK or Sri Lankan nationals, and at least 18 years old. Participants were required to self-identify their nationality, and all participants had to be fluent in English language. The final sample comprised 300 UK and 149 Sri Lankans.

Measures

Demographic information on age, gender, religion and nationality was obtained. In addition, the following measures were administered in English.

Compassionate Engagement and Action Scales (CEAS: Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Catarino, Duarte, Matos, Kolts, Stubbs, Ceresatto, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia and Basran2017) measured participants' compassionate engagement and action in the three flows: self-compassion (engagement α = 0.77, action α = 0.90), compassion to others (engagement α = 0.90, action α = 0.94) and compassion from others (engagement α = 0.89, action α = 0.91), with 13 items measuring each flow. Answers ranged on a Likert-scale from 1 (never) to 10 (always).

Fears of Compassion Scales (FOCS: Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Matos and Rivis2011) measured the fears of self-compassion (15 items), compassion from others (13 items) and compassion to others (10 items) on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (don't agree at all) to 4 (completely agree). This scale indicated a good reliability for all three items (α = 0.85 for fear of self-compassion, α = 0.87 for fear of compassion from others and α = 0.78 for fear of compassion to others).

The Forms of Self-Criticising/Attacking and Self-Reassuring Scale (FSCRS: Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Clarke, Hempel, Miles and Irons2004) assessed self-criticism and self-reassurance on three dimensions: inadequate self, hated self and reassured self. It is a 22-item Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all like me) to 4 (extremely like me) and is designed to measure people's thoughts and feelings about themselves in times of distress. A good reliability has been reported for all three dimensions (e.g. α = 0.90 for inadequate self, α = 0.86 for hated self, and α = 0.86 for reassured self).

The Others as Shamer Scale (OAS: Allan et al., Reference Allan, Gilbert and Goss1994) tested participants' perception of how others see them, referred to as external shame. This is an 18 item, 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always), with a high internal consistency of α = 0.96.

The Social Safeness and Pleasure Scale (SSPS: Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Mitra, Ritcher, Franks, Mills, Bellew and Gale2009) measured the extent to which people perceive their social world as safe and warm. This 12-item scale ranging from 0 (almost never) to 4 (almost all the time) has acquired a high alpha value of α = 0.92.

Finally, anxiety and depression were assessed using the Generalised Anxiety Disorder-7 scale: (GAD-7: Spitzer et al., Reference Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams and Löwe2006), and Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9: Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001), respectively. Both scales are scored on a Likert scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) and have obtained an excellent internal reliability of α = 0.89 (Löwe et al., Reference Löwe, Decker, Müller, Brähler, Schellberg, Herzog and Herzberg2008).

Procedure

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Southampton (ID: 52533.A1). Participants were conveniently recruited from multiple online platforms (e.g. Facebook and Linkedin). A series of questionnaires including a demographic questionnaire, CEAS, FOCS, FSCRS, OAS, SSPS, GAD-7 and PHQ-9 were presented respectively, after obtaining participants' informed consent.

Data analysis plan

Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) tested the first aim to determine whether there were differences between the Sri Lankan and UK groups in their three flows of compassion and inhibitors and facilitators (using scores of FOCS, FSCRS, OAS, SSPS, PHQ-9 and GAD-7), controlling for age and gender. Six hierarchical multiple linear regressions (one for each flow of compassion in each country) were then conducted between the two groups, to test the second aim and exploring similarities and differences in the predictors of compassion (scores of FOCS, FSCRS, OAS, SSPS, PHQ-9 and GAD-7 as predictors). In the first block, religion, age and gender were entered so that the demographics could be controlled for. Depression and anxiety scores were controlled in the second block. The final block contained all the controlled variables and the remaining scales (FOCS, FSCRS, OAS and SSPS).

Results

There were more females (n = 97, 65% Sri Lankan; n = 272, 91% UK) compared to males (n = 52, 35% Sri Lankan; n = 27, 9% UK) in both samples [χ2= (1, N = 448) = 45.819, p < 0.001]. Sri Lankans were significantly older than the UK participants [t(447) = 6.784, p < 0.05], with the ages ranging from 18 to 50 years in Sri Lankans (M = 24.82, s.d. = 4.70) and 18–62 years in the UK participants (M = 20.95, s.d. = 6.11). Chi-squared for religion was significant, χ2 = (7, N = 449) = 333.320, p < 0.001. Majority of the Sri Lankans were Buddhists (74%) and the majority of the UK sample identified themselves as atheists (62%). As the significant differences in the demographic factors (religion, age and gender) could potentially affect the overall results, these factors were controlled.

Aim 1: testing compassion and, inhibitors and facilitators of compassion between Sri Lankan and UK samples

ANCOVA tests were conducted to determine if there would be a difference between the Sri Lankan and UK groups on their levels of compassion and associated inhibitors and facilitators, controlling for the demographics (see Table 1). The Sri Lankan group reported higher self-compassion and self-reassurance compared to the UK group, although of inhibitors, they also reported higher fears across all three flows of compassion and perceived external shame. In contrast, the UK group indicated greater levels of social safeness. No significant differences were found for compassion to and from others, and depression and anxiety between the two groups.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations and ANCOVA results

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

The results were in line with a cross-cultural study that found that Singaporean participants were more self-compassionate compared to the Australian participants (Steindl et al., Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020). They also found that although people from the collectivistic Singaporean background were expected to show higher compassion to others, the Australian participants were not only more likely to receive compassion from others, but also more compassionate to others than the Singaporeans. Similarly to the current study, Singaporean participants also indicated a greater fear of compassion towards others (Steindl et al., Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020). These distinctions will be discussed in the discussion section.

Aim 2: predictors of the three flows of compassion in the UK and Sri Lankan samples

Predictors of self-compassion in Sri Lankan and UK participants

A hierarchical multiple linear regression was carried out to predict self-compassion based on the subscales of FOC, FSCRS, OAS and SSPS scales whilst controlling for religion, age, gender, anxiety and depression. In the Sri Lankan participants, a significant regression equation [F(14, 134) = 8.88, p < 0.001] was resulted in an R 2 of 0.48 (Table 2). Following Buddhism, being older in age, high self-reassurance and lack of fear of self-compassion predicted greater self-compassion. Results implied that Sri Lankan participants who were less fearful of showing self-compassion were more self-reassured and therefore, more self-compassionate.

Table 2. Regression results for predictors of self-compassion in Sri Lankan participants

Note. sr2: small effect size = 0.02, medium effect size = 0.15, large effect size = 0.35. SEB: standard error of B, FCTO: fear of compassion to others, FCFO: fear of compassion from others, FSC: fear of self-compassion.

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

In the UK participants, a significant regression equation [F(14, 284) = 14.73, p < 0.001] resulted in an R 2 of 0.42, for self-compassion (Table 3). Higher self-reassurance and external shame predicted self-compassion in UK participants with small–medium and small-effect sizes, respectively.

Table 3. Regression results for predictors of self-compassion in UK participants

Note. sr2: small effect size = 0.02, medium effect size = 0.15, large effect size = 0.35.

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

The significant but positive multivariate relationship between self-compassion and higher external shame in UK participants is striking as a positive relationship is inconsistent with the existing literature (Allan et al., Reference Allan, Gilbert and Goss1994; Ferreira et al., Reference Ferreira, Pinto-Gouveia and Duarte2013), which strongly reports external shame as an inhibitor of self-compassion. This is also a change in direction from the zero-order correlation between self-compassion and external shame, which was significantly negative, with a medium-large effect size (r = −0.39), consistent with the previous literature. Thus, any possible explanations for this significant directional change were further explored. A simple mediation analyses using PROCESS indicated that self-reassurance significantly mediated the relationship between shame and self-compassion. In step 1 of the mediation model, the regression of perceived external shame on self-compassion, ignoring the mediator, was significantly negative, b = −0.35, t(298) = −7.38, p < 0.001. This meant that self-compassion was lower if the perceived shame was high. Step 2 showed that the regression of external shame on the mediator, self-reassurance, was also significantly negative, suggesting that when participants' perceived shame was high, their levels of self-reassurance was low, b = −0.27, t(298) = −13.81, p < 0.001. Step 3 of the mediation process, however, showed that the mediator (self-reassurance), controlling for external shame, was significantly positive, indicating that participants were more self-compassionate, when they were more self-reassured, b = 1.23, t(297) = 10.80, p = 0.000. As a result, step 4 of the analyses revealed that, controlling for the mediator (self-reassurance), external shame was not a significant predictor of self-compassion, b = −0.00, t(297) = −0.0354, p = 0.9718 in the UK participants. Thus, results explained that although higher external shame inhibits self-compassion, the significantly higher levels of self-reassurance in the UK group meant that their self-compassion was high even in the presence of higher external shame.

Predictors of compassion to others in Sri Lankan and UK participants

A hierarchical multiple linear regression indicated a significant regression F(14, 134) = 3.76, p < 0.001, with an R 2 of 0.28, for predictors of offering compassion to others in Sri Lankans (Table 4). Participants with greater fears of self-compassion were less likely to show others compassion, with higher self-inadequacy predicting higher compassion towards others.

Table 4. Regression results for predictors of compassion to others in Sri Lankan participants

Note. sr2: small effect size = 0.02, medium effect size = 0.15, large effect size = 0.35.

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

In the UK participants, a significant regression [F(14, 284) = 2.80, p < 0.001] with an R 2 of 0.12 was found for offering compassion to others (Table 5). UK participants were more likely to be compassionate towards others, if they were female, less fearful of offering giving compassion to others, and more anxious.

Table 5. Regression results for predictors of compassion to others in UK participants

Note. sr2: small effect size = 0.02, medium effect size = 0.15, large effect size = 0.35.

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

Predictors of compassion from others in Sri Lankan and UK participants

A similar linear regression indicated (Table 6) a significant regression [F(14, 134) = 2.73, p < 0.001] with an R 2 of 0.22 in Sri Lankan participants. Females were more likely to receive compassion from others, whilst higher social safeness also predicted compassion from others in Sri Lankan participants.

Table 6. Regression results for predictors of compassion from others in Sri Lankan participants

Note. sr2: small effect size = 0.02, medium effect size = 0.15, large effect size = 0.35.

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

A significant regression equation [F(14, 284) = 15.54, p < 0.001] with an R 2 of 0.43 was found for compassion from others (Table 7) in UK participants. Being younger, lack of fear of receiving others' compassion, low external shame, lower depression, higher social safeness and higher anxiety all predicted compassion from others, in the UK participants (Tables 6 and 7).

Table 7. Regression results for predictors of compassion from others in UK participants

Note. sr2: small effect size = 0.02, medium effect size = 0.15, large effect size = 0.35.

Variable that has a significant p value of (<.05) has been presented in bold.

Discussion

This study investigated the differences and similarities between the three flows of compassion (self-compassion, compassion to others and compassion from others), and inhibitors (fear of self-compassion, fear of compassion to others, fear of compassion from others, self-criticism and external shame), facilitators of compassion (self-reassurance and social safeness) and psychopathology (depression and anxiety) between a cross-cultural sample of Sri Lankan and UK participants. In comparison with the UK participants, Sri Lankan participants indicated higher levels of self-compassion, self-reassurance, external shame and fears of compassion (when controlling for age and gender). In contrast, the UK participants reported higher social safeness.

This study also explored individual predictors of the three flows of compassion for each country, as this may be helpful in adapting interventions with cultural sensitivity. When controlling for religion, age, gender, anxiety and depression, some similar and some different predictors were identified for each flow of compassion, as discussed below.

Self-compassion

The significantly higher levels of self-compassion in Sri Lankans may be explained by the fact that 74% of them were Buddhists, compared to the 1% in the UK group, in which 62% of the UK participants self-identified as atheists. When testing the second hypothesis, the multiple regressions also indicated that following Buddhism strongly predicted self-compassion in Sri Lankans. Buddhist compassion teaches that one should fully cultivate self-compassion, prior to practising it on others (Salzberg, Reference Salzberg2012; Bhikkhu, Reference Bhikkhu2018). Thus, this study suggested that the strong Buddhist influence on self-compassion might at least partially explain the cross-cultural difference in the two groups. In addition to following Buddhism, older age and lack of fear of self-compassion predicted self-compassion in Sri Lankan participants. Hwang et al. (Reference Hwang, Kim, Yang and Yang2016) found that middle-aged adults, as compared to young adults in Japan, practiced self-compassion as a more vital construct towards leading a prosperous and psychologically healthy life.

Similar to the current study, Steindl et al. (Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020) also found higher self-compassion in the Singaporean sample compared to the Australian sample. Although one would presume people from individualistic backgrounds (e.g. Australia) to be more self-compassionate (Steindl et al., Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020), Montero-Marin et al. (Reference Montero-Marin, Kuyken, Crane, Gu, Baer, Al-Awamleh, Akutsu, Araya-Véliz, Ghorbani, Chen and Kim2018) emphasised that the individualistic social dynamic could suppress self-compassion due to the high levels of competition-based motives, social comparisons and possibly higher self-criticism. Gilbert et al. (Reference Gilbert, Catarino, Duarte, Matos, Kolts, Stubbs, Ceresatto, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia and Basran2017) also found that self-compassion and self-reassurance were significantly higher in a collectivistic Portuguese student sample in comparison with the UK and USA student samples. In consideration of individualistic communities, a study conducted among the UK students found that some participants perceived self-compassion as a self-indulgent construct (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Matos and Rivis2011). Thus, prospective research should explore whether the lower self-compassion in the UK group was due to a belief that self-compassion should not be in one's best interest (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Mayer, Allen, Terry, Chilton and Leary2016).

Interestingly, self-reassurance predicted self-compassion among participants in both countries. Whilst compassion is a sensitivity to suffering with the motivation to relieve that suffering, self-reassurance possesses the ability to soothe or reassure oneself during times of distress (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Clarke, Hempel, Miles and Irons2004). Thus, self-compassion and self-reassurance have indicated strong correlations (Hermanto and Zuroff, Reference Hermanto and Zuroff2016), which is unsurprising as self-reassurance buffers depression and self-criticism, both of which have shown negative correlations with self-compassion (Petrocchi et al., Reference Petrocchi, Dentale and Gilbert2019). In support, Gilbert et al. (Reference Gilbert, Catarino, Duarte, Matos, Kolts, Stubbs, Ceresatto, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia and Basran2017) found that self-compassion and self-reassurance were significantly higher in a Portuguese sample, which also indicated the lowest depression and anxiety scores in comparison with the UK and USA samples.

The results of this study indicated that in the UK group, higher perceived shame predicted higher self-compassion. This positive relationship is theoretically contradicting as the literature suggests that one's experiences of themselves as living negatively in the minds of others (external shame) is strongly correlated with low self-compassion and increased psychopathology (Ferreira et al., Reference Ferreira, Pinto-Gouveia and Duarte2013). Therefore, in the context of societal shame, people may internalise that shame and become more self-critical as opposed to being self-compassionate (Matos et al., Reference Matos, Duarte, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia, Petrocchi, Basran and Gilbert2017). It was surprising that the UK participants' self-compassion was predicted by higher perceived external shame. Thus, further exploratory mediational analyses were conducted, which suggested that the higher self-reassurance in fact, explains how the negative direction between shame and self-compassion can turn into a positive relationship. This is further evidence highlighting the potential vital role of self-reassurance, even in the presence of external shame, as a mechanism to increase self-compassion. The SMT (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2014b) also details that self-reassurance activates a self-to-self caregiving mentality during times of distress, which in turn encourages people to direct compassion inwardly towards themselves.

Compassion to others

Asian cultures are rich in interpersonal connectedness, social conformity and caring for others, compared to Western societies, such as the UK, that encourage individuality and autonomy (Markus and Kitayama, Reference Markus and Kitayama1991; Gardner et al., Reference Gardner, Gabriel and Lee1999). Thus, one would expect Sri Lankans to be more compassionate towards others, given that they were also more self-compassionate. However, there was no significant difference in the levels of compassion to others between the two groups. Steindl et al. (Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020) also found that Australians were more compassionate towards others compared to the Singaporeans, who come from a collectivistic society. Although they expected that the collectivistic social dynamic would encourage compassion towards others, the results led them to believe that the compassion offered in such cultures may be ‘submissive’ than ‘genuine’. In other words, when the compassion is referred to as submissive, it implies that the motive of the compassion given is based on obligation or submission, and possibly due to a fear of not being liked or valued if the compassion is not offered (Catarino et al., Reference Catarino, Gilbert, McEwan and Baiao2014). Sri Lankans in this study also indicated higher fears of compassion to others which may explain the non-significance in the results.

Contradictorily, however, Gilbert et al. (Reference Gilbert, Catarino, Duarte, Matos, Kolts, Stubbs, Ceresatto, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia and Basran2017) found that Portuguese participants from a collectivistic background indicated significantly higher levels of compassion across all three flows of compassion, which was also reflected in their significantly low levels of depression and anxiety, compared to participants from the UK and USA samples. Although the three flows of compassion were related in Gilbert et al.'s study, other studies indicated that self-compassion and compassion to others may not be correlated (Neff and Pommier, Reference Neff and Pommier2013; López et al., Reference López, Sanderman, Ranchor and Schroevers2018) and that self-compassion is independent from developing compassion towards others (Abele and Wojciszke, Reference Abele and Wojciszke2007).

In the current study, Sri Lankans' likelihood of compassion towards others was predicted by greater fear of self-compassion and self-inadequacy. It is possible that people who feel inadequately about themselves have greater sympathy for the suffering of others and therefore, develop more compassion, in the same way having high anxiety is linked to developing sympathetic considerations towards others (Gambin and Sharp, Reference Gambin and Sharp2016). This may also explain why Sri Lankans expressed compassion to others, even when they were fearful of showing themselves compassion. Cross cultural studies have identified compassion to others as a submissive function in Asian people (Catarino et al., Reference Catarino, Gilbert, McEwan and Baiao2014), and implied that people submissively show compassion to others, in order to avoid being rejected, although this may not increase their life satisfaction (Asano et al., Reference Asano, Kotera, Tsuchiya, Ishimura, Lin, Matsumoto, Matos, Basran and Gilbert2020). Thus, a plausible explanation of fear of self-compassion predicting higher compassion towards others in the Sri Lankan group may be that, despite the fear of treating themselves with compassion, they may have felt compelled to offer it to others, to avoid social rejection. Previous studies identified that some Sri Lankans offer compassion to others, out of obligatory and submissive reasons (Kariyawasam et al., Reference Kariyawasam, Ononaiye, Irons, Stopa and Kirby2021).

In the UK participants, higher anxiety, being female and lack of fear of compassion to others predicted compassion towards others. In consideration of the gender difference, Western studies (Sprecher and Fehr, Reference Sprecher and Fehr2005) found that the nurturing and caring tendencies in females increased their compassion to others. Fear of compassion is known to inhibit compassion and stem from insecure attachments with others (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Matos and Rivis2011). Thus, it is unsurprising that the lack of fear predicted compassion to others in the UK participants, especially given that they reported higher social safeness.

Compassion from others

As with offering compassion, Sri Lankan participants were expected to experience higher compassion from others (Markus and Kitayama, Reference Markus and Kitayama1991; Pathirana, Reference Pathirana2016). However, there was no significant difference in compassion from others between the two groups. Steindl et al. (Reference Steindl, Yiu, Baumann and Matos2020) found higher compassion from others in Australian participants compared to that in Singaporean participants, concluding that the collectivistic nature and the perception of help seeking behaviour as being weak or shameful in the Asian communities may have resisted the Singaporean participants from seeking help or being open to receiving compassion from others. However, Gilbert et al. (Reference Gilbert, Catarino, Duarte, Matos, Kolts, Stubbs, Ceresatto, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia and Basran2017) found highest compassion from others in a Portuguese sample when compared to the UK and USA populations. Although Portugal is considered to have a collectivistic culture (Hofstede, Reference Hofstede2011), the higher density of the ‘shame’ component in Asian countries should be explored further, to determine to understand this cultural distinction.

Social safeness predicted compassion from others in both groups. Previous studies also propose that social safeness mediates the capacity to receiving compassion (Kelly and Dupasquier, Reference Kelly and Dupasquier2016) and that the lack of social safeness increases trust issues and the perception that others are judgemental and rejecting (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2014a). Thus, results suggest that participants perceived others as compassionate givers when they felt safe within their social relationships, as social safeness increases affiliative interactions with others (Kelly and Dupasquier, Reference Kelly and Dupasquier2016), which in turn activates the soothing system (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2014a).

Furthermore, being younger in age predicted compassion from others in the UK participants, together with high levels of anxiety, low levels of depression, lack of external shame and lack of fear of receiving compassion from others. It feels fair to address whether younger participants may have experienced stronger parental attachments and connections with the society, which may explain the higher perception of compassion from others. In fact, warm parental relationships enhance the soothing system and increase social safeness (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Berntson, Sheridan and McClintock2000). Future studies on student populations should, therefore, investigate participants' relationship with their parents, for a better understanding of this phenomenon. The current study also noted that participants who were less depressed were more likely to perceive higher compassion from others. Self-critical and depressed people tend to show a lack of ability to receive affection and compassion from others (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1982) and resist compassion, even when it is offered (Gilbert and Procter, Reference Gilbert and Procter2006). This also implies that people who are depressed and self-critical may perceive others as not compassionate as a way of resisting compassion from others. Anxiety is positively associated with affective empathy and sympathy (Gambin and Sharp, Reference Gambin and Sharp2016), indicating that people with anxiety may also have increased considerations towards the suffering of others.

Another cultural difference was that being female in the Sri Lankan group, compared to males, was a predictor of compassion from others, which is supported by females' natural propensity towards engaging compassionately with others (Stellar et al., Reference Stellar, Manzo, Kraus and Keltner2012; Neff and Pommier, Reference Neff and Pommier2013). It is noteworthy that there was no gender difference in the UK group, which raises the question whether the autonomous social background may have prevented UK females from seeking compassion, leading them to believe that others are not compassionate towards them. In fact, distance is identified as a positive cultural value in the UK, which links with respect for individual autonomy (Paxman, Reference Paxman2007). The impact of autonomy on seeking compassion, therefore, needs further exploration.

Strengths and limitations

This was the first cross-cultural study to investigate the three flows of compassion in a Sri Lankan sample in comparison with a UK sample. This was also the first study exploring Gilbert's (Reference Gilbert2014a) SMT in a Sri Lankan population using a questionnaire-based approach. The use of a series of validated measures and the detailed explanation of the nuances of the three flows of compassion further strengthened the overall study quality. This study also contributed to the understanding of the seemingly strong influence of Buddhism on self-compassion, signifying the importance of further explorations to integrate these vital elements when conducting compassion-based interventions. Additionally, future research may incorporate measures to investigate the associations between religion, culture, well-being and the different components of compassion to further understand these factors as potential facilitators/inhibitors of compassion.

In consideration of the weaknesses, as this study was cross-sectional, drawing conclusions on the causality of compassion, inhibitors and facilitators was problematic (Matos et al., Reference Matos, Duarte, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia, Petrocchi, Basran and Gilbert2017). Importantly, as data collection continued during the COVID-19 pandemic (Jia et al., Reference Jia, Ayling, Chalder, Massey, Broadbent, Coupland and Vedhara2020), it is possible that compassion levels varied from the usual levels in all participants, especially in the UK group due to the high-pandemic impact at the time of data collection. This may also explain the non-significance in depression and anxiety between the two countries. Although studies have found people in collectivistic countries to be more distressed than people in the West (Birkett, Reference Birkett2013), the alarming situation of the pandemic in the UK might suggest that the UK participants' depression and anxiety levels may have been higher than usual. In fact, a UK study found that during the pandemic, depression and anxiety exceeded the population norms especially among young people (Jia et al., Reference Jia, Ayling, Chalder, Massey, Broadbent, Coupland and Vedhara2020), who comprised most of the UK sample of this study.

Although religion was accounted as a possible variable of cross-cultural differences observed in the results, this study did not assess differences in cultural or group norms (e.g. collectivistic/individualistic), and other cultural practices which may have had an impact on the overall study results. Furthermore, the study was mostly advertised among university students, limiting the sample to young people in both countries which limits the generalisability of this research. This study was also conducted in English language, allowing only English-speaking Sri Lankans to participate.

Clinical implications and recommendations for future research

This study aimed to facilitate an enhanced cross-cultural understanding of compassion across its three flows (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Catarino, Duarte, Matos, Kolts, Stubbs, Ceresatto, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia and Basran2017), which is predominantly a Western approach. Considering the impact of compassion-based approaches (e.g. CFT) on reduced levels of anxiety and depression in Western communities (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Matos and Rivis2011), the application of these approaches cross-culturally (e.g. Sri Lanka) may result in increased well-being in people around the world. The findings tentatively suggest that compassion-based trainings may be helpful, but also identified that cultural differences should be considered when tailoring individual treatments. It appears that religion, Buddhism in particular in this study, and other demographic factors should be accounted for. This is vital in therapeutic contexts as these may be useful protective factors in enhancing a person's well-being. When implementing interventions such as CFT, clinicians should also address inhibitors such as fears of offering and receiving compassion, and negative shame-based emotional experiences (such as self-criticism and external shame) stimulating such fears, in order to assist self-generating compassion and reception towards compassion in clients (Matos et al., Reference Matos, Duarte, Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia, Petrocchi, Basran and Gilbert2017).

Conclusion

This study identified that cultural differences and similarities were present between UK and Sri Lankan participants in their levels of self-compassion, compassion to and from others. Sri Lankans were significantly more self-reassured and self-compassionate, although they also reported higher external shame and fears of compassion. UK participants found more safeness in others, despite their individualistic social dynamic (Gardner et al., Reference Gardner, Gabriel and Lee1999). Regardless of the cultural differences, those who felt highly self-reassured were more self-compassionate. Buddhism predicted greater self-compassion in Sri Lankans although external shame and insecure attachments inhibited their compassionate experiences with others. Overall, this study signified the importance of paying close attention to cultural and religious influences when exploring compassion across cultures. Irrespective of the individualistic-independent and collectivistic-interdependent cultural context, this study highlighted the potential role that significant others and society play in one's level of compassion and well-being. As compassion cultivation across all three flows has resulted in increased well-being and reduced psychological distress (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2005a), it is vital that differences between countries are considered when introducing Western psychotherapeutic approaches into non-Western settings such as Sri Lanka.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Abele, A and Wojciszke, B (2007) Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93, 751763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allan, S, Gilbert, P and Goss, K (1994) An exploration of shame measures – II: psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences 17, 719722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asano, K, Kotera, Y, Tsuchiya, M, Ishimura, I, Lin, S, Matsumoto, Y, Matos, M, Basran, J and Gilbert, P (2020) The development of the Japanese version of the compassionate engagement and action scales. PLoS ONE 15, e0230875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhikkhu, A (2018) Ekottarika-agama discourse without parallels. Buddhist Studies Review 35, 125134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkett, MA (2013) Self-compassion and empathy across cultures: comparison of young adults in China and the United States. International Journal of Research Studies in Psychology 3, 2534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J (1982) Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 52, 664678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT, Berntson, GG, Sheridan, JF and McClintock, MK (2000) Multilevel integrative analyses of human behavior: social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social and biological approaches. Psychological Bulletin 126, 829843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castilho, P, Pinto-Gouveia, J and Duarte, J (2013) Exploring self-criticism: confirmatory factor analysis of the FSCRS in clinical and nonclinical samples. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 22, 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catarino, F, Gilbert, P, McEwan, K and Baiao, R (2014) Compassion motivations: distinguishing submissive compassion from genuine compassion and its association with shame, submissive behavior, depression, anxiety and stress. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 33, 399412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheon, B, Im, D, Harada, T, Kim, J, Mathur, V, Scimeca, J, Parrish, T, Park, H and Chiao, J (2011) Cultural influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy. NeuroImage 57, 642650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, RJ and Harrington, A (2002) Visions of Compassion: Western Scientists and Tibetan Buddhists Examine Human Nature. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Zoysa, P (2021) Psychology in Sri Lanka. [online] Academia.edu. Available at https://www.academia.edu/13013805/Psychology_in_Sri_Lanka (accessed 23 November 2021).Google Scholar
Ferreira, C, Pinto-Gouveia, J and Duarte, C (2013) Self-compassion in the face of shame and body image dissatisfaction: implications for eating disorders. Eating Behaviors 14, 207210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B, Cohn, M, Coffey, K, Pek, J and Finkel, S (2008) Open hearts build lives: positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95, 10451062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gambin, M and Sharp, C (2016) The differential relations between empathy and internalizing and externalizing symptoms in inpatient adolescents. Child Psychiatry & Human Development 47, 966974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, W, Gabriel, S and Lee, A (1999) ‘I’ value freedom, but ‘we’ value relationships: self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science 10, 321326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P (1998) What is shame? Some core issues and controversies. In Gilbert, P and Andrews, B (eds), Shame: Interpersonal Behavior, Psychopathology, and Culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 338.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P (2000) Social mentalities: internal ‘social’ conflicts and the role of inner warmth and compassion in cognitive therapy. In Gilbert, P and Bailey, KG (eds), Genes on the Couch: Explorations in Evolutionary Psychotherapy. Brunner-Routledge, pp. 118150.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P (2005 a) Compassion: Conceptualisations, Research and Use in Psychotherapy. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P (2005 b) Compassion and cruelty: a biopsychosocial approach. In Gilbert, P (ed.) Compassion: Conceptualisations, Research and Use in Psychotherapy. London, UK: Brunner-Routledge, pp. 974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P (2007) The evolution of shame as a marker for relationship security; A biopsychosocial approach.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P (2009) Introducing compassion-focused therapy. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment 15, 199208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P (2014 a) The origins and nature of compassion focused therapy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 53, 641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P (2014 b) Social Mentalities: Internal ‘social’ Conflict and the Role of Inner Warmth and Compassion in Cognitive Therapy. In Genes on the couch. Routledge, pp. 128160.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P (2015) An evolutionary approach to emotion in mental health with a focus on affiliative emotions. Emotion Review 7(3), 230237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P (2016) Human Nature and Suffering. Oxfordshire, England: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P (n.d.) The Compassionate Mind: A New Approach to Life's Challenges. London: Constable.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P and Irons, C (2005) Focused therapies and compassionate mind training for shame and self-attacking.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P and Procter, S (2006) Compassionate mind training for people with high shame and self-criticism: overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy: An International Journal of Theory & Practice 13, 353379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P, Clarke, M, Hempel, S, Miles, JN and Irons, C (2004) Criticizing and reassuring oneself: an exploration of forms, styles and reasons in female students. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, 3150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P, McEwan, K, Mitra, R, Franks, L, Richter, A and Rockliff, H (2008) Feeling safe and content: a specific affect regulation system? Relationship to depression, anxiety, stress, and self-criticism. The Journal of Positive Psychology 3, 182191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P, McEwan, K, Mitra, R, Ritcher, A, Franks, L, Mills, A, Bellew, R and Gale, C (2009) An exploration of different types of positive affect in students and in patients with bipolar disorder. Clinical Neuropathology: Journal of Treatment Evaluation 6, 135143.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P, McEwan, K, Matos, M and Rivis, A (2011) Fears of compassion: development of three self-report measures. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 84, 239255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P, McEwan, K, Catarino, F and Baião, R (2014) Fears of negative emotions in relation to fears of happiness, compassion, alexithymia and psychopathology in a depressed population: a preliminary study.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P, Catarino, F, Duarte, C, Matos, M, Kolts, R, Stubbs, J, Ceresatto, L, Duarte, J, Pinto-Gouveia, J and Basran, J (2017) The development of compassionate engagement and action scales for self and others. Journal of Compassionate Health Care 4, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunaratnam, HR, Gunaratnam, S and Somasundaram, D (2003) The psychosocial effects of landmines in Jaffna. Medicine, Conflict and Survival 19, 223234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermanto, N and Zuroff, DC (2016) The social mentality theory of self-compassion and self-reassurance: the interactive effect of care-seeking and caregiving. The Journal of Social Psychology 156, 523535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermanto, N, Zuroff, DC, Kopala-Sibley, DC, Kelly, AC, Matos, M, Gilbert, P and Koestner, R (2016) Ability to receive compassion from others buffers the depressogenic effect of self-criticism: a cross-cultural multi-study analysis. Personality and Individual Differences 98, 324332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G (2011) Dimensionalizing cultures: the Hofstede model in context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture 2, 2307–0919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, S, Kim, G, Yang, JW and Yang, E (2016) The moderating effects of age on the relationships of self-compassion, self-esteem, and mental health. Japanese Psychological Research 58, 194205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, R, Ayling, K, Chalder, T, Massey, A, Broadbent, E, Coupland, C and Vedhara, K (2020) Mental health in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic: cross-sectional analyses from a community cohort study. BMJ Open 10, e040620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kariyawasam, L, Ononaiye, M, Irons, C, Stopa, L and Kirby, SE (2021) Views and experiences of compassion in Sri Lankan students: an exploratory qualitative study. PLoS ONE 16: e0260475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kee, CHY (2004) Cultural features as advantageous to therapy: a Singaporean perspective. Journal of Systemic Therapies 23, 6779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, AC and Dupasquier, J (2016) Social safeness mediates the relationship between recalled parental warmth and the capacity for self-compassion and receiving compassion. Personality and Individual Differences 89, 157161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S and Markus, HR (2000) The pursuit of happiness and the realization of sympathy: cultural patterns of self, social relations, and well-being. Culture and Subjective Well-Being 1, 113161.Google Scholar
Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL and Williams, JB (2001) The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine 16, 606613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lama, D and Vreeland, N (2008) An Open Heart: Practicing Compassion in Everyday Life. Boston: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Leighton, TD (2012) Faces of Compassion: Classic Bodhisattva Archetypes and Their Modern Expression – An Introduction to Mahayana Buddhism. New York: USA: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
López, A, Sanderman, R, Ranchor, AV and Schroevers, MJ (2018) Compassion for others and self-compassion: levels, correlates, and relationship with psychological well-being. Mindfulness 9, 325331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Löwe, B, Decker, O, Müller, S, Brähler, E, Schellberg, D, Herzog, W and Herzberg, PY (2008) Validation and standardization of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Screener (GAD-7) in the general population. Medical Care 46(3), 266274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, A, Greischar, LL, Rawlings, NB, Ricard, M and Davidson, RJ (2004) Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 1636916373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacBeth, A and Gumley, A (2012) Exploring compassion: a meta-analysis of the association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review 32, 545552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, HR and Kitayama, S (1991) Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 98, 224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matos, M, Duarte, C, Duarte, J, Pinto-Gouveia, J, Petrocchi, N, Basran, J and Gilbert, P (2017) Psychological and physiological effects of compassionate mind training: a pilot randomised controlled study. Mindfulness 8, 16991712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesquita, B (2001) Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80, 68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montero-Marin, J, Kuyken, W, Crane, C, Gu, J, Baer, R, Al-Awamleh, AA, Akutsu, S, Araya-Véliz, C, Ghorbani, N, Chen, ZJ and Kim, MS (2018) Self-compassion and cultural values: a cross-cultural study of self-compassion using a multitrait-multimethod (MTMM) analytical procedure. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 2638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neff, KD (2011) Self-compassion, self-esteem, and well-being. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 5, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neff, KD and Pommier, E (2013) The relationship between self-compassion and other-focused concern among college undergraduates, community adults, and practicing meditators. Self and Identity 12, 160176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neff, KD, Pisitsungkagarn, K and Hsieh, YP (2008) Self-compassion and self-construal in the United States, Thailand, and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 39, 267285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pathirana, BD (2016) Sri Lankan adolescents’ relationships with their parents, siblings and peers: an exploratory study. The International Journal of Indian Psychology 3, 3850.Google Scholar
Paxman, J (2007) The English. London, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Petrocchi, N, Dentale, F and Gilbert, P (2019) Self-reassurance, not self-esteem, serves as a buffer between self-criticism and depressive symptoms. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 92, 394406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porges, SW (2007) The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology 74, 116143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prebish, CS and Baumann, M (eds) (2002) Westward Dharma: Buddhism Beyond Asia. California, USA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rector, NA, Bagby, RM, Segal, ZV, Joffe, RT, and Levitt, A (2000) Self-criticism and dependency in depressed patients treated with cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy. Cognitive Therapy and Research 24, 571584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, KJ, Mayer, S, Allen, AB, Terry, M, Chilton, A and Leary, MR (2016) Resisting self-compassion: why are some people opposed to being kind to themselves? Self and Identity 15, 505524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salzberg, S (2012) The Force of Kindness: Change Your Life With Love & Compassion. Colorado, USA: Sounds True.Google Scholar
Sinclair, S, Torres, MB, Raffin-Bouchal, S, Hack, TF, McClement, S, Hagen, NA and Chochinov, HM (2016) Compassion training in healthcare: what are patients’ perspectives on training healthcare providers? BMC Medical Education 16, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R, Kroenke, K, Williams, J and Löwe, B (2006) A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of Internal Medicine 166, 1092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sprecher, S and Fehr, B (2005) Compassionate love for close others and humanity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 22, 629651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steindl, S, Yiu, R, Baumann, T and Matos, M (2020) Comparing compassion across cultures: similarities and differences among Australians and Singaporeans. Australian Psychologist 55, 208219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stellar, J, Manzo, V, Kraus, M and Keltner, D (2012) Class and compassion: socioeconomic factors predict responses to suffering. Emotion (Washington, D.C.) 12, 449459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triandis, H (1993) Collectivism and individualism as cultural syndromes. Cross-Cultural Research 27, 155180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, P (2021) The Nature and Practice of Compassion: Integrating Western and Eastern Positive Psychologies, Dr. Paul Wong. [online] Drpaulwong.com. Available at http://www.drpaulwong.com/the-nature-and-practice-of-compassion-integrating-western-and-eastern-positive-psychologies/ (accessed 22 November 2021).Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2018) Regional Strategy on Preventing Suicide. Regional Office for South East Asia: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means, standard deviations and ANCOVA results

Figure 1

Table 2. Regression results for predictors of self-compassion in Sri Lankan participants

Figure 2

Table 3. Regression results for predictors of self-compassion in UK participants

Figure 3

Table 4. Regression results for predictors of compassion to others in Sri Lankan participants

Figure 4

Table 5. Regression results for predictors of compassion to others in UK participants

Figure 5

Table 6. Regression results for predictors of compassion from others in Sri Lankan participants

Figure 6

Table 7. Regression results for predictors of compassion from others in UK participants