Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T07:04:46.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prevalence of perceived coercion among psychiatric patients: literature review and meta-regression modelling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Giles Newton-Howes*
Affiliation:
Imperial College London, UK Otago University, Wellington, New Zealand
James Stanley
Affiliation:
Otago University, Wellington, New Zealand
*
Giles Netwon-Howes ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims and method

To assess how common the subjective experience of coercion is in psychiatric care and what affects its prevalence. A review of published data was undertaken to assess prevalence of coercion and potential confounding variables. The heterogeneity of results was studied using meta-regression to quantify the relative impact of four potential explanatory variables.

Results

The raw prevalence of perceived coercion ranged from 16 to 90%. A quarter of legally detained patients did not feel coerced into psychiatric care, whereas a quarter of voluntary in-patients reported coercion in care. Coercion was more common in studies outside the USA, among patient populations subject to legal detention and populations studied using the MacArthur Perceived Coercion Scale as opposed to other measures. Timing of the interview was not associated with coercion.

Clinical implications

Coercion in psychiatric care remains highly prevalent but varies widely by study. Consistency in measurement is necessary to allow better comparison between studies.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2012

Perceived coercion in care is an issue of particular importance in psychiatry, partly due to the growing use of interventions that intuitively increase the likelihood of this occurring. Reference Salize and Dressing1 The most obvious example of actions that may increase the perception of coercion is the more widespread use of a court-mandated process of detention, present in many countries worldwide. It is not, however, only the process of legal detention in hospital that can lead to the subjective experience of coercive care; Reference Katsakou and Priebe2 locked wards, Reference Haglund and Von Essen3 physical restraint Reference Sailas and Fenton4 and out-patient commitment Reference Swartz, Wagner, Swanson, Hiday and Burns5 are examples of interventions that can be seen to reduce a patient's liberty, increasing the likelihood of patients perceiving the care they receive negatively. As public policy drives an ever-more community-oriented psychiatric service, it is possible that the number of coercive actions undertaken to maintain patients in fewer hospital beds will grow, potentially increasing perceived coercions in some settings. This makes understanding coercion particularly important for practising psychiatrists. Reference Zinkler and Priebe6

Coercion is the negative subjective experience of loss of autonomy caused by an involuntary action, in this case from the mental health services towards a patient. Reference Rhodes7 An objective coercive action is a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for a patient to experience coercion. Reference Newton-Howes8 For example, legal detention, an objectively coercive act, is necessary for a patient to subsequently feel coerced, but the use of legal detention, although objectively coercive, does not, ipso facto, engender the subjective experience of coercion. With the development of the patients’ rights movement, the views of patients are becoming better understood Reference Glass and Arnkoff9 and there is growing importance of allowing patients to choose their own treatment. Reference Nilstun and Syse10 This has led to developments such as non-hospital-based treatments that are more acceptable to patients and are related to reductions in perceived coercion. Reference Gliburt, Slade, Rose, Lloyd-Evans, Johnson and Osborn11 Similarly, mechanisms are being developed to ensure patients’ rights are not infringed as part of psychiatric care. 12

The issue of coercion as a potentially unwanted consequence of management is therefore becoming urgently important, both in terms of its impact on treatment and the need to understand the factors that influence it. This is true for intuitively coercive actions, such as detention, but also for management interventions that may not be considered coercive, such as informal admission. Despite this, the prevalence of this unwanted ‘side-effect’ is unclear. The aim of this review is therefore to systematically collate those papers that outline the prevalence of perceived coercion to ascertain how common this is and understand the variation in reported rates. An exploration of the factors that may increase or decrease these rates from both a methodological and an epidemiological perspective is also considered.

Method

Selection of papers

Selection of papers into the review was kept broad to ensure all papers reporting on the experiences of patients were included. We chose papers describing adults between 16 and 65 years of age in adult psychiatric care (or coercion reported about such care). The papers had to report on the experience of coercion from the patient's perspective, rather than examining potentially coercive interventions. Papers that did not fulfil the inclusion criteria were rejected. All included papers had ethical approval.

It was decided a priori to collect information on coercion presented in a dichotomised fashion. This decision was made for three reasons. Most importantly, in this review coercion was considered as a subjective experience for a patient when reflecting on a paternalistic action. As such, patients reported it to be present or not, although the degree and nature of perceived coercion clearly varies from patient to patient, and philosophically this experience has many shades of grey. Reference Szmukler and Applebaum13 Second, empirically coercion tends to be reported with a bimodal distribution in studies using scalar instruments to measure coercion, usually the MacArthur Perceived Coercion Scale (MPCS) 14 or coercion ladder, Reference Høyer, Kjellin, Engberg, Kaltiala-Heino, Nilstun and Sigurjónsdóttir15 suggesting that for many patients the experience of coercion is either present or absent. Finally, this approach makes statistical analysis manageable, particularly when comparing results using different measures, and as such allows for the relative effect of a number of variables to be assessed and compared with each other as to their relative impact on the patient's experience.

The MPCS is a well-validated instrument developed by the MacArthur Research Network in America. It comprises five questions drawn from the larger Admission Experience Survey and specifically addresses the experience of perceived coercion. Each question is a true/false statement and studies tend to show a bimodal distribution with the point of difference at 2. 14 The coercion ladder is a visual analogue scale ranging usually from 1 to 10, with the fixed ends representing ‘no coercion’ and ‘totally coerced’. Although the instrument is somewhat different in approach, it also tends to provide bimodal data and has also been validated. Reference Høyer, Kjellin, Engberg, Kaltiala-Heino, Nilstun and Sigurjónsdóttir15 For each paper, continuous data were dichotomised according to the authors’ reported cut-off points when reported in this fashion using the MPCS or the coercion ladder. Where this was unclear, the authors were contacted and asked for clarification.

Although no specific exclusion criteria were identified at the onset of the systematic review, papers were excluded if they did not report an overall coercion figure (e.g. reporting each question of an inventory separately without reporting a combined total). For papers where coercion was not reported, but it was thought the data may exist, the authors were contacted to ask whether they could supply the data. For pragmatic reasons papers not reported in English could not be included in the analysis.

The search was conducted electronically. MEDLINE, PsycINFO and Cinahl were accessed via the EBSCO platform. The free-text key words ‘coerci*’, ‘liberty’ and ‘duress’ were used to identify papers on coercion and the free-text term ‘psych*’ was used to identify the psychiatric literature. These sets were then combined with the Boolean term ‘AND’. The titles of all identified papers were read; the abstract and/or full text of any paper considered to be a possible for inclusion was read, and the references of these papers were examined to cross-reference for other potential studies.

For all included papers data were extracted on the prevalence of coercion, total numbers of patients reporting coercion, and total number of patients in the sample. To assess how the prevalence of coercion might be influenced by social and methodological factors, information was recorded (where available) on: the setting of care (in-patient or out-patient), the timing of the interview, the country in which the sample was collected, the instrument used to measure coercion and the legal status of the patients involved. The management strategy that led to coercion was in all cases either admission to hospital or provision of pharmacotherapy.

The timing of data collection and the instrument used to measure coercion are methodological variables that may conceivably influence prevalence rate. Country of study Reference Bergk, Flammer and Steinert16 and patient legal status Reference Allen and Smith17 are epidemiological factors that could be expected to alter prevalence rates. Although treatment setting represents a common intervention potentially associated with coercion, there were only two out-patient studies (both conducted in the USA) examining this variable and these were not, therefore, included in the meta-regression.

Statistical analysis

The METAREG package implemented in Stata 10.1 for Windows Reference Harbord and Higgins18 was used to calculate overall prevalence of coercion across studies, and also meta-regression of coercion prevalence according to those explanatory variables listed earlier. Raw data from each study were used to calculate the log odds and associated standard error of being coerced. Reference Clayton and Hills19 Two studies reported 100% coercion, and so 0.5 was added to the total denominator in these studies to allow calculation of the log odds. Reference Thompson and Sharps20

Covariates used in the meta-regression were patients’ legal detention status (voluntary, legally detained, mixed sample); geographical study location (Nordic countries, USA, and other countries - UK, Switzerland, Australia and New Zealand); interview timing (less than 7 days after admission; 7 days or longer after admission); and coercion instrument used (MPCS and coercion ladder).

The meta-regression treated multiple results from the same paper as independent. For example, six papers reported separate coercion figures for legally detained and voluntarily admitted patients. A pooled odds ratio for this comparison is reported based on these papers alone for comparison with the meta-regression. Likewise, for one paper Reference Kjellin, Høyer, Engberg, Kaltiala-Heino and Sigurjónsdóttir21 data were reported from several Nordic countries which were treated as four independent samples.

Results

In total, 5032 potential papers were highlighted by the initial electronic search. After review of the titles and abstracts, 75 were considered as ‘possibles’ and read in full; 18 papers including 3489 patients were left for the final analysis. Total patients included by study ranged from 11 Reference Haglund, Von Knorring and Von Essen22 to 733 Reference Priebe, Katsakou, Amos, Leese, Morriss and Rose23 (Table 1).

TABLE 1 Papers included in the final summaryFootnote a

Total in study, n Coerced, % Country
Kjellin et al, 2006 Reference Kjellin, Høyer, Engberg, Kaltiala-Heino and Sigurjónsdóttir21 764 49 Nordic countries
Haglund et al, 2003 Reference Haglund, Von Knorring and Von Essen22 11 64 Sweden
Priebe et al, 2009 Reference Priebe, Katsakou, Amos, Leese, Morriss and Rose23 733 87 UK
Lidz et al, 1998 Reference Lidz, Mulvey, Hoge, Kirsch, Monahan and Eisenberg24 , Footnote b 171 22 USA
Swartz et al, 2003 Reference Swartz, Swanson and Hannon25 99 44 USA
Link et al, 2008 Reference Link, Castilled and Stuber26 184 51 USA
Hoge et al, 1997 Reference Hoge, Lidz, Eisenberg, Gardner, Monahan and Mulvey27 156 33 USA
Kjellin et al, 2004 Reference Kjellin, Andersson, Bartholdson, Candefjord, Holmstrøm and Jacobsson28 282 41 Sweden
Eriksson & Westrin, 1995 Reference Eriksson and Westrin29 199 47 Sweden
Bindman et al, 2005 Reference Bindman, Reid, Szmukler, Tiller, Thornicroft and Leese30 98 47 UK
Ivar Iversen et al, 2002 Reference Ivar Iversen, Høyer, Sexton and Grønli31 222 49 Norway
Soergaard, 2004 Reference Soergaard32 190 16 Norway
Shannon, 1976 Reference Shannon33 100 48 Australia
Svensson & Hansson, 1994 Reference Svensson and Hansson34 50 42 Sweden
McKenna et al, 2003 Reference McKenna, Simpson and Coverdale35 69 81 New Zealand
Kuosmanen et al, 2007 Reference Kousmanen, Hätönen, Malkavaara, Kylmä and Välimäki36 51 69 Finland
Bonsack & Borgeat, 2009 Reference Bonsack and Borgeat37 91 74 Switzerland
Olfsson & Jacobsson, 2001 Reference Olofsson and Jacobsson38 , Footnote b 19 90 Sweden
Total sample size 3489

a In-patient treatment in all studies apart from Swartz 2003 and Link 2008, where it was out-patient.

b Prevalence rate confirmed by personal correspondence.

Published raw prevalence rates of coercion varied from 22% Reference Lidz, Mulvey, Hoge, Kirsch, Monahan and Eisenberg24 (C. Lidz, coercion data obtained through personal communication, 2011) to 87%. Reference Priebe, Katsakou, Amos, Leese, Morriss and Rose23 Two papers from an out-patient setting reported raw prevalence rates of 44% Reference Swartz, Swanson and Hannon25 and 51%, Reference Link, Castilled and Stuber26 but they are not included in the subsequent analysis. The raw combined prevalence rate was 53% (95% CI 40-65). Including the two out-patient studies gave a prevalence of 52% (95% CI 41-63). The clearest divergence in coercion prevalence was between legally detained and informally admitted patients. The raw combined prevalence for coercion in the legally detained group was 74% (95% CI 63-82), whereas the coercion prevalence for the informal patient group was 25% (95% CI 20-31).

Meta-regression was undertaken to examine the effect of timing of questioning, instrument used to measure coercion, country of study and legal status (Table 2). Legal status at admission was strongly associated with perceived coercion: legally detained in-patients were more than eight times as likely as voluntarily admitted patients to experience coercion. Coercion was reported more rarely in the USA than in other Western countries in the analysis. From a methodological perspective, interview timing was not associated with significant differences in coercion with OR=1.9 (95% CI 0.8-4.4). Finally, the instrument used to assess coercion appears to influence the prevalence of coercion: those assessed with the coercion ladder or another assessment method were less likely to report coercion than those assessed with the MPCS.

TABLE 2 Odds ratios from meta-regression analysis for experiencing subjective coercion

Factor Odds ratio 95% CI P
Legal status of admission
    Detained 8.6 4.6-15.9 <0.001
    Mixture of detained and informal 3.1 1.4-6.9 0.008
    Informal ReferenceFootnote b
Timing of interview
    7 days or longer, or mixed 1.9 0.8-4.4 0.148
    Less than 7 days ReferenceFootnote b
Country
    Nordic countries 3.3 1.3-8.4 0.016
    Other countriesFootnote a 5.6 2.1-15.2 0.002
    USA ReferenceFootnote b
Instrument used to assess coercion
    Coercion ladder or other instrument 0.4 0.2-0.9 0.03
    MPCS ReferenceFootnote b

MPCS, MacArthur Perceived Coercion Scale.

a UK, Switzerland, Australia and New Zealand.

b Reference denotes reference level against which odds ratios were calculated for factor.

Six studies reported coercion figures for both legally detained and voluntarily admitted patients Reference Kjellin, Høyer, Engberg, Kaltiala-Heino and Sigurjónsdóttir21,Reference Hoge, Lidz, Eisenberg, Gardner, Monahan and Mulvey27-Reference Ivar Iversen, Høyer, Sexton and Grønli31 allowing a direct estimation of the odds ratio for experiencing coercion between these legal status groups. The pooled odds ratio calculated by meta-analysis for these studies was 8.5 (95% CI 4.5-15.8), comparable with the odds ratio for legally detained v. voluntary patients in the meta-regression (Table 2).

Discussion

This paper describes how common it is for patients in psychiatric care to experience the treatment they receive as coercive and highlights the variables that may influence this. It is the first systematic review and analysis of data published in this area that allows a clear understanding of this issue. It highlights that coercion is common not only in legally detained patients, but also among patients who are in hospital of their own free will, for treatment they are presumed to have consented to.

Legally detained patients are, unsurprisingly, more likely to report coercion than those voluntarily admitted. This is both intuitive and unsurprising. Both meta-regression and more straightforward meta-analysis of the odds ratio from studies where coercion data were collected in detained as well as non-detained samples make this finding clear. Legal detention plays a large part in the experience of coercion but is not the only important factor. Country of study was also a significant variable, with coercion less common in the USA, even after controlling for the relative distribution of legal detention, when compared with both Nordic countries and the UK and other countries in this review. It is unclear what factors may be influencing this, although the variation is significant and warrants closer examination. The EUNOMIA project, Reference Kallert, Glöckner, Onchev, Raboch, Karastergiou and Solomon39 funded by the European Commission, is exploring the cross-national variation in Europe and may address some of these questions, although cultural factors including patient expectation, legislation and psychiatric practice are likely to be important variables. Recent research suggests some measures of social pressure vary by country, Reference Burns, Yeeles, Molodynski, Nightingale, Vazquez-Montes and Sheehan40 with increased pressure (‘leverage’) applied in a US setting. This highlights the importance of clearly defining the construct examined, in this case coercion as opposed to leverage, and how these are defined. Szmukler & Applebaum have highlighted the issues in the need for clarity in this area, Reference Szmukler and Applebaum13 identifying the coercive ‘hierarchy’ and where coercion sits in this. Our study examines papers at the most coerced end of this hierarchy and this potentially explained the differences found in this meta-analysis compared with the individual study of Szmukler & Applebaum. Our study also grouped European studies together, in much the same fashion as the EUNOMIA study. Although this increases statistical power, it may weaken the generalisability of the finding to particular patient groups. A combination of meta-analysis and individual studies is likely to prove the most balanced understanding of perceived coercion to guide clinical practice.

The period in which information on coercion was collected from patients appeared to have little influence on the experience of coercion, suggesting that there is no reporting bias for coercive experiences at the early stages of treatment compared with the later stages. This is surprising as recall bias may reinforce affective memory of interventions, enhancing them in a patient's mind. From a research perspective, this suggests there is no pressure to collect information about coercion from patients with any particular speed following a particular intervention and this may be of value in the design of future studies.

The tool used to measure coercion appeared to have an impact on prevalence: studies using the MPCS were associated with higher prevalence rates than studies using the coercion ladder or some other method of assessing coercion. This may reflect the wider scope of the MPCS than a visual analogue scale of the coercion ladder or a simple yes/no response. The MPCS is a well-validated, peer-reviewed tool which clearly measures subjective experience in a reproducible way, with good interrater reliability. It may be that this tool also measures other constructs, such as threats and social leverage, and this may explain greater prevalence rates. Reference Lidz, Mulvey, Hoge, Kirsch, Monahan and Eisenberg24 Nonetheless, having a scientifically validated tool improves rigour in studying this potentially vague concept and allows for combination of findings to provide for a greater understanding of this experience as is presented in this paper. Such variation does, however, suggest that a single tool be used when measuring this subjective experience to improve reliability of coercion as a construct.

Limitations

These findings are limited by several factors. First, to examine the subjective experience of coercion, all papers with purely objective definitions of coercion (such as legal detention) were excluded. Although this ignores some evidence on coercion, it increases the robustness of the findings by examining one construct - the patient's experience. This minimises the problems of heterogeneity that diminish the usefulness of many systematic reviews. Second, all of the studies included in this review came from predominantly White, Western, high-income countries. This potentially reduces the generalisability of these findings to low- and middle-income countries or countries with vastly different legal or social milieus. Similarly, the majority of papers examined in-patients within a public health system, again limiting the generalisability of these findings to out-patient or private clinic settings. It was not possible to accurately describe the in-patient setting for each study included, although it is worth noting the papers from high-income Western countries may improve the similarity of the setting examined. It is also worth noting all included papers were from general adult settings, as opposed to forensic or child settings, which again was designed to improve the similarity of the environmental influences. Despite this, it is possible that unaccounted for environmental changes may have led to counterintuitive findings and need to be considered as a limitation to the finding of this study. These limitations do, however, improve the homogeneity of the populations studied, improving the robustness of the findings within this group. Limitation to English language papers may have excluded some potential articles, although only German and Spanish language papers were found in the review process and overlooked on the basis of language, not papers from Africa, Asia or South America.

From a statistical perspective, the study design for the meta-analysis is essentially observational, in that the studies are not randomised to the different levels of the explanatory factors. In line with standard epidemiological thinking, evidence from the meta-regression is therefore less reliable than evidence from a study where multiple factors are assessed within a single unifying methodological approach. The risk in meta-regression is that the results described might be attributable to residual confounding of the factors of interest with some unknown factor. Reference Thompson and Higgins41 This would, however, be unlikely in this analysis owing to the magnitude of the odds ratios found for several factors.

Implications for practitioners and patients

The findings of this systematic review and meta-analysis raise important issues for clinical in-patient mental health workers, researchers in the field, public policy makers and patients.

For clinicians, the finding that one in four voluntarily admitted in-patients experience coercion in management has ethical implications for practice. It would suggest that clarity around the issues of explicit consent for interventions is important and that implied consent, on the basis of voluntary status, may be misplaced. It also reminds clinicians that experiences of coercion are common for many in-patients, and need to be reflected on within in-patient treatment teams to ensure that practices that reduce coercion are regularly applied. Reference Lidz42

For researchers, the need for clarity about the construct under investigation is important and the use of a common tool, such as the MPCS, improves this. Timing of data collection seems less relevant. Future reviews will benefit from authors reporting on issues regarding those factors that appear to influence coercion rates, particularly common interventions (e.g. in-patient v. out-patient care) and demographic features. In this review, only legal detention has sufficient data to allow such analysis and the finding of increased coercion is not surprising.

For public policy and lawmakers, this systematic review serves as a timely reminder that intuition may not be correct. Much of the experience of coercion is in relation to social constructs, commonly the law, and its revision is not under medical control. The interactions between psychiatrists and patients are necessarily governed by broader cultural factors. The very high rate of coercion in some cultural settings needs further examination to elicit what specific factors are associated with it. This type of analysis allows for evidence-based changes in healthcare systems that can potentially improve perceptions of healthcare and may reduce the stigma associated with mental disorders both within and without the psychiatric profession. Reference Newton-Howes, Weaver and Tyrer43

For patients, this review highlights the complex nature of mental health interactions and the importance of patients’ subjective experiences. In-patient treatment is neither easy nor straightforward, and balancing the need for state-of-the-art care, cultural reality and patient choice is, clearly, fraught with difficulty. Reference Olsen44 Many of the decisions made may be felt as coercive Reference Newton-Howes and Mullen45 and the challenge for psychiatric in-patient care is to continuously improve, through research and practice, to optimise the patient's experience.

Acknowledgements

We thank Professors S. Priebe, C. Lidz, H. Poulsen and B. Oloffsen for answering queries about their published work and Viv Kerr and the team at the library in the Hawke's Bay District Health Board (New Zealand) for their assistance.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Salize, HJ, Dressing, H. Epidemiology of involuntary placement of mentally ill people across the European Union. Br J Psychiatry 2004; 184: 163–8.Google Scholar
2 Katsakou, C, Priebe, S. Outcome of involuntary hospital admission – a review. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006; 114: 232–41.Google Scholar
3 Haglund, K, Von Essen, L. Locked entrance doors at psychiatric wards – advantages and disadvantages according to voluntarily admitted patients. Nord J Psychiatry 2005; 59: 511–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Sailas, EES, Fenton, M. Seclusion and restraint for people with serious mental illnesses. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2000; 1: CD001163.Google Scholar
5 Swartz, MS, Wagner, HR, Swanson, JW, Hiday, VA, Burns, BJ. The perceived coerciveness of involuntary outpatient commitment: findings from an experimental study. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 2002; 30: 207–17.Google Scholar
6 Zinkler, M, Priebe, S. Detention of the mentally ill in Europe – a review. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2002; 106: 38.Google Scholar
7 Rhodes, M. The nature of coercion. J Value Inq 2000; 34: 369–81.Google Scholar
8 Newton-Howes, G. Coercion in psychiatric care: where are we now, what do we know, where do we go? Psychiatrist 2010; 34: 217–20.Google Scholar
9 Glass, C, Arnkoff, D. Consumers' perspectives on helpful and hindering factors in mental health treatment. J Clin Psychol 2000; 56: 1467–80.Google Scholar
10 Nilstun, T, Syse, A. The right to accept and the right to refuse. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000; 101 (suppl): 31–4.Google Scholar
11 Gliburt, H, Slade, M, Rose, D, Lloyd-Evans, B, Johnson, S, Osborn, DPJ. Service users' experiences of residential alternatives to standard acute wards: qualitative study of similarities and differences. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197 (suppl 53): s2631.Google Scholar
12 Department of Health. Deprivation of Liberty Safeguards: Code of Practice to Supplement the Main Mental Capacity Act 2005 Code of Practice. TSO (The Stationery Office), 2008.Google Scholar
13 Szmukler, G, Applebaum, PS. Treatment pressures, leverage, coercion and compulsion in mental health care. J Ment Health 2008; 17: 233–44.Google Scholar
14 MacArthur Research Network on Mental Health and the Law. The MacArthur Coercion Study: May 2004 Update of the Executive Summary. MacArthur Research Network, 2004 (http://www.macarthur.virginia.edu/coercion.html).Google Scholar
15 Høyer, G, Kjellin, L, Engberg, M, Kaltiala-Heino, R, Nilstun, T, Sigurjónsdóttir, M, et al. Paternalism and autonomy: a presentation of a Nordic study on the use of coercion in the mental care system. Int J Law Psychiatry 2002; 25: 93108.Google Scholar
16 Bergk, J, Flammer, E, Steinert, T. Ratings of coercive interventions by inpatients and staff in Germany. Psychiatr Serv 2009; 60: 1401–2.Google Scholar
17 Allen, M, Smith, V. Opening Pandora's box: the practical and legal dangers of involuntary outpatient commitment. Psychiatr Serv 2001; 52: 342–6.Google Scholar
18 Harbord, R, Higgins, J. METAREG: Stata Module to Perform Meta-analysis Regression. Boston College Department of Economics, 2004.Google Scholar
19 Clayton, D, Hills, M. Statistical Models in Epidemiology. Oxford University Press, 1993.Google Scholar
20 Thompson, S, Sharps, S. Explaining heterogeneity in meta-analysis: a comparison of methods. Stat Med 1999; 18: 2693–708.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21 Kjellin, L, Høyer, G, Engberg, M, Kaltiala-Heino, R, Sigurjónsdóttir, M. Differences in perceived coercion at admission to psychiatric hospitals in the Nordic countries. Soc Psychiatry Psychiat Epidemiol 2006; 41: 241–7.Google Scholar
22 Haglund, K, Von Knorring, L, Von Essen, L. Forced medication in psychiatric care: patient experiences and nurse perceptions. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs 2003; 10: 6572.Google Scholar
23 Priebe, S, Katsakou, C, Amos, T, Leese, M, Morriss, R, Rose, D, et al. Patients' views and readmissions 1 year after involuntary hospitalisation. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 194: 4954.Google Scholar
24 Lidz, CW, Mulvey, EP, Hoge, SK, Kirsch, BL, Monahan, J, Eisenberg, M, et al. Factual sources of psychiatric patients' perceptions of coercion in the hospital admission process. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 1254–60.Google Scholar
25 Swartz, M, Swanson, J, Hannon, M. Does fear of coercion keep people away from mental health treatment? Evidence from a survey of persons with schizophrenia and mental health professionals. Behav Sci Law 2003; 21: 459–72.Google Scholar
26 Link, B, Castilled, D, Stuber, J. Stigma and coercion in the context of outpatient treatment for people with mental illness. Soc Sci Med 2008; 67: 409–19.Google Scholar
27 Hoge, SK, Lidz, CW, Eisenberg, M, Gardner, W, Monahan, J, Mulvey, E, et al. Perception of coercion in the admission of voluntary and involuntary psychiatric patients. Int J Law Psychiatry 1997; 20: 167–81.Google Scholar
28 Kjellin, L, Andersson, K, Bartholdson, E, Candefjord, IL, Holmstrøm, H, Jacobsson, L, et al. Coercion in psychiatric care – patients' and relatives' experiences from four Swedish psychiatric services. Nord J Psychiatry 2004; 58: 153–9.Google Scholar
29 Eriksson, K, Westrin, C-G. Coercive measures in psychiatric care. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995; 92: 225–30.Google Scholar
30 Bindman, J, Reid, Y, Szmukler, G, Tiller, J, Thornicroft, G, Leese, M. Perceived coercion at admission to psychiatric hospital and engagement with follow-up: a cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2005; 40: 160–6.Google Scholar
31 Ivar Iversen, K, Høyer, G, Sexton, H, Grønli, OK. Perceived coercion among patients admitted to acute wards in Norway. Nord J Psychiatry 2002; 56: 433–9.Google Scholar
32 Soergaard, K. Patients' perception of coercion on acute psychiatric wards: an interview study. Nord J Psychiatry 2004; 58: 299304.Google Scholar
33 Shannon, P. Coercion and compulsory hospitalization: some patients' attitudes. Med J Aust 1976; 2: 798800.Google Scholar
34 Svensson, B, Hansson, N. Patients satisfaction with inpatient psychiatric care. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1994; 90: 379–84.Google Scholar
35 McKenna, B, Simpson, A, Coverdale, J. Patients' perceptions of coercion on admission to forensic psychiatric hospital: a comparison study. Int J Law Psychiatry 2003; 26: 355–72.Google Scholar
36 Kousmanen, L, Hätönen, H, Malkavaara, H, Kylmä, J, Välimäki, M. Deprivation of liberty in psychiatric hospital care: the patient's perspective. Nurs Ethics 2007; 14: 597607.Google Scholar
37 Bonsack, C, Borgeat, F. Perceived coercion and need for hospitalization related to psychiatric admission. Int J Law Psychiatry 2005; 28: 342–7.Google Scholar
38 Olofsson, B, Jacobsson, N. A plea for respect: involuntarily hospitalized psychiatric patients' narratives about being subjected to coercion. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs 2001; 8: 357–66.Google Scholar
39 Kallert, T, Glöckner, M, Onchev, G, Raboch, J, Karastergiou, A, Solomon, Z, et al. The EUNOMIA project on coercion in psychiatry: study design and preliminary data. World Psychiatry 2005; 4: 168–72.Google Scholar
40 Burns, T, Yeeles, K, Molodynski, A, Nightingale, H, Vazquez-Montes, M, Sheehan, K, et al. Pressure to adhere to treatment (‘leverage’) in English mental healthcare. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199: 145–50.Google Scholar
41 Thompson, S, Higgins, J. How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat Med 2002; 21: 1559–73.Google Scholar
42 Lidz, C. Coercion in psychiatric care: what have we learned from research? J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 1998; 26: 631–7.Google Scholar
43 Newton-Howes, G, Weaver, T, Tyrer, P. Attitudes of staff towards patients with personality disorder in community mental health teams. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2008; 42: 572–7.Google Scholar
44 Olsen, C. Towards an ethical standard for coerced mental health treatment: least restrictive or most therapeutic? J Clin Ethics 1998; 9: 235–46.Google Scholar
45 Newton-Howes, G, Mullen, R. Coercion in psychiatric care: systematic review of correlations and themes. Psychiatr Serv 2011; 62: 465–70.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Papers included in the final summarya

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Odds ratios from meta-regression analysis for experiencing subjective coercion

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.