Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-495rp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-02T16:32:30.713Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Methodological issues in Rma etymology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 June 2024

Nathaniel Sims*
Affiliation:
University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper examines the state-of-the-art for the historical study of the Rma (Qiang) language (< Trans-Himalayan/Sino-Tibetan) and points out some methodological issues in earlier work. The paper discusses how vowel correspondences have been obfuscated by loanwords, onomatopoeic forms, and analogical levelling. It also discusses the analysis of compound forms and points out how certain compound forms have been incorrectly etymologized. It deals with broader, more fundamental issues in prior work such as top-down rather than bottom-up reconstructions, and problematic conceptualizations of what constitutes reconstructions. The article offers potential solutions to the issues discussed and points out where future work would be most profitable.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of SOAS University of London

1. Introduction

Rma (also called Qiang) is a Trans-HimalayanFootnote 1 language spoken in north-western Sichuan, China. Although Rma varieties are relatively well documented, issues of etymology and historical linguistics for Rma are still at an early stage.

The difficulties that the Rma varieties pose to the historical linguist are manifold. In addition to being internally diverse, unevenly documented, and without written tradition, Rma varieties as a whole are typified by radical phonological progressiveness. For example, while certain varieties preserve ancient complex onsets, all varieties have lost all codas from proto-Trans-Himalayan (Benedict Reference Benedict1983; LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 23, inter alia). These phonological innovations can obscure cognancy with other related languages, or even across varieties. Furthermore, Rma is spoken in a highly multilingual region and has been subject to areal influence from different Trans-Himalayan languages (Chirkova Reference Chirkova2012).

Advances in Rma historical-comparative work have also been hampered by issues external to the structure and history of the language and which stem from methodological problems with historical linguists working on Rma. To date, the most information about Rma etymology is to be found in Chang (Reference Chang1967),Footnote 2 Evans (Reference Evans2001), Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2003), and the Sino-Tibetan Etymological Dictionary and Thesaurus (hereafter STEDT). Some etymological notes are given by LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003) and Sims (Reference Sims2014) for the Ronghong and Yonghe varieties respectively. These contributions will be critically discussed throughout.

This paper is but a small part of the large task of building and improving upon earlier works on Rma historical linguistics. The aim here is not an exhaustive review of the Rma etymologies proposed so far, but to point out some general methodological tendencies in earlier work on Rma which may help the future historical linguist.Footnote 3

1.1. Outline

This paper is organized as follows. Section 2 concerns certain Rma vowel correspondences and illustrates how irregular forms may be explained. Section 3 discusses issues relating to the analysis of compounds. Section 4 discusses more general methodological issues with prior work and Section 5 gives some concluding remarks.

2. Vowel correspondences

This section addresses the vowel correspondences of Rma varieties. It examines the approach of Evans (Reference Evans2001) and puts forward an alternative approach.

2.1. Evans’ correspondences

Evans (Reference Evans2001) contains the most comprehensive study of Rma diachrony to date, with over 1,000 proposed correspondence sets for Rma. Nonetheless, there are methodological issues that undermine the usefulness of some of Evans’ reconstructions, especially for the vowels. In general, Evans (Reference Evans2001) identifies forms with the same vowel across varieties, reconstructs an identical vowel, and treats exceptions as exhibiting proto-variation. For example, Evans (Reference Evans2001: 176) states that Longxi Rma /o/ :: Mianchi Rma /o/ and that both reflect *o. Evans gives a small number of problematic sets (5) to support this claim. Evans (Reference Evans2001: 177) also states that Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /u/ and that both reflect *u. Again, there is a rather small number of problematic sets (5) given to support this claim. Nonetheless, Evans goes on to state that:

In spite of these correspondences, there are about forty-eight sets in which it is not possible to distinguish between PSQ [Proto-Southern Rma] *u and *o, because both /u, o/ are present in the supporting forms …. For most of these sets [sic] equivocal sets, the protovowel is determined by simple majority, or by peeking at the vowels present in Northern Qiang [Rma]. This alteration is not unheard of in other branches of TB. Benedict has observed that “the high medial vowels *u and *i of TB are well maintained in Tibetan, Kachin and Lushei, but partial or complete replacement by lower vowels (o ~ e ~ a) is characteristic of Burmese, Garo and many other TB languages”.

Table 1 gives a summary of correspondences for these vowels found in the forms reconstructed by Evans.

Table 1. Vowel correspondences found in Evans 2001

There are no fewer than eleven distinct correspondence patterns in Table 1, including three different, unpredictable, outcomes for both *u and *o, two for *u/o and, three for *o/u. Thus, the starred forms do not predict the attested forms. This approach is methodologically unsound, as reconstructions which do not predict the attested forms are not useful. As Hill (Reference Hill2012: 64) makes clear:

The goal of comparative linguistics is not the invention of unattested languages but rather the explanation of systematic relationships among attested languages; progress in reconstruction is a by-product of increasingly precise statements of such relationships.

Evans (Reference Evans2006a: 113) recognizes that “some revision of PSQ [Proto-Southern-Rma vowels] is needed”, but has not yet revised his reconstructions. The following section gives an alternative analysis that explains the relationship between these Longxi and Mianchi vowels.

2.2. The present analysis

I propose that Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/ and that Longxi /u/ corresponds with Mianchi /o/ for native forms. Other correspondence patterns are the result of borrowings from other languages or dialect mixing, onomatopoeia, or analogical levelling.

2.2.1. Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/

Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/. Evidence is given in Table 2. Note that Longxi H and L tones correspond to Mianchi H and L.Footnote 4

Table 2. Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/

This correspondence is validated by a regular correspondence with Ronghong /u/. Consider Table 3.

Table 3. Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/ :: Ronghong /u/

2.2.2. Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/

Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/. Consider the evidence in Table 4. Note again the regular tonal correspondences between Longxi and Mianchi.

Table 4. Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/

The correspondence is again confirmed by comparison with Ronghong. See Table 5 for examples.

Table 5. Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/ :: Ronghong /(w)ə/

Table 5 also suggests a change * > ə took place after bilabials in Ronghong. The vowel correspondences proposed for native forms are given in Table 6.

Table 6. Vowel correspondences for inherited forms

This raises the question, what about the other nine correspondence patterns in Table 1? The following section attempts to answer this question.

2.3. Irregularities

Loanwords introduce exceptions to regular sound correspondences. Tibetan and Chinese loans have long been known in Rma (Sun Reference Sun1988), but Rma-internal loans have not yet been identified.

2.3.1. Tibetan loans

Tibetan loans are more frequent in the Rma varieties of Heishui county (Liu Reference Liu1981), and also in Songpan County (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Baofeng, Mingjun and Xi2019). See Liu (Reference Liu1981) on Tibetan loans into Mawo Rma.

Table 7 gives three different forms which are probably Tibetan loanwords and shows how these forms have different vowel correspondences from those we find for inherited words.

Table 7. Probable Tibetan loans

Regarding “dragon”, Evans (Reference Evans2001: 235) notes that “It is probable that this is an ancient borrowing from Tibetan”,Footnote 5 and does not reconstruct a proto-form.

For “demon”, Evans (Reference Evans2001: 289) reconstructs *du L. LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 355) give the Ronghong form du-gu̻-mi “ghost, spirit” and note that mi = “eyes”.Footnote 6 The vowel correspondences are irregular and suggest that these forms are Tibetan loans.Footnote 7 The same can be said for “hole, cave”.Footnote 8 Recognizing the aforementioned Tibetan loans explains the otherwise exceptional vowel correspondences.

2.3.2. Chinese loans

Chinese loans have long been discussed in the literature (Sun Reference Sun1988; LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 46–7). Consider Longxi lò pú “radish” and Mianchi lò pú “radish”. These forms, both from Chinese 蘿蔔 luóbo “radish”, show irregular correspondence for both [o] and [u]. Evans (Reference Evans2001) discusses many instances of borrowings from Chinese. Table 8 gives forms which have irregular vowel correspondences within Rma due to being borrowings from Chinese.Footnote 9

Table 8. Chinese loans

Nonetheless, certain loanwords from Chinese have gone unnoticed, and, in some cases, Evans reconstructs Proto-Southern Rma forms for the Chinese loans. Table 9 gives forms which have not been recognized as Chinese loans. For these forms, Evans reconstructs a proto-Southern form. However, treating these as native forms ignores the irregular vowel correspondences.

Table 9. Probable Chinese loans

It should be noted that in Southwestern Mandarin, the variety of Mandarin in contact with Rma, the words “gong”, “elder brother”, “loose”, and “clump, heap” share the same vowel nucleus: [o].

Some of the forms in Table 9 are straightforwardly loans from Chinese. For example, “gong” is a loan due to the vowel correspondence, but also because this musical instrument is generally accepted to be Chinese in origin (see Kunst Reference Kunst1947). Evidence that “trough” is a loanword comes from the fact it is found in Ronghong -tsʰu “manger” (horse-trough) (LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 347). Note the irregular vowel correspondence for Ronghong.

Some of the forms are possibly Chinese loans, but the relationship is not as certain. These include: “clump, heap”, “row of grain”, “to dip pen”, and “elder brother”.

For example, it seems possible that the forms for “to dip pen” are loans from Chinese 書 shū “book, to write”. The meaning “to write” for the Chinese form is more archaic. This comparison is admittedly speculative, but seems more plausible than reconstructing a literary term for a language without a written tradition.

Nonetheless the explanation of loans from Chinese seems the most likely at this point. Even if these are ultimately not Chinese loans, they should not be reconstructed in this way as they introduce multiple correspondences between proto-Southern Rma and the modern varieties such that the reflexes are not predictable based on the proto-forms.

2.3.3. Cross-dialectal borrowings

Cross-dialectal borrowings are more difficult to detect than borrowings from closely related languages, but are an important part of sorting out the historical phonology of a language. I can find in prior work no identification of cross-dialectal borrowings. In this section, I invoke cross-dialectal borrowings in order to explain vowel correspondences which are exceptional to the patterns summarized in Table 10.

Table 10. Potential cross-dialectal loans

While the Mianchi :: Ronghong correspondences are regular, the Longxi forms have o where we would expect u. There is no indication that these forms are borrowings from another language. One solution is to propose that the Longxi forms are Mianchi loans. There are two reasons for this. The first is phonological: *H.L tones correspond regularly to collapsed monosyllables in Ronghong (cf. “flea”, “listen” in Table 3).Footnote 10 Thus the etymological tone for “winter” should be H.L, as seen faithfully reflected in Mianchi, and not the aberrant L.H in Longxi. For this reason, we would expect the directionality of borrowing to be from Mianchi into Longxi, and not the inverse. The second reason is sociological. Liu (Reference Liu1998) reports 3,300 Longxi speakers and 15,700 Mianchi speakers. These numbers are outdated, yet it is clear that Mianchi is a much larger variety. This makes a loan from Longxi > Mianchi less plausible. The Mianchi and Longxi varieties are geographically close, and are both spoken in Wenchuan county. Thus, positing interdialectal loans from Mianchi to Longxi is not unreasonable.

2.3.4. Onomatopoeic forms

Evans (Reference Evans2001) reconstructs several onomatopoeic forms, and thereby introduces irregular vowel correspondences. Consider the forms in Table 11.

Table 11. Onomatopoeic forms

Evans (Reference Evans2001: 268) notes that “cat”, “appears to be onomatopoeic”. Evans’ (Reference Evans2001) reconstruction of these onomatopoeic forms is problematic.Footnote 11 The importance of excluding onomatopoeic forms has long been known to the comparative method (Lottner Reference Lottner1862; see discussion by Lehmann Reference Lehmann1976). Authorities on the comparative method caution against comparing onomatopoeic forms, which are frequently renewed by more imitative forms and thus generally unreliable in historical linguistics (Hock Reference Hock1991: 50; Harrison Reference Harrison, Joseph and Janda2003: 216; Campbell Reference Campbell2013: 317; Millar and Trask Reference Millar and Trask2015: 207; Rankin Reference Rankin, Joseph and Janda2017: 184). Most of the forms in Table 11 are pointed out by Campbell (Reference Campbell2013: 321) as cross-linguistically likely to be onomatopoeic (overlapping forms in bold):

A way to reduce the sound-imitative factor is to omit from consideration words which cross-linguistically are often imitative in form, for example, words meaning “blow”, “breathe”, “suck”, “laugh”, “cough”, “sneeze”, “break/cut/chop/split”, “cricket”, “crow” (and many bird names in general), “frog/toad”, “lungs”, “baby/infant”, “beat/hit/pound”, “call/shout”, “breathe”, “choke”, “cry”, “drip/drop”, “hiccough”, “kiss”, “shoot”, “snore”, “spit” and “whistle”, among others.

2.3.5. Unresolved issues

This section presents an unsolved problem regarding vowel correspondences for certain numerals in Rma. Consider the data in Table 12.

Table 12. Rma numerals

Note that these numbers do not pattern like native forms, nor like loans from Tibetan or Chinese. Looking closer, we see alternations in compounds with these three numbers as well. Consider Table 13.

Table 13. Rma numerals (continued)

We see consonantal and vocalic alternations in Ronghong, as well as consonantal and tonal alternations in Longxi and Mianchi.Footnote 12 Note that lack of tonal correspondences between Longxi and Mianchi. This is suggestive of either early borrowings from another language, inter-dialectal borrowing, analogical levelling, or some combination.Footnote 13

2.3.6. Summary

Evans’ (Reference Evans2001) study of Rma historical phonology is a landmark work to which this and future work on Rma is indebted. Nonetheless, Evans’ (Reference Evans2001: 177) “majority rules” approach to reconstruction leads him, paradoxically, to reconstruct forms with non-etymological correspondences (Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /u/ and Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /o/) with the most confidence, and to posit proto-variation for forms with etymological correspondences (Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/, Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/).

Evans (Reference Evans2001) recognizes the lack of regularity for his correspondences, and states that “Not all reconstructions are equally definite” (p. 95), and that “not all reconstructions are created equal” (p. 98). This is essentially true. Some are loans from Tibetan and Chinese, some are probably interdialectal loans, some are onomatopoeic, and some have possibly been subject to analogical levelling. True cognates are “created equal” by definition. They share a historical development and have been subject to the same regular sound changes.

The following section pivots from vowel correspondences to look at etymologies proposed for compound forms.

3. Compounds

Shafer (Reference Shafer1974) noted Rma's phonological progressiveness, propensity for compounding, and the general difficulty in recognizing compounds in Rma. On the whole, the importance of compounding in the evolution of the Rma language has been underestimated (but see Zheng Reference Zheng2017 on compounds in Longxi Rma). This section examines several compound forms that have been overlooked or misinterpreted.

3.1. A “far”-fetched etymology

Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2003: 195) gives a problematic etymology of the Mawo Rma word for “far”. Writing about his PTB root *g-wəy-n FAR:

There is actually some evidence that this root had an initial velar at the PTB and PST stages, as suggested by the following Qiangic forms: Qiang [Rma] Mawo [guə˞] χe; Qiang [Rma] Taoping χuɑ33; Muya qʰuɐ⁵⁵rɐ⁵3; Queyu kua⁵⁵kua⁵3; Shixing qʰuɑ⁵⁵ [ZMYYC #817], all perhaps < PTB *g-wəy. There is also an attractive comparison with Chinese 遠 OC giw̯ǎn [GSR #256f-g] (Mand. yúan), perhaps with suffixal *-n.

The bracketed text (italicized in the original) indicates that it is the first of the two Mawo Rma syllables that is meant to lend evidence for a velar prefix. This is odd given that the Taoping Rma form χuɑ33 “far”, which has a uvular onset, is also cited as evidence of a velar onset. Of the languages cited here, only Queyu has velars. Baxter and Sagart (Reference Baxter and Sagart2014) reconstruct Chinese 遠 yuǎn “far” < Middle Chinese *hjwonX < Old Chinese *C.ɢʷanʔ, with a uvular initial.Footnote 14

Issues of uvularity aside, the above analysis is rooted in a misunderstanding of the Mawo form. Mawo Rma gwə˞ χe is a compound in which the first syllable is “road” and the second syllable is “far”. Consider the Ronghong Rma forms gwa-ha “far”, and gwe-ɲi “near” (LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 381), these forms contain the same morpheme found in Ronghong gwəːʴ “road”. Consider also the parallel situation in Mianchi Rma: ʐí-χwà “far, distant”, ʐí-zɛ̀ “near”. Both contain the Mianchi word ʐì “road”. The Mianchi forms are correctly analysed by Evans (Reference Evans2001: 361) as compounds with “road” as the initial element. In this case, a lack of recognition of the internal structure of the Rma form has led to problematic comparisons with other languages.

3.2. A “cloudy” etymology

Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2003: 271; STEDT #5656) lists Mawo zdɤm “cloud” as a reflex of his PTB *s-dim CLOUD, without discussion of the fact that the -m in Mawo is secondary. As Jacques (Reference Jacques2015b) has pointed out, the -m in this form must be secondary, as Rma lost all codas (see LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003 and references therein).

The Mawo form zdɤm “cloud” is nearly identical to forms found in Rgyalrong, i.e. Japhug Rgyalrong zdɯm “cloud” and Kyomkyo Situ zdeʔm “cloud”.Footnote 15 Thus, one possibility is that the Mawo form is borrowed from Rgyalrong. Another possibility is that the -m coda in Mawo Rma “cloud” comes from the second syllable of a compound form. Evans (Reference Evans2001) analyses the apparently cognate forms Longxi dá mù̻ “cloud” and Mianchi dá mò “cloud” as compounds with the second element meaning “dark”. Note that the change *Cd > d is regular in Longxi and Mianchi (see Evans Reference Evans2001: 162–3). In this view, the Mawo coda -m would come from a second element meaning “dark”. Both explanations are possible, but in either case, the -m in Mawo zdɤm “cloud” would not be a reflex of the ancient coda -m posited by Matisoff.

3.3. A “corralled” etymology

On the basis of Longxi tɕʰý “to enclose (sheep)” and Taoping ŋu⁵⁵ tɕi⁵⁵ “enclosure”, Evans (Reference Evans2001: 280) reconstructs a proto-Southern Rma form *tɕ/tɕʰ i/y H. Segmentally, this comparison is problematic. The Longxi verb is related to Mawo tɕʰu “to pen (sheep)”. The unrelated Taoping noun appears to be a compound of zɿ31 ŋu33 “bovine” and tɕi⁵⁵ ko33  “home”.

3.4. A “thorny” etymology

Evans (Reference Evans2001: 390) gives an etymology of “numbing pepper” (Zanthoxylum bungeanum) as being related to “mutton”. Mianchi tsʰɛ-̀nə́ “numbing pepper” is glossed “mutton?-red” and Evans notes that “(one kind tastes like mutton)”. An issue with this etymology is that the Mianchi form tsʰɛ̀ is not “mutton” but “goat”. It seems more likely that the first syllable is related to Chinese 刺 < Middle Chinese *tsʰje H < Old Chinese *[tsʰ]ek-s “thorn” or Tibetan tsher.ma “thorn” and that any similarity with “goat” is coincidental. The second syllable is indeed “red”. Thus, this compound is “red thorn”, which makes more intuitive sense.Footnote 16 See Jacques and D'Alpoim Guedes (Reference Jacques and Guedes2023) for a discussion of the etymology of Zanthoxylum.

3.5. An “edgy” etymology

STEDT (#0594) links the first syllable of Longxi Rma tɕà qó “knife edge” with PTB *m-dzya EDGE/SIDE. The Longxi form is a compound in which the first syllable comes from tɕà pjá “knife”Footnote 17 and the second syllable, -, means “edge”. Consider the Longxi form tɕà pjá tsò “back of knife blade”. Thus, the comparison does not work well semantically. Even granting a comparison of Longxi “knife” with *m-dzya EDGE/SIDE, the segmental correspondence (PTB *m-dz :: Longxi -) is not well supported. Consider the data in Table 14.

Table 14. Longxi Rma :: Matisoff's PTB

This etymology should be abandoned due to the weak semantics and problematic segmental correspondences.

3.6. A “bollocks” etymology

STEDT (#1654) draws a connection between the Rma form for “testicles” and Matisoff's Proto-Tibeto-Burman *pu EGG. While many languages have replaced “testicle” with “egg” (i.e. Spanish), there are some issues with this analysis for Rma.

First, neither the onset nor the vowel fit well with the other Rma :: PTB correspondences proposed by Matisoff. Consider the words in Table 15. Longxi forms are from Evans (Reference Evans2001), except for “smell bad” which is from Zheng (Reference Zheng2017).

Table 15. Longxi Rma :: Matisoff's PTB

We see that Longxi /b/ generally corresponds with Matisoff's *b, and that Longxi /u/ generally corresponds with Matisoff's PTB *u. Thus, a comparison of bɚ̀ “testicle” and his PTB *pu EGG does not work well segmentally.

Second, looking within Rma, we see a more likely etymology. The rhotic vowel may be explained as resulting from a collapse of two syllables. Evans (Reference Evans2001: 410) gives the Longxi possessive forms for “testicles” as both bə̀ ə̀˞ and bə̀ ɹə̀. This suggests that the rhotic vowel is from a second syllable which has undergone coalescence.Footnote 18 Evans and Sun (Reference Evans, Sun and Sybesma2015) give the Hongyan Rma form for “bollock” as ʐə, which seems to be the origin of the second syllable. The first syllable, with the b- initial, is possibly related to Japhug Rgyalrong tɯ-mbɯ “penis” and Tangut 5362 biʶj 2.33 < *mbej “penis” (see Jacques Reference Jacques2014: 168; Gong Reference Gong2020). In brief, the Rma forms seem to come from an old compound and thus comparisons with forms meaning “egg” in other languages are probably incorrect.

4. Broader methodological issues

Taking a step back from any one sound correspondence or etymon, we see a more general problem with the way certain comparisons have been made in Rma historical linguistics. This section discusses some of these methodological problems including: (1) top-down comparisons; and (2) lack of commitment to regular sound change.

4.1. Comparisons with Proto-Tibeto-Burman

The modus operandi of Rma etymologists has been to give top-down comparisons of Rma with reconstructions of proto-languages. For instance, Evans (Reference Evans2001) references Proto-Tibeto-Burman forms from Benedict (Reference Benedict1972), and also references proto-Tibeto-Burman forms from Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2003: 228), Proto-Lolo-Burmese forms (2003: 305, 307), as well as his own reconstructions of proto-Qiangic (2003: 228, 296, 302, 314) and of proto-Northern-Rma (2003: 226). Evans (Reference Evans2001) does make direct comparisons with an impressive number of attested languages.Footnote 19 Nonetheless, because of the intermittent nature of these comparisons, it is not possible to ascertain the sound correspondences with any one language. In a similar fashion, LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003) give a lexicon of the Ronghong variety with intermittent comparisons with Benedict's Proto-Tibeto-Burman, except for the numerals, which follow Matisoff (Reference Matisoff1997). Evans (Reference Evans2006a) gives is a study of the history of certain Rma vowels filtered through Matisoff's (Reference Matisoff2003) starred forms. Sims (Reference Sims2014) gives a lexicon of the Yonghe variety of Rma and posits a smattering of connections between Rma forms and Matisoff's (Reference Matisoff2003) starred forms throughout.

A general problem with this approach is that reconstructed forms may change in light of new data or analyses.Footnote 20 A more specific problem is that neither Benedict's nor Matisoff's starred forms were arrived at through the standard comparative method (Chang Reference Chang1973; Miller Reference Miller1974; Sagart Reference Sagart2006; Reference Sagart2008; Hill Reference Hill2019; Fellner and Hill Reference Fellner and Hill2019). See also an interview with Li Fangkui on the methodological problems (Chan and LaPolla Reference Chan and LaPolla1998).Footnote 21

Aside from the matter of how these forms were arrived at, the issue still stands that the starred forms do not predict attested forms. As an example, let us consider the 70 connections posited between Benedict's PTB and Ronghong Rma by LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003) in their lexicon of the Ronghong variety. Let us consider only the forms linked with PTB *a. This is widely agreed to be the least controversial vowel in the family (Matisoff Reference Matisoff2003: 162; Sagart Reference Sagart2006; Hill Reference Hill2019: 236) and should be the most straightforward. We will further limit ourselves to cases where LaPolla and Huang posit a link with open syllables. This yields 22 different examples, in which we find no fewer than six different reflexes of Benedict's *a in Ronghong.Footnote 22 Examples are given in Table 16.

Table 16. PTB :: Ronghong

Bringing these examples together reveals a major division between Ronghong /ɑ/ and /ə/ unaccounted for by Benedict's starred forms.Footnote 23 Because these are all open syllables, it is a problem that cannot be said to result from lost codas.

Vowel raising and fronting has been observed in many Trans-Himalayan languages in the region. This tendency has been called “brightening”, and has been argued to be a common innovation in Qiangic languages (Matisoff Reference Matisoff, Lin, Hsu, Lee, Sun, Yang and Ho2004). One might propose that the apparent vowel split of Benedict's PTB *a into ɑ and ə in Ronghong is the result of brightening. Nonetheless, this is problematic for two reasons. First, recent work has shown that brightening is not a shared innovation but a set of parallel developments (see Chirkova Reference Chirkova2012: 5 fn. 5; Chirkova and Handel Reference Chirkova and Handel2019; Lai Reference Lai2022; Hill Reference Hill2022). Second, there is no obvious phonological condition for “brightening” in Ronghong Rma. Consider the forms for “fat” and “salt” in Table 16. These two forms are segmentally identical in Benedict's system but have different forms in Ronghong.

This belies a more fundamental problem with the way reconstructions are conceptualized. Because reconstructions are merely a shorthand way of indexing the known facts of regular correspondences between cognates, they, by definition, predict attested forms. Reconstructions are not meant to be an amalgam of the phonetic material in attested forms, as we see in Evans’ *q/k i/ou H pu L “cuckoo” or Matisoff's Proto-Tibeto-Burman *m/p/s-(l/d)ap ARM / HAND / WING.Footnote 24

4.2. Regularity of sound change

Some linguists who have worked on Rma have not strongly committed to the principles of regularity of sound change, and this has hindered progress in discovering sound laws. Evans (Reference Evans2001) in particular has expressed doubts about Ausnahmslosigkeit as it relates to Rma vowels:

Mianchi (as well as other Southern Qiang dialects) is subject to irregular vowel harmony … Although vowel harmony is always anticipatory, it does not appear to be regular in any of the dialects … Because vowels in non-final syllables are subject to this irregular vowel harmony, proto-rhymes are much more difficult to compare and reconstruct than are proto-initials. (Evans Reference Evans2001: 65).

… Even at the microlevel undertaken here, many questions arise as to the strength of attestation of certain roots, and many sound laws are far from regular for one dialect. (Evans Reference Evans2001: 96).

Nevertheless, there is often unexplained variation in the [monopthong vowel] reflexes, which is due in part to vowel harmony processes that have not been applied evenly throughout the lexicon (Evans Reference Evans2001: 99).

Evans states that the tones of Rma are “unstable … and often unpredictable” (2001: 231) and subject to “irregular and unpredictable tone sandhi” (2001: 244). Although much has been made of the irregularity or unpredictability of Rma tones, the forms in Tables 2 and 3 attest to the regularity of tonal correspondences for true cognate forms.

Evans (Reference Evans2001: 257) also alludes to grammatically conditioned sound change: “At present I have no explanation for this apparent palatalization, other than the unpredictability of phonological developments in functors”. This type of explanation is not in line with the comparative method (see Hill Reference Hill2014).

Evans (Reference Evans2006b: 113) makes a broader claim that:

Correspondence sets in Qiang [Rma] (and in Qiangic) are often riddled with forms that deviate slightly from the dominant sound laws (Chang Reference Chang1967, Evans Reference Evans2001).

This is correct, but it highlights an underlying problem with the correspondence sets themselves, not a fundamental unruliness of the Rma language.

5. Conclusion

This paper has discussed issues in Rma historical phonology and emphasized several facets of Rma historical linguistics which deserve more attention, including vowel correspondences, loans, analogy, and compounds. This section points out some useful next steps.

Since Evans’ (2001) landmark monograph, there have been many publications on previously undocumented Rma varieties: Ronghong (LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003), Qugu (LaPolla and Poa Reference LaPolla and Poa2003; Huang and Zhou Reference Huang and Facheng2006; Zhou Reference Zhou2010), Puxi (Huang Reference Huang2004), Yonghe (Sims Reference Sims2014, Reference Sims2018), Longxi (Zheng Reference Zheng2017), Xiaoxing (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Baofeng, Mingjun and Xi2019), Luobozhai (Wang Reference Wang2017), among others. Thus, the time is ripe for further historical comparative work that incorporates advancements in the documentation of Rma and builds on Evans (Reference Evans2001), while sifting out the problematic correspondences.

Compounds are an important aspect of Rma word formation, but a lack of recognition of compounds has hampered historical-comparative work. Recent studies of compounds in Tibetan (i.e. Bialek Reference Bialek2018) could serve as a model for how an in-depth treatment of compounds in Rma should be carried out.

Rather than relying on top-down comparisons with hypothetical languages, a study comparing two well-documented varieties of Rma, such as Longxi and Ronghong, with one traditionally written Trans-Himalayan language, such as Tibetan, Tangut, or Burmese, is long overdue. Such an undertaking would hopefully bring advances to the study of Rma historical phonology in the same way that Jacques (Reference Jacques2014) has brought great advancements to the study of Rgyalrongic through a comparison of Tangut and Japhug (see Hill Reference Hill2015).

Scepticism of the regularity of sound change has left many irregularities in Rma unexplained. In some cases, irregularity has been touted to be a basic trait of the Rma language or of the Qiangic subgroup. These issues have been raised enough that it bears affirming: regular phonetic sound change, borrowings, and analogy are sufficient to explain the historical evolution of the Rma language and its close relatives.

While many Rma sound laws have been discovered by Chang (Reference Chang1967), Sun (Reference Sun1981), Evans (Reference Evans2001), and LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003), the lack of an index of these sound laws has impeded comparative work. An inventory of Rma sound laws, paralleling what Hill (Reference Hill2011) has provided for Tibetan, would be a boon to the field.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Jesse P. Gates, Nathan W. Hill, Guillaume Jacques, and Yunfan Lai for their comments on earlier drafts of this paper. All mistakes are my own. This paper was written while I was affiliated with the CRLAO. I wish to especially thank Guillaume Jacques for sharing his office space and being so generous with his time.

Footnotes

1 The family is also called Sino-Tibetan. The name of the family is a point of contention. I use “Trans-Himalayan” simply because it is neutral with respect to ethnonyms. The sub-grouping of the family is not relevant to this paper.

2 Both Chang and Evans reconstruct the phonological systems of Rma varieties of Li and Wenchuan counties. Chang was working with a smaller data set from fewer dialects. Evans’ work supersedes that of Chang, but in some cases introduces new errors. A detailed review of the two systems is beyond the scope of this paper.

3 Rma data are cited directly with the change of [CuV] and [CiV] sequences to medials [w] and [j] (see Sun Reference Sun2003; Evans Reference Evans2006b on this issue). Middle Chinese reconstructions follow Baxter (Reference Baxter1992). Old Chinese reconstructions follow Baxter and Sagart (Reference Baxter and Sagart2014). For clarity, I add the English infinitive to the gloss of Rma verbs. Thus, Mianchi “sleep” (Evans Reference Evans2001: 329) –> “to sleep”.

4 The forms where Longxi /tsʰ/ :: Mianchi /s/ < *Cs clusters (Evans Reference Evans2001). The onset correspondences for the form “tael” are regular (see Evans Reference Evans2001: 115).

5 STEDT (#3629) lists these forms as supporting *m-bru(ŋ/k) DRAGON/THUNDER. The correspondence of Longxi /u/ to Mianchi /u/ is irregular and suggests that this word is indeed from Tibetan ‘brug “dragon”.

6 This word is also loaned from Tibetan into Japhug Rgyalrong βdɯt “une sorte de monstre” (Jacques Reference Jacques2015a: 33).

7 Contrast this with an inherited form: Longxi “poison”, Ronghong dwə “poison”. These forms are related to Tibetan dug “poison” and Chinese 毒 “poison” < Middle Chinese *dowk < Old Chinese *[d]ˤuk.

8 Evans (Reference Evans2001) does not give an explicit reconstruction for “hole”. Nonetheless, he lists the Longxi and Mianchi forms as evidence for his Proto-southern Rma *ʐ (2001: 116), *ɑ (p. 172), *p (p. 102), and *u (p. 177) and does not identify this form as a loan. Tibetan brag.phug, a compound of “cliff” + “hole”, is loaned into other Burmo-Qiangic languages: Tshobdun Rgyalrong pra-pʰuʔ “cave”, Jiulong Prinmi ʐə¹¹ pu⁵⁵, Zuosuo Prinmi ʐə¹³ pu⁵⁵, Ludian Prinmi ʐə¹³ pʰu⁵⁵ (Lù Reference Lù2001: 486–7), Wenquan Prinmi ɡɐrəpŭ “grotto” (Daudey and Gerong Pincuo Reference Daudey and Pincuo2022: 200). Sims (Reference Sims2020: 75) considered the Rma and Prinmi forms cognate, but this is probably incorrect.

9 Forms which are borrowed with [o] vowels in Longxi and Mianchi have the rhyme o in Sichuanese Mandarin, despite the different spellings in Pinyin. Thus, Sichuanese tsʰo213 “chisel”, sõŋ45 “loose”, etc.

10 LaPolla and Huang give a voiceless [u] in the form for “listen” kʂuɕtɕu̻. This feature may be an artifice of a sub-phonemically rounded coda due to the [u] vowel (cf. discussion in LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 32). See Sun (Reference Sun2003) for a critique of the way voiceless vowels have been analysed in the literature on Rma.

11 For example, *q/k i/ou H pu L “cuckoo” does not predict the attested forms and construes irregular segmental correspondences as proto-variation (see Fellner and Hill Reference Fellner and Hill2019: 98–101 on this methodological issue). LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 335) have pointed out that Ronghong Rma kuput ~ kupət “cuckoo” is onomatopoeic. Consider also Tibetan khu.byug, French coucou, Hungarian kakukk, Vietnamese chim cu, Turkish guguk, etc.

12 Note that the Ronghong numbers lack the complex onset in the “teen” forms. It seems probable that the Longxi form ɦà sé “thirteen” is inherited and that ɦà tsʰé “thirteen” has been created on analogy with “three” and “thirty”.

13 See Bradley (Reference Bradley2005) and Jacques (Reference Jacques, Sizhi and Pelkey2017) on irregular numerals in Burmo-Qiangic.

14 Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2003: 20–21) states that “Postvelars are generally secondary developments of the TB *velar series”, but only offers an account for the development of uvulars in Lahu.

15 Thanks to an anonymous reviewer for this observation.

16 Lai Yunfan (personal communication) kindly drew my attention to Wobzi Khroskyabs rtsʰɑv́ “poivre” (Lai Reference Lai2017: 780).

17 Compare with Taoping Rma tɕa³¹ dʑo³³ “knife”.

18 The form for “testicles” has a long rhotic vowel in the Yadu variety: bəə˞, which can also be taken as a trace of a disyllabic compound.

19 These include: Mandarin Chinese (Evans Reference Evans2001: 302), Amdo Tibetan (290, 320), Written Tibetan (224, 229, 319) Written Burmese (305), Prinmi (228), Lahu (234, 305), Akha (255. 288, 292), Rgyalrong (296), Muya (272, 296), Stau (Ergong) (272, 294), Ersu (302), Queyu (302), Namuyi (308), and Shixing (300, 308).

20 See, for example, the major change to the reconstructed vowel system of Proto-Tibeto-Burman between Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2003) and Matisoff (Reference Matisoff2015).

21 See Matisoff (Reference Matisoff1975, Reference Matisoff2007), for responses to Miller (Reference Miller1974) and Sagart (Reference Sagart2006), respectively. See Handel (Reference Handel2019) on Fellner and Hill (Reference Fellner and Hill2019).

22 Although the forms for the numbers “five” and “hundred” come from Matisoff (Reference Matisoff1997) and not Benedict (Reference Benedict1972), they are open syllables with *a in both systems.

23 Some of the correspondences could, perhaps, be dismissed as anomalous. The forms for “ear” and “nose” both involve compounding in Rma and could reasonably be ignored. The prohibitive prefix, which has multiple allomorphs (LaPolla and Huang Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: 175) could be excluded. The *r- prefix might explain the vowel quality in “hundred”. See LaPolla and Huang (Reference LaPolla and Chenglong2003: fn 23) on this form. Note that LaPolla and Huang state that “ear” and “goose” and “moon” are only “possibly” related to the starred forms.

24 Note that Matisoff's ARM / HAND / WING represents a hypothetical word with the following variants: *map, *pap, *sap, *mlap, *plap, *slap, *mdap, *pdap, *sdap.

References

Baxter, William H. and Sagart, Laurent. 2014. Old Chinese: A New Reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baxter, William H. III. 1992. A Handbook of Old Chinese Phonology. (Trends in Linguistics. Studies and Monographs 64.) Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, Paul K. 1972. Sino-Tibetan: A Conspectus. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, Paul K. 1983. “Qiang monosyllabization: a third phase in the cycle”, Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 7/2, 113–4.Google Scholar
Bialek, Joanna. 2018. Compounds and Compounding in Old Tibetan: A Corpus-Based Approach. (Tibetica.) Marburg: Indica et Tibetica Verlag.Google Scholar
Bradley, David. 2005. “Why do numerals show ‘irregular’ correspondence patterns in Tibeto-Burman? Some southeastern Tibeto-Burman examples”, Cahiers de Linguistique Asie Orientale 34/2, 221–38.Google Scholar
Campbell, Lyle. 2013. Historical Linguistics: An Introduction. Third edition. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Chan, Ning-ping and LaPolla, Randy J.. 1998. “Fang-kuei Li, linguistics east and west: American Indian, Sino-Tibetan and Thai, an oral history conducted in 1986 by Ning-ping Chan and Randy LaPolla.” Online Archive of California, <https://oac.cdlib.org/view?docId=hb3489n99m&chunk.id=div00064&brand=oac4&doc.view=entire_text>..>Google Scholar
Chang, Kun. 1967. “A comparative study of the Southern Ch'iang dialects”, Monumenta Serica XXVI, 422–43.Google Scholar
Chang, Kun. 1973. “Review of Sino-Tibetan: A Conspectus (Benedict)”, Journal of Asian Studies 32, 335–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chirkova, Katia. 2012. “The Qiangic subgroup from an areal perspective: a case study of languages of Muli”, Language and Linguistics 13/1, 133–70.Google Scholar
Chirkova, Katia and Handel, Zev. 2019. “‘Brightening’ in Ersu, Lizu, Duoxu and neighboring languages”, The 5th Workshop on Sino-Tibetan Languages of Southwest China (STLS-2019), Nankai University, PRC, Aug 2019, Tianjin, China.Google Scholar
Daudey, Henriëtte and Pincuo, Gerong. 2022. “‘A long foot crossing mountain’: forty-three annotated Pumi riddles”, Himalayan Linguistics 20/3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Jonathan P. 2001. Introduction to Qiang Phonology and Lexicon: Synchrony and Diachrony. Tokyo: ILCAA, Tokyo University of Foreign Studies.Google Scholar
Evans, Jonathan P. 2006a. “Origins of vowel pharyngealization in Hongyan Qiang”, Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 29/2, 95127.Google Scholar
Evans, Jonathan P. 2006b. “Vowel quality in Hongyan Qiang”, Language & Linguistics 7/4, 731–54.Google Scholar
Evans, Jonathan P. and Sun, Jackson T.-S. 2015. “Qiāng 羌 Language”, in Sybesma, Rint (ed.), Encyclopedia of Chinese Language and Linguistics Online. Brill. https://doi.org/10.1163/2210-7363_ecll_COM_00000347Google Scholar
Fellner, Hannes A. and Hill, Nathan W.. 2019. “Word families, allofams, and the comparative method”, Cahiers de Linguistique Asie Orientale 48/2, 91124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gong, Xun. 2020. “Uvulars and uvularization in Tangut phonology”, Language and Linguistics 21/2, 175212.Google Scholar
Handel, Zev. 2019. “A brief response to Fellner and Hill's ‘Word families, allofams, and the comparative method’”, Cahiers de Linguistique Asie Orientale 48/2, 125–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S.P. 2003. “On the limits of the comparative method”, in Joseph, Brian D. and Janda, Richard D. (eds), The Handbook of Historical Linguistics, 213–43. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Nathan W. 2011. “An inventory of Tibetan sound laws”, Journal of the Royal Asiatic Society 21/4, 441–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Nathan W. 2012. “Evolution of the Burmese vowel system”, Transactions of the Philological Society 110/1, 6479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Nathan W. 2014. “Grammatically conditioned sound change”, Language and Linguistics Compass 8/6, 211–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Nathan W. 2015. “The contribution of Tangut to Trans-Himalayan comparative linguistics”, Archiv Orientalni 83/1, 187200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Nathan W. 2019. The Historical Phonology of Tibetan, Burmese, and Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Nathan W. 2022. “Two notes on Proto-Ersuic”, Cahiers de Linguistique: Asie Orientale 51/1, 105–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hock, Hans Henrich. 1991. Principles of Historical Linguistics. Second rev. and updated ed. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Bufan and Facheng, Zhou. 2006. Qiangyu yanjiu (Studies in Qiang). Zhongguo Qiangxue wenku. Chengdu: Sichuan renmin chubanshe.Google Scholar
Huang, Chenglong. 2004. “A reference grammar of the Puxi variety of Qiang”. Doctoral dissertation, City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Huang, Chenglong, Baofeng, Wang, Mingjun, Mao and Xi, Zhang. 2019. Sichuan Songpan Qiangyu. Zhongguo binwei yuyan zhi. Shaoshu minzu yuyan xilie. Beijing: Shangwu yinshuguan.Google Scholar
Jacques, Guillaume. 2014. Esquisse de phonologie et de morphologie historique du tangoute. Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, Guillaume. 2015a. Dictionnaire Japhug–Chinois–Français, version 1.0. Paris: Projet HimalCo. URL http://himalco.huma-num.fr/.Google Scholar
Jacques, Guillaume. 2015b. “Les ressemblances fortuites entre mots apparentés”. URL https://panchr.hypotheses.org/616.Google Scholar
Jacques, Guillaume. 2017. “The morphology of numerals and classifiers in Japhug”, in Sizhi, Ding Picus and Pelkey, Jamin (eds), Sociohistorical Linguistics in Southeast Asia: New Horizons for Tibeto-Burman Studies in Honor of David Bradley. (Brill's Tibetan Studies Library.) Leiden: Brill, 135–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, Guillaume and Guedes, Jade D'Alpoim. 2023. “Sichuan peppercorn and the birth of numbing spices in East Asia”, Ethnobiology Letters 14/1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunst, Jaap. 1947. “A hypothesis about the origin of the gong*”, Ethnos 12/1–2, 7985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, Yunfan. 2017. “Grammaire du khroskyabs de Wobzi”, Doctoral dissertation, Paris: Université Paris III.Google Scholar
Lai, Yunfan. 2022. “When internal reconstruction goes further: proposing the vowel system of Pre-Khroskyabs through examining bound state apophony”, Folia Linguistica 56(s43–s1), 213–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaPolla, Randy and Chenglong, Huang. 2003. A Grammar of Qiang. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaPolla, Randy J. and Poa, Dory. 2003. “Texts in the Qugu variety of Northern Qiang”, in Descriptive and Theoretical Studies in Minority Languages of East and Southeast Asia, 7794. Suita: Osaka Gakuin University.Google Scholar
Lehmann, Winfred P. 1976. A Reader in Nineteenth-Century Historical Indo-European Linguistics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Liu, Guangkun. 1981. “Qiangyu zhong de Zangyu jieci” (Tibetan Loan-words in the Qiang language), Minzu Yuwen 3, 1928.Google Scholar
Liu, Guangkun. 1998. Mawo Qiangyu Yanjiu (Studies in Mawo Qiang). Chengdu Shi: Sichuan minzu chubanshe.Google Scholar
Lottner, Carl. 1862. “Ausnahmen der ersten Lautverschiebung”, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Sprachforschung auf dem Gebiete des Deutschen, Griechischen und Lateinischen 11, 161205.Google Scholar
, Shàozūn. 2001. 普米語方言研究 Pŭmǐyŭ fāngyán yánjiū (A study on Pumi dialectology). Beijing: Minzu chubanshe.Google Scholar
Matisoff, James A. 1975. “Benedict's Sino-Tibetan: a rejection of Miller's Conspectus inspection”, Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 2/1, 155–72.Google Scholar
Matisoff, James A. 2015. “On the demise of the Proto-Tibeto-Burman mid vowels”, Bulletin of the National Museum of Ethnology 39/3, 375–95.Google Scholar
Matisoff, James A. 1997. Sino-Tibetan Numeral Systems: Prefixes, Protoforms and Problems. (Pacific Linguistics.) Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Matisoff, James A. 2003. Handbook of Proto-Tibeto-Burman. (University of California Publications in Linguistics.) Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Matisoff, James A. 2004. “‘Brightening’ and the place of Xixia in the Qiangic branch of Tibeto-Burman”, in Lin, Ying-chin and Hsu, Fang-min and Lee, Chun-chih and Sun, Jackson T.-S. and Yang, Hsiu-fang and Ho, Dah-an (eds), Studies on Sino-Tibetan Languages: Papers in Honor of Professor Hwang-Cherng Gong on His Seventieth Birthday. Taipei: Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica.Google Scholar
Matisoff, James A. 2007. “Response to Laurent Sagart's review of Handbook of Proto-Tibeto-Burman: System and Philosophy of Sino-Tibetan reconstruction”, Diachronica 24/2, 435–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millar, Robert McColl and Trask, R.L. (eds). 2015. Trask's Historical Linguistics. 3rd edition. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Roy Andrew. 1974. “Sino-Tibetan: Inspection of a conspectus”, Journal of the American Oriental Society 94/2, 195209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rankin, Robert L. 2017. “The comparative method”, in Joseph, Brian D. and Janda, Richard D. (eds), The Handbook of Historical Linguistics, 181212. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sagart, Laurent. 2006. “Review of Handbook of Proto-Tibeto-Burman: System and philosophy of Sino-Tibeto-Burman Reconstruction by James A. Matisoff”, Diachronica 23/1, 206–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagart, Laurent. 2008. “Reply to Matisoff on the Handbook of Proto-Tibeto-Burman: System and Philosophy of Sino-Tibetan Reconstruction”, Diachronica 25/1, 153–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shafer, Robert. 1974. Introduction to Sino-Tibetan. Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz.Google Scholar
Sims, Nathaniel A. 2014. “A phonology and lexicon of the Yonghe variety of Qiang”, Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 37/1, 3474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sims, Nathaniel A. 2018. “Yonghe Qiang language and culture: a collection of language and cultural material from the Qiang people of Yonghe valley in Rgnaba Prefecture, Mao County, Sichuan, China”, URL http://hdl.handle.net/2196/00-0000-0000-0012-5FAD-9.Google Scholar
Sims, Nathaniel A. 2020. “Recondsidering the diachrony of Tone in Rma”, Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society 13/1, 5385.Google Scholar
Sino-Tibetan Etymological Dictionary and Thesaurus (STEDT) https://stedt.berkeley.edu/Google Scholar
Sun, Hongkai. 1981. Qiangyu Jianzhi (A Brief Description of the Qiang language). Beijing: Minzu chubanshe.Google Scholar
Sun, Hongkai. 1988. “Lun Qiangzu shuangyuzhi–jiantan Hanyu dui Qiangyu de yingxiang” [A discussion of bilingualism among the Qiang people – with comments on the influence of the Chinese language on the Qiang language]. Minzu Yuwen [Minority Languages of China] 4, 5565.Google Scholar
Sun, Jackson T.-S. 2003. “Issues in Mawo Qiang phonology”, Journal of Taiwanese Languages and Literature 1/1, 227–42.Google Scholar
Wang, Baofeng. 2017. “Luobozhai Qiangyu Yanjiu” (Studies in Luobozhai Qiang). Doctoral dissertation, Central University for Nationalities of China.Google Scholar
Zheng, Wuxi. 2017. A Grammar of Longxi Qiang. (LINCOM Studies in Asian Linguistics.) Munich: LINCOM.Google Scholar
Zhou, Facheng. 2010. Han Qiang Cidian (Han–Qiang Dictionary). Beijing: Zhong guo wen lian chu ban she.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Vowel correspondences found in Evans 2001

Figure 1

Table 2. Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/

Figure 2

Table 3. Longxi /o/ :: Mianchi /ou/ :: Ronghong /u/

Figure 3

Table 4. Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/

Figure 4

Table 5. Longxi /u/ :: Mianchi /o/ :: Ronghong /(w)ə/

Figure 5

Table 6. Vowel correspondences for inherited forms

Figure 6

Table 7. Probable Tibetan loans

Figure 7

Table 8. Chinese loans

Figure 8

Table 9. Probable Chinese loans

Figure 9

Table 10. Potential cross-dialectal loans

Figure 10

Table 11. Onomatopoeic forms

Figure 11

Table 12. Rma numerals

Figure 12

Table 13. Rma numerals (continued)

Figure 13

Table 14. Longxi Rma :: Matisoff's PTB

Figure 14

Table 15. Longxi Rma :: Matisoff's PTB

Figure 15

Table 16. PTB :: Ronghong