Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-g9frx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-13T11:57:17.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evaluation of a nationwide whole-school approach to mental health and well-being in 40 149 Australian secondary school students: cluster quasi-experimental study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2025

Roshini Balasooriya Lekamge*
Affiliation:
School of Public Health and Preventative Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia Alfred Mental and Addiction Health, Alfred Health, Melbourne, Australia
Md Nazmul Karim
Affiliation:
School of Public Health and Preventative Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Leo Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Translational Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia Alfred Mental and Addiction Health, Alfred Health, Melbourne, Australia
Dragan Ilic
Affiliation:
School of Public Health and Preventative Medicine, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
*
Correspondence: Roshini Balasooriya Lekamge. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Adolescence is the peak life stage for the development of mental illness. Whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being, modelled on the World Health Organization’s Health-Promoting Schools Framework, hold vast potential in this developmentally sensitive period. However, the evidence base for these interventions is inconclusive.

Aims

Our study examines the effectiveness of The Resilience Project School Partnership Program, a whole-school intervention involving students, teachers and parents, centred around concepts of gratitude, empathy, emotional literacy and mindfulness.

Methods

A quasi-experimental study with an intervention and a control arm was used to evaluate the programme in 40 149 students across 102 schools in 2023. Data collected included sociodemographic information and outcomes derived from validated scales, comprising life satisfaction, hope, coping skills, anxiety and depression. Intervention schools were stratified by the number of years they had implemented the programme, and mixed-effects regression models were used to evaluate the programme.

Results

After adjusting for confounders, participants at schools who had been implementing the programme for 6 years or longer demonstrated significantly better outcomes across all five domains (life satisfaction: B = 0.627, 95% CI 0.465–0.789; hope: B = 2.135, 95% CI 0.895–3.347; coping skills: B = 0.438, 95% CI 0.250–0.625; anxiety: odds ratio = 0.658, 95% CI 0.559–0.774; depression: odds ratio = 0.534, 95% CI 0.459–0.620). Only depression was significantly lower among participants at schools in their fourth or fifth year of implementing the programme (odds ratio = 0.941, 95% CI 0.935–0.948).

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that whole-school interventions may require long-term investment to realise their potential and highlight implementation duration as an important consideration for future evaluations of whole-school interventions.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

A mental health crisis is emerging among Australian youth and is reflective of a global trend. Reference McGorry, Coghill and Berk1 Adolescence is a time of particular vulnerability to poor mental health, with a recent meta-analysis identifying the peak age at onset for mental illness to be 14.5 years. Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo and Salazar de Pablo2 An Australian-based national study found that 32.4% of Australian males and 45.5% of Australian females aged 16–24 years had experienced a mental illness in the preceding 12 months, the highest prevalence in any age category. 3

Whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being

Schools have long been considered to be influential settings for promotion of mental health and well-being in adolescents, as most adolescents spend a substantial proportion of their time in school. Reference Hamoda, Hoover, Bostic, Rahman and Saaed4 Whole-school interventions are particularly attractive by virtue of their holistic focus, which shifts attention away from individual behaviour change and towards a socio-ecological approach to health promotion. Reference McLeroy, Bibeau, Steckler and Glanz5,Reference Samdal and Rowling6 Whole-school approaches are multicomponent interventions modelled on the World Health Organization’s Health-Promoting Schools Framework. This framework was updated in 2021 to comprise eight global standards, 7 which can be categorised into four different levels (Fig. 1). Interventions are generally classified as whole-school if they incorporate a component addressing each of the following levels: curriculum, ethos and environment and community. Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8,Reference Goldberg, Sklad, Elfrink, Schreurs, Bohlmeijer and Clarke9 Whole-school interventions are theoretically more likely to improve adolescent mental health and well-being than other forms of school-based approaches. Reference Samdal and Rowling6,Reference Goldberg, Sklad, Elfrink, Schreurs, Bohlmeijer and Clarke9,Reference Barry, Clarke, Petersen and Jenkins10 This has been attributed to their use of a holistic, systems-based approach and involvement of the key stakeholders in an adolescent’s life, including their peers, parents and teachers.

Fig. 1 Global standards of the Health-Promoting Schools Framework (reproduced from Lekamge et al. Reference Lekamge, Jain, Sheen, Solanki, Zhou and Romero11 under the Creative Commons CC BY license).

Despite a strong theoretical basis for whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being, meta-analyses exploring their effectiveness have provided inconclusive evidence. Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8,Reference Weare and Nind12,Reference Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor and Schellinger13 A 2015 meta-analysis concluded that although whole-school approaches significantly reduced the odds of tobacco use or being bullied among school-aged populations, there was no evidence of effectiveness in other domains, including alcohol or drug use, aggression and bullying others and mental health outcomes. This inconclusive evidence base is commonly attributed to the complexity of evaluating whole-school interventions Reference Wignall, Kelly and Grace14 and the challenges encountered in their implementation. Reference Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor and Schellinger13

A domain that has received comparatively less attention is implementation duration. Public health initiatives adopting a socio-ecological approach address more than simply individual-level factors affecting behavioural and health outcomes. Realisation of such systems-level change is likely to require time. Despite this, a systematic review of community-based health promotion programmes found that these typically lasted for only 2 to 3 years, with review authors calling into question whether such short durations were sufficient to demonstrate impact on behavioural and health outcomes. Reference Merzel and D’Afflitti15 A 2015 meta-analysis similarly found that whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being among secondary school-aged populations could involve interventions as short as 15 weeks, with the vast majority of interventions implemented for 2 to 3 years. Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8 Given the complex changes that whole-school approaches aspire to achieve, including changes to the school ethos and environment, as well as school relationships with parents and the local community, it is plausible that they may require long-term investment to realise their potential. It is thus important to investigate whether the evidence base surrounding whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being is inconclusive because of programme ineffectiveness, or whether temporal investment represents a factor key to programme success.

Current study

While whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being offer a promising strategy in improving adolescent mental health, the evidence base for these interventions is inconclusive. Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8 There is a particular need to investigate the impact of implementation duration on the effectiveness of whole-school approaches, as this may reveal valuable insights for key stakeholders, including schools and public health and policy makers. Here, we evaluate the Resilience Project School Partnership Program, a whole-school intervention promoting mental health and well-being in Australia. 16 The programme involves students, teachers and parents and is centred around the teaching and practice of four core elements: gratitude, empathy, emotional literacy and mindfulness (GEEM). In its 10 years of implementation, the intervention has reached more than 1500 schools and early learning centres, and in 2023, the programme was implemented in more than 800 primary and secondary schools across Australia. The objective of this study was to assess whether the intervention group demonstrated better mental health outcomes across life satisfaction, hope, coping skills, anxiety and depression than the control group. A unique feature of this study was that the intervention schools included had implemented the programme for 2–8 years, enabling us to explore whether schools with longer implementation duration exhibited better mental health outcomes.

Methods

We conducted a quasi-experimental study that consisted of an experimental and control arm, with post-test data but no pre-test data, Reference Harris, McGregor, Perencevich, Furuno, Zhu and Peterson17 consistent with study design B1 as described by Harris et al. Reference Harris, McGregor, Perencevich, Furuno, Zhu and Peterson17 The experimental arm consisted of a whole-school intervention delivered by the Resilience Project. 18 Outcomes were measured independently by Resilient Youth Australia, 19 annually, via an online, user-report system. To partake in the intervention or survey, schools had to enter into a service agreement with the Resilience Project and/or Resilient Youth Australia. Assignment of schools to conditions was by administrator selection, whereby a school’s executive team self-selected their schooling cluster into the experimental or control condition at the commencement of the study. Reference Cook, Campbell and Shadish20 Any school that was in at least its second year of implementing the programme, that was implementing the programme for all grade levels, and that agreed at the outset to having students participate in data collection was included in the intervention group. The control group consisted of schools that agreed to having their students undertake data collection but were not actively implementing the programme and had not previously implemented the programme.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human participants were approved by the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (project ID: 37824). Informed consent by participants was deemed not necessary by the Research Ethics Committee owing to the anonymous nature of the data collected.

Participants

Participants were required to meet all aspects of the eligibility criteria, including: (a) enrolment in grades 7 to 12 in an Australian secondary school in 2023; (b) age 11 to 19 years in 2023; and (c) attending a school that was in at least its second year of implementing the programme and that offered the programme to all grade levels, or attending a school that was assigned as part of the control group. The sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are detailed in Table 2.

Intervention

The programme consisted of a whole-school approach to cultivating happiness and resilience in young people. At the curriculum level, the programme comprised 50 year-level-specific lessons to facilitate students’ understanding and practice of GEEM. At the ethos and environment level, schools were provided with activities to foster GEEM outside the formal curriculum; resources to embed GEEM chat boards and gratitude boards into the physical environment; professional development opportunities to build staff capacity for implementing the programme; and resources to facilitate the practice of GEEM as a staff cohort. At the community level, parents and carers were provided with access to an online hub comprising informational videos, family well-being activities and links to additional resources. The programme is embedded among governmental initiatives to promote mental health and well-being in schools, with Be You and the Schools Mental Health Menu serving as examples of national and subnational initiatives, respectively. 16,Reference Hoare, Thorp, Bartholomeusz-Raymond, McCoy and Berk21 Each intervention school was allocated a Schools Partnership Manager to assist with programme implementation. A detailed description of the programme in accordance with the TIDieR checklist Reference Hoffmann, Glasziou, Boutron, Milne, Perera and Moher22 and sample lesson plans are provided in Supplementary Material 1 and 2, available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2024.843.

Data sources

Data were collected from January to December in 2023 through an online survey administered in schools. The Resilience Survey was co-developed with the School of Psychology, Social Work and Social Policy at the University of South Australia. The 60-item survey incorporates five scientifically validated measures: the Cantril Self-Anchoring Scale (CSAS), Children’s Hope Scale (CHS), Coping Strategies Inventory (CSI) Disengagement Subscale, Generalised Anxiety Disorder (GAD-2) and Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-2). We evaluated the programme’s effect based on these outcome measures. Survey questions from these measures are included in Supplementary Material 3.

Cantril Self-Anchoring Scale (adapted)

The adapted CSAS is a single-item rating scale in which students rank their life satisfaction from 1 to 8, with a higher score indicating greater life satisfaction. An 11-point version of the CSAS has demonstrated reliability and convergent validity with six previously validated measures of subjective well-being in a sample of 7670 adolescents in Scotland. Reference Levin and Currie23 The scale used here was adapted in that the wording was simplified and an eight-point version was used (Supplementary Material 3).

Children’s Hope Scale

The CHS is a six-item scale, with each item recorded on a six-point Likert scale. Scores are summed from the six items, producing a total score out of 36, with a higher aggregate score indicating greater hope. The CHS has demonstrated reliability, convergent and discriminant validity in studies comprising children and adolescents. Reference Snyder, Hoza, Pelham, Rapoff, Ware and Danovsky24,Reference Valle, Huebner and Suldo25 Cronbach’s alpha in our sample was 0.897 (95% CI 0.895–0.899).

Coping Strategies Inventory Disengagement Subscale (adapted)

The CSI disengagement subscale encompasses four items, with each item recorded on a four-point Likert scale. Items are summed to produce an overall score out of 16, with a higher aggregate score indicating greater coping skills. Studies have demonstrated the reliability Reference Tobin, Holroyd, Reynolds and Wigal26 and criterion and construct validity Reference Tobin, Holroyd and Reynolds27 of the CSI. Hierarchical factor analysis has demonstrated two tertiary scales, one of which is the disengagement scale used here. Reference Tobin, Holroyd, Reynolds and Wigal26 The adapted version includes minor simplifications to the wording of the original disengagement subscale (Supplementary Material 3). Cronbach’s alpha in our sample was 0.523 (95% CI 0.515–0.530).

Generalised Anxiety Disorder

The GAD-2 is a two-item screening tool for generalised anxiety disorder, with each item recorded on a four-point Likert scale. The scores are first summed and then dichotomised, with an aggregate score of 3 or more demonstrating acceptable sensitivity, specificity and discriminant validity as a screening tool for generalised anxiety disorder. Reference Staples, Dear, Gandy, Fogliati, Fogliati and Karin28,Reference Plummer, Manea, Trepel and McMillan29 Here, a score of <3 was coded as 0 (indicating the absence of anxiety), and a score of ≥3 was coded as 1 (indicating the presence of anxiety). Cronbach’s alpha in our sample was 0.861 (95% CI 0.858–0.863).

Patient Health Questionnaire

The PHQ-2 is a two-item screening tool for major depressive disorder, with each item recorded on a four-point Likert scale. The scores are then summed, with a total of 3 or more demonstrating acceptable sensitivity, specificity and discriminant validity as a screening tool for major depressive disorder. Reference Staples, Dear, Gandy, Fogliati, Fogliati and Karin28,Reference Richardson, Rockhill, Russo, Grossman, Richards and McCarty30 A score of <3 was coded as 0 (indicating the absence of depression), and a score of ≥3 was coded as 1 (indicating the presence of depression). Cronbach’s alpha in our sample was 0.729 (95% CI 0.724–0.735).

Statistical methods

Baseline sociodemographic characteristics of the intervention and control groups were compared using chi-squared tests for categorical variables and independent t-tests for numerical variables. For comparison of outcomes between intervention and control schools, intervention schools were first stratified into categories according to their duration of implementing the programme: (a) second or third year, (b) fourth or fifth year, (c) sixth, seventh or eighth year. Each stratum was then compared with the control group to assess whether differential outcomes were observed across the three strata. These strata were selected as a recent meta-analysis found that the majority of whole-school interventions promoting mental health and well-being were implemented by schools for 2–3 years, Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8 whereas 6+ years of implementation would result in a subset of students who had received the programme for their entire secondary school experience. Given that individual data were nested within schools, which themselves were nested within states, mixed-effects models were used to compare group differences in outcomes. The mixed-effects models used a fixed effect for group (intervention versus control), a random effect for school to account for clustering of responses within schools (level 2), and a random effect for state to account for clustering of responses within states (level 3). The effects of clustering were negligible at the state level, but apparent at the school level (Table 1). This indicates that the majority of variation occurred at the individual level.

Table 1 Intraclass coefficients at the school-level for outcome measures

Mixed-effects linear regression was used for continuous outcomes (all of which were approximately normally distributed), and mixed-effects logistic regression was used for binary outcomes. The models used maximum likelihood estimation and robust standard errors. Confounders that were adjusted for as fixed effects in all mixed-effects models were grade, gender, socioeconomic and rurality status, with each treated as a categorical covariate. Socioeconomic and rurality status were categorised according to the Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage 31 and Remoteness Area, 32 respectively, based on the location of a participant’s school. All analyses were conducted using Stata Basic Edition Version 18 for Windows (StataCorp LLC, College Station, Texas, USA).

Results

Participant characteristics

A total of 40 149 students participated in the study, consisting of 18 875 participants from 55 schools assigned to the intervention group and 21 274 participants from 47 schools assigned to the control group (Table 2). The mean age was slightly lower in the intervention group than in the control group (14.11 v. 14.47 years, range 11–19). There was a higher proportion of male participants in the intervention compared with the control group (48.33% v. 37.14%), with roughly equivalent proportions of non-binary participants (3.86% v. 3.33%). The control group had a higher proportion of participants from schools in metropolitan areas (89.98% v. 42.17%) and the highest socioeconomic quintile (54.57% v. 16.31%).

Table 2 Sociodemographic variables of the intervention and control groups

P < 0.001 for all t-tests and chi-squared analyses between intervention and control groups.

Participants at schools in their second or third year of implementing the intervention did not show significant differences with respect to any of the five outcomes compared with the control group (Table 3). Participants at schools in their fourth or fifth year of implementing the intervention demonstrated significantly lower odds of depression than the control group (odds ratio = 0.941, 95% CI 0.935–0.948), but there were no significant differences in relation to other outcome domains. Participants at schools in at least their sixth year of implementing the intervention demonstrated significantly better scores across all outcome domains. This included higher scores across positive mental health outcomes (life satisfaction: B = 0.627, 95% CI 0.465–0.789; hope: B = 2.135, 95% CI 0.895–3.374; coping skills: B = 0.438, 95% CI 0.250–0.625) and significantly lower odds of mental illness than the control group (anxiety: odds ratio = 0.658, 95% CI 0.559–0.774; depression: odds ratio = 0.534, 95% CI 0.459–0.620). A graphical representation of these results is provided in Figs 2 and 3. The full mixed-effects models are included in Supplementary Material 4.

Table 3 Results from mixed-effects linear and logistic regression models, with intervention schools stratified by number of years implementing the programme (reference: control group); all results are adjusted for grade, gender, socioeconomic and rurality status

OR, odds ratio.

**P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Fig. 2 Graphical representation of linear mixed-effects model results. The reference for the null effect is represented by a maroon dashed line.

Fig. 3 Graphical representation of logistic mixed-effects model results. The reference for the null effect is represented by a maroon dashed line.

Discussion

The findings of our nationwide evaluation indicate that participants receiving The Resilience Project School Partnership Program demonstrated consistently better mental health outcomes than the control group, provided that schools had invested in long-term implementation of the programme. Students at schools in at least their sixth year of implementing the programme demonstrated significantly better mental health outcomes than the control group, with this pattern consistent across all five outcome domains. A significant difference in outcomes emerged for participants at schools implementing the programme for 4 to 5 years, who demonstrated significantly lower odds for depression than the control group. By contrast, participants at schools implementing the programme for 2 to 3 years demonstrated no significant difference in outcomes compared with the control group.

A 2015 meta-analysis concluded that whole-school interventions in school-aged populations were ineffective with respect to the majority of mental health and well-being outcomes Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8 , a surprising finding given the robust theoretical basis underlying whole-school interventions. Reference Samdal and Rowling6,Reference Barry, Clarke, Petersen and Jenkins10 One possible explanation for this is that the majority of whole-school interventions examined were implemented by schools for only 2 to 3 years, Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8 whereas our findings suggest that this could be too short a duration to achieve the complex systems-level changes that these interventions aspire to achieve. Evaluation of a whole-school intervention as ineffective after 2 to 3 years of implementation may not necessarily indicate programme ineffectiveness; rather, the programme may not have been implemented for long enough to achieve measurable change. Our findings indicate that long-term investment in whole-school interventions could lead to substantial benefit; for example, participants at schools implementing the programme for 6 years or longer had 34% lower odds for anxiety and 47% lower odds for depression than the control group. Given the manifold consequences of positive mental health and mental illness, long-term investment in these programmes holds vast potential to reduce the psychosocial and economic burden attributable to mental health. Reference Hoare, Thorp, Bartholomeusz-Raymond, McCoy and Berk21

Our study offers a conceptual model of why schools with longer implementation of the programme demonstrated better mental health outcomes, whereas those with shorter periods of implementation did not (Fig. 4). First, students at schools in at least their sixth year of implementing the programme are more likely to have had multi-year exposure to programme components. For example, a grade 12 student at a school in its sixth year of implementing the programme will have received the programme for their entire secondary school experience, provided they had remained at the same school. In 2023, the grade 7–12 apparent retention rate in Australia was 79.1%. 33 Given that changes to one’s emotions, thoughts and behaviours are known to require time and iterative effort, Reference Kwasnicka, Dombrowski, White and Sniehotta34 longer exposure to the programme is more likely to be effective in modifying these faculties central to mental health and well-being.

Fig. 4 Conceptual model of why longer programme implementation may result in better effects.

Second, owing to their implementation in complex, multilevel systems and the involvement of multiple stakeholders, whole-school approaches commonly encounter challenges with implementation. Reference Durlak and DuPre35 It is likely that schools with longer implementation of whole-school interventions will become more proficient in their implementation and in troubleshooting the challenges that arise. Stronger implementation has subsequently been linked to better mental health outcomes, with a meta-analysis of school-based social and emotional learning programmes demonstrating that effect sizes were twice as high for interventions with high-quality implementation when compared to those with low-quality implementation. Reference Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor and Schellinger13

Finally, longer implementation of the programme is likely to translate into sustainable changes in school norms, for example, norms pertaining to the school ethos and environment. A 2019 umbrella review found that a highly demanding academic environment is a risk factor for poor adolescent mental health and well-being, whereas positive relations with teachers and peers, as well as perceptions of safety, belonging and connectedness, serve as protective factors. Reference Barry, Clarke, Petersen and Jenkins10 The influence of technology is also vital to consider, with a recent meta-analysis linking problematic smartphone use in youth to increased odds of stress, anxiety, depression and poor sleep and advocating that teachers help to limit exposure in school environments. Reference Sohn, Rees, Wildridge, Kalk and Carter36 Sustainable improvements to domains such as these can thus offer benefit to the mental health and well-being of old and new students alike. Nevertheless, although long-term investment in whole-school approaches can offer various advantages, a fundamental challenge lies in how to sustain resources. To overcome this, it is critical that stakeholders such as the government support schools in their ability to invest in whole-school initiatives in the long term, for example, through funding schemes and ensuring that schools are sufficiently staffed.

In placing the findings of our study in perspective, there are several limitations to consider. The first is the lack of randomisation inherent in the study design, which introduces the possibility of selection bias. However, the appropriateness of using randomised controlled trials (RCTs) to evaluate whole-school interventions is contentious. Some suggest that RCTs are incapable of capturing the complexity of systems-based approaches to public health issues, Reference Barry, Clarke, Petersen and Jenkins10 whereas others offer the counter-perspective that RCTs are an appropriate trial design and can accommodate the effects of local programme adaptation. Reference Langford, Bonell, Jones, Pouliou, Murphy and Waters8 A second limitation is the lack of pre-test results from the intervention and control group. The groups may differ from each other in systematic ways other than the intervention, and these differences may provide alternative explanations for the observed effect. For example, the control group was more heavily skewed towards female participants than the intervention group (59.53% v. 47.80%) and more heavily skewed towards students from metropolitan schools (89.98% v. 42.17%). Although one may presume that intervention schools were more well-resourced than control schools, and that the observed effect may instead be attributable to this, our findings indicate that the intervention schools in our sample were from less-well-resourced areas, including a higher proportion from lower socioeconomic, rural and remote settings. Nonetheless, we have attempted to counteract these two limitations by controlling for plausible confounders including grade, gender, socioeconomic and rurality status. A third limitation is the low Cronbach’s alpha observed for the CSI disengagement subscale in our sample, indicating that this finding should be interpreted with caution. Given that the reliability and validity of the scale have been demonstrated elsewhere, Reference Tobin, Holroyd, Reynolds and Wigal26,Reference Tobin, Holroyd and Reynolds27 the low internal consistency observed in our study may be explained by the use of a sample with different sociocultural characteristics, different contextual factors or adaptation of the original scale. A fourth limitation is that owing to the anonymous nature of the survey and collection of data at one time point (post-test), the collected responses only represent those students who were present and willing to complete the survey on the day. We were thus unable to explore the characteristics of non-responders and the potential impact of response bias, implementing complete-case analysis to analyse the data. Our data-set also did not specifically identify students with special needs or those whose first language was not English. However, the survey providers have confirmed that teachers provided support to students affected by these circumstances to assist them in completing the survey. A final limitation is that data on ‘non-standard’ participants, who had attended their index school for a period of time shorter than that for which the programme had been implemented, were not collected. However, we note that this would be likely to bias the results in a more conservative direction.

The strengths of our study include its large sample size and inclusion of intervention schools that had implemented the programme for a duration anywhere from 2 to 8 years. This enabled identification of an important relationship between implementation duration and programme effectiveness, which warrants further investigation through robust longitudinal studies. We suggest that possible explanatory factors raised by our conceptual model could also be explored, for example: (a) through collection of data on how long individual students have received the programme and assessment of the subsequent impact on programme effectiveness; (b) by conducting a process evaluation to assess whether schools with longer implementation duration demonstrate higher-quality implementation and (c) by measuring school-level factors to assess whether schools with longer implementation duration demonstrate sustainable changes in school norms. Given the theoretical promise of whole-school initiatives, future research is required to further our understanding of the role of implementation duration in the success of these initiatives.

In summary, adolescents receiving The Resilience Project School Partnership Program demonstrated consistently better mental health outcomes than the control group, provided that schools had invested in the programme in the long term. We recommend that future research continue to investigate the importance of implementation duration in the success of whole-school initiatives, in addition to factors that may mediate this effect. Our study provides insights for stakeholders such as public health and policy makers, in particular, that whole-school approaches to mental health and well-being in adolescent populations may require long-term investment for their potential to be realised.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2024.843.

Data availability

The data-sets analysed in the current study are not publicly available but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

We thank Professor Margaret Barry for her feedback on our manuscript.

Author contributions

Conceptualisation: all authors; methodology: R.B.L. and M.N.K.; formal analysis and investigation: R.B.L. and M.N.K.; writing – original draft preparation: R.B.L.; writing – review and editing: all authors; funding acquisition: the first author’s PhD programme is funded by the Monash University Graduate Research Scholarship and Westpac Future Leaders Scholarship; resources: all authors; supervision: M.N.K., L.C. and D.I.

Funding

R.B.L. is funded by an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship and the Westpac Future Leaders Scholarship for her PhD programme.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

McGorry, PD, Coghill, D, Berk, M. Mental health of young Australians: dealing with a public health crisis. Med J Aust 2023; 219: 246–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, M, Radua, J, Olivola, M, Croce, E, Soardo, L, Salazar de Pablo, G, et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry 2022; 27: 281–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). National Study of Mental Health and Wellbeing. ABS, 2023 (https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/health/mental-health/national-study-mental-health-and-wellbeing/2020-2022).Google Scholar
Hamoda, HM, Hoover, S, Bostic, J, Rahman, A, Saaed, K. Development of a World Health Organization mental health in schools programme in the Eastern Mediterranean Region. Eastern Mediterr Health J 2022; 28: 225–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeroy, KR, Bibeau, D, Steckler, A, Glanz, K. An ecological perspective on health promotion programs. Health Educ Q 1988; 15: 351–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samdal, O, Rowling, L. The Implementation of Health Promoting Schools: Exploring the Theories of What, Why and How. Routledge, 2012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO) and the United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO). Making Every School a Health-Promoting School: Global Standards and Indicators for Health-Promoting Schools and Systems. WHO and UNESCO, 2021 (https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240025059).Google Scholar
Langford, R, Bonell, C, Jones, H, Pouliou, T, Murphy, S, Waters, E, et al. The World Health Organization’s Health Promoting Schools framework: a Cochrane systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health 2015; 15: 130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, JM, Sklad, M, Elfrink, TR, Schreurs, KM, Bohlmeijer, ET, Clarke, AM. Effectiveness of interventions adopting a whole school approach to enhancing social and emotional development: a meta-analysis. Eur J Psychol Educ 2019; 34: 755–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barry, MM, Clarke, AM, Petersen, I, Jenkins, R. Implementing Mental Health Promotion 2nd ed. Springer Nature, 2019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lekamge, RB, Jain, R, Sheen, J, Solanki, P, Zhou, Y, Romero, L, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the effectiveness of whole-school interventions promoting mental health and preventing risk behaviours in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc [Epub ahead of print] 2025. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-025-02135-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weare, K, Nind, M. Mental health promotion and problem prevention in schools: what does the evidence say? Health Promot Int 2011; 26(suppl 1): i2969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durlak, JA, Weissberg, RP, Dymnicki, AB, Taylor, RD, Schellinger, KB. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev 2011; 82: 405–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wignall, A, Kelly, C, Grace, P. How are whole-school mental health programmes evaluated? A systematic literature review. Pastoral Care Educ 2022; 40: 217–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merzel, C, D’Afflitti, J. Reconsidering community-based health promotion: promise, performance, and potential. Am J Public Health 2003; 93: 557–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Education, State Government of Victoria. Schools Mental Health Menu Tiles. Mental Health in Schools. Department of Education, State Government of Victoria, 2023 (https://www.education.vic.gov.au/school/teachers/health/mentalhealth/mental-health-menu/Pages/Menu-Tiles.aspx?queryid=positive).Google Scholar
Harris, AD, McGregor, JC, Perencevich, EN, Furuno, JP, Zhu, J, Peterson, DE, et al. The use and interpretation of quasi-experimental studies in medical informatics. J Am Med Inf Assoc 2006; 13: 1623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resilient Youth Australia. Resilience Survey. Resilient Youth. Resilient Youth Australia, 2024 (https://resilientyouth.org/survey).Google Scholar
Cook, TD, Campbell, DT, Shadish, W. Experimental and Quasi-experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. Houghton Mifflin, 2002.Google Scholar
Hoare, E, Thorp, A, Bartholomeusz-Raymond, N, McCoy, A, Berk, M. Opportunities in the Australian national education initiative for promoting mental health in schools. Lancet Child Adolesc Health 2020; 4: 11–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, TC, Glasziou, PP, Boutron, I, Milne, R, Perera, R, Moher, D, et al. Better reporting of interventions: template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ 2014; 348: g1687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, KA, Currie, C. Reliability and validity of an adapted version of the Cantril Ladder for use with adolescent samples. Social Indic Res 2014; 119: 1047–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, CR, Hoza, B, Pelham, WE, Rapoff, M, Ware, L, Danovsky, M, et al. The development and validation of the Children’s Hope Scale. J Pediatr Psychol 1997; 22: 399421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valle, MF, Huebner, ES, Suldo, SM. Further evaluation of the Children’s Hope Scale. J Psychoeduc Assess 2004; 22: 320–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobin, DL, Holroyd, KA, Reynolds, RV, Wigal, JK. The hierarchical factor structure of the coping strategies inventory. Cognit Therapy Res 1989; 13: 343–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobin, DL, Holroyd, K, Reynolds, R. User’s Manual for the Coping Strategies Inventory. Ohio University, 1984.Google Scholar
Staples, LG, Dear, BF, Gandy, M, Fogliati, V, Fogliati, R, Karin, E, et al. Psychometric properties and clinical utility of brief measures of depression, anxiety, and general distress: the PHQ-2, GAD-2, and K-6. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2019; 56: 13–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plummer, F, Manea, L, Trepel, D, McMillan, D. Screening for anxiety disorders with the GAD-7 and GAD-2: a systematic review and diagnostic meta analysis. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2016; 39: 2431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, LP, Rockhill, C, Russo, JE, Grossman, DC, Richards, J, McCarty, C, et al. Evaluation of the PHQ-2 as a brief screen for detecting major depression among adolescents. Pediatrics 2010; 125: e1097103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). Remoteness Areas: Australian Statistical Geography Standard (ASGS) Edition 3. ABS, 2023 (https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/standards/australian-statistical-geography-standard-asgs-edition-3/jul2021-jun2026/remoteness-structure/remoteness-areas#interactive-map).Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). Schools: Data on Government and Non-government Students, Staff and Schools. ABS, 2024 (∼https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/people/education/schools/latest-release#:∼:text=In%20Australia%20in%202023%3A,13.1%20students%20to%20one%20teacher).Google Scholar
Kwasnicka, D, Dombrowski, SU, White, M, Sniehotta, F. Theoretical explanations for maintenance of behaviour change: a systematic review of behaviour theories. Health Psychol 2016; 10: 277–96.Google ScholarPubMed
Durlak, JA, DuPre, EP. Implementation matters: a review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. Am J Commun Psychol 2008; 41: 327–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohn, SY, Rees, P, Wildridge, B, Kalk, NJ, Carter, B. Prevalence of problematic smartphone usage and associated mental health outcomes amongst children and young people: a systematic review, meta-analysis and GRADE of the evidence. BMC Psychiatry 2019; 19: 356–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Global standards of the Health-Promoting Schools Framework (reproduced from Lekamge et al.11 under the Creative Commons CC BY license).

Figure 1

Table 1 Intraclass coefficients at the school-level for outcome measures

Figure 2

Table 2 Sociodemographic variables of the intervention and control groups

Figure 3

Table 3 Results from mixed-effects linear and logistic regression models, with intervention schools stratified by number of years implementing the programme (reference: control group); all results are adjusted for grade, gender, socioeconomic and rurality status

Figure 4

Fig. 2 Graphical representation of linear mixed-effects model results. The reference for the null effect is represented by a maroon dashed line.

Figure 5

Fig. 3 Graphical representation of logistic mixed-effects model results. The reference for the null effect is represented by a maroon dashed line.

Figure 6

Fig. 4 Conceptual model of why longer programme implementation may result in better effects.

Supplementary material: File

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 1

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material
Download Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 14.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 2

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material
Download Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 8.1 MB
Supplementary material: File

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 3

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material
Download Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 14.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 4

Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material
Download Balasooriya Lekamge et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 449.9 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.