Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T23:29:01.491Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Impact of Oral Argument Attendance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2023

Damon Cann*
Affiliation:
Professor, Department of Political Science, Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA
Greg Goelzhauser
Affiliation:
Professor, Department of Political Science, Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA
*
Corresponding author: Damon Cann; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

How does oral argument attendance impact public perceptions of the judiciary? Judicial independence is partly contingent on public support, but the conditions that generate institutional good will are not well understood. We examine how judicial outreach and court exposure inform public attitudes. Leveraging a field-experiment randomizing in-person attendance at oral arguments conducted by a federal circuit court of appeals on a university campus, we find that exposure increases perceptions of institutional legitimacy and the extent to which judicial decisions are motivated by law versus politics. The results have important implications for judicial politics and policy debates concerning reform initiatives involving circuit riding, courtroom cameras, and public outreach.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Law and Courts Organized Section of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

How does oral argument attendance impact public perceptions of the judiciary? Judicial independence is partly contingent on public support (Caldeira Reference Caldeira1986; Stephenson Reference Stephenson2004; Vanberg Reference Vanberg2004), but the conditions that generate institutional good will are not well understood. The public can provoke political attacks on courts that threaten judicial independence (Staton Reference Staton2004; Clark Reference Clark2009; Helmke Reference Helmke2010). To forge support, courts can, for example, decide cases consistent with public opinion (McGuire and Stimson Reference McGuire and Stimson2004; Epstein and Martin Reference Epstein and Martin2010; Casillas, Enns, and Wohlfarth Reference Casillas, Enns and Wohlfarth2011) or engage in various outreach efforts (Savchak and Edwards Reference Savchak and Edwards2016; Curry and Fix Reference Curry and Fix2019; Glennon and Strother Reference Glennon and Strother2019). To the extent public understanding of the judicial process is limited, outreach may be appealing to judges, though the extent to which it impacts public attitudes is unclear.

We evaluate the causal effect of oral argument attendance on public perceptions of the judiciary using a field experiment. Specifically, we randomized in-person attendance at oral argument proceedings conducted by a traveling federal circuit court of appeals on a university campus. This type of outreach initiative is common among state and lower federal courts. Consistent with positivity theory (Gibson and Caldeira Reference Gibson and Caldeira2009; Gibson, Lodge, and Woodson Reference Gibson, Lodge and Woodson2014; Gibson and Nelson Reference Gibson and Nelson2017), we find that oral argument attendance increases perceptions of institutional legitimacy. We also find that exposure increases the extent to which people think judicial decisions are motivated by law as opposed to politics. The results are consistent across two oral argument sessions, with nonoverlapping control and treatment groups in each session.

This project has important implications for our understanding of judicial politics and the effect of exposure to government proceedings. With respect to the former, the results enhance our understanding of why oral argument matters (Johnson Reference Johnson2004; Black, Johnson, and Wedeking Reference Black, Johnson and Wedeking2012; Ringsmuth and Johnson Reference Ringsmuth and Johnson2013) and the impact of court exposure on public attitudes (Black et al. Reference Black, Johnson, Owens and Wedekingn.d.; Benesh Reference Benesh2006; Krewson Reference Krewson2019). The results also contribute to policy debates concerning reform initiatives involving circuit riding, courtroom cameras, and outreach initiatives. With respect to the latter, the results contribute to our broader understanding of how exposure to government proceedings (Blair, Karim, and Morse Reference Blair, Karim and Morse2019; Malesky and Taussig Reference Malesky and Taussig2019; Peyton, Sierra-Arevalo, and Rand Reference Peyton, Sierra-Arevalo and Rand2019) as well as deliberation and transparency initiatives (Simon and Sulkin Reference Simon and Sulkin2002; Gottlieb Reference Gottlieb2016; Hollyer, Rosendorff, and Vreeland Reference Hollyer, Rosendorff and Vreeland2019) impact the public.

Oral argument exposure

Oral argument can serve a variety of purposes. Internally, judges may use oral argument to acquire information for purposes of informing decisions on the merits, coalition formation, opinion writing, and separation of powers considerations (Johnson Reference Johnson2004; Black, Johnson, and Wedeking Reference Black, Johnson and Wedeking2012; Ringsmuth and Johnson Reference Ringsmuth and Johnson2013). Externally, oral argument can enhance institutional legitimacy, increase public awareness, guard against noncompliance, and provide judges with a platform for political position taking (Black et al. Reference Black, Johnson, Owens and Wedekingn.d.; Krehbiel Reference Krehbiel2016; Jacobi and Sag Reference Jacobi and Sag2019).

The external impact of oral argument remains relatively understudied notwithstanding its potential importance. As discussions of judicial legitimacy proliferate, oral argument exposure has been at the forefront. With respect to the Supreme Court, for example, it has been said that “the public spectacle of oral argument assures the parties in the case at hand that their arguments have been heard and considered,” while also “allow[ing] the public to see the Court as an impartial tribunal exploring issues of national importance through a balanced adjudicative process” (Jacobi and Sag Reference Jacobi and Sag2019, 1168).

Institutional maintenance is complicated at the appellate level by a public information deficit. It may be that “to know courts is to love them, because to know them is to be exposed to a series of legitimizing messages focused on the symbols of justice, judicial objectivity, and impartiality” (Gibson, Caldeira, and Baird Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Baird1998, 345). As one federal circuit court judge put it, however, “People simply do not understand what appellate courts are supposed to do” (Medina Reference Medina1961, 155). As the only part of the appellate decision-making process open to the public, oral argument may inform the public’s perception of how these courts operate.

Judges engage in a variety of outreach activities for institutional maintenance purposes (Savchak and Edwards Reference Savchak and Edwards2016; Curry and Fix Reference Curry and Fix2019; Glennon and Strother Reference Glennon and Strother2019), but traveling to conduct oral arguments is a particularly intriguing example because it exposes people to official court proceedings. State and lower federal courts regularly conduct oral argument at law schools.Footnote 1 Moreover, several state courts have expanded their oral argument outreach initiatives to the public more broadly. Example institutionalized programs include Appeals on Wheels by the Indiana Court of Appeals, Courts in the Community by the Hawaii Supreme Court, and Justice on Wheels by the Wisconsin Supreme Court. Officials hope these initiatives enhance judicial legitimacy and improve public perception (Deits and Keenan Reference Deits and Keenan2004, 243; Nelson Reference Nelson2005, 170), but the extent to which they do so is unclear.

Bringing courts to the people is an old idea. Supreme Court circuit riding may have primarily been a cost-saving measure, but it was also thought to be of “great value in keeping the federal judiciary in touch with the local communities” (Warren Reference Warren1924, 58). When justices lobbied to abolish the practice, legislators resisted in part because of the public-interfacing component. In 1848, for example, one senator argued that abolishing circuit riding would result in the Court “losing… that responsive confidence of the people, which adds so essentially to the sanction of all the acts of the officers of government.”Footnote 2 Although circuit riding has long been abolished, calls for renewal regularly emphasize the value of public exposure (Calabresi and Presser Reference Calabresi and Presser2006; Stras Reference Stras2006).

Positivity theory provides a theoretical lens through which one can assess the potential impact of oral argument exposure on public attitudes. The core idea is simple: “[I]ncreased exposure to the judicial process, whatever the circumstances and even when citizens are displeased, results in collateral exposure to the symbols of judicial legitimacy, thereby tending to reinforce rather than undermine institutional support” (Gibson and Caldeira Reference Gibson and Caldeira2009, 4). Symbol exposure “triggers learned associated thoughts, which for most people in the United States have become connected with these symbols largely through socialization processes and experience, and which are typically ones of legitimacy and positivity” (Gibson, Lodge, and Woodson Reference Gibson, Lodge and Woodson2014, 842). As a result, comprehending court proceedings is not a necessary condition for attitude change.

Legitimizing symbols can be specific or abstract. Examples of specific legitimizing symbols that tend to be present at oral argument include “judges’ black robes” (Gibson and Nelson Reference Gibson and Nelson2017, 593), judges occupying an elevated position in the courtroom (Gibson, Caldeira, and Spence Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Spence2003b, 553), and litigants using “honorific forms of address” (Gibson, Lodge, and Woodson Reference Gibson, Lodge and Woodson2014, 838). Experimental evidence indicates that exposure to such symbols can heighten the positive relationship between support and decision acquiescence for low-frequency consumers (Gibson, Lodge, and Woodson Reference Gibson, Lodge and Woodson2014), sever the link between decision disagreeableness and diffuse support for those expressing relatively low levels of disappointment with a court’s judgment (Gibson and Nelson Reference Gibson and Nelson2016), and enhance perceptions of institutional legitimacy (Armaly Reference Armaly2018).

Court proceedings also expose consumers to more abstract legitimizing symbols such as “decorum” (Nelson and Gibson Reference Nelson, Gibson, Howard and Randazzo2017, 134); “judicial objectivity” (Gibson, Caldeira, and Baird Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Baird1998, 345); “emphasizing reliance on the Constitution, precedent, and legal norms” (Nicholson and Hansford Reference Nicholson and Hansford2014, 621); and “impartiality and insulation from ordinary political pressures” (Gibson and Caldeira Reference Gibson and Caldeira2009, 9). Consistent with courtroom settings, studies in other institutional contexts have found legitimizing effects associated with exposures emphasizing transparency (Gottlieb Reference Gottlieb2016; Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Brook, Hansen, Hoopes and Tidwell2019; Hollyer, Rosendorff, and Vreeland Reference Hollyer, Rosendorff and Vreeland2019), deliberation (Tyler, Rasinski, and Spodick Reference Tyler, Rasinski and Spodick1985; Simon and Sulkin Reference Simon and Sulkin2002; Persson, Esaiasson, and Gilljam Reference Persson, Esaiasson and Gilljam2013), legal frames (Baird and Gangl Reference Baird and Gangl2006; Zink, Spriggs, Scott Reference Zink, Spriggs and Scott2009; Farganis Reference Farganis2012), and procedural justice (Gibson Reference Gibson1989; Tyler and Rasinski Reference Tyler and Rasinski1991; Baird Reference Baird2001).

There is little existing empirical evidence on the impact of court exposure on public attitudes. Most relevant for our purpose, Black et al. (Reference Black, Johnson, Owens and Wedekingn.d.) conducted survey experiments exposing observers to short video clips of oral argument exchanges in two state supreme courts. Leveraging variation in audio versus video presentations, contentious versus neutral interpersonal exchanges, and dynamic versus static camera angles, the authors find mixed evidence concerning the impact of exposure on perceptions of legitimacy. With respect to court exposure more broadly, Benesh (Reference Benesh2006) finds that respondents who report having been a juror express more confidence in local courts, while those who report having been a direct party express less confidence. And Grimmelikhuijsen and Klijn (Reference Grimmelikhuijsen and Klijn2015) find that a randomized instruction to watch a television show featuring real trial court proceedings, commentary, and interviews with participating judges yielded increased trust in judges as the self-reported number of shows watched increased.

Building on this literature, we hypothesize that oral argument attendance will increase perceptions of institutional legitimacy and the extent to which judges are thought to base decisions on law versus politics. While there have been relatively few examinations of how court exposure impacts perceptions of decision-making compared to legitimacy, the underlying theory is similar. Legitimizing symbols presumably activate the “myth of legality,” which “holds that cases are decided by the application of legal rules formulated and applied through a politically and philosophically neutral process of legal reasoning” (Scheb and Lyons Reference Scheb and Lyons2000, 929). While so-called “legalist” and “realist” perspectives on judging are theoretically distinct from the concept of legitimacy (Gibson and Caldeira Reference Gibson and Caldeira2011; Cann and Yates Reference Cann and Yates2016), the same psychological processes that lead symbols to enhance legitimacy can reasonably be construed to reinforce perceptions that judges make decisions based on law rather than politics. Evidence indicates, for example, that exposure to a lunch or speech with a Supreme Court justice increases the extent to which decisions are thought to be driven by law relative to ideology (Krewson Reference Krewson2019).

The field experiment

To examine the causal effect of oral argument exposure on perceptions of the judiciary, we conducted a field experiment randomizing attendance at proceedings conducted by the U.S. Court of Appeals for the Tenth Circuit at Utah State University. The Tenth Circuit is one of thirteen federal circuit courts of appeals. It is an intermediate appellate court, sitting between state-based district courts and the Supreme Court in the federal judicial hierarchy. The Tenth Circuit hears appeals arising from district courts in six states.Footnote 3 Oral arguments are heard at the court’s discretion, generally in three-judge panels. According to Tenth Circuit officials attending this event, the court travels for oral argument as an outreach initiative for students and the public.

At this event, a three-judge panel held two oral argument sessions, with two cases in each session and a short break between sessions. Case details are provided in the appendix. As is common with circuit court proceedings, the cases were not particularly salient. Advance session-specific tickets were required for admission to the event, which filled to capacity. Student access was primarily allocated through this study.Footnote 4 Prior to the visit, we recruited undergraduate and graduate students to participate in a study concerning courts on event day in exchange for a $10.00 Amazon gift card.Footnote 5 In the recruitment email, students were told they would be randomly assigned to attend an oral argument session or participate in a different court-related activity. Participating students selected one of two time slots corresponding to the oral argument sessions. Once students were assigned to a session slot, they were were randomized within sessions at a 50–50 split between treatment and control groups.Footnote 6

The treatment and control groups were instructed to appear on event day at separate but nearby campus locations. On event day, control group participants completed a survey immediately upon arrival at their scheduled time and were subsequently dismissed. Treatment group participants were admitted to their respective oral argument sessions, after which they were provided the same survey with a day’s end completion deadline. All surveys were completed electronically on event day.Footnote 7 Overall, 248 students participated in the experiment. The first session respectively included 48 and 58 students in the control and treatment groups; the second session respectively included 70 and 72 students in the control and treatment groups.Footnote 8 As described in more detail in the appendix, control and treatment groups were balanced within and across sessions by gender, race, ideology, and prior court exposure. Although student samples present obvious generalizability issues, ours is “relevant for theory” (Coppock and McClellan Reference Coppock and McClellan2019, 3) because students are a target audience for traveling courts.

We followed standard practice in measuring our outcomes of interest. To capture perceptions of legitimacy, we used an established battery of statements (see, for example, Gibson, Caldeira, and Spence Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Spence2003a; Gibson and Caldeira Reference Gibson and Caldeira2009; Gibson and Nelson Reference Gibson and Nelson2015) that capture whether institutions enjoy “a widely accepted mandate to render judgments for a political community” (Gibson, Caldeira, and Spence Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Spence2003a). The items include:

  • Judges on the federal courts who consistently make decisions at odds with what a majority of the people want should be removed from their position as a judge.

  • If the federal courts started making a lot of decisions that most people disagree with, it might be better to do away with the federal courts altogether.

  • The right of the federal courts to decide certain types of controversial issues should be reduced.

  • The federal courts get too mixed up in politics.

Given the institutional context of our experiment, we modified these questions as developed for a study concerning the Supreme Court by replacing “Supreme Court” with “federal courts” (cf. Scherer, Benesh, and Steigerwalt Reference Scherer, Benesh and Steigerwalt2010).Footnote 9 Response choices were five-point Likert scales ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. A principal components analysis on the scores shows that items load on a single dimension.Footnote 10 We used factor scores from the principal components analysis to create a legitimacy index and normalized scores to the [0,1] interval.Footnote 11

To operationalize our decision-making hypothesis, we used a battery of questions developed by Gibson and Caldeira (Reference Gibson and Caldeira2011) to measure what they call “legal realism.” In essence, this battery captures the extent to which people think judicial decision-making is driven by law versus politics. The items include:

  • Judges always say that their decisions are based on the law and the Constitution, but in many cases, judges are really basing their decisions on their own personal beliefs.

  • Judges’ values and political views have little to do with how they decide cases before the federal courts.

  • Judges’ party affiliations have little to do with how they decide cases before the federal courts.

Again, given the institutional context of our experiment, we modified the questions as designed by replacing “Supreme Court” with “federal courts” (cf. Scherer, Benesh, and Steigerwalt Reference Scherer, Benesh and Steigerwalt2010). Response choices were five-point Likert scales ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. We used principal components analysis to create an index concerning decision-making attitudes and normalized scores to the [0,1] interval.Footnote 12 As detailed in the appendix, the result is similar using an alternative measure of beliefs about judicial decision-making developed by Cann and Yates (Reference Cann and Yates2016).

Analysis and results

Legitimacy

Beginning with the legitimacy hypothesis, Figure 1 plots means, 95% confidence intervals, and underlying data distributions (jittered for clarity) for the control and treatment groups. Between control and treatment, the perceived legitimacy mean increases from 0.48 to 0.60 ( $ p $ $ < $ .001). Substantively, this change indicates that oral argument attendance increased perceived legitimacy by 57% of a standard deviation. As detailed in the appendix, these results hold across sessions. There is evidence of heterogeneous session effects, with perceived legitimacy higher in the first session, but exposure to the second session nonetheless increased perceived legitimacy by 39% of a standard deviation.

Figure 1. The Effect of Oral Argument Attendance on Legitimacy.

Judicial decision-making

Turning to the decision-making hypothesis, Figure 2 plots means, 95% confidence intervals, and underlying data distributions for the control and treatment groups. Between control and treatment, the mean index score increases from 0.34 to 0.46 ( $ p $ $ < $ .001). Substantively, this indicates that oral argument attendance increased the extent to which individuals perceive judicial decision-making to be driven by law versus politics by 61% of a standard deviation. As detailed in the appendix, the result holds across sessions. Here, however, there is no evidence of heterogeneous session effects.

Figure 2. The Effect of Oral Argument Attendance on Beliefs About Judicial Decision-Making.

Conclusion

Judicial independence is widely thought to be contingent on public support. Given increasing political attacks on the judiciary, courts are engaging in outreach to enhance and maintain institutional legitimacy. What remains unclear, however, is the extent to which courts can unilaterally manage public relations – particularly given that people are often unfamiliar with how they operate. We consider whether court exposure can change public attitudes with a field experiment randomizing in-person attendance at oral argument proceedings conducted by a traveling federal circuit court of appeals. We find that oral argument attendance increases perceived legitimacy and the extent to which judges are thought to make decisions based on law versus politics.

The normative implications are mixed. From a public policy perspective, commentators have long debated reform to increase the public’s exposure to court proceedings. The results presented here suggest that proposals to institute circuit riding and install cameras in courtrooms may increase perceived legitimacy and change the way people think about how judges make decisions, which may be arguments in their favor. But these policy questions are complex, and a comprehensive assessment will require more empirical evidence concerning a variety of potential costs and benefits. With respect to cameras in courtrooms, for example, impact may depend on a variety of circumstances (Black et al. Reference Black, Johnson, Owens and Wedekingn.d.), which cautions against interpreting the evidence presented here as indicating that oral argument exposure is necessarily positive.

A potentially concerning normative implication is that oral argument exposure may generate misleading public perceptions of the judiciary. Although we find that exposure increases the extent to which judges are thought to base decisions on law versus politics, for example, it is not clear that such updating is warranted based on what transpires at oral argument. Interestingly, some Supreme Court justices have argued against courtroom cameras out of concern that excerpts may mislead viewers (Segall Reference Segall2016, 793–794), but any misdirection may work the other way as well. To the extent oral arguments disproportionately emphasize neutral principles relative to our broader empirical understanding of the determinants of judicial decision-making, exposure may distort public perception.

The results have important implications for judicial politics. While much of the empirical research on oral arguments emphasizes internal dynamics, we show that these proceedings can impact the public as well. We also contribute to the literature on positivity theory with application to a new setting using a real-world exposure, both of which enhance generalizability. Moreover, notwithstanding the literature’s emphasis on legitimacy, it is important to understand how exposure impacts other outcomes of interest (cf. Black et al. Reference Black, Johnson, Owens and Wedekingn.d., 17). We advance this goal by examining how oral argument attendance impacts public perceptions of judicial decision-making. Last, our research design is transportable to other areas where exposure to court business may impact public attitudes, such as briefs, conference deliberations, and internal correspondence bargaining over opinion language.

Notwithstanding this project’s contributions, there are several important limitations. Generalizability is limited by reliance on college students who self-selected into exposure. Although students are “relevant for theory” (Coppock and McClellan Reference Coppock and McClellan2019, 3) here insofar as they are a target demographic for judicial outreach efforts, it may be, for example, that older participants would be less impressionable. Theoretically, there is reason to expect heterogeneous treatment effects based on factors such as case salience and topic familiarity, but the proceedings analyzed here did not involve constitutional or otherwise politically contested issues. Moreover, we do not explore treatment effect persistance or isolate the mechanisms underlying observed attitude changes. Future research will help develop a more comprehensive evaluation of how exposure to court proceedings impacts the public.

Footnotes

*

Thanks to participants at the 2020 QuantLaw Conference hosted by the University of Arizona College of Law, and colloquium participants at Brigham Young University, for helpful feedback. Replication files are available on the Journal of Law & Courts Dataverse page.

1 See, for example, Ninth Circuit Holds Oral Arguments at Law School for First Time Since COVID-19, https://www.uscourts.gov/news/2023/02/27/ninth-circuit-holds-oral-arguments-law-school-first-time-covid-19.

2 Congressional Globe, 30th Congress, 1st Session 596 (1848).

3 These states are Colorado, Kansas, New Mexico, Oklahoma, Utah, and Wyoming.

4 In addition to the tickets we allocated, a select group of student leaders obtained tickets from the event organizer. Other tickets were dispersed to university officials, community groups, and secondary schools.

5 Student recruitment occurred using the primary email address on file with the university. Of the resulting sample, about 33 percent were freshmen, 20 percent sophomores, 15 percent juniors, and 15 percent seniors, with the remainder being graduate students or selecting the “other” category (e.g., non-degree-seeking students). Over fifty majors were represented in the sample. The university has a Law and Constitutional Studies major, but only seven students were pursuing that course of study. Sixteen students were political science majors. As a result, the vast majority of participants were not studying law or politics as majors. The university does not have a law school.

6 We used a random number generator to assign participants to treatment and control groups within sessions.

7 With survey completion time varying across treatment and control groups, there is some risk that treatment group participants were exposed to stimuli other than oral argument that could have impacted their perceptions of the judiciary, though the risk seems insubstantial for several reasons. First, there were no salient court-related events that afternoon or evening that would have provided broad exposure to judicial symbols. Second, not much time passed between oral argument and survey completion by the treatment groups – less than two hours for most participants. Third, if an intervening event impacted perceptions of the judiciary, we should observe weaker effects in the treatment–control comparison when omitting treatment group participants who completed surveys later in the day. However, dropping treatment group participants who completed their survey after 5:00 p.m. increases the gap between control and treatment groups on both outcome variables.

8 A post-hoc power analysis using GPower shows that using a two-tailed test and the conventional $ \alpha =.05 $ criterion for statistical significance, our sample sizes (118 and 130, respectively) have “power” of .975, meaning that 97.5% of sample pairs would be able to detect a statistically significant difference if there was, in fact, an effect of at least .5 standard deviations in the population. Respectively, power for the first and second sessions were .719 and .841. Attrition details are presented in the appendix.

9 Although question wording and inclusion varies across studies, the shared theme is that battery items capture diffuse support, which is defined as “institutional loyalty” (Gibson, Caldeira, and Spence Reference Gibson, Caldeira and Spence2003a, 356) or the “reservoir of favorable attitudes or good will that helps [people] accept or tolerate outputs to which they are opposed or the effects of which they see as damaging to their wants” (Easton Reference Easton1965, 273).

10 This holds using the Scree test and Kaiser criterion.

11 As a robustness check, we re-ran the principal components analysis on a polychoric correlation matrix to determine whether the ordinal nature of the Likert items posed a threat to this approach. The scores from the principal components analysis on the polychoric correlation matrix correlate with the index from the original principal components analysis at $ r>.99 $ .

12 The Scree test and Kaiser criterion indicated a one-dimensional solution for this index. As with the legitimacy index, we re-ran the principal components analysis on a polychoric correlation matrix and found that the scores based on the polychoric matrix correlate with scores from a regular Pearson’s r matrix at $ r>.99 $ .

References

Armaly, Miles T. 2018. “Politicized nominations and public attitudes toward the Supreme Court in the Polarization Era.” Justice System Journal 39(3): 193209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, Vanessa A. 2001. “Building institutional legitimacy: The role of procedural justice.” Political Research Quarterly 52(2): 333354.Google Scholar
Baird, Vanessa A., and Gangl, Amy. 2006. “Shattering the myth of legality: The impact of the media’s framing of Supreme Court procedures on perceptions of fairness.” Political Psychology 27(4): 597614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benesh, Sara C. 2006. “Understanding public confidence in American courts.” Journal of Politics 68(3): 697707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, Ryan C., Johnson, Timothy R., Owens, Ryan J., and Wedeking, Justin. n.d. “Televised oral arguments and judicial legitimacy: An initial assessment.” Forthcoming, Political Behavior.Google Scholar
Black, Ryan C., Johnson, Timothy R., and Wedeking, Justin. 2012. Oral Arguments and Coalition Formation on the U.S. Supreme Court. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Blair, Robert A., Karim, Sabrina M., and Morse, Benjamin S.. 2019. “Establishing the rule of law in weak and war-torn states: Evidence from a field experiment with the Liberian National Police.” American Political Science Review 113(3): 641657.10.1017/S0003055419000121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calabresi, Steven G., and Presser, David C.. 2006. “Reintroducing circuit riding: A timely proposal.” Minnesota Law Review 90(5): 13861416.Google Scholar
Caldeira, Gregory A. 1986. “Neither the purse nor the sword: Dynamics of public confidence in the Supreme Court.” American Political Science Review 80(4): 12091226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cann, Damon M., and Yates, Jeff. 2016. These Estimable Courts: Understanding Public Perceptions of State Judicial Institutions and Legal Policy-Making. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199307210.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, Christopher J., Enns, Peter K., and Wohlfarth, Patrick C.. 2011. “How public opinion constrains the U.S. Supreme Court.” American Journal of Political Science 55(1): 7488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Tom S. 2009. “The separation of powers, court curbing, and judicial legitimacy.” American Journal of Political Science 53(4): 971999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, Alexander, and McClellan, Oliver A.. 2019. “Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents.” Research and Politics 6(1): 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curry, Todd A., and Fix, Michael P.. 2019. “May it please the Twitterverse: The use of Twitter by state high court judges.” Journal of Information Technology and Politics 16(4): 379393.10.1080/19331681.2019.1657048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deits, Mary J., and Keenan, Lora E.. 2004. “Getting to know us: Judicial outreach in Oregon.” The Journal of Appellate Practice and Process 6(2): 237248.Google Scholar
Easton, David. 1965. A Systems Analysis of Political Life. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Epstein, Lee, and Martin, Andrew D.. 2010. “Does public opinion influence the Supreme Court? Possibly yes (but we’re not sure why).” University of Pennsylvania Journal of Constitutional Law 13(2): 263282.Google Scholar
Farganis, Dion. 2012. “Do reasons matter? The impact of opinion content on Supreme Court legitimacy.” Political Research Quarterly 65(1): 206216.10.1177/1065912911430667CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L. 1989. “Understandings of justice: Institutional legitimacy, procedural justice, and political tolerance.” Law & Society Review 23(3): 469496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., and Caldeira, Gregory A.. 2011. “Has legal realism damaged the legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court?Law & Society Review 45(1): 195219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., and Caldeira, Gregory A.. 2009. Citizens, Courts, and Confirmations: Positivity Theory and the Judgments of the American People. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.1515/9781400830602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., and Baird, Vanessa A.. 1998. “On the legitimacy of national high courts.” American Political Science Review 92(2): 343358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., and Spence, Lester Kenyatta. 2003a. “Measuring attitudes toward the United States Supreme Court.” American Journal of Political Science 47(2): 354367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Caldeira, Gregory A., and Spence, Lester Kenyatta. 2003b. “The Supreme Court and the US presidential election of 2000: Wounds, self-inflicted or otherwise?British Journal of Political Science 33(4): 535556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., and Nelson, Michael J.. 2017. “Reconsidering positivity theory: What roles do politicization, ideological disagreement, and legal realism play in shaping U.S. Supreme Court legitimacy?Journal of Empirical Legal Studies 14(3): 592617.10.1111/jels.12157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., and Nelson, Michael J.. 2016. “Change in institutional support for the U.S. Supreme Court: Is the Court’s legitimacy imperiled by the decisions it makes?Public Opinion Quarterly 80(3): 622641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., and Nelson, Michael J.. 2015. “Is the U.S. Supreme Court’s legitimacy grounded in performance satisfaction and ideology?American Journal of Political Science 59(1): 162174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James L., Lodge, Milton, and Woodson, Benjamin. 2014. “Losing, but accepting: Legitimacy, positivity theory, and the symbols of judicial authority.” Law & Society Review 48(4): 837866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glennon, Colin, and Strother, Logan. 2019. “The maintenance of institutional legitimacy in Supreme Court justices’ public rhetoric.” Journal of Law and Courts 7(2): 241261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, Jessica. 2016. “Greater expectations: A field experiment to improve accountability in Mali.” American Journal of Political Science 60(1): 143157.10.1111/ajps.12186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimmelikhuijsen, Stephan, and Klijn, Albert. 2015. “The effects of judicial transparency on public trust: Evidence from a field experiment.” Public Administration 93(4): 9951011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, Darren, Brook, Lucas C., Hansen, Ian M., Hoopes, Neal A., and Tidwell, Taylor R.. 2019. “Do citizens see through transparency? Evidence from survey experiments in Peru.” British Journal of Political Science 49(1): 205228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmke, Gretchen. 2010. “The origins of institutional crises in Latin America.” American Journal of Political Science 54(3): 737750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollyer, James R., Rosendorff, B. Peter, and Vreeland, James Raymond. 2019. “Transparency, protest and democratic stability.” British Journal of Political Science 49(4): 12511277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobi, Tonja, and Sag, Matthew. 2019. “The new oral argument: Justices as advocates.” Notre Dame Law Review 94(3): 11611254.Google Scholar
Johnson, Timothy R. 2004. Oral Arguments and Decision Making on the United States Supreme Court. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Krehbiel, Jay N. 2016. “The politics of judicial procedures: The role of public oral hearings in the German Constitutional Court.” American Journal of Political Science 60(4): 9901005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krewson, Christopher N. 2019. “Save this honorable Court: Shaping public perceptions of the Supreme Court off the bench.” Political Research Quarterly 72(3): 686699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malesky, Edmund, and Taussig, Markus. 2019. “Participation, government legitimacy, and regulatory compliance in emerging economies: A firm-level field experiment in Vietnam.” American Political Science Review 113(2): 530551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuire, Kevin T., and Stimson, James A.. 2004. “The least dangerous branch revisited: New evidence on Supreme Court responsiveness to public preferences.” Journal of Politics 66(4): 10181035.10.1111/j.1468-2508.2004.00288.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medina, Harold R. 1961. “Some reflections on the judicial function at the appellate level.” Washington University Law Review 1961(2): 148156.Google Scholar
Nelson, Ingrid A. 2005. “More judicial outreach: ‘Justice on Wheels’ from the Supreme Court of Wisconsin.” Journal of Appellate Practice and Process 7(1): 167170.Google Scholar
Nelson, Michael J., and Gibson, James L.. 2017. “U.S. Supreme Court legitimacy: Unanswered questions and an agenda for future research.” In Routledge Handbook of Judicial Behavior, edited by Howard, Robert M. and Randazzo, Kirk A., 132150. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, Stephen P., and Hansford, Thomas G.. 2014. “Partisans in robes: Party cues and public acceptance of Supreme Court decisions.” American Political Science Review 58(3): 620636.10.1111/ajps.12091CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persson, Mikael, Esaiasson, Peter, and Gilljam, Mikael. 2013. “The effects of direct voting and deliberation on legitimacy beliefs: An experimental study of small group decision-making.” European Political Science Review 5(3): 381399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peyton, Kyle, Sierra-Arevalo, Michael, and Rand, David G.. 2019. “A field experiment on community policing and police legitimacy.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116(40): 1989419898.10.1073/pnas.1910157116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringsmuth, Eve M., and Johnson, Timothy R.. 2013. “Supreme Court oral arguments and institutional maintenance.” American Politics Research 41(4): 651673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savchak, Elisha Carol, and Edwards, Amanda Ross. 2016. “Why are state judges among us?Judicature 100(2): 2130.Google Scholar
Scheb, John M. II, and Lyons, William. 2000. “The myth of legality and public evaluation of the Supreme Court.” Social Science Quarterly 81(4): 928940.Google Scholar
Scherer, Nancy, Benesh, Sara C., and Steigerwalt, Amy. 2010. “How do lower federal courts attain institutional legitimacy when they are largely unknown to the public?” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association.Google Scholar
Segall, Eric J. 2016. “Invisible justices: How our highest court hides from the American people.” Georgia State University Law Review 32(4): 787848.Google Scholar
Simon, Adam F., and Sulkin, Tracy. 2002. “Discussion’s impact on political allocations: An experimental approach.” Political Analysis 10(4): 403412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staton, Jeffrey K. 2004. “Judicial policy implementation in Mexico City and Merida.” Comparative Politics 37(1): 4160.10.2307/4150123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephenson, Matthew C. 2004. “Court of public opinion: Government accountability and judicial independence.” Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization 20(2): 379399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stras, David D. 2006. “Why Supreme Court justices should ride circuit again.” Minnesota Law Review 90(5): 17101751.Google Scholar
Tyler, Tom R., and Rasinski, Kenneth. 1991. “Procedural justice, institutional legitimacy, and the acceptance of unpopular U.S. Supreme Court decisions: A reply to Gibson.” Law & Society Review 25(3): 621630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, Tom R., Rasinski, Kenneth A., and Spodick, Nancy. 1985. “Influence of voice on satisfaction with leaders: Exploring the meaning of process control.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48(1): 7281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanberg, Georg. 2004. The Politics of Constitutional Review in Germany. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warren, Charles. 1924. The Supreme Court in United States History. Volume I. Boston: Little, Brown, and Company.Google Scholar
Zink, James R., Spriggs, James F. II, and Scott, John T.. 2009. “Courting the public: The influence of decision attributes on individuals’ views of court opinions.” Journal of Politics 71(3): 909925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The Effect of Oral Argument Attendance on Legitimacy.

Figure 1

Figure 2. The Effect of Oral Argument Attendance on Beliefs About Judicial Decision-Making.