Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T21:11:18.011Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early language experience in a Papuan community

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 September 2020

Marisa CASILLAS*
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
Penelope BROWN
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
Stephen C. LEVINSON
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: Marisa Casillas, P.O. Box 310, 6500AHNijmegen, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The rate at which young children are directly spoken to varies due to many factors, including (a) caregiver ideas about children as conversational partners and (b) the organization of everyday life. Prior work suggests cross-cultural variation in rates of child-directed speech is due to the former factor, but has been fraught with confounds in comparing postindustrial and subsistence farming communities. We investigate the daylong language environments of children (0;0–3;0) on Rossel Island, Papua New Guinea, a small-scale traditional community where prior ethnographic study demonstrated contingency-seeking child interaction styles. In fact, children were infrequently directly addressed and linguistic input rate was primarily affected by situational factors, though children's vocalization maturity showed no developmental delay. We compare the input characteristics between this community and a Tseltal Mayan one in which near-parallel methods produced comparable results, then briefly discuss the models and mechanisms for learning best supported by our findings.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In their first few years of life, children hear an extraordinary amount of language. The sum of this experience with language (their “input”) is the basis for their lexical, grammatical, and sociolinguistic development. Much developmental language research focuses on the value of child-directed speech (CDS) in particular as a tailored source of linguistic input that can boost lexical and syntactic development (Bates & Goodman, Reference Bates and Goodman1997; Brinchmann, Braeken & Lyster, Reference Brinchmann, Braeken and Lyster2019; Frank, Braginsky, Yurovsky & Marchman, Reference Frank, Braginsky, Yurovsky and Marchmanin press; Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Hoff, Reference Hoff2003; Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea & Hedges, Reference Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea and Hedges2010; Lieven, Pine & Baldwin, Reference Lieven, Pine and Baldwin1997; Marchman, Martínez-Sussmann & Dale, Reference Marchman, Martínez-Sussmann and Dale2004; Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012; Snow, Reference Snow, Snow and Ferguson1977; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013). However, we have also known for decades that children's language environments – e.g., who is around and talking about what to whom – vary dramatically within and across families, and that children in some communities hear very little directed talk without any apparent delays in their linguistic development (e.g., Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Brown & Gaskins, Reference Brown, Gaskins, Enfield, Kockelman and Sidnell2014; de León, Reference de León, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Gaskins, Reference Gaskins, Enfield and Levinson2006; Ochs, Reference Ochs1988; Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs, Schieffelin, Schweder and LeVine1984; Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez & Angelillo, Reference Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez and Angelillo2003; Schieffelin, Reference Schieffelin1990).

A key puzzle for developmental language science is then uncovering how the human cognitive toolkit for language learning can flexibly adapt to the variable circumstances under which it occurs, including circumstances in which CDS is infrequent, is produced in large part by other children, or is primarily restricted to a small number of activities (Brown, Reference Brown, Arnon, Casillas, Kurumada and Estigarribia2014; Casillas, Brown & Levinson, Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019; Gaskins, Reference Gaskins, Enfield and Levinson2006; Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs, Schieffelin, Schweder and LeVine1984). Resolving this puzzle requires researchers to find ways to track the distribution and characteristics of linguistic input over multiple interactional contexts, across developmental time, between families, and across different cultural groups. In what follows we explore two major factors that may impact children's linguistic environments: culturally held ideas about talking to children, and situational features of everyday life. We build a case for testing both sources of variation using clips sampled from recordings of whole waking days at home. We then use this approach to report on the language environments of children under 3;0 in one child-centric subsistence farming society (Yélî (Papuan), Rossel Island, Papua New Guinea), and compare the findings to a parallel set of results from another subsistence farming society that is, by contrast, not child-centric (Tseltal Mayan, Tenejapa, Mexico).

Ideological and situational variation in CDS

Caregivers’ personal and cultural notions about how children should develop as members of the broader language community influence the prevalence and style of their child-directed talk (Gaskins, Reference Gaskins, Enfield and Levinson2006; Harkness & Super, Reference Harkness and Super1996; Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs, Schieffelin, Schweder and LeVine1984; Rowe, Reference Rowe2008). For example, extensive ethnographic research among multiple, distinct Mayan communities of Southern Mexico and Guatemala has forged a consistent view of childrearing and child-directed speech: adult caregivers shape infants’ and young children's worlds in such a way that children learn to attend to what is going on around them rather than expecting to be the center of attention (e.g., Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011, Reference Brown, Arnon, Casillas, Kurumada and Estigarribia2014; de León, Reference de León, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Gaskins, Reference Gaskins2000; Pye, Reference Pye1986; Rogoff et al., Reference Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez and Angelillo2003). These ethnographic findings lay out a broader ideology of caregiving, including a number of component attitudes (e.g., infants as inadequate conversational partners), that lead to the prediction that, on average, typically developing Mayan children are only infrequently directly addressed during their days at home. Indeed, using data from daylong recordings of children under age 3;0, Casillas and colleagues (Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019) found that the Tseltal Mayan children in their sample heard an average of 3.6 minutes per hour of speech directed to them – around one third of the current estimate for North American English (Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Casillas, Soderstrom, Seidl, Warlaumont and Amatuni2019b) – yet they hit established benchmarks for the onset of single- and multi-word utterances (see also Cychosz et al., Reference Cychosz, Cristia, Bergelson, Casillas, Baudet, Warlaumont, Scaff, Yankowitz and Seidl2019). This finding appears to support the idea that attitudes about child-directed talk mediate how frequently children are addressed. However, any direct comparison between these two childrearing contexts is critically confounded: the arrangement of everyday life is highly different between the subsistence farming, rural Tseltal Mayan community and the (sub)urban, middle-class North American populations to which they are being compared.

Children's pattern of linguistic input also varies depending on the social organization of everyday life, which shapes the circumstances for their interactions with others over the course of the day. Prior analyses of daylong recordings in both North American and Tseltal Mayan contexts suggests that different activities impact the rate at which children hear child-directed speech from hour to hour (Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Amatuni, Dailey, Koorathota and Tor2019a; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019; Greenwood, Thiemann-Bourque, Walker, Buzhardt & Gilkerson, Reference Greenwood, Thiemann-Bourque, Walker, Buzhardt and Gilkerson2011; Soderstrom & Wittebolle, Reference Soderstrom and Wittebolle2013). The limited evidence to date shows approximately similar patterns in input rate fluctuation across the waking day: children in both North American and Tseltal Mayan contexts hear their highest rates of linguistic input in the morning and afternoon, with a dip around midday (Greenwood et al., Reference Greenwood, Thiemann-Bourque, Walker, Buzhardt and Gilkerson2011; Soderstrom & Wittebolle, Reference Soderstrom and Wittebolle2013). Intriguingly, the activities associated with dense adult talk in the North American context are highly rare in the Tseltal Mayan sample (e.g., sing-alongs) and the activities associated with the least dense periods in the North American data are associated with peak input periods in the Tseltal Mayan sample (e.g., mealtimes, Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019). In the Tseltal Mayan context specifically, the afternoon-dip pattern likely arises as a consequence of morning and later afternoon communal eating events with multiple adult and child speakers, separated by a longer, relatively quiet midday period of work or rest. The fluctuations in linguistic input Tseltal Mayan children hear over the day thus appear to be driven by the presence of multiple adult and child speakers whose home presence is regulated by the schedule and workload of farming, food preparation, rest, and other domestic activities.

The current study

Here we investigate the language environments of children growing up on Rossel Island, Papua New Guinea. While the Rossel lifestyle is broadly similar to that of the Tseltal Mayans, their orientation to verbal interaction with infants is more similar to that of middle-class North Americans: Rossel caregivers engage in intensive face-to-face verbal interactions with prelinguistic children, as described in more detail below (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted). Rossel families therefore offer a critical new datapoint in our understanding of cross-cultural variation in linguistic inputFootnote 1: If patterns of CDS on Rossel Island are similar to those reported for North American English, it would support the idea that caregiver ideology drives substantial differences in language input across variable contexts. If, instead, CDS patterns are more similar to that of the Tseltal Mayan community, it would support the idea that lifestyle drives substantial differences in language input across variable contexts; specifically, subsistence farming vs. post-industrial lifestyles.

We use manually annotated daylong recordings of Rossel children's language environments to track how much speech they hear from different speakers over the course of a day at home. During these recordings, the target child freely navigates their environment for multiple hours at a time while wearing an audio recorder, a simple method that can be similarly deployed across diverse linguistic and cultural settings (Casillas & Cristia, Reference Casillas and Cristia2019; Cychosz et al., Reference Cychosz, Cristia, Bergelson, Casillas, Baudet, Warlaumont, Scaff, Yankowitz and Seidl2019). We capture both situational variation and variation due to differences in caregiver responsiveness by sampling the daylong recordings in two different ways. First, we randomly sample clips to get a baseline estimate for how much speech children encounter, on average, over the course of the day. Because these clips are indiscriminately distributed over the whole recording, they include variation in input due to both specific activities (e.g., mealtime vs. work periods) and social-organizational effects (e.g., subsistence farming schedule, household composition). Second, we look specifically at patterns of interlocutor responsiveness by manually selecting the day's peak clips of sustained interaction between the target child and one or more co-interactants. By identifying clips in which children are hearably interacting with others, we aim to partly – albeit imperfectly – sample from home interactional contexts in which we know the target child is alert and socially engaged, similar to contexts in which cross-cultural differences in CDS have been shown in the past with these same two communities (e.g., Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted).

On the basis of past comparative work, we predicted that Rossel children would hear frequent CDS from a wide variety of caregiver types throughout the day, which would support the idea that ideologies about child-directed talk drive substantial cross-context variation in language input rate. Prior ethnographic findings also led us to predict that: (a) distributed caregiving practices on Rossel Island would weaken hour-to-hour fluctuations in CDS rate attributed previously to a subsistence farming schedule (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019), (b) children would hear an increasing proportion of CDS from other children as they got older, and (c) other-directed speech (ODS) would be abundant. We also predicted that any ideology-derived differences between the Tseltal and Rossel data would be most apparent during the clips targeting interactant responsiveness, which better approximate the contexts in which past differences between these communities have been found (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011, Reference Brown, Arnon, Casillas, Kurumada and Estigarribia2014; Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted). Consonant with prior daylong child language data across multiple cultural contexts, we also expected little-to-no increase in CDS rate with age, a decrease in ODS rate with age, and for CDS to occur in non-uniform bursts throughout the day (Abney, Smith & Yu, Reference Abney, Smith, Yu, Gunzelmann, Howes, Tenbrink and Davelaar2017; Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Casillas, Soderstrom, Seidl, Warlaumont and Amatuni2019b; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019).

In what follows we review the ethnographic work done in this community previously, describe our methods for following up on that work with daylong recordings, present the current findings, and discuss the similarities and differences that arose. All methods for annotation and analysis in this study closely follow those reported elsewhere for Tseltal children's speech environments (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019).

Method

Corpus

The participants in this study live in a collection of small hamlets on north-eastern Rossel Island, approximately 250 nautical miles off the southern tip of mainland Papua New Guinea with only intermittent access to and contact with the outside world. The traditional language of Rossel Island is Yélî Dnye, an isolate (Papuan), which features a phonological inventory and set of grammatical features unlike any other in the (predominantly Austronesian) languages of the region. The islanders are swidden horticulturalists, cultivating taro, sweet potato, manioc, yam, coconut, and more for their daily subsistence, with protein coming from fishing and (occasionally) slaughtering pigs or wild animals such as possums, goannas, snakes. Children often forage independently for shellfish and wild nuts, extra sources of protein. Most children on Rossel Island grow up speaking Yélî Dnye at home, though English, Tok Pisin, and a number of languages from the nearby islands and mainland are frequently heard from adults and school-aged children. Formal training in English as a second language begins in school, around age 7. Children grow up in patrilocal household clusters (i.e., their family and their father's brothers’ families), usually arranged such that there is some shared open space between households.

During their waking hours, infants are typically carried in a caregiver's arms as they go about daily activities. Infants, even very young ones, are frequently passed between different people (male and female, young and elderly) throughout the day, returning to the mother to suckle when hungry. This baby-lending practice is not restricted to the natal family, or even close relatives; between feedings, one may find an infant several villages away from its mother, with older infants and young children being transferred between distant caregivers for even longer periods (sometimes for several weeks or longer). The arc of a typical day for an infant might include waking, being dressed and fed, then a mix of (a) spending time with nearby adults or older children as they walk around socializing and completing tasks with others and (b) more feeding, perhaps followed by short bouts of sleep in the late morning and afternoon, usually with the mother. Sometimes children are also taken along for gardening after the morning meal. Afternoon meals are cooked from around 15:00 onward, with another feed and more socializing before resting for the night. Starting around age two or three, children spend much of their time in large, independent child playgroups (10+ cousins and neighbors) who freely travel near and around the village searching for nuts and fruits, bathing in nearby rivers, and engaging in group games (e.g., tag, pretend play, etc.).

Interaction with infants and young children on Rossel Island is initiated by women, men, girls, and boys alike in a face-to-face, contingency-seeking, and affect-laden style (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted). Children are considered a shared responsibility, but also a source of joy and entertainment for the wider network of caregivers in their community. In her prior ethnographic work, Brown details some ways in which interactants make bids for joint attention and act as if the infant can understand what is being said (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011). Infants pick up on this pattern of caregiving, initiating interactions with others twice as frequently as Tseltal children, who are encouraged instead to observe the interactions going on around them (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011). Brown and Casillas (Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted) document how Rossel caregivers encourage early independence in their children, observing their autonomy in choosing what to do, wear, eat, and say while finding other ways to promote pro-social behavior (e.g., praise). Overall, Rossel Island could be characterized as a child-centered language environment (Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs, Schieffelin, Schweder and LeVine1984; but see Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted), in which children, even very young ones, are considered interactional and conversational partners whose interests are often allowed to shape the topic and direction of conversation.

The data presented here come from the Rossel subset of the Casillas HomeBank Corpus (Casillas, Brown & Levinson, Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2017), a collection of raw daylong recordings and supplementary data from over 100 children age four and younger growing up on Rossel Island and in the Tseltal Mayan community described elsewhere (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019). The Rossel subcorpus was collected in 2016 and includes daylong audio recordings and experimental data from 57 children born to 43 mothers. These children had 0–2 younger siblings (mean = 0.36; median = 0) and 0–5 older siblings (mean = 2; median = 2); most participating caregivers were on the younger end of those in the community, though two children's primary caregivers were their biological grandparents (mean = 33.9 years; median = 32; range = 24–70 and fathers: mean = 35.6; median = 34; range = 24–57). Based on available demographic data for 40 of the biological mothers we estimate that mothers are typically 21.4 years old when they give birth to their first child (median = 21.5; range = 12–30). On the basis of demographic data for 34 of those mothers, we estimate an average inter-child interval of 2.8 years (median = 2.6; range = 1.75–5.2).

The size of households, defined here as the number of people sharing kitchen and sleeping areas on a daily basis, ranged between 3 and 12 (mean = 7; median = 7). Households are clustered into small patrilocal hamlets which afford a wider group of communal caregivers and playmates. The hamlets themselves are clustered together into patches of more distantly related patrilocal residents. The average hamlet in our corpus comprises 5.8 households (median = 5; range = 3–11); the typical household in our dataset has 2 children under age seven (i.e., not yet attending school) and 2 adults, leading us to estimate that there are around 10 young children and 10 adults present within a hamlet throughout the day. This estimate does not include visitors to the target child's hamlet or relatives that the target child encounters while visiting others. Therefore, while 24.6% of the target children in our corpus are first born to their mothers, these children are incorporated into a larger pool of young children whose care is divided among numerous caregivers.

Among our participating families, most mothers had finished their education at one of the island's schools (6 years of education = 32.6%; 8 years of education = 37.2%)Footnote 2, with about a quarter having attended secondary school off the island (10 years of education = 25.6%; 12 years of education = 2%). Only one mother had less than six years of education. Similarly, most fathers had finished their education at one of the island's schools (6 years of education = 44.2%; 8 years of education = 20.9%) or at an off-island secondary school (10 years of education = 27.9%), with only 7% having less than six years of education. Note that in Table 1 we use a different set of educational levels than is used on the island so that we can more easily compare the present sample to the Tseltal sample used in Casillas et al. (Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019; see Table 1 caption for details). As far as we could ascertain at the time of recording, all but two children were typically developing; one showed signs of significant language delay and one showed signs of multiple developmental delay (motor, language, intellectual). Both of these children's delays were consistently observed in follow-up trips in 2018 and 2019. Their recordings are not included in the analyses reported below.

Table 1. Demographic overview of the 10 children whose recordings are sampled in the current study, including from left to right: child's age (years;months.days); child's sex (M/F); mother's age (years); highest level of maternal education achieved (primary (grades 6–7)/secondary (grades 8–11)/preparatory (grade 12)); and the number of people living in the child's household.

Dates of birth for children were initially collected via caregiver report. We were able to verify the majority of birth dates using the records at the island health clinic. Because not all mothers give birth at the clinic and because dates are logged by hand, some births are not recorded, are inaccurately recorded, or otherwise significantly diverge from what the caregivers report. In these cases we gathered information from as many sources as possible and followed up with the families, often using the dates of neighboring children born around the same time to determine the correct date.

The data we present come from 7–9-hour recordings made at home during daylight hours (6:00–18:00; there is little or no powered light after dark). Children wore the recording device: an elastic vest containing a small stereo audio recorder (Olympus WS-832 or WS-853) and a miniature camera that captured photos from the child's frontal viewpoint at a fixed interval (every 15 seconds; Narrative Clip 1). The camera was outfitted with a fisheye lens (Photojojo Super Fisheye) that allowed us to capture 180 degrees of the child's frontal view. This photo technique increases the ease and reliability of transcription and annotation by giving scene information that aids activity and interlocutor identification. However, because the camera and recorder are separate devices, we had to synchronize them manually. We used an external wristwatch to record the current time at start of recording on each device individually, with accuracy down to the second (photographed by the camera and spoken into the recorder). The camera's software timestamps each image file such that we can calculate the number of seconds that have elapsed between photos. These timestamps were used with the cross-device time synchronization cue to create photo-linked audio files of each recording, which we then formatted as video files (see https://github.com/marisacasillas/Weave for scripts). The informed consent process used with participants, as well as data collection and storage, were conducted in accordance with ethical guidelines approved by the Radboud University Social Sciences Ethics Committee.

Data selection and annotation

From the daylong recordings of 57 Rossel children, we selected 10 representative children between ages 0;0 and 3;0 for transcription and analysis. The 10 children were selected to be spread between the target age range (0;0–3;0) while also representing a range of typical maternal education levels found in the community and being evenly split between male and female children (Table 1). We selected a series of non-overlapping sub-clips from each recording for transcription (Figure 1) in the following order: nine randomly-selected 2.5-minute clips, five manually-selected ‘peak’ turn-taking activity 1-minute clips, five manually-selected ‘peak’ target child vocal activity 1-minute clips, and one manually-selected 5-minute expansion of the highest-activity one-minute clip, for a total of 37.5 minutes of transcribed audio for each child (6.25 audio hours in total).

Figure 1. Recording duration (black line) and sampled clips (colored boxes) for each of the 10 recordings analyzed, sorted by child age in months.

Manual clip selection proceeded as follows: one person (the first author or a Western research assistant) listened through the entirety of each recording, documenting the approximate onset time, duration, and notable features of any short period that they perceived to be a burst of turn taking and/or target-child vocalization; judgments were made subjectively, and with reference to the lack of such activity in other parts of the recording. After compiling a list of candidate bursts for each recording, the first author listened again to each candidate, adding further notes about the diversity of target-child vocalizations and the density of turn taking. Clips that overlapped with previously transcribed segments or that featured significant background noise were eliminated. From the remainder, the five 1-minute clips that best demonstrated sequences of temporally contingent vocalization between the target child and at least one other person were selected as the ‘turn-taking’ clips. From the remaining candidate clips, the five that best demonstrated high density, high maturity, and high diversity vocalizations by the target child were selected as the ‘vocal activity’ clips. After these ten 1-minute clips had been transcribed for each recording (i.e., during the field visit), the first author assessed each for its density of vocal and turn-taking activity and searched for continuation of that activity before and after the one-minute clip. The clip that best balanced dense, minimally repetitious verbal activity with continuation in neighboring minutes was selected to have a 5-minute extension window for further annotation. Finally, all else being equal, we gave preference to clips featuring speech from underrepresented foreground speakers (e.g., adult males; see more details at https://git.io/fhdUm).

We were limited to annotating these sub-clips from only 10 children because of the time-intensive nature of transcribing these naturalistic data; 1 minute of audio typically took approximately 60–70 minutes to be segmented into utterances, transcribed, annotated, and loosely translated into English (~400 hours total). Yélî Dnye is almost exclusively spoken on Rossel Island, where there is no electricity (we use solar panels) and unreliable access to mobile data, so transcription was completed over the course of three 4–6 week visits to the island in 2016, 2018, and 2019.

We used the ACLEW Annotation Scheme (Casillas, Bunce, et al., Reference Casillas, Bunce, Soderstrom, Rosemberg, Migdalek, Alam and Garrison2017) in ELAN (Wittenburg, Brugman, Russel, Klassmann & Sloetjes, Reference Wittenburg, Brugman, Russel, Klassmann and Sloetjes2006) to transcribe and annotate all hearable speech in the clips. Using both the audio and photo context, we segmented out the utterances and ascribed them to individual speakers (e.g., older brother, mother, aunt, etc.). We then annotated the vocal maturity of each utterance produced by the target child (non-canonical babble/canonical babble/single word/multi-word/unsure) and annotated the addressee of all speech from other speakers (addressed to the target child/one or more other children/one or more adults/a mix of adults and children/any animal/other/unsure).

Regarding vocal maturity annotations, a vocalization was considered a ‘single word’ if it contained a single recognizable (transcribed) word or a repetition of the same word (e.g., ‘mine’, ‘mine mine’). It was considered a ‘multi-word’ vocalization if it contained at least two different words (e.g., ‘my mango’), with non-lexical linguistic vocalizations annotated as ‘canonical babble’ (containing at least one consonant with an adult-like transition with its neighboring vocalic sound(s)) or ‘non-canonical babble’, and non-linguistic vocalizations classified as ‘crying’ or ‘laughing’. Vocalizations that were too ambiguous to make a decision were marked as ‘unsure’. Vegetative sounds (e.g., burps, sneezes) were ignored.

The audio and photo context were reviewed to identify, for each utterance, to whom the speaker was talking (i.e., the addressee for each utterance); utterances were only considered directed to the target child when the native Rossel-speaking research assistant and first author felt certain of this judgment given the context. Utterances were otherwise classified as directed to a ‘child’ (1+ children; a group that may include the target child so long as another child is also being addressed), ‘adult’ (1+ adults), ‘both’ (1+ children and 1+ adults; a group that may include the target child), ‘animal’ (1+ animals), ‘other’ (a clear addressee that doesn't fit into the other categories), or ‘unsure’ (not enough evidence to make a judgment about addressee).

Note that all transcription and annotation was done together by the first author and one of three community members (all native Yélî Dnye speakers). The community-based research assistants personally knew all the families in the recordings, and were able to use their own experience, the discourse context, and information from the accompanying photos in reporting what was said and to whom speech was addressed for each utterance. These annotations relied on mutual agreement between the first author and the Rossel research assistant, so there is no direct way to estimate interrater reliability for the 4308 target-child vocalizations and 10133 other-speaker vocalizations discovered in the clips. That said, independent vocal maturity annotations of these same target child vocalizations in a different study revealed a highly similar pattern of results (Cychosz et al., Reference Cychosz, Cristia, Bergelson, Casillas, Baudet, Warlaumont, Scaff, Yankowitz and Seidl2019). Detailed manuals and self-guided training materials, including a ‘gold standard test’ for this annotation scheme can be found at https://osf.io/b2jep/wiki/home/ (Casillas, Bunce, et al., Reference Casillas, Bunce, Soderstrom, Rosemberg, Migdalek, Alam and Garrison2017).

In what follows we first analyze the nine randomly selected 2.5-minute clips from each child to establish a baseline view of their speech environment, focusing on the effects of child age, time of day, household size, and number of speakers on the rate of target child-directed speech (TCDS) and other-directed speech (ODS). Next, we repeat these analyses, focusing instead only on the turn-taking clips to gain a view of the speech environment as it appears during the peak interactions for the day. Then as a first approximation of children's linguistic development, we map a coarse trajectory of children's use of babble, first words, and multi-word utterances. Lastly, we compare our findings to those from the Tseltal Mayan community, and briefly relate our results to the larger literature on child-directed speech and its role in language development.

Statistical models

We conducted all analyses in R, using the glmmTMB package to run generalized linear mixed-effects regressions (Brooks, Kristensen, van Benthem, Magnusson, Berg, Nielsen, Skaug, Mächler & Bolker, Reference Brooks, Kristensen, van Benthem, Magnusson, Berg, Nielsen, Skaug, Mächler and Bolker2017; R Core Team, 2019) and ggplot2 to generate figures (Wickham, Reference Wickham2016). This dataset and analysis are available at https://github.com/marisacasillas/Yeli-CLE. TCDS and ODS minutes per hour are naturally restricted to non-negative (0–infinity) values, causing the distributional variance of those measures to become positively skewed. To address this issue we use negative binomial regressions, which can better fit non-negative, overdispersed data (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Kristensen, van Benthem, Magnusson, Berg, Nielsen, Skaug, Mächler and Bolker2017; Smithson & Merkle, Reference Smithson and Merkle2013). There were also many cases of zero minutes of TCDS across the clips – for example, this occurred in the randomly sampled clips when the child was sleeping in a quiet area. To handle this additional distributional characteristic of the data, we added a zero-inflation component to TCDS analysis which, in addition to the count model of TCDS (e.g., testing effects of age on the input rate), creates a binary model to evaluate the likelihood of clips with no TCDS being used at all. More conventional, gaussian linear mixed-effects regressions with log-transformed dependent variables are provided in the Online Supplemental Materials (Supplementary Materials), but are qualitatively similar to what we report here.

Results

The models included the following predictors: child age (months; centered and standardized), household size (number of people; centered and standardized), number of non-target-child speakers present in that clip (centered and standardized), and time of day at the start of the clip (factor: “morning” = before 11:00; “midday” = 11:00–13:00; “afternoon” = after 13:00). We also included two-way interactions of (a) child age and the number of speakers present and (b) child age and time of day, with a random effect of child. For the zero-inflation model of TCDS, we included the number of speakers present. We limit our discussion to significant effects; full model results are provided in the Online Supplemental Materials (Supplementary Materials).

Target-child-directed speech (TCDS)

In the random sample, these 10 children heard an average of 3.13 minutes of speech directly addressed to them per hour (median = 2.95; range = 1.58–6.26; Figure 2 left panel, purple/solid summaries). For comparison, this is slightly less than reported values using a near-identical method of data collection, annotation, and analysis in a Tseltal Mayan community (3.6 minutes per hour for children under 3;0; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019) and comparable to what has been reported using a similar method in a Tsimane’ community in the Bolivian lowlands (3.1–7.8 minutes per hour for children under 3;0 depending on what speech is counted; Scaff, personal communication).

Figure 2. TCDS min/hr (left) and ODS min/hr (right) across the sampled age range. Each box plot summarizes the data for one child from the randomly sampled clips (purple; solid) or the turn taking clips (green; dashed). Bands on the linear trends show 95% confidence intervals.

The zero-inflated negative binomial regression of TCDS minutes per hour (N = 90, log-likelihood = -195.26, overdispersion estimate = 3.37) suggested significant effects of child age, time of day, and their interaction on the rate at which children are directly addressed. First, the older children heard a small but significantly greater amount of TCDS per hour (Figure 2 left panel purple/solid summaries; B = 0.73, SD = 0.23, z = 3.20, p < 0.01). Secondly, overall, all children were also more likely to hear TCDS in the mornings (Figure 3 top left panel), with significantly higher TCDS rates in the morning compared to both midday (midday-vs-morning: B = 0.80, SD = 0.36, z = 2.23, p = 0.03) and the afternoon (afternoon-vs-morning: B = 0.54, SD = 0.26, z = 2.10, p = 0.04), and no significant difference in TCDS rate between midday and the afternoon. However, the time-of-day pattern changed with child age. Older children were more likely than younger children to show a peak in TCDS during midday, with a decrease in TCDS between midday and the afternoon (midday-vs-afternoon: B = -0.60, SD = 0.29, z = -2.04, p = 0.04) and marginally less TCDS in the morning than at midday (midday-vs-morning: B = -0.59, SD = 0.30, z = -1.94, p = 0.05). There were no further significant effects in either the count or the zero-inflation models.

Figure 3. TCDS min/hr (left panels) and ODS min/hr (right panels) across the recorded day in the random clips (top panels) and turn-taking (bottom panels) clips. Each box plot summarizes the data for children age 1;0 and younger (light) or age 1;0 and older (dark) at the given time of day.

Children heard TCDS from a variety of different speakers. Most TCDS came from adults (mean = 72.65%, median = 75.51%, range = 41.41–100%). On average, 82.35% of the total TCDS minutes from adults came from women. However, older target children were more likely to hear TCDS from other child speakers than younger target children (e.g., TCDS from siblings, cousins, or neighbors; Child-TCDS); a Spearman's correlation showed a significant positive relationship between the average proportion of Child-TCDS in a clip and target child age (Spearman's rho = 0.78; p = 0.01).

Other-directed speech (ODS)

In the random sample, these children heard an average of 35.90 minutes of other-directed speech per hour (Figure 2 right panel, purple/solid summaries; median = 32.37; range = 20.20–53.78): that is more than eleven times the average quantity of speech directed to them, with many clips displaying near-continuous background speech. For comparison, the prior estimate for Tseltal Mayan children using near-parallel methods found an average of 21 minutes of overhearable speech per hour (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019), and a recent study of North American children's daylong recordings found that adult-directed speech (a subset of ODS) occurred at a rate of 7.3 minutes per hour (Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Casillas, Soderstrom, Seidl, Warlaumont and Amatuni2019b).

The negative binomial regression of other-directed speech rate (N = 90, log-likelihood = -370.87, overdispersion estimate = 9.14) revealed effects of child age, number of speakers present, and time of day on the rate of ODS encountered. The rate of ODS significantly decreased with child age (Figure 2 right panel, purple/solid summaries; B = −0.57, SD = 0.17, z = −3.28, p < 0.01) and significantly increased in the presence of more speakers (B = 0.50, SD = 0.05, z = 10.07, p < 0.001). Across the randomly selected clips, there were an average of 6.19 speakers present other than the target child (median = 6; range = 1–19), an average of 59.99% of whom were adults. Comparing again to Tseltal Mayan and to North American English daylong recording findings, in which the average number of speakers present, not including the target child, was 3.9 and 3.44 respectively (Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Amatuni, Dailey, Koorathota and Tor2019a; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019), we can infer that the increased rate of ODS on Rossel Island is due in part to there simply being more speakers present. Time-of-day effects on ODS only came through in an interaction with child age (Figure 3 top right panel). In particular, older children heard a pattern of ODS mirroring the general pattern of TCDS; significantly more ODS in the mornings compared to midday (midday-vs-morning: B = 0.65, SD = 0.20, z = 3.23, p < 0.01) and the afternoon (afternoon-vs-morning: B = 0.37, SD = 0.15, z = 2.50, p = 0.01). There were no other significant effects on ODS rate.

In sum, the random baseline rates of TCDS and ODS in children's speech environments are influenced by child age (TCDS increases, ODS decreases), by time of day (both generally peak in the morning), and by their interaction (older children hear more TCDS and less ODS than younger children at midday). The rate of ODS is also impacted by the number of speakers present. Correlational results suggest that TCDS comes increasingly from other children over the first three years. That said, the baseline rate of TCDS is low, on par with estimates in other small-scale rural communities (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019), while the ODS rate is quite high relative to estimates in prior work.

TCDS and ODS during interactional peaks

If we instead investigate the rates of TCDS and ODS encountered by these children during interactional peaks, a different picture emerges (Figures 2 and 3 green/dashed summaries). Unsurprisingly, the children heard much more TCDS in the turn-taking clips – 14.45 min/hr; more than four times the rate of TCDS in the random baseline (Figure 2, left panel, green/dashed summaries; median = 15.07; range = 9.61–18.73). Children also heard a reduced rate of ODS: 25.27 min/hr (70.39% of the random-sample ODS rate, Figure 2, right panel, green/dashed summaries; median = 19.59; range = 6.68–60.18). The next question was whether the pattern of TCDS and ODS use across age, time of day, and number of speakers in these turn-taking clips differed from what was seen in similarly sampled clips from the Tseltal Mayan community (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019). To investigate the effects of these variables we ran parallel regressions to what was used with the random clips above.

The negative binomial mixed-effects regression of TCDS (N = 55, log-likelihood = −183.25, overdispersion estimate = 2.91) revealed a significant decrease with child age (B = −0.63, SD = 0.27, z = −2.33, p = 0.02) and a significant interaction between child age and time of day; TCDS rate during interactional peaks was marginally higher for older children at morning compared to midday (midday-vs-morning: B = 0.53, SD = 0.28, z = 1.89, p = 0.06) and significantly higher in the afternoon than at midday (midday-vs-afternoon: B = 0.61, SD = 0.28, z = 2.17, p = 0.03; see Figure 3, bottom left panel).

During interactional peaks, as in the random sample, older target children heard more Child-TCDS than younger target children. While, overall, more of the TCDS in interactional peaks came from adults than in the random clips (mean = 82.68%, median = 88.04%, range = 50–100%), a Spearman's correlation showed an even stronger positive relationship between the average proportion of Child-TCDS in a clip and target child age (Spearman's rho = 0.92; p = < 0.001). Notably, women contributed proportionally less TCDS during interactional peaks than they did during the random clips: on average, women contributed 61.55% of the children's TCDS minutes from adults in the turn-taking clips (compared to 82.35% in the random clips). In brief, compared to the random sample, interactional peaks included more directed speech from men and, for older target children, more directed speech from other children.

The negative binomial mixed-effects regression of ODS (N = 55, log-likelihood = -202.60, overdispersion estimate = 4.66) only revealed a significant effect of number of speakers. As before, ODS rates were higher when more speakers were present (B = 0.56, SD = 0.08, z = 6.76, p < 0.001). There were no other significant effects on ODS rate (Figure 3, bottom right panel).

Overall, the results suggest that these children typically hear very little directly addressed speech, but that interactional peaks provide opportunities for dense input. While the majority of directed speech comes from women, an increasing portion of it comes from other children with age, and directed speech from men is more likely during interactional peaks. Directed and overhearable speech are most likely to occur during the morning, before most of the household has dispersed for their work activities, similar to other findings from subsistence farming households (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019). However, older children are more likely than younger children to experience higher input rates at midday, perhaps due to their increased interactions with other children while adults attend to gardening and domestic tasks. Possibly because of the large number of speakers present, these children were also in the vicinity of a great deal of overhearable speech, underscoring the availability of other-addressed speech as a resource for linguistic input in this context.

Vocal maturity

Given the low baseline rate of directed speech, one might expect that Rossel children's early linguistic development, particularly the onset and use of single- and multi-word utterances, shows delays in comparison to children growing up in more CDS-rich environments. We plotted the proportion of all linguistic vocalizations for each child (i.e., discarding laughter, crying, or unknown-types; leaving a total of 4308 vocalizations) that fell into the following categories: non-canonical babble, canonical babble, single-word utterance, or multi-word utterance. Children are expected to traverse all four types of vocalization during development such that they primarily produce single- and multi-word utterances by age three.

In the onset of use for canonical babble, first words, and multi-word utterances, these Rossel children's vocalization data closely resemble expectations based on populations of children who hear more CDS (Figure 4). Canonical babble appears in the second half of the first year, peaking before first words appear, around the first birthday, and multi-word utterances appear a few months after that (Frank et al., in press; Kuhl, Reference Kuhl2004; Pine & Lieven, Reference Pine and Lieven1993; Slobin, Reference Slobin, Flores d'Arcais and Levelt1970; Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello, Brooks and Barrett1999; Warlaumont, Richards, Gilkerson & Oller, Reference Warlaumont, Richards, Gilkerson and Oller2014). Rossel children also far exceeded the minimal canonical babbling ratio (CBR) associated with major developmental delay (proportional use of speech-like vocalizations > 0.15 by 0;10; Cychosz et al., Reference Cychosz, Cristia, Bergelson, Casillas, Baudet, Warlaumont, Scaff, Yankowitz and Seidl2019; Oller, Eilers, Basinger, Steffens & Urbano, Reference Oller, Eilers, Basinger, Steffens and Urbano1995); the minimum CBR among Rossel children 0;9 and older was 0.22 (mean = 0.63; median = 0.68; range = 0.22–0.86).

Figure 4. Proportion of vocalization types used by children across age (NCB = Non-canonical babble, CB = Canonical babble, SW = single word utterance, MW = multi-word utterance).

Over all annotated clips, children produced an average of 7.18 linguistic vocalizations per minute (median = 7.79; range = 4.57–8.95), which is a vocalization rate lower than recorded for short recordings of US infant-caregiver interaction (Oller et al., Reference Oller, Eilers, Basinger, Steffens and Urbano1995) but similar to estimates for Tseltal Mayan children (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019).

Discussion

We analyzed the speech environments of 10 Rossel children under age 3;0 to investigate: (a) how often children were spoken to directly, (b) how much other overhearable speech is available to them, and (c) how these sources of linguistic input are shaped by child age and interactional context. We then additionally conducted a preliminary investigation into (d) whether this (relatively) low rate of directed input appears to impact their early production milestones.

By investigating the language environments of children in this child-centric subsistence farming context, we aimed to provide a new and critical comparative datapoint to a research area that has previously confounded differences in child-directed speech ideology with differences in broad lifestyle features (post-industrial/nuclear vs. subsistence-farming/multi-generational, Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019; Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012). Our idea was that, if Rossel children's language environments pattern like North American ones, it would support that idea that caregiver ideology drives substantial differences in language input, whereas if they patterned like Tseltal Mayan environments, it would instead support the idea that lifestyle drives substantial differences. Overall, our findings point toward broad effects of lifestyle on the quantity of directed and overheard speech children hear. Evidence for the influence of CDS ideologies only begins to emerge when we look at patterns in who speaks to the target child, not in overall rates of linguistic input.

Input rate similarities across subsistence farming communities

Based on prior ethnographic work, we hypothesized that Rossel children would hear frequent child-directed speech (Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted). In fact, Rossel children were rarely directly addressed over the course of the day. We found a baseline rate of TCDS comparable to that found in a Tseltal Mayan community where infrequent use of TCDS is one means of socializing children into attending to their surroundings (Rossel: 3.13 TCDS min/hr vs. Tseltal: 3.63). As in the case of Tseltal Mayan children, this relatively low rate of TCDS was not associated with any delay in the appearance of vocal maturity milestones, including the use of single- and multi-word utterances. Since we know from prior, in-depth ethnographic work that caregivers’ ideas about talking to young children do, in fact, differ enormously in these two communities (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011, Reference Brown, Arnon, Casillas, Kurumada and Estigarribia2014; Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted), we attribute the similarity in baseline rates of TCDS to the fact that all these children are growing up in multi-generational, subsistence farming households. This inference is bolstered by the fact that fluctuations in TCDS rate over the day in the Rossel data are highly similar to those reported for Tseltal – peak rates in the morning, with older children eliciting more TCDS during midday hours than younger children (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019), and with ODS rate following a similar contour. While a basic afternoon-dip pattern has been shown in at least some North American home recordings (Greenwood et al., Reference Greenwood, Thiemann-Bourque, Walker, Buzhardt and Gilkerson2011; Soderstrom & Wittebolle, Reference Soderstrom and Wittebolle2013), the activities and total number of speakers present during periods of peak linguistic input are likely to be different across these economic contexts; an important avenue for future research. In line with prior work linking high caregiver workload to less CDS, our prediction is that the Tseltal and Rossel fluctuations derive from some of the (broadly) similar tasks associated with their subsistence farming lifestyles (see also findings from Kaluli, Samoan, Gusii, and Yucatec communities in, e.g., LeVine et al., Reference LeVine, Dixon, LeVine, Richman, Leiderman, Keefer and Brazelton1996; Ochs, Reference Ochs1988; Schieffelin, Reference Schieffelin1990; Gaskins, Reference Gaskins, Enfield and Levinson2006).

We had hypothesized that cultural differences in quantity of caregiver talk to children would be most visible in the turn-taking clips, which were selected in particular for their view into caregiver responsiveness patterns. Against expectations, we found a similar overall rate of TCDS in the Rossel turn-taking clips compared to that of the Tseltal Mayan children (Rossel: 14.45 TCDS min/hr vs. Tseltal: 13.28). In both cultural contexts, peak TCDS clips displayed around four times the rate of directed speech as the baseline rate, though we note that this relative increase was greater in the case of the Rossel data than the Tseltal data (Rossel: 4.62x the random rate vs. Tseltal: 3.66x).

Input source differences across subsistence farming communities

One distinctive feature of the Rossel data that was not observed for Tseltal is the division of TCDS among women, men, and other children. On Rossel Island, all of these types of speakers attend to the care of young children (Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted). In line with these observations, we find that Rossel children hear more CDS from other children than Tseltal children do (Rossel: 27% of TCDS vs. Tseltal 20%), and that the proportion of TCDS from other children increases with age, a pattern not found for Tseltal children in this age range (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019). Additionally, TCDS from men was far more frequent in the Rossel data, making up nearly 20% of adult TCDS in the random baseline and nearly 40% of adult TCDS in the turn-taking clips.Footnote 3 We take this substantial proportion of TCDS from children and men as evidence that caregiving is indeed divided among many types of speakers in Rossel communities (Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted); note that, together, child and adult male speakers contribute more than half of the TCDS during interactional peaks (see also Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012). In brief, we only get a glimpse into the different caregiving arrangements between the Tseltal and Rossel cultural contexts with respect to who is talking to the target child, and not with respect to how often the child is being talked to.

The age-related increase in TCDS from other children recalls findings from Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow (Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012; see also Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011 and Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted) in which Yucatec Mayan children's directed speech rate increased enormously between ages one and three – much more than the increase observed in these Rossel children's recordings – primarily due to increased input from other children (see also Scaff et al., Reference Scaff, Stieglitz, Casillas and Cristiain preparation). Interestingly, data from the Tseltal community, which is from the same Mayan cultural milieu as the Yucatec families studied in Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow (Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012), show no evidence for increased input from other children in this same age range (0;0–3;0; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019), possibly because Tseltal children only begin to more fully engage in independent, extended play with other children after age three. In contrast, independence is a primary concern for caregivers of young children on Rossel Island; from early toddlerhood Rossel children are encouraged to choose how they dress, when and what to eat, and whom to visit (Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted). The formation of hamlets in a cluster around a shared open area, often close to a shallow swimming area, further nurtures a sense of safe, free space in which children can wander. These features of childhood on Rossel Island support extended independent play with other children from an early age and may help explain the strongly increasing presence of child TCDS in the present data. Further work combining the time-of-day and interactant effects found here with ethnographic interview data are needed to explore these ideas in full.

Replicating daylong language environment patterns

Prior work using daylong audio recordings in both Western and non-Western contexts led us to expect that the quantity of TCDS would be relatively stable across the age range studied, that ODS rate would decrease with age, and that TCDS would be non-uniformly distributed over the recording day (Abney et al., Reference Abney, Smith, Yu, Gunzelmann, Howes, Tenbrink and Davelaar2017; Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Casillas, Soderstrom, Seidl, Warlaumont and Amatuni2019b; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019). Counter to expectations, we found a small but significant increase in TCDS rate with child age in the random clips and a small and significant decrease in TCDS rate with age in the turn-taking clips. The age-related baseline increase in TCDS may derive from more frequent participation in independent play with other children; in prior work, increased proportional input from other children was also associated with an increase in overall input rate (Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012). The age-related decrease in TCDS rate during peak interactional moments was not expected, but may also be attributable to this change in interactional partners with age; if adults are more likely to be the source of TCDS during interactional peaks for younger children, they may also provide more voluminous speech during those peaks than other children do during interactional peaks later in development. Sleep during the day may also help explain these patterns; if older children sleep less than younger children, they may be more likely hear more TCDS during random but not peak-based clips. All of these explanations require follow-up work from a larger sample of children and, ideally, from a larger sample of their interactions throughout the day. Finally, consistent with prior daylong language environment analyses, ODS rate decreased with age, and the random and turn-taking clips across the day revealed substantial fluctuations in TCDS rate (Abney et al., Reference Abney, Smith, Yu, Gunzelmann, Howes, Tenbrink and Davelaar2017; Bergelson et al., Reference Bergelson, Casillas, Soderstrom, Seidl, Warlaumont and Amatuni2019b; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019).

One implication of our findings is that TCDS rate estimates from daylong data do not directly distinguish distinct caregiver attitudes toward talking to young children. While Rossel caregivers view their children, even their young infants, as potential co-interactants in conversational play (Brown & Casillas, Reference Brown, Casillas, Fentiman and Goodyaccepted), the circumstances of everyday life shape the broader linguistic landscape such that most of what children hear is talk between others. We suggest that, in the daylong context, caregivers from these two subsistence farming communities are preoccupied for most of the day with social and domestic commitments in which they are motivated to converse with the other adults and (older) children present; not just to get their daily tasks done but also because these more mature speakers enable more complex verbal interactions and social routines. Rather, we suspect that caregiver attitudes about how to engage children in interaction are more clearly expressed during interactional peaks and, even then, via behaviors more nuanced than what can be captured by input quantity measures alone. In the case of Rossel Island, we saw not only more TCDS but also TCDS from more diverse speaker types during interactional peaks. We suggest, then, that the forces shaping the rate of Rossel children's linguistic input are somewhat different from the forces shaping the content and sources of their linguistic input. In order to comparatively examine culturally distinct codes of verbal interaction in children's at-home speech environments, future work should focus not only on the rate, but also the sources and content of the speech children are exposed to, perhaps using strategic subsampling similar to what was implemented here.

Implications for theories of language learning

Despite hearing relatively little directed linguistic input, these 10 Rossel children show no sign of delay in their achievement of early linguistic milestones, including the use of single- and multi-word utterances. This finding is hard to explain under any theory of language learning that requires very large amounts of TCDS input. While prior evidence predicts a highly robust onset of canonical babble (e.g., Oller et al. Reference Oller, Eilers, Basinger, Steffens and Urbano1995; Oller, Eilers, Neal & Cobo-Lewis, Reference Oller, Eilers, Neal and Cobo-Lewis1998; but see also Lee, Jhang, Relyea, Chen & Oller, Reference Lee, Jhang, Relyea, Chen and Oller2018 and Cychosz et al., Reference Cychosz, Cristia, Bergelson, Casillas, Baudet, Warlaumont, Scaff, Yankowitz and Seidl2019), the stable use of individual phonological segments in speech-like babble and the subsequent appearance of recognizable words is indeed variable between children (McGillion, Herbert, Pine, Vihman, DePaolis, Keren-Portnoy & Matthews, Reference McGillion, Herbert, Pine, Vihman, DePaolis, Keren-Portnoy and Matthews2017; see also McCune & Vihman, Reference McCune and Vihman2001) and, further on, children's early productive vocabulary size predicts their later syntactic development, including early word combinations (Frank et al., Reference Frank, Braginsky, Yurovsky and Marchmanin press; Marchman et al., Reference Marchman, Martínez-Sussmann and Dale2004). In sum, while prior evidence led us to expect a stable onset of canonical babble across diverse input contexts, it would not have led us to expect cross-context stability in the onset of early lexical productions, as found here.

Following a similar set of findings regarding both the language environment and vocal maturity of Tseltal-learning children, Casillas and colleagues (Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019) suggested three ways in which children might proceed in language learning without delay despite hearing relatively little directed speech: (a) an ability to learn from observing others’ language use (see also de León, Reference de León, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Rogoff et al., Reference Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez and Angelillo2003; Shneidman, Reference Shneidman2010; Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012), (b) capitalizing on regularities in language used during day-to-day routines, and (c) benefiting from a natural cycle in which children frequently sleep following short bursts of interactional linguistic input. In this third case, the idea is that short-term memories of directed input are consolidated before significant interference takes place (Gómez, Bootzin & Nadel, Reference Gómez, Bootzin and Nadel2006; Horváth, Liu & Plunkett, Reference Horváth, Liu and Plunkett2016; Kurdziel, Duclos & Spencer, Reference Kurdziel, Duclos and Spencer2013; Mullally & Maguire, Reference Mullally and Maguire2014). These three proposals for Tseltal children, which are not mutually exclusive, may also apply in the case of Rossel children, considering that the overall characteristics of their linguistic environments are not dissimilar.

Mechanisms for language learning that efficiently capitalize on sparse bursts of CDS and/or overhearable speech (e.g., massed learning, as in Schwab & Lew-Williams, Reference Schwab and Lew-Williams2016; or attention to others’ talk, as in Akhtar, Reference Akhtar2005 and Shneidman, Arroyo, Levine & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman, Arroyo, Levine and Goldin-Meadow2012) may help us understand the current findings. Further, theoretical models of language learning that: (a) make the most of each linguistic “datapoint” in the input and (b) enable rapid uptake of streams of talk (e.g., when observing speech between others) may be key to explaining language development in this kind of context. For example, prediction-based models allow the learner to compare the predicted vs. observed properties of each utterance as it unfolds, with recalibration when errors are detected (Chang, Dell & Bock, Reference Chang, Dell and Bock2006; Christiansen & Chater, Reference Christiansen and Chater2016; Elman, Reference Elman1990, Reference Elman1993; McCauley & Christiansen, Reference McCauley and Christiansen2017). Such models hypothetically make the most of each utterance by rapidly updating knowledge on the basis of both the occurrence and non-occurrence of expected events (see Rabagliati, Gambi & Pickering, Reference Rabagliati, Gambi and Pickering2016 for a balanced overview). In contrast, models of learning that rely on pedagogical cueing or frequent and fitted responses to infant vocalizations by an adult caregiver are not easily reconciled with the results presented here, nor indeed those reported for several other rural, traditional communities (Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven & Stieglitz, Reference Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven and Stieglitz2017; Gaskins, Reference Gaskins, Enfield and Levinson2006; Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs, Schieffelin, Schweder and LeVine1984; Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012; Vogt, Mastin & Schots, Reference Vogt, Mastin and Schots2015).

Limitations

Our language outcome measures, which track the onset and relative usage frequency of broad linguistic phenomena, crucially differ from those used in prior work establishing a relationship between child vocabulary and input quality measures (e.g., Cartmill et al., Reference Cartmill, Armstrong, Gleitman, Goldin-Meadow, Medina and Trueswell2013; Hirsh-Pasek, Adamson, Bakeman, Owen, Golinkoff, Pace, Yust & Suma, Reference Hirsh-Pasek, Adamson, Bakeman, Owen, Golinkoff, Pace, Yust and Suma2015; Ramírez, Lytle & Kuhl, Reference Ramírez, Lytle and Kuhl2020; Ramírez-Esparza, García-Sierra & Kuhl, Reference Ramírez-Esparza, García-Sierra and Kuhl2014; Rowe, Reference Rowe2012). Vocabulary development on Rossel Island may be similarly responsive to the type and quantity of CDS children encounter – for example, referentially transparent utterances would theoretically still facilitate the acquisition of word meanings. That said, our impression is that such variation does not play a meaningful role in Rossel children's development as a full-fledged members of the language community. So, future work along those lines would likely be limited to interpreting such effects with respect to the mechanisms underlying lexical category formation, and not as prerequisites for normative language development. With respect to input quality measures, we are similarly unable to assume that the features of language experience considered to be “quality” in a North American middle-class context also happen to promote the suite of language behaviors particular to Yélî Dnye speakers. Instead, we here use target-child-directed speech as a proxy for the quantity of tailored input children hear; that is, we focus here on the quantity of input we know to be designed for the child's attention and ability at the moment the speech was uttered.

Conclusion

We estimate that, on average, children on Rossel Island under age 3;0 hear 3.13 minutes of directed speech per hour, with an average of 14.45 minutes per hour during peak interactive moments during the day. Most directed speech comes from adults, but older children hear more directed speech from other children. There is also an average 35.90 minutes per hour of overhearable speech present. Older children heard more directed speech and less overhearable speech than younger children. Bursts of speech featuring mostly TCDS appear to be present from infancy onward. Despite this relatively low rate of directed speech, these children's vocal maturity appears on track with norms for typically developing children in many other populations (Cychosz et al., Reference Cychosz, Cristia, Bergelson, Casillas, Baudet, Warlaumont, Scaff, Yankowitz and Seidl2019; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Jhang, Relyea, Chen and Oller2018; Warlaumont et al., Reference Warlaumont, Richards, Gilkerson and Oller2014). The present findings thus join the numerous other documented cases of non-delayed language development without frequent child-directed speech (Brown, Reference Brown, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Brown & Gaskins, Reference Brown, Gaskins, Enfield, Kockelman and Sidnell2014; Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Brown and Levinson2019; Cristia et al., Reference Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven and Stieglitz2017; de León, Reference de León, Duranti, Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Gaskins, Reference Gaskins, Enfield and Levinson2006; Ochs, Reference Ochs1988; Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs, Schieffelin, Schweder and LeVine1984; Rogoff et al., Reference Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez and Angelillo2003; Schieffelin, Reference Schieffelin1990; Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, Reference Shneidman and Goldin-Meadow2012).

Our findings diverged in several ways from expectations developed on the basis of prior ethnographic work in this community, including the frequency of child-directed talk and the distribution of talk over the course of the day. When considered together with data from a Tseltal Mayan community, the findings suggest that estimates of input rate that are derived from daylong data are far more sensitive to situational variation (e.g., the number of speakers present, which varies with activity) than they are to established ideological variation in how caregivers talk to children. Whether child language development is better predicted by meaningful individual differences in average situational variation in input rate, ideologically based variation in other verbal behaviors (e.g., who talks to the child), or something in between, is a question for future work. Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic data will have a major role to play in teasing out the causal factors at play in this larger issue relating children's early linguistic experience to their later language development.

The data presented here come from an evolving corpus of Yélî Dnye developmental data; any reader interested in citing descriptive features of the Rossel child language environment (e.g., TCDS rate) or in replicating or extending these analyses is strongly encouraged to visit the following address for up-to-date estimates: https://middycasillas.shinyapps.io/Yeli-Child_Language_Environment/. The information on that linked page will include any new data, annotations, and analyses added after the publication of this study.

Supplementary Material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper, visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000549

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge that the collection and annotation of these recordings was made possible by Taakêmê Ńamono, Ndapw:éé Yidika, and Y:aaw:aa Pikuwa, with further assistance from Ghaalyu Yidika. We also give thanks to the PNG National Research Institute, and the Administration of Milne Bay Province. We are indebted to the participating families and the Rossel community at large for their continuing support. We also acknowledge support from the ACLEW project and thank Maartje Weenink for her help with manual clip selection. This work is supported by a NWO Veni Innovational Scheme grant (275-89-033) to MC, an ERC Advanced Grant (269484 INTERACT) to SCL, and fieldwork funding from the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. This paper was written using the papaja library in RStudio (Aust & Barth, Reference Aust and Barth2018).

Footnotes

1 While a comparison between the Rossel and Tseltal communities is still confounded by numerous other cultural and linguistic differences, their similarity in subsistence lifestyle facilitates comparative interpretations more than either community compared to a post-industrial one.

2 Local schools include elementary (~3 years; ages ~7–10) and primary (~6 years; ages ~10–16) education. Subsequent education is not locally available and students pursuing this route must find accommodations on other islands in the region or on mainland PNG.

3 For comparison, men's TCDS was absent in 4 out of 10 Tseltal children's samples and was outpaced 12-to-1 or more by TCDS from women in the other 6 children's samples.

References

Abney, D. H., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2017). It's time: Quantifying the relevant time scales for joint attention. In Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T., & Davelaar, E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 14891494). London, UK.Google Scholar
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8(2), 199209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aust, F., & Barth, M. (2018). papaja: Create APA manuscripts with R Markdown. Retrieved from https://github.com/crsh/papajaGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5–6), 507584. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909697386628Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., Amatuni, A., Dailey, S., Koorathota, S., & Tor, S. (2019a). Day by day, hour by hour: Naturalistic language input to infants. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12715. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., Casillas, M., Soderstrom, M., Seidl, A., Warlaumont, A. S., & Amatuni, A. (2019b). What do North American babies hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12724. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinchmann, E. I., Braeken, J., & Lyster, S.-A. H. (2019). Is there a direct relation between the development of vocabulary and grammar? Developmental Science, 22(1), e12709. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, M. E., Kristensen, K., van Benthem, K. J., Magnusson, A., Berg, C. W., Nielsen, A., Skaug, H. J., Mächler, M., & Bolker, B. M. (2017). Modeling zero-inflated count data with glmmTMB. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/132753Google Scholar
Brown, P. (2011). The cultural organization of attention. In Duranti, A., Ochs, E., & and Schieffelin, Bambi B (Eds.), Handbook of Language Socialization (pp. 2955). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. (2014). The interactional context of language learning in Tzeltal. In Arnon, I., Casillas, M., Kurumada, C., & Estigarribia, B. (Eds.), Language in interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark (pp. 5182). Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Brown, P., & Casillas, M. (accepted). Childrearing through social interaction on Rossel Island, PNG. In Fentiman, A. J. & Goody, M. (Eds.), Esther Goody revisited: Exploring the legacy of an original inter-disciplinarian (pp. XXXX). New York, NY: Berghahn. Retrieved from https://psyarxiv.com/5rvky/Google Scholar
Brown, P., & Gaskins, S. (2014). Language acquisition and language socialization. In Enfield, N. J., Kockelman, P., & Sidnell, J. (Eds.), Handbook of Linguistic Anthropology (pp. 187226). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139342872.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., Armstrong, B. F., Gleitman, L. R., Goldin-Meadow, S., Medina, T. N., & Trueswell, J. C. (2013). Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(28), 1127811283. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2017). Casillas HomeBank corpus. https://doi.org/10.21415/T51X12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2019). Early language experience in a Tseltal Mayan village. Child Development, OnlineOpen(X), XXXX.Google Scholar
Casillas, M., Bunce, J., Soderstrom, M., Rosemberg, C., Migdalek, M., Alam, F., … Garrison, H. (2017). Introduction: The ACLEW DAS template [training materials]. Retrieved from https://osf.io/aknjv/Google Scholar
Casillas, M., & Cristia, A. (2019). A step-by-step guide to collecting and analyzing long-format speech environment (LFSE) recordings. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), 24. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2017). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development, Early View, 115. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974Google Scholar
Cychosz, M., Cristia, A., Bergelson, E., Casillas, M., Baudet, G., Warlaumont, A. S., Scaff, C., Yankowitz, L. & Seidl, A. (2019). BabbleCor: A Crosslinguistic Corpus of Babble Development in Five Languages. DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/RZ4TX.Google Scholar
de León, L. (2011). Language socialization and multiparty participation frameworks. In Duranti, A., Ochs, E., & and Schieffelin, Bambi B (Eds.), Handbook of Language Socialization (pp. 81111). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342901.ch4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14(2), 179211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48(1), 7199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., & Marchman, V. A. (in press). Variability and consistency in early language learning: The Wordbank project. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Retrieved from https://langcog.github.io/wordbank-book/Google Scholar
Gaskins, S. (2000). Children's daily activities in a Mayan village: A culturally grounded description. Cross-Cultural Research, 34(4), 375389. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939710003400405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaskins, S. (2006). Cultural perspectives on infant–caregiver interaction. In Enfield, N. J. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction (pp. 279298). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Gómez, R. L., Bootzin, R. R., & Nadel, L. (2006). Naps promote abstraction in language-learning infants. Psychological Science, 17(8), 670674. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01764.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, C. R., Thiemann-Bourque, K., Walker, D., Buzhardt, J., & Gilkerson, J. (2011). Assessing children's home language environments using automatic speech recognition technology. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 32(2), 8392. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740110367826CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1996). Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., Yust, P. K. S., & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children's language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 10711083. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 13681378. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horváth, K., Liu, S., & Plunkett, K. (2016). A daytime nap facilitates generalization of word meanings in young toddlers. Sleep, 39(1), 203207. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.5348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdziel, L., Duclos, K., & Spencer, R. M. (2013). Sleep spindles in midday naps enhance learning in preschool children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(43), 1726717272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C.-C., Jhang, Y., Relyea, G., Chen, L.-m., & Oller, D. K. (2018). Babbling development as seen in canonical babbling ratios: A naturalistic evaluation of all-day recordings. Infant Behavior and Development, 50, 140153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeVine, R. A., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., Keefer, C. H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1996). Child care and culture: Lessons from Africa. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Baldwin, G. (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language, 24(1), 187219. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000996002930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., Martínez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7(2), 212224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00340.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2017). Computational investigations of multiword chunks in language learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9(3), 637652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCune, L., & Vihman, M. M. (2001). Early phonetic and lexical development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGillion, M., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J., Vihman, M., DePaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Matthews, D. (2017). What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 88(1), 156166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullally, S. L., & Maguire, E. A. (2014). Learning to remember: The early ontogeny of episodic memory. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 1229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E. (1988). Culture and language development: Language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In Schweder, R. A. & LeVine, R. A. (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 276322). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Basinger, D., Steffens, M. L., & Urbano, R. (1995). Extreme poverty and the development of precursors to the speech capacity. First Language, 15(44), 167187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (1998). Late onset canonical babbling: A possible early marker of abnormal development. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 103(3), 249263.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (1993). Reanalysing rote-learned phrases: Individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 20(3), 551571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008473Google ScholarPubMed
Pye, C. (1986). Quiché Mayan speech to children. Journal of Child Language, 13(1), 85100. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900000313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabagliati, H., Gambi, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2016). Learning to predict or predicting to learn? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 94105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramírez, N. F., Lytle, S. R., & Kuhl, P. K. (2020). Parent coaching increases conversational turns and advances infant language development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(7), 34843491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2014). Look who's talking: Speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17, 880891. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12172CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Paradise, R., Arauz, R. M., Correa-Chávez, M., & Angelillo, C. (2003). Firsthand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 175203. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185205. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 17621774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaff, C., Stieglitz, J., Casillas, M., & Cristia, A. (in preparation). Language input in a hunter-forager population: Estimations from daylong recordings.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B. (1990). The give and take of everyday life: Language, socialization of Kaluli children. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Repetition across successive sentences facilitates young children's word learning. Developmental Psychology, 52(6), 879886. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shneidman, L. A. (2010). Language Input and Acquisition in a Mayan Village (PhD thesis). The University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Shneidman, L. A., Arroyo, M. E., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). What counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language, 40(3), 672686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shneidman, L. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science, 15(5), 659673. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01168.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1970). Universals of grammatical development in children. In Flores d'Arcais, G. B. & Levelt, W. J. M. (Eds.), Advances in Psycholinguistics (pp. 174186). Amsterdam, NL: North Holland Publishing.Google Scholar
Smithson, M., & Merkle, E. (2013). Generalized linear models for categorical and continuous limited dependent variables. New York: Chapman; Hall/CRC. https://doi.org/10.1201/b15694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: From input to interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to Children: Language Input and Interaction (pp. 3149).Google Scholar
Soderstrom, M., & Wittebolle, K. (2013). When do caregivers talk? The influences of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments. PloS One, 8, e80646. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Brooks, P. J. (1999). Early syntactic development: A Construction Grammar approach. In Barrett, M. (Ed.), The Development of Language (pp. 161190). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Vogt, P., Mastin, J. D., & Schots, D. M. A. (2015). Communicative intentions of child-directed speech in three different learning environments: Observations from the Netherlands, and rural and urban Mozambique. First Language, 35(4–5), 341358. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723715596647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warlaumont, A. S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., & Oller, D. K. (2014). A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in Autism. Psychological Science, 25(7), 13141324. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614531023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 21432152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2016). Ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer-Verlag New York. Retrieved from https://ggplot2.tidyverse.orgCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittenburg, P., Brugman, H., Russel, A., Klassmann, A., & Sloetjes, H. (2006). ELAN: A professional framework for multimodality research. In Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (pp. 15561559).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic overview of the 10 children whose recordings are sampled in the current study, including from left to right: child's age (years;months.days); child's sex (M/F); mother's age (years); highest level of maternal education achieved (primary (grades 6–7)/secondary (grades 8–11)/preparatory (grade 12)); and the number of people living in the child's household.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Recording duration (black line) and sampled clips (colored boxes) for each of the 10 recordings analyzed, sorted by child age in months.

Figure 2

Figure 2. TCDS min/hr (left) and ODS min/hr (right) across the sampled age range. Each box plot summarizes the data for one child from the randomly sampled clips (purple; solid) or the turn taking clips (green; dashed). Bands on the linear trends show 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 3

Figure 3. TCDS min/hr (left panels) and ODS min/hr (right panels) across the recorded day in the random clips (top panels) and turn-taking (bottom panels) clips. Each box plot summarizes the data for children age 1;0 and younger (light) or age 1;0 and older (dark) at the given time of day.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Proportion of vocalization types used by children across age (NCB = Non-canonical babble, CB = Canonical babble, SW = single word utterance, MW = multi-word utterance).

Supplementary material: File

Casillas et al. Supplementary Materials

Casillas et al. Supplementary Materials

Download Casillas et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 1.2 MB