Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T06:24:56.829Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural contributions to adults' self-rated mental health problems and strengths: 7 culture clusters, 28 societies, 16 906 adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2023

William E. Copeland*
Affiliation:
University of Vermont, 1 South Prospect Street, Burlington, VT 05401, USA
Masha Y. Ivanova
Affiliation:
University of Vermont, 1 South Prospect Street, Burlington, VT 05401, USA
Thomas M. Achenbach
Affiliation:
University of Vermont, 1 South Prospect Street, Burlington, VT 05401, USA
Lori V. Turner
Affiliation:
University of Vermont, 1 South Prospect Street, Burlington, VT 05401, USA
Guangyu Tong
Affiliation:
Yale University, 135 College St, New Haven, CT 06510, USA
Adelina Ahmeti-Pronaj
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Clinical Center of Kosova, 10000 Prishtine, Kosova
Alma Au
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Social Sciences, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hung Hom, Kowloon, Hong Kong, China
Monica Bellina
Affiliation:
Department of Child Psychiatry, Eugenio Medea Scientific Institute, 7 Padiglione, Via Don Luigi Monza 20, Bosisio Parini, Lecco 23842, Italy
J. Carlos Caldas
Affiliation:
Departamento de Ciências Sociais e do Comportamento, Instituto Superior de Ciências da Saúde – Norte, Rua Central de Gandra, 1317, 4585-116 Gandra, PRD, Portugal
Yi-Chuen Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, National Chung Cheng University, 168 University Road, Min-Hsiung, Chia-Yi 62102, Taiwan
Ladislav Csemy
Affiliation:
Prague Psychiatric Centre, Laboratory of Social Psychiatry, Ustavni 91, 181 03 Praha 8, Prague, Czech Republic
Marina M. da Rocha
Affiliation:
University Paulista (Unip), Institute of Human Sciences, Rua Francisco Bautista, 300, São Paulo, Brazil
Anca Dobrean
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Babes-Bolyai University, 400015, Rupublicii st. 37, Cluj Napoca, Romania
Lourdes Ezpeleta
Affiliation:
Departament de Psicologia Clinica i de la Salut, Universitat Autonoma de Barcelona, Edifici B, Bellaterra 08193, Spain
Yasuko Funabiki
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto University Hospital, 54 Kawaharacho, Shogoin, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto, Japan
Valerie S. Harder
Affiliation:
University of Vermont, 1 South Prospect Street, Burlington, VT 05401, USA
Felipe Lecannelier
Affiliation:
Faculty of Medical Sciences, Universidad de Santiago, Santiago, Chile
Marie Leiner de la Cabada
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Texas Tech University Health Sciences Center, P. O. Box 43091 Lubbock, Texas 79409, USA
Patrick Leung
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Room 356, Sino Building, Shatin, New Territories, Hong Kong, People's Republic of China
Jianghong Liu
Affiliation:
School of Nursing and Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, 418 Curie Blvd., Room 426, Claire M. Fagin Hall, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Safia Mahr
Affiliation:
Departement de Psychologie, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre la Défense, Laboratoire EVACLIPSY, Batiment C, 3e Etage, Salles C.319 & C.321, 200 Avenue de la Republique, Nanterre 92001, France
Sergey Malykh
Affiliation:
Psychological Institute of Russian Academy of Education, Mokhovaya str, 9/4, Moscow 125009, Russia
Jasminka Markovic
Affiliation:
Medical Faculty Novi Sad, University of Novi Sad, Clinical Center of Vojvodina, Hajduk Veljkova 1, Novi Sad 21000, Serbia
David M. Ndetei
Affiliation:
Africa Mental Health Foundation, P.O. Box 48423-00100, Nairobi, Kenya
Kyung Ja Oh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Soedaemun-gu, Seoul, South Korea
Jean-Michel Petot
Affiliation:
Departement de Psychologie, Université de Paris Ouest, Laboratoire EVACLIPSY, Batiment C, 3 Etage, Salles C.319 & C.321, 200 Avenue de la Republique, Nanterre 92001, France
Geylan Riad
Affiliation:
Helwan University, Cairo, Egypt
Direnc Sakarya
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Ankara University Faculty of Medicine, Ankara, Turkey
Virginia C. Samaniego
Affiliation:
Pontificia Universidad Católica Argentina, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Sandra Sebre
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Latvia, Jurmalas Avenue 74/76, Riga, Latvia LV-1083
Mimoza Shahini
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Clinical Center of Kosova, 10000 Prishtine, Kosova
Edwiges Silvares
Affiliation:
University of São Paulo, Instituto de Psicologia, Av. Prof. Mello Moraes 1721, Cidade Universitária, São Paulo, Brazil
Roma Simulioniene
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Klaipeda University, Herkaus Manto str. 84, Klaipeda 92294, Lithuania
Elvisa Sokoli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Tirana, Tirana, Albania
Joel B. Talcott
Affiliation:
Aston Brain Centre, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Aston Triangle, Birmingham, UK B4 7ET
Natalia Vazquez
Affiliation:
Pontificia Universidad Católica Argentina, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Tomasz Wolanczyk
Affiliation:
Department of Child Psychiatry, Medical University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
Ewa Zasepa
Affiliation:
The Maria Grzegorzewska Academy of Special Education, Room 3609, Szczesliwicka 40, 02-353, Warsaw, Poland
*
Corresponding author: William E. Copeland; E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

It is unknown how much variation in adult mental health problems is associated with differences between societal/cultural groups, over and above differences between individuals.

Methods

To test these relative contributions, a consortium of indigenous researchers collected Adult Self-Report (ASR) ratings from 16 906 18- to 59-year-olds in 28 societies that represented seven culture clusters identified in the Global Leadership and Organizational Behavioral Effectiveness study (e.g. Confucian, Anglo). The ASR is scored on 17 problem scales, plus a personal strengths scale. Hierarchical linear modeling estimated variance accounted for by individual differences (including measurement error), society, and culture cluster. Multi-level analyses of covariance tested age and gender effects.

Results

Across the 17 problem scales, the variance accounted for by individual differences ranged from 80.3% for DSM-oriented anxiety problems to 95.2% for DSM-oriented avoidant personality (mean = 90.7%); by society: 3.2% for DSM-oriented somatic problems to 8.0% for DSM-oriented anxiety problems (mean = 6.3%); and by culture cluster: 0.0% for DSM-oriented avoidant personality to 11.6% for DSM-oriented anxiety problems (mean = 3.0%). For strengths, individual differences accounted for 80.8% of variance, societal differences 10.5%, and cultural differences 8.7%. Age and gender had very small effects.

Conclusions

Overall, adults' self-ratings of mental health problems and strengths were associated much more with individual differences than societal/cultural differences, although this varied across scales. These findings support cross-cultural use of standardized measures to assess mental health problems, but urge caution in assessment of personal strengths.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

It is assumed that variations in ratings of mental health problems reflect primarily individual differences. But such variations may also reflect societal and cultural differences. The relevant questions are: How much variation is due to societal and cultural differences? and Does the respective variation due to societal and cultural differences vary among different conditions? If the variations are largely due to societal and cultural differences, then a particular instrument may be less useful for individual treatment planning within particular societies/cultures as the instrument is not sensitive to individual differences within that society or culture. Arthur Kleinman described this concern over 40 years ago (Kleinman, Reference Kleinman1977). He questioned research that uncritically exported mental health instruments from one society or culture to another. This question highlights the contrast between emic approaches that focus on characteristics of a given culture v. etic approaches that focus on more universal aspects of cultures (Pike, Reference Pike1967). This tension spurred the rise of transcultural psychiatry, the landmark 2001 WHO Report on Mental Health, and the burgeoning field of global mental health (Prince et al., Reference Prince, Patel, Saxena, Maj, Maselko, Phillips and Rahman2007; World Health Organization, 2001).

It is now clear that certain mental disorders affect people across all regions of the world (Steel et al., Reference Steel, Marnane, Iranpour, Chey, Jackson, Patel and Silove2014; WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium, 2004). Even so, the construct of mental disorder itself is evolving. The Lancet Commission on Global Mental health recently admitted that ‘the binary approach to the diagnosis of mental disorder … does not adequately reflect the dimensional nature of mental health’ (Patel et al., Reference Patel, Saxena, Lund, Thornicroft, Baingana, Bolton and Eaton2018, p. 4) Mental health, like physical health, exists on a continuum from mild, time-limited symptoms to severe, chronic debilitating conditions. To study the role of culture in mental health problems, one needs standardized measures that cover a broad continuum of human behavior and have been adopted and studied across many societies.

One measure that meets these criteria is the Adult Self-Report (ASR) (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003). The ASR is a 15–20 min questionnaire for ages 18–59 that assesses dimensions of behavioral, emotional, social, and thought problems, and personal strengths. It has been adopted widely and used in over 250 published studies. It has also been adapted for use across many societies. First, each non-English version of the ASR is the result of a process in which indigenous researchers make a translation of the ASR in their language and then obtain an independent back-translation to insure the accuracy of the initial translation (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2015). Next, confirmatory factor analysis has been used to test whether the ASR syndrome structure was supported across 29 societies (Ivanova et al., Reference Ivanova, Achenbach, Rescorla, Turner, Ahmeti-Pronaj, Au and Chen2015). The primary model fit index (root mean square error of approximation) showed good model fit for all samples (<0.03) and good to acceptable fit for secondary indices (e.g. comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI)). Only five (0.06%) of the 8598 estimated parameters were outside the admissible parameter space. These findings support similarities in the factor structure and factor loadings across societies. Additional analyses tested similarities in the mean ratings of individual ASR items and scale scores (Rescorla et al., Reference Rescorla, Achenbach, Ivanova, Turner, Althoff, Árnadóttir and Chen2016). As indicated by a mean correlation of 0.77 between the mean ratings of items for all pairs of societies, the rank order of item ratings was similar across societies. The mean scale scores of the scales themselves do vary across societies in a manner consistent with a normal distribution. Together, these findings support the ASR as a useful tool for dimensional assessment of mental health problems in diverse societies. It also provides a way to test effects of societal and cultural difference on adults' self-ratings.

We use ‘society’ in reference to geopolitically demarcated populations that include but are not limited to countries. ‘Culture’ is defined as an accumulated set of beliefs, values, and social norms which impact the behavior of a relatively large group of people (Lustig, Koester, & Halualani, Reference Lustig, Koester and Halualani2006). The Global Leadership and Organizational Behavior Effectiveness study (GLOBE) mapped cultures by analyzing responses by 17 000 participants in 62 societies to questionnaires on cultural dimensions proposed by Hofstede including assertiveness, gender egalitarianism, collectivism, and future orientation (Hofstede, Reference Hofstede1984; House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman, & Gupta, Reference House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004). Ten distinct ‘culture clusters’ were derived from these dimensions (e.g. Confucian, Anglo, Latin America, Eastern Europe, Sub-Saharan Africa).

Stankov (Reference Stankov2011) applied the GLOBE findings to test the effects of individual societies, GLOBE-defined culture clusters, and individual differences on personality measures completed by college students in 45 societies. For neuroticism, the personality trait most relevant to mental health, individual differences accounted for 95.3% of the variance, societal differences 2.0%, and culture cluster differences 2.7%. The societal findings for personality are similar to those obtained in another study of 130 602 adults in 22 societies (Kajonius & Mac Giolla, Reference Kajonius and Mac Giolla2017). A similar analytic approach was applied in two studies of ASR-related measures of child/youth mental health problems. For 11–18-year-olds' self-ratings, individual differences accounted for 92.5% of variance across 17 problem scales, societal differences 6.0%, and cultural differences only 1.5%, indicating modest effects of society and culture (Ivanova et al., Reference Ivanova, Achenbach, Turner, Almqvist, Begovac, Bilenberg and Chahed2022). For 6–18-year-olds rated by their parents, individual differences accounted for 90% of the variance across problem scales, societal differences 6%, and cultural differences 4% (Rescorla, Althoff, Ivanova, & Achenbach, Reference Rescorla, Althoff, Ivanova and Achenbach2019).

Purpose of the present study

In 28 societies representing seven GLOBE culture clusters, we tested whether society and culture cluster would account for significant variance in adults' self-rated mental health problems and personal strengths, over and above individual differences. Prior research on 11–18- and 6–18-year-olds suggest modest effects of society and culture. But adults have had much longer exposure to the norms and influences of their society and culture than children and youths. To further examine the effects of society and culture cluster on self-rated mental health problems and strengths, we tested society and gender as predictors in one multi-level model and culture cluster and gender in a second multi-level model.

Method

Samples

Indigenous researchers independently arranged to have ASRs completed by 16 906 18- to 59-year-olds in the 28 societies listed in Table 1. These samples were pooled as part of an international consortium of mental health researchers. Samples averaged 42% male, and Ns ranged from 293 (Egypt) to 2020 (USA). As shown in Table 1, rigorous random sampling methods were used in some societies, resulting in representative population samples. However, in other societies, various methods of convenience sampling were necessary, resulting in samples of unknown representativeness. Follow-up analyses tested models in 15 representative samples. Additional details of individual studies are available from cited references and/or listed primary investigators.

Table 1. ASR samples

a Only age ranges were available for Angola.

b Unpublished data.

c The identical sample sizes for Japan and Korea are coincidental, not errors.

Using the GLOBE culture cluster taxonomy (House et al., Reference House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004), we classified the 28 societies into the following seven culture clusters: Anglo (N = 2362, two societies); Confucian Asia (N = 3182, five societies); Eastern Europe (N = 4475, nine societies); Latin America (N = 2094, four societies); Latin Europe (N = 2094, four societies); Middle East (N = 676, two societies); and Sub-Saharan Africa (N = 826, two societies) (see Table 1). Following the standard ASR procedure, cases that were missing ratings for >8 problem items were excluded from the analyses. Indigenous researchers followed their institutions' procedures for protection of human participants including obtaining informed consent. The lead authors' university human participants board approved the overall project. All data were de-identified.

Instrument and tested model

Indigenous mental health workers conducted translations of the ASR to their language and then obtained independent back-translations to insure the accuracy of the initial translation. The ASR's 120 problem items are rated 0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, or 2 = very true or often true, based on the preceding 6 months. These items tap diverse emotional, behavioral, social, and thought problems, such as I worry about my family; I am stubborn, sullen, or irritable; I argue a lot; and I have thoughts that other people would think are strange. The 11 personal strengths items (e.g. I make good use of my opportunities; I work up to my ability; I am pretty honest; I meet my responsibilities to my family; I try to be fair to others; and I am a happy person) are rated on the same 0–1–2 scale with high ratings indicating positive characteristics.

Our analyses focused on 17 ASR problem scales and one personal strengths scale. The scales included eight syndromes derived from exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses of ratings by adults in US population and clinical samples (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003). The syndromes are anxious/depressed, withdrawn, somatic complaints, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking behavior, aggressive behavior, and intrusive behavior. We also analyzed three broad-spectrum scales: total problems (comprised of all problem items); internalizing (anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, and somatic complaints syndrome items); and externalizing (rule-breaking behavior, aggressive behavior, and intrusive syndrome items). Six DSM-oriented scales comprise ASR problem items identified by an international panel of experts as being very consistent with particular DSM-5 diagnostic categories (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach2013; Achenbach, Bernstein, & Dumenci, Reference Achenbach, Bernstein and Dumenci2005). The DSM-oriented scales are designated as depressive problems, anxiety problems, somatic problems, avoidant personality, AD/H problems, and antisocial personality. Adults' ratings of strengths items comprise an 11-item personal strengths scale.

For Japan, items assessing illegal behavior [6. I use drugs (other than alcohol and nicotine) for nonmedical purposes; 57. I physically attack people; 82. I steal; and 92. I do things that may cause me trouble with the law] were omitted from the ASR because their endorsement by respondents would have legally obligated the investigators to report them to authorities. To account for these excluded items, we re-wrote our scale-scoring syntax from simply taking the sum of items comprising each scale to, instead, taking the mean of the items comprising each scale (when there were valid responses available for at least 50% of such items) and multiplied that value times the total number of items comprising the scale.

Based on US data, Achenbach and Rescorla (Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003) reported αs of 0.89–0.97 for the internalizing, externalizing, and total problem scales, 0.51–0.88 for the syndromes, and 0.68–0.84 for the DSM-oriented scales. The ASR's 1-week test–retest correlations were 0.89–0.94 for the broad-band scales, 0.78–0.91 for the syndromes, and 0.77–0.86 for the DSM-oriented scales. ASR items and scales discriminated significantly between demographically similar clinically referred and nonreferred samples of US adults. Additional ASR findings across societies are reported by Achenbach and Rescorla (Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2015); Ivanova et al. (Reference Ivanova, Achenbach, Rescorla, Turner, Ahmeti-Pronaj, Au and Chen2015); and Rescorla et al. (Reference Rescorla, Achenbach, Ivanova, Turner, Althoff, Árnadóttir and Chen2016).

Analyses

The effects of individual, society, and culture cluster contributions to differences on ASR scales were tested with hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) estimated using PROC MIXED in SAS 9.4. (SAS Institute, 2013). Mental health problem scores are positively skewed in general population samples (where many people obtain relatively low scores), but HLM has been found robust to deviations from normality, especially for large samples (Ketelsen, Reference Ketelsen2014; Man, Schumacker, Morell, & Wang, Reference Man, Schumacker, Morell and Wang2022; Schielzeth et al., Reference Schielzeth, Dingemanse, Nakagawa, Westneat, Allegue, Teplitsky and Araya-Ajoy2020). Each ASR scale was tested separately in a multilevel model. Individual differences (i.e. differences between individuals within a society) and unspecified effects (i.e. measurement error) were entered at level 1. Societal differences were entered at level 2. Culture cluster differences were entered at level 3. All multilevel models included intercepts and used the restricted maximum likelihood estimator that provides more robust results. The percent of variance due to predictors at each level was calculated as the ratio of the respective level-specific variance component over total variance. First, we tested the null model in which no predictors were entered at level 1, and society and culture cluster were modeled as random effects at levels 2 and 3. In addition to the random-effects model, all results were retested in fixed-effects models. For a small number of clusters, the fixed-effects model can be more robust than the random-effects model that assumes normality in cluster-specific random intercepts (McNeish & Kelley, Reference McNeish and Kelley2019). Next, we added age and gender as fixed effects at level 1 and reran the model for each ASR scale. Finally, we tested whether the economic status of societies was a stronger predictor of ASR scale scores than culture cluster. To do this, models were rerun with World Bank income group classification of societies based on purchasing power parity (PPP) included as a level 2 variable (World Bank, 2020).

To better understand how society and culture cluster interacted with age and gender in their relations to ASR scores, we used analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to test associations of internalizing, externalizing, and total problem scores with society (28 societies) and culture cluster (seven clusters), plus age, gender, and all possible interactions.

Results

Figure 1 presents internalizing and externalizing scores by culture cluster. The seven clusters are arranged in ascending order for mean internalizing problems score. Significant differences were observed between most culture clusters for both internalizing and externalizing problems using Student–Newman–Keuls (SNK) post hoc tests. For internalizing, there was only exception. The following clusters that did not differ significantly from each other: Sub-Saharan Africa and Middle East. For externalizing, exceptions were Eastern Europe and Latin America; Latin America and Sub-Saharan Africa; and Anglo and Confucian. The rank-ordering of culture cluster for externalizing differed from the rank-ordering for internalizing.

Figure 1. Mean raw scale score by culture cluster for Internalizing and Externalizing Problems.

Table 2 presents the variance components estimated for the multilevel null model for individual differences (level 1), societies (level 2), and culture cluster (level 3). Averaged across the 17 problem scales (i.e. all scales except personal strengths), the percent of variance accounted for by individual differences was 90.7%, by society was 6.3%, and by culture cluster was 3.0%. Results based on fixed-effects models (which are less constrained with a small number of clusters) were similar for problem scales and can be found in online Supplementary Table S1.

Table 2. Percent of variance accounted for by individual, society, and GLOBE culture cluster effects in hierarchical linear models of adult self-ratings on the ASR

All effects of individual differences and society were significant (p < 0.001). Effects of culture cluster did not reach the p < 0.05 level of significance.

For specific problem scales, the variance accounted for by individual differences ranged from 80.3% for DSM-oriented anxiety problems to 95.2% for DSM-oriented avoidant personality; by society: 3.2% for DSM-oriented somatic problems to 8.0% for DSM-oriented anxiety problems; and by culture cluster: 0.0% for DSM-oriented avoidant personality to 11.6% for DSM-oriented anxiety problems. Individual differences explained most of the variance in scores for problem scales, while society explained most of the remaining variance. The variance accounted for by individual differences (80.8%) in personal strengths was substantially smaller than for the 17 problem scales (90.7%). Hence, the variance accounted for by society (10.5%) and by culture cluster (8.7%) was greater for personal strengths than for society and culture cluster averaged across the problem scales (6.3% and 3.0%, respectively). In the fixed-effects models, the variance accounted for by culture cluster for personal strengths was greater still at 15.9%. Figure 2 displays all ASR scales ranked from lowest to highest for total variance accounted for by society and culture cluster in random-effects models. The total variance accounted for by society and culture cluster ranged from close to 5% for DSM avoidant personality to near 20% for personal strengths and DSM anxiety. When all models were retested only including 15 representative samples, the results were similar to findings in Table 2 and online Supplementary Table S1.

Figure 2. ASR scales ranked by lowest to highest for total contribution of society and culture cluster.

The multilevel models were rerun with age and gender as fixed effects at the individual level. Their addition did not significantly change the variance components for individual differences, society, or culture cluster for any scale. We then added the World Bank's PPP Index as a level 2 variable. In these models, individual differences accounted for 93.2% of the variance, society for 3.7%, culture for 2.1%, and the World Bank PPP Index for 1.0%, averaged across 17 problem scales (see online Supplementary Table S2). These findings confirm that most of the variance in ASR problem scale scores was associated with individual differences. For personal strengths, individual differences accounted for 75.8% of the variance, society for 7.2%, culture for 15.5%, and the World Bank PPP Index for 1.4%.

Table 3 presents the variance components from ANOVAs of internalizing, externalizing, and total problems scales. Predictors were society (28 societies) or culture cluster (seven clusters), plus age, gender, and all possible interactions. Results of ANOVAs for internalizing, externalizing, and total problems scores indicated that effects of society (7.5, 5.8, and 7.4%, respectively) were larger than effects of culture cluster (2.9, 1.7, and 2.8%, respectively). In terms of main effects, age had larger effects on externalizing and total problems than on internalizing problems (~5.1% v. 1.8%), whereas gender had larger effects on internalizing problems than on externalizing and total problems (~1.8% v. 0.2%). All of the 24 effects involving age and gender interactions were ⩽1%.

Table 3. Percent of variance accounted for by individual and society or GLOBE culture cluster in ANOVAs of ASR internalizing, externalizing, and total problems scores

p < 0.0001; p < 0.01; p < 0.05.

Discussion

Our goal was to conduct the broadest test to date of the effects of society and culture on differences in adults' self-rated mental health problems and strengths. Several findings are noteworthy. First, most of the variance in adult problem ratings (~90%) was associated with individual differences. Of the remaining variance, society accounted for, on average, double the variance of culture cluster. These estimates, however, varied across constructs: society and culture accounted for only 5% of the variance in DSM avoidant personality but up to 20% of the variance in DSM anxiety problems. The effects of society and culture on the personal strengths scale were twice as large as for the problem scales. Overall, the effects of society and culture on adults' self-rated problems were small to moderate, but they varied broadly – from small to large – across the different scales (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

We know of no other studies that have tested the effects of societal and cultural effects on adults' self-ratings of mental health problems. We know of one such study of parent ratings of children (Rescorla et al., Reference Rescorla, Althoff, Ivanova and Achenbach2019) and one of youth self-ratings (Ivanova et al., Reference Ivanova, Achenbach, Turner, Almqvist, Begovac, Bilenberg and Chahed2022). The results converge in three ways. Firstly, all three studies found that about 90% of the variance in problem scale scores was associated with individual differences (parent-ratings: 92.5%; youth self-ratings: 89.8%; adult self-ratings: 90.6%). This is striking given that the two child/youth studies included societies and culture clusters not included here. Next, society generally accounted for more of the variance in ratings of mental health problems than culture in all three studies (parent-report: 6.1% v. 4.2%; youth self-report: 6.0% v. 1.5%; adult self-report: 6.3% v. 3.0%). Finally, there were similarities in the rank ordering of results for the individual mental health scales. The DSM anxiety scale showed the largest combined effects of society and culture in both the adult and the youth self-rating studies (it was third in the child parent-rating study). Also, both the youth study and our study supported larger societal and cultural contributions to personal strengths (16.6% in youth self-ratings and 19.2% in adult self-ratings) than to problem scales.

Why did society and culture account for twice as much of the variance in ratings of strengths as in mental health problems? A similar discrepancy was observed by Stankov (Reference Stankov2011). There, societal and cultural effects on personality scales were smaller than on social attitude and norm scales. Stronger societal/cultural effects on social constructs than on personality scales might be expected. The ASR and Youth Self-Report (YSR) strengths scales, however, do not assess social constructs but rather self-ratings of strengths (e.g. I make good use of my opportunities, I work up to my ability, I am pretty honest). Our notions of strengths may reflect values that are shared or defined within different societal and cultural groups. Our notions of mental health problems, by contrast, may be more universal because of how these problems impair functioning and cause distress. Self-ratings of strengths may also be more affected by social desirability, but it is not clear why this would be the case. Future research should attempt to clarify why the effects of society and culture on self-ratings of strengths were larger than on self-ratings of problems. In any case, the findings for personal strengths suggest caution in comparing personal strengths across societies and cultures.

Limitations

Our study's strengths included (1) use of a standardized mental health measure that has been adopted and tested extensively across many societies; and (2) data from 16 906 adults across 28 societies that represent seven culture clusters. There are also limitations to consider. Our samples were collected under varying conditions in diverse societies by indigenous researchers. Some of our samples were of unknown representativeness because they were obtained using convenience rather than random sampling methods. Second, our findings are limited to the specific problems and strengths assessed by the ASR. Many societies and cultural groups were not included in the study. The inclusion of additional societies and cultures might yield different results. Also, other ways of classifying cultures might yield different results. In HLM analyses, effects associated with individual differences included residual variance. The residual variance may be due to other variables such as the adult's family, work, or local community. Third, our work has previously demonstrated similarity in factor structure and factor loadings, but did not formally test other aspects of measurement invariance. Finally, ‘society’ and ‘culture’ may be associated with genetic differences, as well as with socio-cultural demarcations of populations.

Conclusion

Over the past 30 years, the study of mental health problems has been extended to many societies around the world (Prince et al., Reference Prince, Patel, Saxena, Maj, Maselko, Phillips and Rahman2007; World Health Organization, 2001). In this time, the global health burden for mental health conditions has increased (Vos et al., Reference Vos, Lim, Abbafati, Abbas, Abbasi, Abbasifard and Abdelalim2020). The unmet need for mental health treatment around the world is large and, sadly, growing. Despite the myriad difference between societies and cultures with respect to geographic location, political/economic systems, history, population, ethnicity/race, and religion, the mental health conditions identified and studied across societies and cultures appear rather similar even if there are differences in the mean levels of those conditions. In previous work, our international consortium has obtained large α levels for ASR scales and a good fit for the syndrome structure across societies. The current study suggests that societal and cultural effects on problem scores are modest. Together, our findings suggest that cross-cultural use of standardized measures like the ASR to assess individual mental health problems is warranted but suggest more caution regarding personal strengths.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001332

Acknowledgements

Dr Copeland had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Drs Copeland, Achenbach, and Ivanova contributed to the concept and design, all authors contributed to the acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of the data, Drs Copeland, Achenbach, and Ivanova contributed to the manuscript, all authors critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content, and all authors provided final approval for the submitted manuscript.

Financial support

This work was funded in part by the nonprofit University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families which publishes the Adult Self-Report and from which authors Copeland, Achenbach, and Ivanova receive funding. The remaining authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest.

References

Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2015). Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M. (2013). DSM-oriented guide for the Achenbach system of empirically based assessment (ASEBA). Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., Bernstein, A., & Dumenci, L. (2005). DSM-oriented scales and statistically based syndromes for ages 18 to 59: Linking taxonomic paradigms to facilitate multitaxonomic approaches. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84(1), 4963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ezpeleta, L., de la Osa, N., & Doménech, J. M. (2014). Prevalence of DSM-IV disorders, comorbidity and impairment in 3-year-old Spanish preschoolers. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(1), 145155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values (Vol. 5). Newbury Park California: Sage.Google Scholar
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Ivanova, M. Y., Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A., Turner, L. V., Ahmeti-Pronaj, A., Au, A., … Chen, Y.-C. (2015). Syndromes of self-reported psychopathology for ages 18–59 in 29 societies. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37(2), 171183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivanova, M. Y., Achenbach, T. M., Turner, L., Almqvist, F., Begovac, I., Bilenberg, N., … Chahed, M. (2022). Effects of individual differences, society, and culture on youth-rated problems and strengths in 38 societies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(11), 12971307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kajonius, P., & Mac Giolla, E. (2017). Personality traits across countries: Support for similarities rather than differences. PLoS ONE, 12(6), e0179646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ketelsen, K. L. (2014). A Monte Carlo simulation to examine the effects of violating the normality assumption in 2-level hierarchical linear models with unbalanced designs. Greeley Colorado: University of Northern Colorado.Google Scholar
Kim, H. J., Kim, Y. A., & Oh, K. J. (2009). Preliminary study for the Korean version of the Adult Self Report (ASR). Paper presented at the Korean Psychological Association., Seoul.Google Scholar
Kleinman, A. M. (1977). Depression, somatization and the ‘new cross-cultural psychiatry’. Social Science & Medicine (1967), 11(1), 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lustig, M. W., Koester, J., & Halualani, R. (2006). Intercultural competence: Interpersonal communication across cultures. London, England: Pearson/A and B.Google Scholar
Mahr, S., Petot, J., Camart, N., & Zebdi, R. (2018). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the French version of the Achenbach and Rescorla's adult self-report. Psychologie Francaise, 63(1), 2336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Man, K., Schumacker, R., Morell, M., & Wang, Y. (2022). Effects of compounded nonnormality of residuals in hierarchical linear modeling. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 82(2), 330355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeish, D., & Kelley, K. (2019). Fixed effects models versus mixed effects models for clustered data: Reviewing the approaches, disentangling the differences, and making recommendations. Psychological Methods, 24(1), 2035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patel, V., Saxena, S., Lund, C., Thornicroft, G., Baingana, F., Bolton, P., … Eaton, J. (2018). The Lancet Commission on global mental health and sustainable development. The Lancet, 392(10157), 15531598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pike, K. L. (1967). Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human behavior. The Hague, Netherlands: Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prince, M., Patel, V., Saxena, S., Maj, M., Maselko, J., Phillips, M. R., & Rahman, A. (2007). No health without mental health. The Lancet, 370(9590), 859877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. A., Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Turner, L. V., Althoff, R. R., Árnadóttir, H. A., … Chen, Y.-C. (2016). Problems and adaptive functioning reported by adults in 17 societies. International Perspectives in Psychology, 5(2), 91109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. A., Althoff, R. R., Ivanova, M. Y., & Achenbach, T. M. (2019). Effects of society and culture on parents’ ratings of children's mental health problems in 45 societies. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(8), 11071115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samaniego, V., & Vázquez, N. (2012). Adult psychopathology: Is there any agreement between self-reports and reports by other informants. Paper presented at the International Congress of Psychology.Google Scholar
SAS Institute (2013). SAS/STAT version 9.4. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.Google Scholar
Schielzeth, H., Dingemanse, N. J., Nakagawa, S., Westneat, D. F., Allegue, H., Teplitsky, C., … Araya-Ajoy, Y. G. (2020). Robustness of linear mixed-effects models to violations of distributional assumptions. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 11(9), 11411152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Šimulionienė, R., Brazdeikienė, L., Rugevičius, M., Gedutienė, R., & Žakaitienė, A. (2010). The psychometric properties of the Lithuanian version of ASEBA adult forms. Psichologija, 42, 2343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokoli, E., Bodinaku, B., Paço, J., Gjergji, O., & Çala, L. (2016). Psychometric properties of the Albanian version of the adult self-report for ages 18–59. Academic Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies, 5(1), 5157.Google Scholar
Stankov, L. (2011). Individual, country and societal cluster differences on measures of personality, attitudes, values, and social norms. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 5566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steel, Z., Marnane, C., Iranpour, C., Chey, T., Jackson, J. W., Patel, V., & Silove, D. (2014). The global prevalence of common mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 476493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vos, T., Lim, S. S., Abbafati, C., Abbas, K. M., Abbasi, M., Abbasifard, M., … Abdelalim, A. (2020). Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet, 396(10258), 12041222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium (2004). Prevalence, severity, and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA, 291(21), 25812590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Bank. (2020). Purchasing power parities and the size of world economies: Results from the 2017 international comparison program. Washington, DC: The World Bank.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2001). The World Health Report 2001: Mental health: new understanding, new hope.Google Scholar
Zasepa, E., & Wolanczyk, T. (2011). Assessment of problem behaviour in adults: Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Polish adaptations of the adult self-report (ASR) and the adult behaviour checklist (ABCL). International Journal of Child Health and Human Development, 4(3), 295306.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. ASR samples

Figure 1

Figure 1. Mean raw scale score by culture cluster for Internalizing and Externalizing Problems.

Figure 2

Table 2. Percent of variance accounted for by individual, society, and GLOBE culture cluster effects in hierarchical linear models of adult self-ratings on the ASR

Figure 3

Figure 2. ASR scales ranked by lowest to highest for total contribution of society and culture cluster.

Figure 4

Table 3. Percent of variance accounted for by individual and society or GLOBE culture cluster in ANOVAs of ASR internalizing, externalizing, and total problems scores

Supplementary material: File

Copeland et al. supplementary material

Copeland et al. supplementary material
Download Copeland et al. supplementary material(File)
File 19.1 KB