Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T04:28:50.475Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attachment expectations moderate links between social support and maternal adjustment from 6 to 18 months postpartum

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2024

Yufei Gu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates Department of Applied Psychology, Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, New York, NY, USA
Theodore E.A. Waters*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates Department of Applied Psychology, Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, New York, NY, USA
Victoria Zhu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates
Brittany Jamieson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
Danielle Lim
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Toronto Metropolitan University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Gabrielle Schmitt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Toronto Metropolitan University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Leslie Atkinson*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Toronto Metropolitan University, Toronto, ON, Canada
*
Corresponding authors: Theodore E. A. Waters; Email: [email protected]; Leslie Atkinson; Email: [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Theodore E. A. Waters; Email: [email protected]; Leslie Atkinson; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Significant links exist between one’s perception of available social support and mental health outcomes, including during the transition to motherhood. Yet, attachment theory posits that individuals do not benefit equally from social support. As such, we examined the influence of attachment representations (i.e., secure base script knowledge) as they potentially moderate links between social support and psychological distress in a 1-year longitudinal study of an ethnically diverse (56% White) sample of infant-mother dyads. We hypothesized that higher social support would predict lower maternal psychological distress and this relation would be strongest in those with higher secure base script knowledge. Results indicated that maternal perceptions of social support were significantly negatively correlated with psychological distress. Analyses revealed that secure base script scores significantly moderated these associations. Interestingly, for those high in script knowledge, low social support predicted greater psychological distress. For those low in script knowledge, social support was unrelated to psychological distress. This pattern suggested that those who expect care (i.e., high secure base script knowledge) but receive minimal support (i.e., low perceived social support) find motherhood uniquely dysregulating. Practitioners may do well to examine individuals’ attachment expectations in relation to their current social support.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The arrival of a new child can be a challenging time that heightens postpartum mothers’ risk of mental health problems, including depression, anxiety, and emotion dysregulation (e.g., Brockington, Reference Brockington2004; Wenzel et al., Reference Wenzel, Haugen, Jackson and Brendle2005). These aspects of maternal psychological adjustment have significant implications for child adaptation into maturity (e.g., Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011). In recognition of the potential negative impact of postpartum stress (Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Strivens-Vilchez, Gonzalez-Perez, Martinez-Augustin, Peralta-Ramirez and Slattery2017), researchers have emphasized the role of social support in alleviating stress and improving maternal adjustment during infancy and beyond (e.g., Cutrona, Reference Cutrona1984; Priel & Besser, Reference Priel and Besser2002). Social support from different sources, including family and romantic partnership, serves as a protective factor against emotion dysregulation, postpartum depressive symptoms, and psychological distress (e.g., Dennis & Ross, Reference Dennis and Ross2006; Kuscu et al., Reference Kuscu, Akman, Karabekiroglu, Yurdakul, Orhan, Ozdemir, Akman and Ozek2008). However, there are significant individual differences in how social support is sought and the effectiveness with which it is utilized.

Attachment research has long emphasized the origins and implications of individual differences in attachment security – the ability to seek out, accept, and benefit from support – which could lead to differential impacts of social support on maternal adjustment (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002; Rapoza et al., Reference Rapoza, Vassell, Wilson, Robertson, Manzella, Ortiz-Garcia and Jimenez-Lazar2016; Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Holland, Fortuna, Fraley, Clausell and Clarke2007). Individuals’ early interactions with their caregivers shape their expectations of the availability and effectiveness of support in their relationships with others across the lifespan (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1973, Reference Bowlby1980, Reference Bowlby1988; Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2009). Securely attached individuals tend to experience sensitive and responsive caregiving during infancy and form expectations of support from their attachment figures. As a result, they actively seek support in times of distress and are more easily comforted. In contrast, insecurely attached individuals experience less consistent support, or even outright rejection, during childhood. They come to expect the same in the future, thus refraining from seeking and receiving support when distressed (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Steel and DiLillo2013; Nivison et al., Reference Nivison, Vandell, Booth-LaForce and Roisman2021). Given its association with one’s ability to utilize support, attachment security may serve as an important moderator of the association between the availability of social support and maternal adjustment at critical junctures in family life, including the arrival of a new child (see Dujardin et al., Reference Dujardin, Santens, Braet, De Raedt, Vos, Maes and Bosmans2016 for a similar discussion in the context of middle childhood).

Perceived social support and postpartum adjustment

Social support plays an important role in buffering life stress, enhancing resistance and coping ability, thus yielding better physical and mental health outcomes (e.g., Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Gottlieb, Underwood, Cohen, Underwood and Gottlieb2000; House et al., Reference House, Landis and Umberson1988). Research on the transition to motherhood highlights the predictive value of perceived social support as it relates to postpartum adjustment and psychological well-being among mothers. Castle et al. (Reference Castle, Slade, Barranco-Wadlow and Rogers2008) examined the impact of prenatal perceived social support on postnatal stress among first-time parents, and results indicated that higher levels of perceived social support during pregnancy were significantly associated with lower levels of postpartum distress. A similar pattern was observed in a recent study conducted in China, where in a large sample of mothers (n = 2546), lower levels of perceived social support during pregnancy were associated with an elevated risk of postpartum depression (Gan et al., Reference Gan, Xiong, Song, Xiong, Yu, Gao, Hu, Zhang, Tian, Gu, Zhang and Chen2019).

Emotion regulation refers to individuals’ ability to adapt the types and expressions of emotions they experience depending on circumstance (Gross et al., Reference Gross, Richards, John, Snyder, Simpson and Hughes2006). According to previous research, emotion regulation serves as a preventative factor for postpartum depression, and psychopathology in general (Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Aldao and Nolen-Hoeksema2010; Haga et al., Reference Haga, Ulleberg, Slinning, Kraft, Steen and Staff2012). Apart from its predictive power for maternal adjustment, perceived social support facilitates emotion regulation, thereby improving psychological well-being during the postpartum period. In a systematic review of interpersonal emotion regulation as it mediates the association between social support and depression, Marroquín (Reference Marroquín2011) highlighted the importance of social responses in the development of emotion regulation and argued that social support might introduce interpersonal influences on emotion regulation. This notion aligns with research on emotion regulation among postpartum women, which suggests that mothers’ emotion regulatory ability reflects the level of stress, anxiety, and depression they experience (Grande et al., Reference Grande, Olsavsky, Erhart, Dufford, Tribble, Phan and Kim2021; Marques et al., Reference Marques, Monteiro, Canavarro and Fonseca2018). Thus, it is imperative to consider emotion regulation as a maternal adjustment outcome and to understand how attachment security moderates the link between perceived social support and emotion regulation among postpartum mothers.

Attachment theory and support seeking and receiving

Researchers have long emphasized the need to understand social support from a cognitive perspective, including mental representations of relationship dynamics and attachment. Sarason et al. (Reference Sarason, Pierce, Shearin, Sarason, Waltz and Poppe1991) proposed a link between perceived social support and the appraisal of interpersonal relationships. They suggested that individual differences in the interpretation and expectation of interpersonal support could account for the differences in how people utilize and benefit from that social support. According to attachment theory, individuals’ “internal working model” of attachment is constructed based on their early interactions with their caregivers (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1973, Reference Bowlby1980, Reference Bowlby1988). Such mental representations act as a guide for how to organize support seeking behaviors in the context of distress and exploration, how to provide support to others, and what to expect from intimate relationships in the future.

Building on attachment theory, Collins and Feeney (Reference Collins and Feeney2000) proposed an interpersonal model of support seeking and caregiving interactions in romantic relationships. When encountering a stressful situation, individuals’ working models of attachment become activated and will guide them to express the distress. After receiving the signal for support, their partners will respond and provide support or care based on their own attachment working model. The support seeker will then interpret such support or care based on their attachment model and their distress will be relieved in the case of securely attached individuals or potentially exacerbated in insecure individuals. According to this model, individual differences in attachment representations play an essential role in how and whether a person seeks support, and how that support is perceived and utilized. Furthermore, the model proposes that attachment representations moderate the link between support seeking and psychological adjustment, in terms of individuals’ willingness to acknowledge and express their needs for support, their tendency to accept others’ support, and their emotional outcomes after the support seeking and caregiving interaction.

Research has supported this model with several studies indicating that variations in support seeking and acceptance map onto individual differences in romantic attachment. Using a self-reported measure of attachment style, Davila and Kashy (Reference Davila and Kashy2009) examined the relations between relationship-specific attachment styles and daily social support experiences, including support seeking, provision, and reception, among heterosexual dating couples. Results indicated individuals with high comfort with intimacy and low anxiety about abandonment felt more comfortable seeking support from their partners and reported higher felt support and security on a daily basis. Crowell et al. (Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002) reported similar results in a sample of engaged couples, using the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George et al., Reference George, Main and Kaplan1996). They found a significant positive association between attachment, support seeking, and acceptance during conflict discussions. Furthermore, Ditzen et al. (Reference Ditzen, Schmidt, Strauss, Nater, Ehlert and Heinrichs2008) explored the interaction effect of attachment styles and social support on psychological responses to stress using a self-reported measure. Results indicated that individuals with low attachment anxiety and avoidance exhibited a stronger decline in stress levels after exposure to a stressful situation when social support was available.

Attachment also plays a role in how parents cope with the stress associated with child-rearing. Rholes et al. (Reference Rholes, Simpson and Friedman2006) assessed attachment anxiety and avoidance using a self-reported questionnaire and found that mothers with lower avoidance prenatally were more likely to report lower levels of parenting stress and perceive parenting as more satisfying. When adjusting to the arrival of a newborn, the data suggest that mothers may be more likely to reach out for, and benefit from, support when attachment anxiety and avoidance are low, and expectations for support are high. Thus, it is reasonable to predict that attachment may serve as a moderator of the associations between perceived social support and psychological adjustment to distress during the postpartum period and beyond.

While self-reported attachment styles questionnaires focus on individuals’ attachment-related feelings and thoughts in romantic relationships (Cassidy & Shaver, Reference Cassidy and Shaver1999), assessments of secure base script knowledge focus on knowledge of a cognitive script which specifies the sequence of steps involved in support seeking interactions and what to expect from those around (see Steele et al., Reference Steele, Waters, Bost, Vaughn, Truitt, Waters, Booth-LaForce and Roisman2014). Given this, secure base script knowledge may play a critical role in the way mothers navigate the difficult transition following the birth of a child. Unfortunately, there is limited research on the role of secure base script knowledge in psychological adjustment. Previous studies have shown the associations between secure base script knowledge and mental health outcomes, including psychopathological symptoms in middle childhood (Ruiz et al., Reference Ruiz, Waters and Yates2020; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Bosmans, Vandevivere, Dujardin and Waters2015), and depressive symptoms in adulthood (Dagan et al., Reference Dagan, Groh, Madigan and Bernard2021). However, the impact of secure base script knowledge has not been examined during the postpartum period where greater stress might be present and the need for support likely increases. Thus, it is imperative to examine the role of secure base script knowledge in moderating the associations between perceived social support and maternal adjustment during infancy.

The present study

Despite the abundant research on the influence of perceived social support on maternal adjustment, the moderators of this relation remain largely unknown. While attachment theory could account for the individual differences in how perceived social support impacts adjustment outcomes, no prior studies have examined the moderating effect of attachment on the associations between perceived social support and maternal psychological and emotional well-being. To address the gap in existing literature, the present study aims to assess the associations between perceived social support and maternal adjustment in the postpartum period and to examine the moderating role of attachment representation (i.e., secure base script knowledge) on these associations in a longitudinal study. Waters and Waters (Reference Waters and Waters2006) argued that one component of attachment representations that shapes support-seeking and caretaking behaviors takes the form of a cognitive script. Previous studies have shown that secure base script knowledge is associated with early caregiving quality, healthy romantic relationship functioning, and caregiving for one’s own children (for review, see Waters & Roisman, Reference Waters and Roisman2019). As such, we chose to operationalize attachment security as one’s level of knowledge of the secure base script using the Attachment Script Assessment (ASA; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Waters, Waters, Thompson, Simpson and Berlin2021).

We operationalized maternal adjustment in several ways. Specifically, we focused on postpartum depressive symptoms, emotion dysregulation, and externalizing and internalizing problems to yield a more comprehensive understanding of maternal distress following the arrival of a new child and the mechanisms by which perceived social support buffers distress during child-rearing. Based on attachment theory, we hypothesized that: (1) perceived social support will associate negatively with psychological distress and emotion dysregulation during the postpartum period; (2) mothers with lower secure base script knowledge will report more maternal maladjustment during the postpartum period, compared with mothers with higher secure base script knowledge; and (3) the association between perceived social support and maternal adjustment will be moderated by secure base script knowledge such that people with higher secure base script knowledge will benefit more from perceived social support.

Method

Participants

Data were drawn from a longitudinal study on mothers of young infants conducted in the Greater Toronto Area. During 2018–2021, 144 mother-infant dyads were recruited through Internet platforms (e.g., Kijiji), parenting programs (e.g., prenatal classes), infant-related events (e.g., baby shows), and flyer postings across the city. The inclusion criteria for participation were (a) the mother was proficient in English; (b) the mother was at least 18 years of age; (c) the dyad had no major medical conditions; (d) the infant weighed more than 2,500 g at birth; and (e) the infant was roughly six months old. Power analysis based on a larger, four-visit study indicated the need for 193 participants, but recruitment was interrupted by the outbreak of COVID-19.

Both observational and questionnaire data were collected at three time points: early infancy (Time 1; M = 6.64 months, SD = 1.35), late infancy (Time 2; M = 12.49 months, SD = 1.56), and toddlerhood (Time 3; M = 19.57 months, SD = 2.17). Demographic information on maternal education level, family income, and ethnicity was reported at the initial assessment and included as covariates in all regression analyses. Among the 144 mother-child dyads, 55% of the children were male, and 68% of the mothers were first-time mothers. Due to the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic midway through data collection, of the 144 dyads who were recruited and seen at Time 1, 27 dyads (18.6%) dropped out at the time of transition in data collection from home visits to an online format. Prior to the pandemic, participants discontinued because they were unresponsive or expressed a loss of interest (n = 5, 3.4%), or moved away from the Greater Toronto Area (n = 7, 4.8%). Therefore, 106 mother-child dyads completed all three waves. No significant differences were found in maternal education level or family income between the initial sample and those who completed all three waves, yet there was a significant difference (t = 2.04; p = .04) in ethnicity, with a higher attrition rate for ethnically diverse dyads. In the current sample, Little’s MCAR test was not statistically significant, providing no evidence of systematic missingness, χ 2 (92) = 93.50, p = .44. Thus, we concluded that our data were missing at random.

At the initial assessment, maternal age ranged from 18.50 to 49.75 years (M = 33.95, SD = 4.36). Mothers reported the racial/ethnic status of their biological parents and were classified into the following groups: White (55.6%), Asian (27.1%), Black (4.9%), Latinx (5.6%), Middle Eastern (5.6%), and other (1.2%). Most mothers were university-educated or above (79.2%) and either married or in a common-law relationship (92.4%). Annual family income before tax ranged from under 5,000 to over 250,000 CAD (median 100,001–150,000 CAD). When data were collected, the median family income in Toronto was 106,900 CAD per year (Statistics Canada, Reference Statistics2019). Taken together, our sample is demographically normative-risk, and broadly represents the mother-infant population in the Toronto area.

Procedure

Data were collected between June 2018, and August 2021. All procedures were approved by the University Research Ethics Board at Toronto Metropolitan University. Written informed consent was obtained from mothers before participation. At Time 1 (before the outbreak of COVID-19), 128 dyads completed the first home visit, where mothers completed questionnaires and participated in the Attachment Script Assessment (ASA). A small proportion of mothers (n = 16, 11%) completed the ASA remotely over the phone after the pandemic onset. At Time 2, 80 dyads completed the second home visit where mothers completed questionnaires pre-pandemic, and 39 mothers completed questionnaires remotely using Qualtrics, an online survey platform during COVID-19. At Time 3, 41 mothers completed questionnaires in the home visit, and 63 mothers completed questionnaires remotely, again, due to the ongoing pandemic. For the current study, we utilized demographic information, the ASA, and the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support collected at Time 1, the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale collected at Time 2, and Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale and Adult Self-Report collected at Time 3 (see Figure 1 for illustration). Dyads received monetary compensation for their participation at each time of the study.

Figure 1. Timeline of data collection.

Measures

Perceived social support

Perceived social support was assessed using the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet et al., Reference Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet and Farley1988) at 6 months postpartum (Time 1). The MSPSS is a 12-item self-report questionnaire measuring the perception of social support from family, friends, and significant other. The MSPSS has been adapted and validated in high- and normative-risk samples in both Western and non-Western settings (Canty-Mitchell & Zimet, Reference Canty-Mitchell and Zimet2000; Dambi et al., Reference Dambi, Corten, Chiwaridzo, Jack, Mlambo and Jelsma2018; Kazarian & McCabe, Reference Kazarian and McCabe1991). Using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree), mothers were instructed to rate the extent to which they agree or disagree with statements on social support (e.g., I get the emotional help and support I need from my family). The extant literature has identified an association between higher levels of perceived social support and lower levels of mental health problems, including postpartum depression, anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychopathology (Eker & Arkar, Reference Eker and Arkar1995; Gökdağ, Reference Gökdağ2021; Haga et al., Reference Haga, Ulleberg, Slinning, Kraft, Steen and Staff2012; Stewart & Suldo, Reference Stewart and Suldo2011; Yağmur & Ulukoca, Reference Yağmur and Ulukoca2010). The total score of MSPSS was calculated for each mother by averaging the item ratings, with higher scores indicating higher level of perceived social support. Cronbach’s alpha for perceived social support was 0.92 at 6 months postpartum.

Secure base script knowledge

The ASA is a narrative-based measure of secure base script knowledge among adults (Waters & Waters, Reference Waters and Waters2006). By providing a list of prompt words indicating the beginning, middle, and end of an attachment-relevant story (e.g., a baby in need of comfort after losing its teddy bear), the ASA encourages participants to describe fictional third-person accounts of seeking and providing secure base support when distressed. If participants possess a well-developed secure base script, such knowledge should be activated by the prompt words and the detail and elaborations in their story should match with the temporal-causal structure of the secure base script (Waters & Waters, Reference Waters and Waters2006).

At 6 months postpartum, mothers were instructed to tell six stories (four secure base stories and two neutral stories) in third-person narration. Sets of prompt-word outlines were provided to the participants, indicating the beginning, interruption, distress, and resolution contained in the secure base script. The four secure base stories include two stories involving mother-child interactions (Baby’s Morning, Doctor’s Office) and two stories involving adult-partner interactions (Jane and Bob’s Camping Trip, Sue’s Accident). The two neutral, non-secure base stories (Trip to the Park, Afternoon Shopping) were administered as control stories to minimize hypothesis guessing. Mothers were told that they did not have to use every word in the outline and did not have to follow the words in the order they were presented, but they were encouraged to use the word-prompt outlines as a guide and to tell the story with as many details as possible to make the best possible story.

Using the Waters and Waters’ (Reference Waters and Waters2006) seven-point secure base script scale for ASA, the ratings of the stories told by participants were based on how well they expressed secure base script knowledge through their constructed stories. Scores of 7 were given to stories with rich secure base content (e.g., child or adult signaling their distress, parents or partners responding promptly and properly, problems being resolved successfully, etc.). Scores of 3 were given to event-focused narratives with minimal secure base content. Scores of 1 were given to stories that contained atypical content inconsistent with a secure base script (e.g., parents or partners causing the distress or making the problem even worse). Two independent coders (ICCs = .89–.94) coded each story by storyline rather than individual participants so that coders were blind to the same participant’s scores on other stories. All discrepancies were resolved through discussion. Cronbach’s alpha for these four stories was .65, and the average of the stories was used as the final score for ASA.

Postpartum depressive symptoms

Postpartum depressive symptoms were measured with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987) at 12 months postpartum (Time 2). The EPDS is a 10-item self-report questionnaire assessing depression and anxiety symptoms among mothers in the year following childbirth, and has been validated in both clinical and community samples among mothers with diverse racial/ethnic backgrounds (Beck & Gable, Reference Beck and Gable2001; Chiu et al., Reference Chiu, Sheffield, Hsu, Goldstein, Curtin and Wright2017; Di Florio et al., Reference Di Florio, Putnam, Altemus, Apter, Bergink, Bilszta, Brock, Buist, Deligiannidis, Devouche, Epperson, Guille, Kim, Lichtenstein, Magnusson, Martinez, Munk-Olsen, Newport, Payne and Meltzer-Brody2017; McCabe-Beane et al., Reference McCabe-Beane, Segre, Perkhounkova, Stuart and O’Hara2016). Each item was rated on a 4-point Likert scale, with 0 indicating low frequency of experiencing emotional symptoms of depression (e.g., sadness, hopelessness), and 3 indicating high frequency of depressive symptoms. Mothers’ scores for each item were summed to generate the final scores, with higher scores indicating more postpartum depressive symptoms over the past week. Cronbach’s alpha for maternal depressive symptoms was .87 at 12 months postpartum. Overall, mothers reported low to moderate levels of postpartum depressive symptoms (M = 6.76, SD = 4.67).

Emotion regulation problems

Emotion regulation problems were assessed using the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS – 16; Victor & Klonsky, Reference Victor and Klonsky2016) at 18 months postpartum (Time 3). The DERS utilizes an integrative conceptualization of emotion regulation by assessing the modulation of emotional arousal, the awareness, understanding, and acceptance of emotions, and the capability to prevent one’s action from fluctuations in emotional state (Hallion et al., Reference Hallion, Steinman, Tolin and Diefenbach2018). The DERS consists of 16 items, comprising six subscales: (1) nonacceptance of emotional responses; (2) difficulty engaging in goal-directed behavior; (3) impulse control difficulties; (4) lack of emotional awareness; (5) limited access to emotion regulation strategies; (6) lack of emotional clarity. Mothers were asked to rate the frequency of emotion regulation problems listed in each item using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = almost never, 5 = almost always). Research on emotion regulation among postpartum women shows that mothers’ perceived stress, anxiety, and depression are reflected in their emotion regulatory ability (Grande et al., Reference Grande, Olsavsky, Erhart, Dufford, Tribble, Phan and Kim2021; Haga et al., Reference Haga, Ulleberg, Slinning, Kraft, Steen and Staff2012; Marques et al., Reference Marques, Monteiro, Canavarro and Fonseca2018). A total score was calculated for each mother, with higher scores suggesting greater problems with emotion regulation. Cronbach’s alpha for difficulties in emotion regulation was 0.93 at 18 months postpartum. Overall, mothers reported low to moderate levels of emotion regulation problems (M = 28.73, SD = 10.28).

Internalizing and externalizing problems

The Adult Self Report (ASR) was used at Time 3 to measure mother’s internalizing and externalizing psychopathology symptoms. The ASR is a component of the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment taxonomy and consists of 120 items that assess adaptive functioning and behavior problems among adults (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003). The ASR consists of eight syndrome scales, comprising two broadband scales: internalizing and externalizing problems. The internalizing problems scale is a sum of the syndrome scales of anxious/depressed (18 items), withdrawn (9 items), and somatic complaints (18 items), while the externalizing problems scale comprises the syndrome scales of aggressive behavior (15 items), rule-breaking behavior (14 items), and intrusive behavior (6 items). Using a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not true, 2 = very true or often true), mothers rated how well the descriptions of emotional, behavioral, and social problems apply to themselves. Total raw scores for maternal internalizing problems and externalizing problems were calculated separately for each mother, with higher scores indicating higher levels of psychopathology. In the current sample, on average, mothers reported low to moderate levels of internalizing (M = 10.92, SD = 8.77) and externalizing problems (M = 6.28, SD = 6.21). None of the mothers endorsed behavioral problems at a clinically significant level. Cronbach’s alphas for internalizing and externalizing subscales were .90 and .87, respectively.

Maternal maladjustment

An overall maternal maladjustment variable was created by averaging the z-scores of postpartum depression, emotion dysregulation, and internalizing and externalizing behaviors, with higher scores indicating higher levels of maladjustment. Cronbach’s alpha for this composite variable was 0.83.

Covariates

A composite for socioeconomic status (SES) was created by averaging the z-scores for maternal highest education and annual family income. SES and ethnicity (0 = non-White, 1 = White) were included as potential covariates in the regression analyses.

Results

Perceived social support and relations with maternal maladjustment

Descriptive statistics and measure intercorrelations are presented in Table 1. Maternal perceptions of social support at 6 months postpartum were significantly negatively correlated with overall maternal maladjustment, as well as postpartum depressiven symptoms, emotion regulation problems, and internalizing and externalizing problems specifically. Furthermore, secure base script knowledge was significantly positively correlated to perceived social support, and overall maternal maladjustment, while no significant correlations were observed between secure base script knowledge and the outcome variables including postpartum depressive symptoms, emotion regulation problems, internalizing and externalizing problems.

Table 1. Summary of correlations and descriptive statistics

Note. *p < .05. **p < .01. Ethnicity: 0 = non-White, 1 = White; Maternal Maladjustment is a z-score composite of postpartum depression, emotion dysregulation, internalizing problems and externalizing problems; SocioEconomic Status (SES) is a z-score composite of education level and family income; Analyses were conducted with listwise deletion. Ns range from 97 to 144.

Regarding demographic variables, consistent with previous research on the economic and social determinants of perceived social support (McConnell et al., Reference McConnell, Breitkreuz and Savage2011; Shields & Price, Reference Shields and Price2005), socioeconomic status was significantly positively correlated with perceived social support, and perceived social support was higher for mothers who identified as White, compared with those identified as non-White. Socioeconomic status was also significantly negatively correlated with overall maternal maladjustment, as well as postpartum depression, emotion regulation problems, and internalizing problems. No significant correlations were found between ethnic groups and psychological outcomes (Table 1).

Moderation effect of secure base script knowledge on the associations between perceived social support and maternal maladjustment

We examined associations between perceived social support and postpartum maternal adjustment (assessed as a composite of maternal adjustment outcomes), as moderated by secure base script knowledge. To address missing data, we performed multiple hierarchical regression analysis with Full Information Maximum Likelihood estimation (FIML) using the Lavaan package in R (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). We first ran a moderation test with perceived social support at Time 1 as the predictor, overall maternal maladjustment as the outcome, and secure base script knowledge as the moderator. Demographic covariates (i.e., socioeconomic status and ethnicity) were also included in the models. In Step 1, perceived social support and secure base script knowledge were included to predict maternal maladjustment. In Step 2, the interaction term of perceived social support and secure base script knowledge was entered. In Step 3, socioeconomic status and ethnicity were added to the model, and in Step 4, we included the interaction terms of perceived social support and socioeconomic status, and of perceived social support and ethnicity. Results are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2. Results of multiple hierarchical regression models with FIML predicting composite maternal maladjustment

Note. Numbers in bold indicate statistically significant results (p < .05). N = 144.

Results from Step 1 showed significant main effects between perceived social support and maternal maladjustment. After adding the interaction term between perceived social support and secure base script knowledge in Step 2, results indicated that both perceived social support and secure base script knowledge predicted maternal maladjustment during infancy, and there was a perceived social support and secure base script knowledge interaction effect on maternal maladjustment. After including the demographic covariates (i.e., socioeconomic status and ethnicity) in the model in Steps 3 and 4, the interaction effect of perceived social support and secure base script knowledge on maternal maladjustment remained significant, and results also indicated a main effect of socioeconomic status on maternal maladjustment.

Simple Slope Analysis was performed to examine the moderating effect of SBSK at −1SD, the mean, and +1SD on the association between perceived social support and overall maternal maladjustment (see Figure 2). Results revealed that for participants with higher (but not lower) secure base script knowledge, social support was negatively associated with maternal maladjustment during the postpartum period.

Figure 2. SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and maternal maladjustment. Note. SBSK: Secure Base Script Knowledge; Results based on listwise deletion (n = 97); Statistical significance of simple slopes indicated by *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Moderation effect of secure base script knowledge on the associations between perceived social support and maternal adjustment outcomes

Having demonstrated the moderating effect of secure base script knowledge on the relation between maternal perceived social support and a composite measure of maternal adjustment, we assessed this same moderation model as it pertains to the component parts of the composite, namely, postpartum depression, emotion regulation problems, and internalizing and externalizing problems. Emotion dysregulation, internalizing problems (including depression), and externalizing problems are well-validated as distinctive constructs that have been used separately in previous research; and meta-analytic evidence suggests that attachment representations are differentially associated with internalizing and externalizing problems (e.g., Madigan et al., Reference Madigan, Brumariu, Villani, Atkinson and Lyons-Ruth2016).

Significant main effects were found between perceived social support and postpartum depression, emotion regulation problems, and internalizing problems, but not for externalizing problems. Main effects linking secure base script knowledge to postpartum depression, emotion regulation, and internalizing and externalizing problems were positive and statistically significant. The perceived social support-secure base script knowledge interaction was significantly related to internalizing and externalizing problems, and marginally significantly related to level of emotion dysregulation; the interaction was not significantly related to postpartum depression (see Table 3).

Table 3. Results of multiple regression models with FIML estimating the influence of interactions between SBSK and perceived social support on depression, emotion regulation, internalizing and externalizing problems

Note. Numbers in bold indicate statistically significant results (p < .05). PSS: Perceived Social Support; SBSK: Secure Base Script Knowledge; SES: SocioEconomic Status. N = 144.

Simple Slope Analysis was performed to examine the moderating effect of secure base script knowledge at −1SD, the mean, and +1SD on the association between perceived social support and emotion dysregulation, and internalizing and externalizing problems separately (see Figures 3a–c). Results converged on a similar pattern of results compared to the overall maternal maladjustment analyses. Mothers high in secure base script knowledge benefited more from the increase in perceived social support and reported less emotion dysregulation, internalizing and externalizing problems. The same pattern was observed for those mothers with moderate levels of secure base script knowledge for overall maladjustment, emotion dysregulation, and internalizing problems, but not for externalizing problems. Such results indicated that people with minimal expectations of receiving support from their families and friends showed no increase in distress when social support was not received. Meanwhile, they did not seem to benefit when social support was given. On the other hand, people with higher levels of secure base script knowledge, and thus greater expectations of social support, tended to have worse psychological outcomes when support was not received, yet did benefit when support was given.

Figure 3. (a) SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and emotion dysregulation. (b) SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and internalizing problems. (c) SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and externalizing problems. Note. SBSK: Secure Base Script Knowledge; Results based on listwise deletion (n = 97); Statistical significance of simple slopes indicated by *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Discussion

The present study examined the potential impact of social support on maternal psychological adjustment during the infant period and explored the potential moderating effect of secure base script knowledge on these associations during this critical transition in family life. Our study provides longitudinal data from 6 months to 18 months postpartum on one potential mechanism by which perceived social support protects mothers from mental health dysfunction and emotion dysregulation. Consistent with previous research suggesting that lack of social support has an adverse impact on maternal well-being (e.g., Castle et al., Reference Castle, Slade, Barranco-Wadlow and Rogers2008; Elsenbruch et al., Reference Elsenbruch, Benson, Rücke, Rose, Dudenhausen, Pincus-Knackstedt, Klapp and Arck2007), our findings indicate that perceived social support negatively correlates with postpartum depression, emotion dysregulation, and internalizing and externalizing problems. Furthermore, while no significant correlations between secure base script knowledge and the individual outcome variables were detected, secure base script knowledge was significantly positively correlated to overall maternal adjustment, as well as perceived social support. These results are somewhat inconsistent with the extant literature which indicates that attachment representations are associated with psychological adjustment (see Atkinson et al., Reference Atkinson, Paglia, Coolbear, Niccols, Parker and Guger2000; Dagan et al., Reference Dagan, Facompré and Bernard2018).

As hypothesized, there was a significant interaction effect between perceived social support and secure base script knowledge, such that secure base script knowledge moderated the effect of perceived social support on overall maternal maladjustment during the postpartum period. More specifically, secure base script knowledge significantly moderated the effect of perceived social support on internalizing and externalizing problems at 18 months postpartum, and the interaction effect between perceived social support and secure base script knowledge on emotion dysregulation at 18 months postpartum was marginally significant (p = .06). We did not find a significant interaction effect between perceived social support and secure base script knowledge on postpartum depression measured at 12 months postpartum. However, findings were in the predicted direction. Of course, the fact that one relation is significant and the other not does not mean the findings are statistically different from one another; the failure to find a significant relation in the case of depression may represent a Type II error. Alternatively (or concurrently), the fact that depression was assessed at Time 2, and all other maladjustment indices were assessed at Time 3, may influence findings. It is possible that the manifestation of depression changes with child age. For example, as parenting becomes more complex, involving not only warm and timely responsiveness, as in earlier infancy, but also greater limit-setting, this potentially complex challenge may exert a greater impact on maternal well-being (Maunder et al., Reference Maunder, Hunter, Atkinson, Steiner, Wazana, Fleming, Moss, Gaudreau, Meaney and Levitan2017; Waters & Cummings, Reference Waters and Cummings2000).

Consistent with our predictions, mothers with moderate to high secure base script knowledge increasingly benefited from greater levels of social support, while mothers who scored low on secure base script knowledge showed no significant benefit from increasing levels of social support. Our data indicate that mothers who expect care and support (i.e., higher secure base script knowledge) and receive support consistent with those expectations, are less likely to experience emotional distress during the uniquely stressful infant period of child-rearing relative to those who receive less support. Meanwhile, a mismatch between high maternal expectations of support and lower levels of perceived support led to elevated risk of psychological distress and adjustment problems.

Surprisingly, our findings suggested that among mothers with low levels of perceived social support, those with lower secure base script knowledge reported lower levels of overall maternal maladjustment, emotion dysregulation, and externalizing problems, compared to those with higher secure base script knowledge. Such findings were inconsistent with previous studies which report negative associations between attachment representations and emotion dysregulation, as well as psychological distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, Reference Mikulincer and Shaver2019; Ruiz et al., Reference Ruiz, Waters and Yates2020; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Bosmans, Vandevivere, Dujardin and Waters2015).

Crowell et al. (Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002) argued that the concordance between one’s attachment history (i.e., attachment security to one’s primary caregivers) and one’s attachment security in their current romantic partnership played a critical role in relationship functioning and adjustment. Those who were securely attached in both relationship domains (high concordance) tended to show higher relationship satisfaction and perceive their partner as less aggressive/hostile during conflict and distress. Interestingly, those with insecure attachments in both relationship domains (high concordance, again) showed increased levels of aggression and conflict-oriented relationship strategies but did not report the lowest levels of satisfaction/highest levels of distress. Rather, discordant individuals, those with secure attachment histories who were insecurely attached in their current romantic partnership, showed the highest levels of verbal aggression and distress during conflict, reported lower levels of commitment and felt intimacy, and were the most likely to get divorced (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002; see also (Castellano et al., Reference Castellano, Velotti, Crowell and Zavattini2014). Crowell and her colleagues (Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002) argued that a mismatch between one’s more general attachment expectations, which developed in the context of parental care during childhood, and the specific attachment expectations developed with a current romantic partner/co-parent could lead to greater conflict and distress because the expectations of secure base support were not being met. This mismatch between attachment expectations and current reality could lead to greater conflict under stress (e.g., transition to parenthood). In addition, because the specific attachment relationship was characterized by insecurity, the resolution of this conflict was less effective and may not be positive.

The finding that mothers with greater secure base script knowledge increasingly benefited from higher levels of social support, while mothers with lower secure base script knowledge showed no such benefit, is potentially consonant with the evolutionary life history model of development (Del Giudice, Reference Del Giudice2009; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009; Szepsenwol & Simpson, Reference Szepsenwol and Simpson2019). The model suggests that early experience (pre- and postnatal) prepares the organism for an expected environment, one which is more or less resource-rich, whether psychologically, physically, or both. The most advantageous combination involves high resource availability both early and later in life. Importantly, a mismatch of resource availability across these life stages can be detrimental as the organism may not be well adapted to exploit them, even if the shift is from low to high resources. This model has important physiological, psychological, and relational ramifications as well as transgenerational implications (Atkinson, Reference Atkinson2012; Del Giudice, Reference Del Giudice2009; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009; Simpson, Reference Simpson2019). In the present circumstance, a mismatch between secure base script knowledge (which is partly derived from early caregiving experience), and perceived social support (reflective of the current environment) produces poor psychological outcomes consistent with the life history perspective. Although secure base script knowledge in late adolescence and adulthood is partially rooted in early caregiving experiences, it is important to acknowledge that the relation between early caregiving experiences and secure base script knowledge is modest (e.g. Steele et al., Reference Steele, Waters, Bost, Vaughn, Truitt, Waters, Booth-LaForce and Roisman2014). Nevertheless, several studies indicated that stability in attachment is consistent with the prototype model of development such that the contributions of early experience on secure base script knowledge endures across development and is not overwritten by later experience (Fraley, Reference Fraley2002; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Yang, Finet, Verhees and Bosmans2022). Thus, even a moderate effect size can be considered meaningful in terms of providing a window into one’s early life history.

Although we did not study the concordance of attachment representations in the way Crowell and colleagues did (Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002), the current findings map onto the aforementioned frameworks. Mothers with the highest expectations of support in our study, but with the lowest levels of specific support, were in the greatest distress. Our findings highlight the importance of considering both mothers and their current romantic partnership in attachment-based interventions. Enhancement of secure base script knowledge among mothers through intervention has shown significant benefits in caregiving (Raby et al., Reference Raby, Waters, Tabachnick, Zajac and Dozier2021). When considering attachment intervention development, our results, along with the literature on attachment expectation concordance, suggest that it is essential to extrapolate beyond the context of the mother-infant dyad and intervene with the broader social support system to create more adaptive expectations as well as a network ready to meet those expectations. Thus, clinicians and practitioners may do well to examine whether an individual’s attachment expectations match the actual resources of support in their daily environment.

On the other hand, mothers with minimal expectations of support (i.e. low on script knowledge) showed no psychological benefit from social support. This finding is consistent with prior work indicating that insecurely attached individuals tend to perceive less social support, demonstrate less frequent and persistent help-seeking behavior, and accept less help when it is offered (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Treboux, Gao, Fyffe, Pan and Waters2002; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Rholes and Nelligan1992; Vogel & Wei, Reference Vogel and Wei2005). Our results indicate that secure base script knowledge was positively associated with overall maternal maladjustment. When examining results from our Simple Slope Analysis, mothers who scored lowest on secure base script knowledge reported lower levels of distress at lowest level of perceived social support, compared to their higher secure base script knowledge counterparts. This finding is noteworthy as prior research led us to predict that insecurely attached mothers would report higher levels of psychological distress than securely attached mothers (e.g., Atkinson et al., Reference Atkinson, Paglia, Coolbear, Niccols, Parker and Guger2000). There are several potential explanations for the counter-intuitive pattern of low self-reported distress among mothers with low secure base script knowledge we observed, including: (1) heightened emotion regulation; (2) employing a deactivating/dismissing attachment strategy; (3) social desirability.

Adopting a heightened emotion regulation strategy, including suppressing emotion, disengaging from attachment figures, and using superficial problem-solving strategies, could explain the low levels of self-reported psychological distress among mothers with low secure base script knowledge observed in our sample. In a longitudinal study conducted by Girme et al. (Reference Girme, Jones, Fleck, Simpson and Overall2021), infants who were insecurely attached at 12 and 18 months old displayed higher levels of emotion suppression 30 years later when discussing relationship-threatening situations with their partners. In our sample, it is possible that mothers with low secure base script knowledge tended to avoid emotional experiences and conceal their emotions in communication, and thus reported lower levels of distress during their child’s infancy. Mothers low in secure base script knowledge may have developed this strategy to manage their distress in the absence of secure base support and thus reported lower levels of psychological distress compared to mothers high in secure base script knowledge, whose emotion regulation strategies were more intertwined with their attachment figures. As a result, it is imperative to assess individuals’ strategies for emotion regulation or stress management and include these strategies as moderators in future research.

Another potential explanation for the observed low levels of distress among mothers with low secure base script knowledge in our sample could be the adoption of a deactivating/dismissing attachment strategy. According to Bowlby (Reference Bowlby1980), individuals with insecure attachment tend to deactivate their response (i.e., ignore attachment-related information) and disconnect their affect from the source of distress. A dismissing attachment strategy refers to neglecting or rejecting the conscious awareness of attachment-related information to minimize distress and prevent oneself from being upset and potentially disorganized (Steele & Steele, Reference Steele and Steele2008). Previous research demonstrated the discrepancy between self-reported and other-reported psychiatric symptomatology among individuals with deactivating/dismissing attachment strategies (Dozier & Lee, Reference Dozier and Lee1995). Findings suggested that those relying more on deactivating/dismissing strategies were more likely to report less symptomatology, compared with the amount observed by experts. Thus, our results might have also been influenced by the use of self-report questionnaires to measure maternal psychological adjustments. Mothers with lower secure base script knowledge could have experienced higher levels of postpartum depression, emotion dysregulation, or internalizing and externalizing problems. However, the adoption of a deactivating/dismissing strategy could have led to self-reported lower levels of distress compared with mothers with higher secure base script knowledge who did not utilize a deactivating/dismissing attachment strategy.

Finally, mothers with low secure base script knowledge may have been more likely to conform to social desirability, reporting a joyful, satisfying motherhood, while in fact, their ability to enjoy parenthood and deal with parenting stress was lower (e.g., Martin et al., Reference Martin, Anderson, Groh, Waters, Young, Johnson, Shankman, Eller, Fleck, Steele, Carlson, Simpson and Roisman2018). Thus, mothers with low secure base script knowledge in our sample could have been experiencing greater distress and lower confidence in their caregiving (i.e., parenting self-efficacy; Kohlhoff & Barnett, Reference Kohlhoff and Barnett2013). However, due to social desirability, when mothers were asked to report their distress, they were less likely to accurately report their level of distress and thus reported lower levels of psychological distress.

Despite the strengths of this study and the intriguing results demonstrating the moderating effect of secure base script knowledge on perceived social support as it relates to maternal adjustment, several limitations should be acknowledged. First, due to the onset of COVID-19, attrition in our sample size was relatively high (26.7%). Although the initial sample and those with complete data across all three waves did not significantly differ on maternal education level and family income, the initial sample was more ethnically diverse than those with complete data. Furthermore, although ethnically diverse, our sample was drawn from a normative-risk and highly educated population in Toronto, Canada. According to the most updated Employment Insurance Benefits, mothers in Canada are entitled to have a 55% paid maternity leave for up to 15 weeks, and a parental leave of either 55% paid for up to 40 weeks or 33% paid for up to 69 weeks, which they can share with their partner/spouse (Government of Canada, 2023). In this sense, Canada is a unique context for parents with newborns where mothers are provided with extensive time to recover from childbirth, adjust to the arrival of the newborn, and build their relationship with the baby. Thus, our sample could be at lower risk of postpartum maladjustment compared to samples without these advantages. Furthermore, mothers in this sample who scored low on secure base script knowledge may have been suppressing their emotions or adopting a dismissing strategy, thereby distorting self-reported distress. However, under more stressful circumstances, perhaps these coping strategies would be more likely to break down and cause a sharp increase in the level of reported distress. We proposed that future work should adopt behavioral measures other than self-reported questionnaires and be extended to higher-risk populations as well as mothers transitioning back to work following maternity leave to further understand the interaction effect of secure base script knowledge and perceived social support on maternal adjustment for mothers low in secure base script knowledge.

The clinical implications of these findings are noteworthy. Most broadly, the findings are relevant to the search for personalized, stratified, or precision intervention, with its emphasis on adapting prevention and treatment to individual differences (Academy of Medical Sciences, 2015) in response to the question of what works for whom (Roth & Fonagy, Reference Roth and Fonagy2005). In this regard, Hogan et al. (Reference Hogan, Linden and Najarian2002) noted that “Despite a massive literature on the benefits of support, there is surprisingly little hard evidence about how, and how well, social support interventions work.” After reviewing 100 social support interventions, Hogan et al. (2002) concluded, “there is … not enough evidence to conclude which interventions work best for what problems.” Importantly too, precision medicine stresses the notion of change, and by extension life history, with its focus on the “uniquely evolving health status of each patient” (Kosorok & Laber, Reference Kosorok and Laber2019).

With specific regard to precision intervention and social support, the clinician must be sensitive to the client’s expectations regarding the possibility/utility of social support as developed earlier in life, and their match to current social circumstances. A distressed individual with historically low expectations regarding social support may not benefit from individual or group interventions designed to foster supportive relations (including attachment-based approaches wherein the therapist aims to provide a secure base). Depending on the difficulty and speed of needed resolution, such a client may benefit more from functional support. In contrast, an individual with high social support expectations that are currently unmet may benefit from a support intervention fully and/or faster than individuals with lower social support expectations.

In conclusion, this study showed that maternal perceptions of social support are negatively associated with psychological distress, as prior research indicates (e.g., Cutrona, Reference Cutrona1984; Dennis & Ross, Reference Dennis and Ross2006; Kuscu et al., Reference Kuscu, Akman, Karabekiroglu, Yurdakul, Orhan, Ozdemir, Akman and Ozek2008; Priel & Besser, Reference Priel and Besser2002). However, this association is selective. Mothers with higher script knowledge and low social support reported greater psychological distress; for mothers with lesser script knowledge, social support was unrelated to psychological distress. This pattern suggests that those who expect care (i.e., high secure base script knowledge) but receive minimal support (low perceived social support) find motherhood uniquely dysregulating, emotionally and in terms of internalizing and externalizing difficulties.

Author contribution

The research team would like to note that the first and second authors of this manuscript contributed equally to this work and wish to share the role of first author and have listed themselves in alphabetical order.

Funding statement

This study draws on research supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council, Canada. This work was supported by a fellowship from the American Psychological Foundation.

Competing interests

None.

References

Academy of Medical Sciences (2015, May). Stratified, personalised or P4 medicine: A new direction for placing the patient at the centre of healthcare and health education (Technical report). Archived from the original on 27 October 2016. Retrieved 26 September, 2023.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 974983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002 Google Scholar
Atkinson, L. (2012). Strategic decisions: Life history, interpersonal relations, intergenerational neurobiology, and ethics in parenting and development. Parenting, 12(2-3), 185191. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2012.683356 Google Scholar
Atkinson, L., Paglia, A., Coolbear, J., Niccols, A., Parker, K. C., & Guger, S. (2000). Attachment security: A meta-analysis of maternal mental health correlates. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(8), 10191040. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00023-9 Google Scholar
Beck, C. T., & Gable, R. K. (2001). Comparative analysis of the performance of the Postpartum Depression Screening Scale with two other depression instruments. Nursing Research, 50(4), 242250. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006199-200107000-00008 Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol 3. Loss: Sadness and depression. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brockington, I. (2004). Postpartum psychiatric disorders. The Lancet, 363(9405), 303310. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(03)15390-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canty-Mitchell, J., & Zimet, G. D. (2000). Psychometric properties of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support in urban adolescents. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28(3), 391400. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005109522457 Google Scholar
Caparros-Gonzalez, R. A., Romero-Gonzalez, B., Strivens-Vilchez, H., Gonzalez-Perez, R., Martinez-Augustin, O., Peralta-Ramirez, M. I., & Slattery, D. A. (2017). Hair cortisol levels, psychological stress and psychopathological symptoms as predictors of postpartum depression. PloS One, 12(8), Article e0182817. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182817 Google Scholar
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Rough Guides.Google Scholar
Castellano, R., Velotti, P., Crowell, J. A., & Zavattini, G. C. (2014). The role of parents’ attachment configurations at childbirth on marital satisfaction and conflict strategies. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 10111026.Google Scholar
Castle, H., Slade, P., Barranco-Wadlow, M., & Rogers, M. (2008). Attitudes to emotional expression, social support and postnatal adjustment in new parents. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 26(3), 180194. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646830701691319 Google Scholar
Chiu, Y. H. M., Sheffield, P. E., Hsu, H. H. L., Goldstein, J., Curtin, P. C., & Wright, R. J. (2017). Subconstructs of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale in a multi-ethnic inner-city population in the US. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 20, 803810. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-017-0765-2 Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Gottlieb, B. H., & Underwood, L. G. (2000). Social relationships and health. In Cohen, S., Underwood, L., & Gottlieb, B. H. (Eds.), Social support measurement and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 325). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000). A safe haven: An attachment theory perspective on support seeking and caregiving in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(6), 10531073. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1053 Google Scholar
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150(6), 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782 Google Scholar
Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., Gao, Y., Fyffe, C., Pan, H., & Waters, E. (2002). Assessing secure base behavior in adulthood: Development of a measure, links to adult attachment representations and relations to couples’ communication and reports of relationships. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 679693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutrona, C. E. (1984). Social support and stress in the transition to parenthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93(4), 378390. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.93.4.378 Google Scholar
Dagan, O., Facompré, C. R., & Bernard, K. (2018). Adult attachment representations and depressive symptoms: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 236, 274290.Google Scholar
Dagan, O., Groh, A. M., Madigan, S., & Bernard, K. (2021). A lifespan development theory of insecure attachment and internalizing symptoms: Integrating meta-analytic evidence via a testable evolutionary mis/match hypothesis. Brain Sciences, 11(9), 1226.Google Scholar
Dambi, J. M., Corten, L., Chiwaridzo, M., Jack, H., Mlambo, T., & Jelsma, J. (2018). A systematic review of the psychometric properties of the cross-cultural translations and adaptations of the Multidimensional Perceived Social Support Scale (MSPSS). Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 16(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-018-0912-0 Google Scholar
Davila, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2009). Secure base processes in couples: Daily associations between support experiences and attachment security. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(1), 7688. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014353 Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M. (2009). Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(1), 167. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09000016 Google Scholar
Dennis, C. L., & Ross, L. (2006). Women’s perceptions of partner support and conflict in the development of postpartum depressive symptoms. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 56(6), 588599. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2006.04059.x Google Scholar
Di Florio, A., Putnam, K., Altemus, M., Apter, G., Bergink, V., Bilszta, J., Brock, R., Buist, A., Deligiannidis, K. M., Devouche, E., Epperson, C. N., Guille, C., Kim, D., Lichtenstein, P., Magnusson, P. K. E., Martinez, P., Munk-Olsen, T., Newport, J., Payne, J., …Meltzer-Brody, S. (2017). The impact of education, country, race and ethnicity on the self-report of postpartum depression using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Psychological Medicine, 47(5), 787799. https://doi.org/10.1017%2FS0033291716002087 Google Scholar
Ditzen, B., Schmidt, S., Strauss, B., Nater, U. M., Ehlert, U., & Heinrichs, M. (2008). Adult attachment and social support interact to reduce psychological but not cortisol responses to stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 64(5), 479486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.11.011 Google Scholar
Dozier, M., & Lee, S. W. (1995). Discrepancies between self-and other-report of psychiatric symptomatology: Effects of dismissing attachment strategies. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 217226. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940000643X Google Scholar
Dujardin, A., Santens, T., Braet, C., De Raedt, R., Vos, P., Maes, B., & Bosmans, G. (2016). Middle childhood support-seeking behavior during stress: Links with self-reported attachment and future depressive symptoms. Child Development, 87(1), 326340. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12491 Google Scholar
Eker, D., & Arkar, H. (1995). Perceived social support: Psychometric properties of the MSPSS in normal and pathological groups in a developing country. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 30(3), 121126. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00802040 Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A., Brumbach, B., & Schlomer, G. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7 Google Scholar
Elsenbruch, S., Benson, S., Rücke, M., Rose, M., Dudenhausen, J., Pincus-Knackstedt, M. K., Klapp, B. F., & Arck, P. C. (2007). Social support during pregnancy: Effects on maternal depressive symptoms, smoking and pregnancy outcome. Human Reproduction, 22(3), 869877. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/del432 Google Scholar
Evans, S. E., Steel, A. L., & DiLillo, D. (2013). Child maltreatment severity and adult trauma symptoms: Does perceived social support play a buffering role? Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(11), 934943. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.005 Google Scholar
Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(2), 123151.Google Scholar
Gan, Y., Xiong, R., Song, J., Xiong, X., Yu, F., Gao, W., Hu, H., Zhang, J., Tian, Y., Gu, X., Zhang, J., & Chen, D. (2019). The effect of perceived social support during early pregnancy on depressive symptoms at 6 weeks postpartum: A prospective study. BMC Psychiatry, 19(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186%2Fs12888-019-2188-2 Google Scholar
George, C., & Main, M., & Kaplan, N (1996). Adult attachment interview. Interpersona: An International Journal on Personal Relationships.Google Scholar
Girme, Y. U., Jones, R. E., Fleck, C., Simpson, J. A., & Overall, N. C. (2021). Infants’ attachment insecurity predicts attachment-relevant emotion regulation strategies in adulthood. Emotion, 21(2), 260272. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000721 Google Scholar
Gökdağ, C. (2021). How does interpersonal emotion regulation explain psychological distress? The roles of attachment style and social support. Personality and Individual Differences, 176, Article 110763. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2021.110763 Google Scholar
Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 Google Scholar
Government of Canada (2023). EI maternity and parental benefits: What these benefits offer – Canada.ca. https://www.canada.ca/en/services/benefits/ei/ei-maternity-parental.html Google Scholar
Grande, L. A., Olsavsky, A. K., Erhart, A., Dufford, A. J., Tribble, R., Phan, K. L., & Kim, P. (2021). Postpartum stress and neural regulation of emotion among first-time mothers. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 21(5), 10661082. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-021-00914-9 Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in everyday life. In Snyder, D. K., Simpson, J., & Hughes, J. N. (Eds.), Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 1335). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11468-001 Google Scholar
Haga, S. M., Ulleberg, P., Slinning, K., Kraft, P., Steen, T. B., & Staff, A. (2012). A longitudinal study of postpartum depressive symptoms: Multilevel growth curve analyses of emotion regulation strategies, breastfeeding self-efficacy, and social support. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 15(3), 175184. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-012-0274-2 Google Scholar
Hallion, L. S., Steinman, S. A., Tolin, D. F., & Diefenbach, G. J. (2018). Psychometric properties of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) and its short forms in adults with emotional disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 539. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00539 Google Scholar
Hogan, B. E., Linden, W., & Najarian, B. (2002). Social support interventions: do they work?. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(3), 381440.Google Scholar
House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241(4865), 540545. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3399889 Google Scholar
Kazarian, S. S., & McCabe, S. B. (1991). Dimensions of social support in the MSPSS: Factorial structure, reliability, and theoretical implications. Journal of Community Psychology, 19(2), 150160. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6629(199104)19:23.0.CO;2-J Google Scholar
Kohlhoff, J., & Barnett, B. (2013). Parenting self-efficacy: Links with maternal depression, infant behaviour and adult attachment. Early Human Development, 89(4), 249256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.01.008 Google Scholar
Kosorok, M. R., & Laber, E. B. (2019). Precision medicine. Annual Review of Statistics and Its Application, 6(1), 263286. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-statistics-030718-105251 Google Scholar
Kuscu, M. K., Akman, I., Karabekiroglu, A., Yurdakul, Z., Orhan, L., Ozdemir, N., Akman, M., & Ozek, E. (2008). Early adverse emotional response to childbirth in Turkey: The impact of maternal attachment styles and family support. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology, 29(1), 3338. https://doi.org/10.1080/01674820701535480 Google Scholar
Madigan, S., Brumariu, L. E., Villani, V., Atkinson, L., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2016). Representational and questionnaire measures of attachment: A meta-analysis of relations to child internalizing and externalizing problems. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 367.Google Scholar
Marques, R., Monteiro, F., Canavarro, M. C., & Fonseca, A. (2018). The role of emotion regulation difficulties in the relationship between attachment representations and depressive and anxiety symptoms in the postpartum period. Journal of Affective Disorders, 238, 3946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.013 Google Scholar
Marroquín, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of social support in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(8), 12761290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.005 Google Scholar
Martin, J., Anderson, J. E., Groh, A. M., Waters, T. E. A., Young, E., Johnson, W. F., Shankman, J. L., Eller, J., Fleck, C., Steele, R. D., Carlson, E. A., Simpson, J. A., & Roisman, G. I. (2018). Maternal sensitivity during the first 3½ years of life predicts electrophysiological responding to and cognitive appraisals of infant crying at midlife. Developmental Psychology, 54(10), 19171927. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000579 Google Scholar
Maunder, R. G., Hunter, J. J., Atkinson, L., Steiner, M., Wazana, A., Fleming, A. S., Moss, E., Gaudreau, H., Meaney, M. J., & Levitan, R. D. (2017). An attachment-based model of the relationship between childhood adversity and somatization in children and adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 79(5), 506513.Google Scholar
McCabe-Beane, J. E., Segre, L. S., Perkhounkova, Y., Stuart, S., & O’Hara, M. W. (2016). The identification of severity ranges for the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 34(3), 293303. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646838.2016.1141346 Google Scholar
McConnell, D., Breitkreuz, R., & Savage, A. (2011). From financial hardship to child difficulties: Main and moderating effects of perceived social support. Child: Care, Health and Development, 37(5), 679691. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01185.x Google Scholar
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2019). Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 610.Google Scholar
Nivison, M. D., Vandell, D. L., Booth-LaForce, C., & Roisman, G. I. (2021). Convergent and discriminant validity of retrospective assessments of the quality of childhood parenting: Prospective evidence from infancy to age 26 years. Psychological Science, 32(5), 721734. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620975775 Google Scholar
Priel, B., & Besser, A. (2002). Perceptions of early relationships during the transition to motherhood: The mediating role of social support. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health, 23(4), 343360. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10021 Google Scholar
Raby, K. L., Waters, T. E., Tabachnick, A. R., Zajac, L., & Dozier, M. (2021). Increasing secure base script knowledge among parents with Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-Up. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 554564.Google Scholar
Rapoza, K. A., Vassell, K., Wilson, D. T., Robertson, T. W., Manzella, D. J., Ortiz-Garcia, A. L., & Jimenez-Lazar, L. A. (2016). Attachment as a moderating factor between social support, physical health, and psychological symptoms. SAGE Open, 6(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244016682818 Google Scholar
Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Friedman, M. (2006). Avoidant attachment and the experience of parenting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(3), 275285. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205280910 Google Scholar
Roisman, G. I., Holland, A., Fortuna, K., Fraley, R. C., Clausell, E., & Clarke, A. (2007). The Adult Attachment Interview and self-reports of attachment style: An empirical rapprochement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 678697. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.678 Google Scholar
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02 Google Scholar
Roth, A., & Fonagy, P. (2005). What works for whom?: A critical review of psychotherapy research (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ruiz, S. K., Waters, T. E., & Yates, T. M. (2020). Children’s secure base script knowledge as a mediator between early life stress and later behavior problems. Attachment & Human Development, 22, 627642.Google Scholar
Sarason, B. R., Pierce, G. R., Shearin, E. N., Sarason, I. G., Waltz, J. A., & Poppe, L. (1991). Perceived social support and working models of self and actual others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 273287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.273 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, M. A., & Price, S. W. (2005). Exploring the economic and social determinants of psychological well-being and perceived social support in England. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society), 168(3), 513537. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-985X.2005.00361.x Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A. (2019). Bringing life history theory into relationship science. Personal Relationships, 26(1), 420. https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12269 Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attachment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(3), 434446. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.3.434 Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2009). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Statistics, Canada (2019). Market income, government transfers, total income, income tax and after-tax income by economic family type. https://doi.org/10.25318/1110019001-eng Google Scholar
Steele, H., & Steele, M. (Eds.). (2008). Clinical applications of the adult attachment interview. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Steele, R. D., Waters, T. E., Bost, K. K., Vaughn, B. E., Truitt, W., Waters, H. S., Booth-LaForce, C., & Roisman, G. I. (2014). Caregiving antecedents of secure base script knowledge: A comparative analysis of young adult attachment representations. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2526.Google Scholar
Stewart, T., & Suldo, S. (2011). Relationships between social support sources and early adolescents’ mental health: The moderating effect of student achievement level. Psychology in the Schools, 48(10), 10161033. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20607 Google Scholar
Szepsenwol, O., & Simpson, J. A. (2019). Attachment within life history theory: An evolutionary perspective on individual differences in attachment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 6570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.03.005 Google Scholar
Victor, S. E., & Klonsky, E. D. (2016). Validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS-18) in five samples. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38, 582589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-016-9547-9 Google Scholar
Vogel, D. L., & Wei, M. (2005). Adult attachment and help-seeking intent: The mediating roles of psychological distress and perceived social support. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(3), 347357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.3.347 Google Scholar
Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure base from which to explore close relationships. Child Development, 71(1), 164172.Google Scholar
Waters, H. S., & Waters, E. (2006). The attachment working models concept: Among other things, we build script-like representations of secure base experiences. Attachment & Human Development, 8, 185197. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730600856016 Google Scholar
Waters, H. S., Waters, T. E. A., & Waters, E. (2021). From internal working models to script-like attachment representations. In Thompson, R. A., Simpson, J. A., & Berlin, L. (Eds.), Attachment: The fundamental questions (pp. 111119). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Waters, T. E., Yang, R., Finet, C., Verhees, M. W., & Bosmans, G. (2022). An empirical test of prototype and revisionist models of attachment stability and change from middle childhood to adolescence: A 6-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 93(1), 225236.Google Scholar
Waters, T. E. A., Bosmans, G., Vandevivere, E., Dujardin, A., & Waters, H. S. (2015). Secure base representations in middle childhood across two Western cultures: Associations with parental attachment representations and maternal reports of behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 51(8), 10131025. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039375 Google Scholar
Waters, T. E. A., & Roisman, G. I. (2019). The secure base script concept: An overview. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 162166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.08.002 Google Scholar
Wenzel, A., Haugen, E. N., Jackson, L. C., & Brendle, J. R. (2005). Anxiety symptoms and disorders at eight weeks postpartum. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(3), 295311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.001 Google Scholar
Yağmur, Y., & Ulukoca, N. (2010). Social support and postpartum depression in low socioeconomic level postpartum women in Eastern Turkey. International Journal of Public Health, 55(6), 543549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-010-0182-z Google Scholar
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(1), 3041. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Timeline of data collection.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of correlations and descriptive statistics

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of multiple hierarchical regression models with FIML predicting composite maternal maladjustment

Figure 3

Figure 2. SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and maternal maladjustment. Note. SBSK: Secure Base Script Knowledge; Results based on listwise deletion (n = 97); Statistical significance of simple slopes indicated by *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Figure 4

Table 3. Results of multiple regression models with FIML estimating the influence of interactions between SBSK and perceived social support on depression, emotion regulation, internalizing and externalizing problems

Figure 5

Figure 3. (a) SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and emotion dysregulation. (b) SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and internalizing problems. (c) SBSK moderating effect between perceived social support and externalizing problems. Note. SBSK: Secure Base Script Knowledge; Results based on listwise deletion (n = 97); Statistical significance of simple slopes indicated by *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.