Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T20:45:48.103Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gender, Race, and Interruptions at Supreme Court Confirmation Hearings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2024

CHRISTINA L. BOYD*
Affiliation:
University of Georgia, United States
PAUL M. COLLINS JR.*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts Amherst, United States
LORI A. RINGHAND*
Affiliation:
University of Georgia, United States
*
Corresponding author: Christina L. Boyd, Professor of Political Science, Thomas P. and M. Jean Lauth Public Affairs Professor, Department of Political Science, University of Georgia, United States, [email protected].
Paul M. Collins, Jr., Professor of Legal Studies and Political Science, Department of Political Science, University of Massachusetts Amherst, United States, [email protected].
Lori A. Ringhand, J. Alton Hosch Professor of Law, Josiah Meigs Distinguished Teaching Professor, School of Law, University of Georgia, United States, [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this research letter, we examine whether gender and racial bias affect interruption rates at one of the most visible events in American politics: US Supreme Court confirmation hearings. Using original data from 1939 to 2022, we find that male and white participants are more likely to interrupt women and person of color speakers, respectively, relative to male and white speakers. This finding holds for both senators and nominees as interrupters. Our results provide evidence that biased interruptive behavior occurs in even the most public and salient of political settings and that it can be mitigated (or intensified) by shared (or opposite) partisanship among speaking pairs. We also find interruption inequalities are not isolated to women as the interrupted, revealing that people of color in political and legal settings are subject to heightened rates of interruptions as well.

Type
Letter
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association

INTRODUCTION

US Supreme Court confirmation hearings before the Senate Judiciary Committee are incredibly important. With high levels of public interest and gavel-to-gavel television coverage, the hearings provide a unique opportunity for the world to observe and develop opinions about senators, nominees, and the usually opaque Supreme Court. For nominees, the hearings are their primary chance to defend their judicial record or develop credibility—all of which can help gain senator confirmation votes and develop public support for their service on the Court. For senators, the Judiciary Committee is a coveted assignment, and Supreme Court confirmation hearings offer them an excellent opportunity to increase constituent support, raise their national profile, and even launch a White House bid (Collins and Ringhand Reference Collins and Ringhand2016; Schoenherr, Lane, and Armaly Reference Schoenherr, Lane and Armaly2020).

But not all hearing participants have equal opportunity to speak unimpeded, as Supreme Court nominee Ketanji Brown Jackson found out during her 2022 hearing. In the course of just minutes of questioning from Senator Graham (R-SC), Jackson was interrupted three times.Footnote 1 Some senators also have this experience. For example, in her 2018 questioning of Brett Kavanaugh, Senator Hirono (D-HI) was interrupted multiple times by the nominee.

In this high-profile setting, interruptions matter, especially when disproportionally directed to the speech of women and person of color senators and nominees. Interruptions silence the original speaker and elevate the power of the interrupter, which may aggravate the challenges already facing women and people of color in high-profile settings. Women and person of color senators are at a numerical disadvantage in Congress and face challenges in winning elections and governing once elected (e.g., Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2011; McDermott Reference McDermott1998; Palmer and Simon Reference Palmer and Simon2006; Shah Reference Shah2015). Interruption inequities can be an additional blow. For the nominees, being interrupted may stall their efforts to overcome the competence bias women and people of color face in interviews across professions (Boyd, Collins, and Ringhand Reference Boyd, Collins and Ringhand2018; Christensen, Szmer, and Stritch Reference Christensen, Szmer and Stritch2012; Haynie Reference Haynie2002; Lawless Reference Lawless2004; Nelson Reference Nelson2015). More broadly, since legitimacy and public support rest on representative inclusion in governing institutions (Badas and Stauffer Reference Badas and Stauffer2018; Barnes Reference Barnes2016; Clayton, O’Brien, and Piscopo Reference Clayton, O’Brien and Piscopo2018; Harris and Sen Reference Harris and Sen2019; Means, Eslich, and Prado Reference Means, Eslich, Prado, Sterett and Walker2019; Scherer Reference Scherer2023; Stauffer Reference Stauffer2021; Widner Reference Widner2023), public displays of bias against women and people of color can result in lower rates of ambition for members of those groups (Fox and Lawless Reference Fox and Lawless2014; Williams Reference Williams2008) and aggravate a sense that our government does not represent them (Campbell and Wolbrecht Reference Campbell and Wolbrecht2006).

We examine interruptions coming from societal majority group members like men and white people and the likelihood that they will occur more frequently for women and person of color participants in Supreme Court confirmation hearings, particularly when those participants are of the opposite party of the would-be interrupter. We utilize almost 40,000 senator and nominee statements from 1939 to 2022 to analyze confirmation hearing interruptions. Our findings indicate that cross-party women and people of color face substantially higher rates of interruptions than others.

These results are an important addition to the existing literature in three ways. First, we are the first to show that biased interruptions come from both senators and nominees. Even in a situation in which male and white individuals are presumptively in a position of less power than their senatorial interrogators—testifying for a coveted Supreme Court seat for which they need senatorial votes—they still interrupt female senators and senators of color at higher rates than others. Second, while prior political science literature studying interruptions has, to our knowledge, exclusively focused on gendered patterns, our findings confirm that nominees and senators of color are also subject to heightened interruption rates. Third, we show that bias in interruptions occurs even in high-profile settings that garner substantial media attention and interest from the American public.

INTERRUPTIONS, RACE, GENDER, AND PARTISANSHIP

Whether in Congress, the courtroom, or elsewhere, turn-taking—where a single person serves as the speaker while others act as listeners—is the norm in group speaking situations (Beattie Reference Beattie1982). Allowing speech without interruption signals respectful discourse (Bresnahan and Cai Reference Bresnahan and Cai1996; Goldberg Reference Goldberg1990) and is cognitively necessary since we struggle to “talk and listen simultaneously” (Beattie Reference Beattie1982, 93). Interruptions break conversational turn-taking norms. When another person interrupts before the prior speaker’s turn is finished, the interruption stops the speaker from continuing and disorganizes the future speech (Smith-Lovin and Brody Reference Smith-Lovin and Brody1989). Interruptions thus can be a conversational “power play” (Mendelberg, Karpowitz, and Oliphant Reference Mendelberg, Karpowitz and Oliphant2014) that control or alter speech and grant dominance to the interrupter (Karpowitz, Mendelberg, and Mattioli Reference Karpowitz, Mendelberg and Mattioli2015; Leman and Ikoko Reference Leman and Ikoko2010).

In political settings, interruptions enable control over the discussion and achievement of outcome goals. In congressional hearings, for example, interruptions loudly signal conflict among participants, with policy conflict likely to emerge thereafter (Miller and Sutherland Reference Miller and Sutherland2023). During Supreme Court oral arguments, justices’ interruptions prevent their colleagues from coordinating or signaling their preferences (Black, Johnson, and Wedeking Reference Black, Johnson and Wedeking2012; Johnson, Black, and Wedeking Reference Johnson, Black and Wedeking2009) and reduce the likelihood of justices voting together (Jacobi and Rozema Reference Jacobi and Rozema2018).

Race and Gender Bias

Interruptions can happen among any speaker pairings. However, when majority group members (like men and white people) are the would-be interrupters, interruptions may be particularly likely to be targeted at women and person of color speakers. The combination of bias, status, negative stereotypes, and incentives to grow power and derogate subordinates can result in powerful group members inferring (consciously or not) entitlement to seize the conversational floor from women and person of color speakers (Anderson and Leaper Reference Anderson and Leaper1998; Christensen, Szmer, and Stritch Reference Christensen, Szmer and Stritch2012; Moyer et al. Reference Moyer, Szmer, Haire and Christensen2021; Smith-Lovin and Brody Reference Smith-Lovin and Brody1989). Inequities in who is interrupted may further entrench the power of majority classes and signal weakness among those with less power (Mendelberg, Karpowitz, and Oliphant Reference Mendelberg, Karpowitz and Oliphant2014).

Women and people of color face more interruptions than male and white speakers in many settings (e.g., Leman and Ikoko Reference Leman and Ikoko2010). This pattern holds in political and legal contexts, although political science research to date has largely focused on gendered interruptions and ignored race-related effects. Women—including congresspeople (Miller and Sutherland Reference Miller and Sutherland2023) and witnesses (Mattei Reference Mattei1998)—are interrupted more than men during congressional hearings (but see Kathlene Reference Kathlene1994). In courts, female judges, justices, and lawyers are interrupted more than men (Bogoch Reference Bogoch1999; Cortina et al. Reference Cortina, Lonsway, Magley, Freeman, Collinsworth, Hunter and Fitzgerald2002; Feldman and Gill Reference Feldman and Gill2019; Jacobi and Schweers Reference Jacobi and Schweers2017; Patton and Smith Reference Patton and Smith2017).

The Conditioning Effect of Partisanship

The activation of inequalities in interruptions is likely dependent on the relationship status and worldview perspectives of the speaker and would-be interrupter. Individuals who do not know each other well are more likely to have higher interruption levels (James and Clarke Reference James, Clarke and Tannen1993). By contrast, “friends” interrupt each other less frequently and alternate speaking turns (Bresnahan and Cai Reference Bresnahan and Cai1996; Smith-Lovin and Brody Reference Smith-Lovin and Brody1989). This has been confirmed in Supreme Court oral arguments, where interruptions rise among ideological foes (Johnson, Black, and Wedeking Reference Johnson, Black and Wedeking2009) and when justices’ ideology and gender are different (Jacobi and Schweers Reference Jacobi and Schweers2017).

On the Judiciary Committee, we expect that shared party will lessen the gendered and racial effects of majority group members’ interruptive behavior, while opposite-party affiliation will heighten such effects. The modern Senate is highly partisan, and its committees rely on party-based teamwork (Lee Reference Lee2009). Senators will be more likely to view shared-party nominees as “friends” than those appointed by opposing-party presidents (regardless of race or gender). The same should hold for nominees interacting with senators. With shared party, there will be little hostility to the nomination and less need for aggressiveness in exchanges. By contrast, opposite-party speakers may be seen as “different,” without the mitigating salve of a shared partisanship. Questioning senators are incentivized by partisan loyalties to behave assertively, and nominees will instinctively view cross-party senators as foes. These opposite-party, female and person of color speakers, are thus likely to receive a higher rate of interruptions than their shared party counterparts.

DATA AND METHODS

To study majority group members’ interruption patterns, we examine every public Supreme Court confirmation hearing held before the Senate Judiciary Committee to date (1939–2022).Footnote 2 The unit of analysis is the nominee–senator dyad, with one observation per nominee-senator pair per hearing.Footnote 3 Our main analysis focuses on intrusive interruptions (attempts to take over the conversation) and excludes backchannel interruptions (signs of active conversational listening).Footnote 4 The dependent variables are the percentage of time the senator/nominee intrusively interrupted the nominee/senator they were paired with as a function of all statements they made at each hearing. Our primary analysis is limited, for our gender-centric models, to interruptions made by male senators (or nominees) and, for our race-centric models, to interruptions by white senators (or nominees). We estimate ordinary least squares regression models and cluster the standard errors on the would-be interrupter.

We include independent variables that capture the gender/race of the nominee or senator and their party status relative to the individual they are interacting with. For example, in the model that examines interruptions of nominees by male senators, we include three variables: Female Nominee, Same Party; Female Nominee, Different Party; and Male Nominee, Different Party (with Male Nominee, Same Party as the baseline category). We expect that all three of these variables will be positively signed relative to the baseline, with female nominees from the opposite party of the questioning senator being interrupted more than all other nominee types. We use a similar modeling strategy for models focusing on interruptions by white senators, male nominees, and white nominees. We also include control variables in the models to account for other participant and contextual factors that may affect interruption patterns (detailed in Supplementary Appendix). Our variables’ expected directional effects are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1. Expected Directions of Independent Variables

INTERRUPTIONS BY SENATORS

Figure 1 reports the coefficients and 95% confidence intervals for the statistical models focused on senators intrusively interrupting nominees.Footnote 5 The top panel focuses on gender differences and reveals that male senators interrupt female, different-party nominees 4.5 percentage points more often than same-party male nominees (the baseline category). We also find that male senators interrupt male, different-party nominees 3.8 percentage points more than male, same-party nominees, but there is no statistically significant difference between interruptions of female and male, same-party nominees.Footnote 6 These results provide clear evidence that male senators exhibit gendered interruption patterns, interrupting female nominees from the opposite party more often than male, same-party nominees.

Figure 1. Intrusive Interruptions by Senators, 1939–2022

Note: Circles are regression coefficients and lines represent 95% confidence intervals (robust standard errors, clustered on speaker). Unit of analysis is senator–nominee dyad, and dependent variable is percentage of interruptions made by senators during their nominee interaction. See Table A1 in the Supplementary Appendix for more details.

Our regression results plotted in Figure 1’s bottom panel similarly reveal strong evidence of racial differences in the interruption behavior of white senators. In particular, white senators interrupt opposite-party nominees of color 6.5 percentage points more than white, same-party nominees. This is, by far, the largest coefficient in the model and one that is statistically significantly larger than every other nominee grouping in the model based on equality of coefficient tests (see Supplementary Appendix).Footnote 7 Compared to white nominees from the opposite-party, nominees of color from the opposite party are interrupted a shocking 108% more frequently.

INTERRUPTIONS BY NOMINEES

We now turn the tables on hearing participants and explore in Figure 2 intrusive interruptions of senators by the nominees. As one would expect in the job interview–like format of a confirmation hearing, interviewees are less interruptive than their interviewers: nominees interrupt senators about half as much as they are interrupted by them (2.1% of statements compared to 4.3%). Those interruptions are not equally distributed, however, with male nominees and white nominees’ interruption patterns revealing sharp gendered and racialized behavior. Compared to their behavior toward same-party, male senators, male nominees interrupt female, different-party senators 6.8 percentage points more (Figure 2, top panel). Equality of coefficients tests reveal that female, different-party senators also receive greater interruptions than other groupings (male, different-party and female, same-party senators). There is therefore clear evidence that male nominees treat female senators from the opposite party differently in their interruptive behavior than all other senators.

Figure 2. Intrusive Interruptions by Nominees, 1939–2022

Note: Circles are regression coefficients and lines represent 95% confidence intervals (robust standard errors, clustered on speaker). Unit of analysis is senator–nominee dyad, and dependent variable is percentage of interruptions made by nominees during their senator interaction. See Table A2 in the Supplementary Appendix for more details.

The bottom panel in Figure 2 shows a similar pattern for white nominees as interrupters. Compared to white same-party senators, white nominees interrupt senators of color from the opposite party 5.6 percentage points more. And, per equality of coefficients tests, opposite-party senators of color are interrupted more than same-party senators of color.Footnote 8 As with the interruption patterns for other nominees and senators, these are substantively large effects revealing that white nominees intrusively interrupt opposite-party senators of color almost three times as much as is average for nominees.

CONCLUSIONS

Interrupters hold power to assert dominance, take the conversational floor, and disorganize the speech of another. Despite the salience and stakes attached to Supreme Court confirmation hearings, they are not immune to interruptive behavior by participants. Interruptions are not only frequent in this setting, but they are also unevenly dispersed. As our study reveals, women and person of color speakers, when paired with a male or white speech partner during the hearings,Footnote 9 face a disproportionate number of interruptions relative to other speakers—especially when the speech pair does not share the same political party.

Viewed as a whole, our analyses present multiple contributions of note to the study of American politics, Congress, the legal system, and race and gender bias in society.Footnote 10 First, gender and racial biased interruptive behavior is not isolated to private conversations, in nonpublic arguments at the Supreme Court, or in sparsely attended congressional committee hearings; rather, it also happens at what is likely the most prominent of congressional hearings. Second, this interruptive bias is rampant, extending from members of Congress who hold the upper hand in power and have the “questioner” status during these hearings to nominees themselves in ways that largely mirror what we see from interrupting senators. It is also not limited to gender. While racial bias in interruptions has been largely unexamined in political and legal settings, our findings indicate it is just as potent as is gender bias. And finally, like with so much in politics, shared party and political worldview can mitigate the emergence of interruptive biases, while the lack of this common political perspective among a speaking pair leaves a gulf of space for biases to fill. And that is exactly what we see during the Senate Judiciary Committee’s Supreme Court confirmation hearings.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055424000145.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Research documentation and data that support the findings of this study are openly available at the American Political Science Review Dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/C2IATB.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

A previous version of this project was presented at the 2023 Annual Meeting of the New England Political Science Association, 2022 Conference for Empirical Legal Studies, and faculty workshops at the University of Virginia and George Mason University law schools. We are grateful to our colleagues for their feedback on this research, including Scott Blinder, Jennifer Bowie, Roberto Carlos, James Cooper, Pam Corley, Josh Fischman, Justin Gross, Susan Haire, Rebecca Hamlin, Morgan Hazelton, Rachael Hinkle, Alex Hindman, Gbemende Johnson, Sally Kenney, Ray La Raja, Monika Lindbekk, Wendy Martinek, Lauren McCarthy, Alison Merrill, Greg Mitchell, Paul Musgrave, Michael Nelson, Tatishe Nteta, Rachel Potter, Jesse Rhodes, Meredith Rolfe, Jamie Rowen, Geoffrey Sheagley, Barbara Spellman, Amy Steigerwalt, Susan Sterett, Art Ward, and Leah Wing. We received invaluable research assistance from the following students: Madison Alvis, Matt Baker, Juliana Bird, Brie Bristol, Rosa Brown, Haley Carman, Rayani Chonmany, Jenah Clarkson, Madison Conkel, Mitchell Director, Jordan Dyer, Gillian Gaines, Caroline Greenlaw, Riley Grube, Grace Hayes, Ryan Heilmann, Julianna Hightower, Emma Hupp, Telly Jacobs, Eshaa Joshi, Nadeen Jumai’an, Caitlyn Kinard, Kate Kostel, Chloe Levy, Elisabeth Lopez, Bryce McManus, Maggie Mendoza, Alaina Moore, Brianna Owen, Moksha Padmaraju, Haley Patel, Katie Pitner, Kiana Powers, Elise Puschett, Emma Rowr, Sam Rusling-Flynn, Adam Rutkowski, Kyle Venit, Allison Vick, Nora Vonmoltke-Simms, Lizzy Walker, Veronica Walsh, Megan Wan, Emily Willard, and Jessie Zacune.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no ethical issues or conflicts of interest in this research.

ETHICAL STANDARDS

The authors affirm this research did not involve human subjects.

Footnotes

1 See the Supplementary Appendix for this and other hearing transcript excerpts referenced in the text.

2 Full details on data sourcing, coding, intercoder reliability, and control variable coding and expectations are provided in the Supplementary Appendix.

3 Miller and Sutherland (Reference Miller and Sutherland2023) instead use a “chunk” unit of analysis approach where each exchange between a senator and nominee is an observation. The Supplementary Appendix further details this alternative strategy and the additional control variables related to the dynamics of the exchange between the speaker and would-be interrupter it permits, replicates our modeling utilizing the “chunk” approach, and explains our rationale for utilizing the “dyad” approach in the main text. Of note, our results are similar using the “dyad” and “chunk” approaches, with differences highlighted in the Supplementary Appendix. In all, the Supplementary Appendix includes the results of 40 alternative modeling approaches, most (but not all) of which corroborate the key results in the manuscript.

4 Our findings are robust to the inclusion of backchannel interruptions (see Supplementary Appendix).

5 Full regression results and equality of coefficients tests are provided in the Supplementary Appendix.

6 Equality of coefficients tests indicate that there are not statistically significant differences between the volume of interruptions targeted at female, different-party nominees compared to male, different-party or female, same-party nominees (see Supplementary Appendix).

7 p = 0.06 in the comparison between opposite-party nominees of color and white nominees.

8 The comparison with white, different-party senators falls short of statistical significance (p < 0.10).

9 Relative to the rich literature on majority group members as interrupters, much remains unknown about the behavior of woman and people of color as potential interrupters. In the Supplementary Appendix, we provide an initial examination of the interruptive behavior of women and people of color in our data. Our supplemental analysis is limited not only by theoretical uncertainty in the literature about nonmajority group interruptive behavior but also by empirics: there are a small number of women and person of color senators and nominees to serve as potential interrupters. As the Judiciary Committee continues to diversity in the coming years, this area will be ripe for additional inquiry.

10 Future projects may also find it fruitful to examine whether similar interruption patterns hold for lower court and nonjudicial Judiciary Committee hearings and whether interruption frequency varies in private versus public settings.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, Kristin J., and Leaper, Campbell. 1998. “Meta-Analyses of Gender Effects on Conversational Interruption: Who, What, When, Where, and How.” Sex Roles 39 (3): 225–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badas, Alex, and Stauffer, Katelyn E.. 2018. “Someone Like Me: Descriptive Representation and Support for Supreme Court Nominees.” Political Research Quarterly 71 (1): 127–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, Tiffany D. 2016. Gendering Legislative Behavior: Institutional Constraints and Collaboration. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, Geoffrey W. 1982. “Turn-Taking and Interruption in Political Interviews: Margaret Thatcher and Jim Callaghan Compared and Contrasted.” Semiotica 39 (1): 93114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, Ryan C., Johnson, Timothy R., and Wedeking, Justin. 2012. Oral Arguments and Coalition Formation on the U.S. Supreme Court: A Deliberate Dialogue. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Bogoch, Bryna. 1999. “Judging in a ‘Different Voice’: Gender and the Sentencing of Violent Offences in Israel.” International Journal of Sociology of Law 27 (1): 5178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, Christina L., Collins, Paul M. Jr., and Ringhand, Lori A.. 2018. “The Role of Nominee Gender and Race at U.S. Supreme Court Confirmation Hearings.” Law and Society Review 52 (4): 871901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, Christina L., Collins, Paul M. Jr., and Ringhand, Lori A.. 2024. “Replication Data for: Gender, Race, and Interruptions at Supreme Court Confirmation Hearings.” Harvard Dataverse. Dataset. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/C2IATB.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bresnahan, Mary I., and Cai, Deborah H.. 1996. “Gender and Aggression in the Recognition of Interruption.” Discourse Processes 21 (2): 171–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, David E., and Wolbrecht, Christina. 2006. “See Jane Run: Women Politicians as Role Models for Adolescents.” Journal of Politics 68 (2): 233–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, Robert K., Szmer, John, and Stritch, Justin M.. 2012. “Race and Gender Bias in Three Administrative Contexts: Impact on Work Assignments in State Supreme Courts.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 22 (4): 625–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, Amanda, O’Brien, Diana Z., and Piscopo, Jennifer M.. 2018. “All Male Panels? Representation and Democratic Legitimacy.” American Journal of Political Science 63 (1): 113–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, Paul M, and Ringhand, Lori A.. 2016. “The Institutionalization of Supreme Court Confirmation Hearings.” Law and Social Inquiry 41 (1): 126–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortina, Lilia M., Lonsway, Kimberly A., Magley, Vicki J., Freeman, Leslie V., Collinsworth, Linda L., Hunter, Mary, and Fitzgerald, Louise F.. 2002. “What’s Gender Got to Do with it? Incivility in the Federal Courts.” Law and Social Inquiry 27 (2): 235–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, Adam, and Gill, Rebecca D.. 2019. “Power Dynamics in Supreme Court Oral Arguments: The Relationship between Gender and Justice-to-Justice Interruptions.” Justice System Journal 40 (3): 173–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Richard L., and Lawless, Jennifer L.. 2014. “Uncovering the Origins of the Gender Gap in Political Ambition.” American Political Science Review 108 (3): 499519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Julia A. 1990. “Interrupting the Discourse on Interruptions: An Analysis in Terms of Relationally Neutral, Power and Rapport Oriented Acts.” Journal of Pragmatics 14 (6): 883903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Allison P., and Sen, Maya. 2019. “Bias and Judging.” Annual Review of Political Science 22: 241–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haynie, Kerry L. 2002. “The Color of their Skin or the Content of their Behavior? Race and Perceptions of African American Legislators.” Legislative Studies Quarterly 27 (2): 295314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, Mirya R., Merolla, Jennifer L., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2011. “Sex, Stereotypes, and Security: A Study of the Effects of Terrorist Threat on Assessments of Female Leadership.” Journal of Women, Politics and Policy 32 (3): 173–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobi, Tonja, and Rozema, Kyle. 2018. “Judicial Conflicts and Voting Agreement: Evidence from Interruptions at Oral Argument.” Boston College Law Review 59 (7): 2259–317.Google Scholar
Jacobi, Tonja, and Schweers, Dylan. 2017. “Justice, Interrupted: The Effect of Gender, Ideology and Seniority at Supreme Court Oral Arguments.” Virginia Law Review 103 (7): 1379–496.Google Scholar
James, Deborah, and Clarke, Sandra. 1993. “Women, Men, and Interruptions: A Critical Review.” In Gender and Conversational Interaction, ed. Tannen, Deborah, 231–59. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Timothy R., Black, Ryan C., and Wedeking, Justin. 2009. “Pardon the Interruption: An Empirical Analysis of Supreme Court Justices’ Behavior during Oral Arguments.” Loyola Law Review 55: 331–51.Google Scholar
Karpowitz, Christopher F., Mendelberg, Tali, and Mattioli, Lauren. 2015. “Why Women’s Numbers Elevate Women’s Influence, and When they Do Not: Rules, Norms, and Authority in Political Discussion.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 3 (1): 149–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kathlene, Lyn. 1994. “Power and Influence in State Legislative Policymaking: The Interaction of Gender and Position in Committee Hearing Debates.” American Political Science Review 88 (3): 560–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawless, Jennifer L. 2004. “Women, War, and Winning Elections: Gender Stereotyping in the Post September 11th Era.” Political Research Quarterly 53 (3): 479–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Frances E. 2009. Beyond Ideology: Politics, Principles, and Partisanship in the U.S. Senate. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leman, Patrick, and Ikoko, Theresa. 2010. “Interruption in Women’s Conversations: The Effects of Context in Ethnic Majority and Minority Group Interactions.” Psychology of Language and Communication 14 (1): 6170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattei, Laura R. Winsky. 1998. “Gender and Power in American Legislative Discourse.” Journal of Politics 60 (2): 440–61.Google Scholar
McDermott, Monika L. 1998. “Race and Gender Cues in Low-Information Elections.” Political Research Quarterly 51 (4): 895918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Means, Taneisha, Eslich, Andrew, and Prado, Kaitlin. 2019. “Judicial Diversity in the United States Federal Judiciary.” In Research Handbook in Law and Courts, eds. Sterett, Susan M., and Walker, Lee D., 231–45. Cheltenham, UK: Elgar Publishing.Google Scholar
Mendelberg, Tali, Karpowitz, Christopher F., and Oliphant, J. Baxter. 2014. “Gender Inequality in Deliberation: Unpacking the Black Box of Interaction.” Perspectives on Politics 12 (1): 1844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Michael G., and Sutherland, Joseph L.. 2023. “The Effect of Gender on Interruptions at Congressional Hearings.” American Political Science Review 117 (1): 103–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyer, Laura P., Szmer, John, Haire, Susan, and Christensen, Robert K.. 2021. “‘All Eyes Are on You’: Gender, Race, and Opinion Writing on the US Courts of Appeals.” Law and Society Review 55 (3): 452–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Kjersten. 2015. “Double-Bind on the Bench: Citizen Perceptions of Judge Gender and the Court.” Politics and Gender 11 (2): 235–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, Barbara, and Simon, Dennis. 2006. Breaking the Political Glass Ceiling: Women and Congressional Elections. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Patton, Dana, and Smith, Joseph L.. 2017. “Lawyer, Interrupted: Gender Bias in Oral Arguments at the US Supreme Court.” Journal of Law and Courts 5 (2): 337–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, Nancy. 2023. Diversifying the Courts: Race, Gender, and Judicial Legitimacy. New York: New York University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoenherr, Jessica A., Lane, Elizabeth A., and Armaly, Miles T.. 2020. “The Purpose of Senatorial Grandstanding during Supreme Court Confirmation Hearings.” Journal of Law and Courts 8 (2): 333–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, Paru. 2015. “Stepping up: Black Political Ambition and Success.” Politics, Groups and Identities 3 (2): 278–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith-Lovin, Lynn, and Brody, Charles. 1989. “Interruptions in Group Discussions: The Effects of Gender and Group Composition.” American Sociological Review 54 (3): 424–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stauffer, Katelyn E. 2021. “Public Perceptions of Women’s Inclusion and Feelings of Political Efficacy.” American Political Science Review 115 (4): 1226–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widner, Kirsten. 2023. “The Supreme Court and the Limits of Descriptive Representation.” Polity 55 (2): 380–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, Margaret S. 2008. “Ambition, Gender, and the Judiciary.” Political Research Quarterly 61 (1): 6878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Expected Directions of Independent Variables

Figure 1

Figure 1. Intrusive Interruptions by Senators, 1939–2022Note: Circles are regression coefficients and lines represent 95% confidence intervals (robust standard errors, clustered on speaker). Unit of analysis is senator–nominee dyad, and dependent variable is percentage of interruptions made by senators during their nominee interaction. See Table A1 in the Supplementary Appendix for more details.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Intrusive Interruptions by Nominees, 1939–2022Note: Circles are regression coefficients and lines represent 95% confidence intervals (robust standard errors, clustered on speaker). Unit of analysis is senator–nominee dyad, and dependent variable is percentage of interruptions made by nominees during their senator interaction. See Table A2 in the Supplementary Appendix for more details.

Supplementary material: File

Boyd et al. supplementary material

Boyd et al. supplementary material
Download Boyd et al. supplementary material(File)
File 764 KB
Supplementary material: File

Boyd_et_al._Dataset

Dataset

Download Boyd_et_al._Dataset(File)
File
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.