Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T05:54:14.483Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Novel word learning ability in 24-month-olds: The interactive role of mother’s work status and education level

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2024

Rong HUANG*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational and Counseling Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, USA Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, University of Connecticut, USA
Tianlin WANG
Affiliation:
Department of Educational and Counseling Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, USA
*
Corresponding author: Rong Huang; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Using both online and offline measures, this study investigates how maternal education and work status (stay-at-home, part-time, full-time) are jointly associated with infants’ word learning ability and vocabulary size. One hundred 24-month-old infants completed a lab-based mutual exclusivity task, which assesses infants’ novel word learning ability. Caregivers reported infants’ productive vocabulary size using the MCDIs. There was no evidence for an association between infants’ productive vocabulary size and maternal education, maternal work status, or their interaction. However, infants’ novel word learning ability was significantly related to both maternal factors and their interaction. The positive association between maternal education and word learning performance was attenuated for infants of part-time and full-time working mothers compared to infants with at home mothers. These findings suggest that using real-time measures with high task demand may better capture developmental differences in infants and expand our understanding of maternal factors contributing to early language development.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In 2019, 55.4% of women who were older than 16 were employed, and 49.3% of them were mothers. Of these working mothers, nearly two-thirds (63.8%) had children under the age of 3, according to a report from the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2021). Previous surveys have shown that for families with working mothers and children under age 5, child care arrangements are split between relying on other familial members (29.3% fathers, 42.1% grandparents, siblings, or other relatives) and child care settings (53%) (Laughlin, Reference Laughlin2013). Although there is no consistent evidence showing that mother’s entering the workforce has a negative impact on children’s development (Brooks-Gunn et al., Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010; Côté et al., Reference Côté, Boivin, Nagin, Japel, Xu, Zoccolillo, Junger and Tremblay2007), mothers continue to report guilt and shame when returning to work postpartum (Hoffman, Reference Hoffman1974; Segura, Reference Segura, Glenn, Chang and Forcey2016).

The discussion regarding how maternal employment influences child development is far from settled. Studies have shown that children of working mothers who use non-maternal care resources may have better language outcomes due to the richer language input compared to children who stay at home, while their mother is working (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Myers, Rosenthal and Ginsburg1986; Yoshikawa, Reference Yoshikawa1999). Meanwhile, modeling work based on large samples has also suggested that maternal employment, especially in the case of mothers returning to work in the first year postpartum, relates to a decline in children’s later language and cognitive scores (Baum, Reference Baum2003; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Waldfogel, Brooks-Gunn and Han2005; Waldfogel et al., Reference Waldfogel, Han and Brooks-Gunn2002). A recent longitudinal study has found that infants who experienced a change in their care due to a shift in their mother’s work status (i.e., from home to part-time, home to full-time, or part-time to full-time) were more likely to have better language outcomes by 18 months of age compared to infants with stay-at-home mothers and full-time working mothers (Laing & Bergelson, Reference Laing and Bergelson2019). Laing and Bergelson (Reference Laing and Bergelson2019) speculated that this effect may be due to the improved variability of language input. Social constructivism theory posits that language development is a result of social and cultural environments, emphasizing the importance of caregiver-child interactions and caregiver assistance (Kaufman, Reference Kaufman2004; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello and Bavin2009). Maternal employment can thus be viewed as a critical environmental factor influencing language development as it impacts the amount and quality of infant-mother interactions, caregiver assistance, as well as language input received by children (Baum, Reference Baum2003; Booth et al., Reference Booth, Clarke-Stewart, Vandell, McCartney and Owen2002).

Compared to maternal employment, another family-related factor that has consistently shown a positive relationship with child language development is socioeconomic status (SES) (Hoff, Reference Hoff2003; Letourneau et al., Reference Letourneau, Duffett-Leger, Levac, Watson and Young-Morris2013; Pungello et al., Reference Pungello, Iruka, Dotterer, Mills-Koonce and Reznick2009). SES refers to a family’s social and economic standing, which may be indicated by household income, parental occupation, parental education level, or combinations of these factors. According to bioecological systems theory, a child’s development is influenced by multiple levels of the surrounding environment (e.g., home, neighborhood, community, government, etc.) and their interactions. Both SES and maternal employment can be viewed as components of the distal environment that impact a child’s development by interacting with their immediate environment (e.g., family, school; Bronfenbrenner & Evans, Reference Bronfenbrenner and Evans2000; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner1998). For instance, a mother’s employment status and education level, though not part of the child’s immediate family structure, can influence children’s language development indirectly through influencing the time and quality of a child’s at home interactions with the mother. Moreover, prior research has suggested that SES may modify the associations between maternal employment and developmental outcomes (Baum, Reference Baum2003; Hoff, Reference Hoff2003; Letourneau et al., Reference Letourneau, Duffett-Leger, Levac, Watson and Young-Morris2013). Though recent studies have begun to focus on how these familial factors (i.e., maternal employment and SES) relate to, and may even be responsible for language outcome differences (Berry et al., Reference Berry, Blair, Willoughby, Garrett-Peters, Vernon-Feagans and Mills-Koonce2016; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hendry, Gibson, Gliga, McGillion and Gonzalez-Gomez2021; Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014), it remains unclear if maternal work status and education are associated with infant’s language development differentially. More research is needed to explore these relationships to better understand the complex interplay of factors that influence language development in infants.

Importantly, the existing studies relating maternal factors to infants’ language outcomes primarily relied on offline measures, such as parental reports of vocabulary size to assess young children’s language ability. Such methods may overlook a potentially important area of examination because recent evidence suggests that lab-based online measures are more sensitive and reliable in capturing variations in language development than parental reports. This is because parental reports rely heavily on the parents’ familiarity with and sensitivity to their children’s language development, which affects their reliability (Laing & Bergelson, Reference Laing and Bergelson2019). This underscores the concern that using online reliable measures of language abilities is essential to ensure the validity of the findings regarding maternal factors and child language. To address this issue, the current study uses both offline and online measures to examine how maternal work status and maternal education attainment relate to 24-month-olds’ language learning ability.

Maternal employment and child development

The impact of maternal employment on young children’s development has been studied extensively, although its effects remain a contentious debate among researchers. Some studies claim that children benefit from their mothers being employed (Gregg et al., Reference Gregg and Washbrook2003; Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014; Vandell & Ramanan, Reference Vandell and Ramanan1992), while a body of research suggests that maternal employment (or mothers returning to the workplace) in infants’ early life is negatively associated with child outcomes, including vocabulary, reading skills, and mathematical skills (e.g., Baum, Reference Baum2003; Berger et al., Reference Berger, Brooks-Gunn, Paxson and Waldfogel2008; Brooks-Gunn et al., Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2002, Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Waldfogel, Brooks-Gunn and Han2005; Waldfogel et al., Reference Waldfogel, Han and Brooks-Gunn2002). However, it is important to note that the negative associations between maternal employment and children’s later outcomes were mainly limited to maternal employment during the first year of infants’ lives (e.g., Baum, Reference Baum2003; Waldfogel et al., Reference Waldfogel, Han and Brooks-Gunn2002). Once children grow beyond one year of age, maternal employment in the second and third year of their life yields a positive effect on cognitive skills at 7 or 8 years of age (Waldfogel et al., Reference Waldfogel, Han and Brooks-Gunn2002). Brooks-Gunn et al. (Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2002) further specified that the negative association between maternal employment and children’s outcome is most pronounced during the first 9 months after childbirth. In addition, the type of maternal employment matters. Brooks-Gunn et al. (Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010) demonstrated that mothers’ full-time employment during infant’s first year of life is negatively associated with cognitive outcomes later on, whereas mothers’ part-time employment has a positive effect on children’s cognitive development. Similarly, Waldfogel et al. (Reference Waldfogel, Han and Brooks-Gunn2002) analyzed a large-scale NICHD sample and demonstrated that maternal employment with more than 21 hours per week in the first year may negatively affect children’s vocabulary outcomes in later years.

The above-mentioned negative associations between early maternal employment and later child outcome have received strong criticism from researchers. Many studies have shown that maternal employment has a positive effect on child development and brought out that mothers who are employed tend to be more educated and have higher incomes than those who do not go back to work, all of which in turn positively relate to child outcomes (Gregg et al., Reference Gregg and Washbrook2003; Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014; Vandell & Ramanan, Reference Vandell and Ramanan1992). In addition, working mothers tend to send their children to high quality center-based child cares and exhibit more maternal sensitivities (Brilli et al., Reference Brilli, Del Boca and Pronzato2013; Brooks-Gunn et al., Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010). Children of working mothers who use non-maternal care resources (i.e., center-based child care, home-based care, other caregivers’ care) may benefit from a richer and more diverse language input, such as interactions with peers and teachers, various educational toys and books, and thus develop better language skills (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Myers, Rosenthal and Ginsburg1986; Yoshikawa, Reference Yoshikawa1999). Moreover, studies have indicated that employed mothers score higher on maternal aptitude tests, which positively predict children’s reading achievement (Vandell & Ramanan, Reference Vandell and Ramanan1992).

Although being employed may take mothers’ time and energy away from maternal care, and possibly decrease the quantity and quality of mother-child interactions (Baum, Reference Baum2003; see Heinrich, Reference Heinrich2014 for review), there remains a large number of variabilities for families. A number of studies have shown that working mothers place more emphasis on the quality rather than quantity of shared activities, such as educational and structured playtime with their children (Booth et al., Reference Booth, Clarke-Stewart, Vandell, McCartney and Owen2002; Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014). For hetero co-parenting families with working mothers, fathers’ engagement in parenting and interactions with children tend to increase, which compensate for maternal employment and benefit child development (Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014). Altogether, the specific association between maternal employment and children’s later outcomes depends on when the mother is employed, what type of employment, and how the family manages child care when the mother is not available.

Socioeconomic status and language development

While a few studies have failed to detect SES variations in child language outcomes by highlighting child vocalization counts (Piot et al., Reference Piot, Havron and Cristia2022; Sperry et al., Reference Sperry, Sperry and Miller2019), a large number of studies support the pattern that SES is positively associated with children’s language skills and development (Farah & Noble, Reference Farah, Noble, Mayr, Awh and Keele2005; Letourneau et al., Reference Letourneau, Duffett-Leger, Levac, Watson and Young-Morris2013; Noble et al., Reference Noble, Farah and McCandliss2006; Pungello et al., Reference Pungello, Iruka, Dotterer, Mills-Koonce and Reznick2009). A meta-analysis has shown that SES as a composite measure is positively related to language and literacy development among children aged 3 to 12 years old across different cultures (Letourneau et al., Reference Letourneau, Duffett-Leger, Levac, Watson and Young-Morris2013). As early as 18 months, infants from higher-SES families (i.e., based on maternal education and occupation) tend to outperform their peers from lower-SES families on productive vocabulary size and language processing efficiency (Fernald et al., Reference Fernald, Marchman and Weisleder2013; Hoff, Reference Hoff2003; Rescorla & Alley, Reference Rescorla and Alley2001). As children’s age increases, their vocabulary gap becomes wider (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995). Beyond lexical knowledge, SES is also related to grammatical development, especially for complex sentences, with children from higher SES backgrounds (i.e., identified by maternal education as higher than college degree) producing more complex sentences in their speech than peers with mothers whose educational attainment was equivalent to college or less (Vasilyeva et al., Reference Vasilyeva, Waterfall and Huttenlocher2008). Additionally, studies suggest that children from higher-SES families have better ability to express their thinking and ideas compared to their middle- and lower-SES peers (Pappas et al., Reference Pappas, Ginsburg and Jiang2003; Pungello et al., Reference Pungello, Iruka, Dotterer, Mills-Koonce and Reznick2009).

The possible variations in language development across SES strata are primarily due to the varied language environment children experience (Hoff, Reference Hoff2003). SES backgrounds may lead to varying amounts of economic resources children are exposed to at home. For example, economically advantaged families tend to have more language learning resources, such as more children’s books and longer or more frequent reading time between parent and child (Bradley et al., Reference Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo and García Coll2001). The stimulating experiences provide a positive learning environment, which facilitates children’s language development (Linver et al., Reference Linver, Brooks-Gunn and Kohen2002). In addition, parents from more educated and advantaged backgrounds tend to provide a greater amount and higher quality of language input (e.g., higher lexical diversity and syntactic complexity) when directly interacting with their children (not overheard talk) than parents with less educated and disadvantaged backgrounds (Dailey & Bergelson, Reference Dailey and Bergelson2022; Hoff, Reference Hoff2003; Rowe, Reference Rowe2018). The high-quality language input may better attract children’s attention or highlight critical components in speech and therefore facilitate infants’ language learning and development (e.g., Fernald & Mazzie, Reference Fernald and Mazzie1991; Hoff, Reference Hoff2003).

Interaction between maternal employment and maternal education

Informed by bioecological systems theory and socio constructivism theory, both maternal employment and SES can be regarded as playing important roles in child development. Prior studies suggest that maternal employment and SES may interactively impact children’s development, including language development (Baum, Reference Baum2003; Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014). For instance, high SES may function as a mitigating factor in the relationship between maternal employment and child outcomes. In the event that maternal employment brings a negative impact, a high family income may counteract the negative effects of maternal employment on an infant’s development at an early stage (Baum, Reference Baum2003). This can be explained by the fact that families with higher income are more likely to send their infants to high-quality child care centers, which offsets the missed interactions with mothers due to maternal work. Similarly, highly educated mothers are more likely to balance child care and work well compared to mothers with low educational attainment (Hsin & Felfe, Reference Hsin and Felfe2014), and be more engaged and responsive when interacting with their children (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Weinert, von Maurice and Attig2022).

Similarly, maternal work status may modify the associations between SES/maternal education and children’s language development. When mothers work either full time or part time and children experience nonmaternal care, it can serve as a protective factor against the potential negative impacts of impoverished home environments on child outcomes (Berry et al., Reference Berry, Blair, Willoughby, Garrett-Peters, Vernon-Feagans and Mills-Koonce2016; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Hendry, Gibson, Gliga, McGillion and Gonzalez-Gomez2021; Watamura et al., Reference Watamura, Phillips, Morrissey, McCartney and Bub2011). For instance, research suggests that there may be a connection between children receiving care outside the home and a decrease in aggressive behaviors, particularly in children whose mothers have obtained low educational attainment (Côté et al., Reference Côté, Boivin, Nagin, Japel, Xu, Zoccolillo, Junger and Tremblay2007), and that this is likely due to the quality interaction and guidance provided by other caregivers (Belsky, Reference Belsky2006; Mashburn et al., Reference Mashburn, Pianta, Hamre, Downer, Barbarin, Bryant, Burchinal, Early and Howes2008). Conversely, when mothers are highly educated and available, they can offer diverse and high-quality language input, engaging children in interactive activities during shared reading, such as asking questions or providing feedback (Hart & Risley, Reference Hart and Risley1995; Magnuson et al., Reference Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean and Huston2009; Tracey & Young, Reference Tracey and Young2002). Consequently, infants tend to benefit from the presence and engagement of their highly educated mothers, especially when they are stay-at-home mothers.

Less educated and low-income working parents often have to contend with unstable work schedules, financial strain, and limited time and effort to maintain high-quality interactions with their children (Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017; Newland et al., Reference Newland, Crnic, Cox and Mills-Koonce2013). Thus, children, including those from economically diverse backgrounds, may experience developmental advantages associated with maternal employment, such as enhanced language development, improved social skills, and a reduction in aggressive tendencies (Mashburn et al., Reference Mashburn, Pianta, Hamre, Downer, Barbarin, Bryant, Burchinal, Early and Howes2008; Yoshikawa, Reference Yoshikawa1999). The potential benefits for children’s development may be linked to the increased opportunities for engaging with skilled educators and caregivers that can come with maternal employment. Taken together, it is very likely that maternal education and employment have an interaction effect on children’s language abilities, while it remains unclear how their interaction is associated with children’s language learning.

The current study

Based on bioecological systems theory and previous empirical studies, both maternal work status and education level are considered significant factors in predicting infants’ language development. These factors are closely related to the language environment that infants are exposed to (Dailey & Bergelson, Reference Dailey and Bergelson2022; Rowe, Reference Rowe2012). A significant number of studies have demonstrated the associations between the individual maternal factors (maternal education or employment) and language outcomes (Gregg et al., Reference Gregg and Washbrook2003; Laing & Bergelson, Reference Laing and Bergelson2019; Magnuson et al., Reference Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean and Huston2009). However, to the best of our knowledge, no study has examined the combined influences of both maternal factors on infants’ language development. To obtain a comprehensive understanding of the relationship between maternal factors and early language development, it is crucial to take into account the interactive contributions from various maternal factors. In addition, most existing studies used offline measures for language development, such as comprehension or productive vocabulary size based on parental reports, to assess infants’ language abilities in relation to maternal education and/or work status (e.g., Brooks-Gunn et al., Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010; Magnuson et al., Reference Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean and Huston2009). Compared to parental reports of vocabulary size, a real-time language processing assessment may be more objective and sensitive in reflecting potential learning differences due to variations of maternal factors (Laing & Bergelson, Reference Laing and Bergelson2019).

Using a Mutual Exclusivity (ME) task, Bion et al. (Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013) demonstrated that while 2-year-olds as a group can reliably identify the correct object among two items upon hearing a label, only some of the participants performed at above-chance level in a retention task that involved novel word learning. The large individual differences within this age group on the ME-based novel word learning task is of particular interest for two reasons. The first is that previous studies, together with Bion et al. (Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013), have suggested a partial dissociation between differentiating familiar objects and learning new words (e.g., Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008), and that MCDI-based parental reports were not designed to assess the latter. In addition, modeling work has shown that the learning process for retaining novel object-label mapping differs from that of disambiguation (McMurray et al., Reference McMurray, Horst and Samuelson2012), and that a retention task based on novel word learning is more challenging. This more challenging aspect of lexical development, however, has not been investigated within the context of maternal care and education.

The main goal of the current study is to examine the interaction between maternal work status (i.e., at home, part-time working, full-time working) and maternal education attainment, and how they relate to 24-month-old infants’ language abilities using both online and offline language measures. We have two specific research questions that we aim to address in this study:

  1. 1) Does the interaction of maternal employment and education attainment correlate with both online and offline measures of language abilities in 24-month-old infants? Based on previous evidence and the nature of the assessment of infants’ language abilities, we hypothesize that the interaction between maternal employment and education is more likely to be associated with infants’ ability to learn novel words online, compared to their parent-reported productive vocabulary size.

  2. 2) How do maternal employment and education interact with each other and relate to language learning in 24-month-old children? Based on theoretical frameworks and prior research, we expect that the positive association between mothers’ educational level and infants’ language learning will be influenced by mother’s work status. Specifically, we expect the association between maternal education and infants’ language learning to be most pronounced among infants with stay-at-home mothers.

Crucial to our interest, we have implemented the more challenging, retention, trials in an ME task to assess real-time utilization of skills related to lexical development. We examined 24-month-old infants for two reasons: 1) since previous studies reported negative effects of maternal employment on child development when infants were younger than one year old and positive effects when they enter the 2nd and 3rd year of their lives, we should expect that as infants grow and amass more linguistic knowledge, this older age group may start to benefit from maternal employment and child care experience in all aspects of language development; 2) the more challenging ME-based retention trials from Bion et al. (Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013) were not learnable to 18-month-olds, whereas some 24-month-olds were able to succeed in the task. This age group would therefore allow us to investigate the potential effects in infant language development with abundant developmental differences.

Method

Participants

One hundred and eight 24-month-old infants participated in the study (M = 24.5 months; range = 24.02 – 27.07 months, 59 females). All participants were typically developing children from monolingual English-speaking families in a Midwestern city in the U.S. The racial and ethnic composition of the participating families was comparable to that of the local demographic distribution, with most of the participants (89.5%) being Caucasian, 2% Hispanic, 2% Black, and 6.5% from multiracial backgrounds. Most of the parents obtained a college degree (mothers, 69%; fathers, 64%). Over half of the mothers (59%) reported having a job at the time of study participation and the rest (41%) reported being stay-at-home moms. The majority of the fathers (91%) reported having a job and 3 fathers were unemployed; 6 fathers did not report their job status. Caregivers reported household income on a 7-point scale: 1 = <$24999, 2 = $25000-$49999, 3 = $50000~$74999, 4 = $75000~$99999, 5 = $100000~$124999, 6 = $125000~$149999, 7 = >150000, and the average household income reported was 3.49, indicating an average household income for the sample was around $75000. Recruitment was done at the local children’s museum, a baby and families fair, and through free hospital birthing and neonatal care classes to ensure inclusivity of different SES backgrounds. No participants reported hearing or speech problems. Participating families received monetary compensation for their time.

Measures

Maternal education

Parents completed a survey reporting basic family background information including maternal education, as well as their occupations. Mothers chose the highest degree they have earned from seven options: 1 = eighth grade completion, 2 = high school diploma, 3 = two-year college degree, 4 = some college, 5 = four-year college degree, 6 = master’s degree, 7 = doctoral degree. Their responses were then coded into the years of education completed: 1 = 8 years of education, 2 = 12 years of education, 3 = 14 years of education, 4 = 15 years of education, 5 = 16 years of education, 6 = 18 years of education, 7 = 22 years of education. Two raters checked all participants’ input and coding; interrater agreement reached 100%.

Maternal work status

In the demographics survey, parents provided their current occupation and work hours. Responses in the maternal occupation question including “unemployed”, “caregiver”, or “stay-at-home mom” were coded as “At Home”. Other responses that indicated a specific occupation were coded based on reported working hours. Following Laing and Bergelson (Reference Laing and Bergelson2019), mothers who worked longer than 30 hours per week were coded as “Full Time”, and those who worked 5 to 28 hours per week were coded as the “Part Time”. Two raters checked all participants’ input and coding; interrater agreement reached 100%.

Productive vocabulary

Productive vocabulary was measured using a standardized parental report vocabulary checklist: the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words & Sentences (MCDI; Fenson et al., Reference Fenson, Marchman, Thal, Dale, Reznick and Bates2006). Parents were asked to identify the words that they thought their child is able to produce in daily life. The identified words parents reported were counted and used for participants’ productive vocabulary score.

Novel word learning ability

Infants’ novel word learning ability was measured by a mutual exclusivity (ME) task (adopted from Bion et al., Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013) and took place in the lab. The visual stimuli were pictures of six familiar objects (truck, bubbles, blanket, cheese, balloon, spoon) and two novel objects (see Fig. 1), each centered on a gray background in a 640 × 480 pixel space. The speech stimuli were sentences consisting of brief carrier frames that each ended in the name for one of the six familiar objects or two novel objects (e.g., modi and dofa), followed by simple questions that introduced prosodic variability across trials (e.g., Where is the truck? Can you see it?). A female native speaker of American English first recorded multiple tokens of each sentence. The duration of the target nouns and the intensity of the phrases was normalized using Praat speech analysis software (Boersma, Reference Boersma2001).

Figure 1. Three types of trials in the Mutual Exclusivity (ME) experiment.

Procedure

Accuracy in identifying the correct target picture was assessed using the Looking-While-Listening procedure (see Fernald et al., Reference Fernald, Zangl, Portillo, Marchman, Sekerina, Fernández and Clahsen2008). Participants sat on their caregiver’s lap and viewed pictures of objects as they listened to speech naming one of the pictures. On each trial, a pair of pictures was presented on the screen for approximately 6s, with the speech stimuli starting after 2s, followed by 1s of silence. Each infant was presented with 28 trials, consisting of three different trial types (Fig. 1): on twelve Training trials, each novel object served as the target six times, with both a familiar and novel object presented during labeling. On eight Recognition trials, each novel object was paired with a familiar object, and the familiar objects were named during labeling. On eight Retention trials, both novel objects were shown side by side, with one of them named during labeling.

The Recognition and Retention trials were interspersed after the Training trials. The target object was named only once per trial. Pairings of novel and familiar objects were counterbalanced across participants. The side of presentation of the target was also randomized, with the constraint that the target did not appear on the same side of the screen in more than two consecutive trials. To maintain attention, six filler trials with colorful and visually complex scenes appeared after every four trials, accompanied by attention-getting phrases such as “Hey, look at that! That’s cool!” spoken in a child-directed manner. Caregivers wore opaque glasses so that they could not influence infants’ looking behavior. The entire procedure lasted 5 minutes.

Coding and analysis

Infants’ looking behavior during the trials was video-recorded. Trained coders who were unaware of the experimental conditions analyzed videos of infants’ eye movements at 33 ms intervals. They recorded whether the child was looking at the left or right image, shifting between images, or off-task (Fernald et al., Reference Fernald, Zangl, Portillo, Marchman, Sekerina, Fernández and Clahsen2008). The study excluded trials where the infant was not looking at either image at noun onset (24.8% of the dataset) or looked away for more than 1000 ms continuously within the 3000 ms analysis window (26.7% of trials). A table with summary statistics for the included trials by three maternal work status subgroups is provided in Appendix A. Twenty-five percent of trials from all participants were randomly selected and independently coded for whether an infant was looking at the picture on the left or the picture on right, transitioning between pictures, or off task. The agreement between coders within a single frame was greater than 99%.

Consistent with previous studies, trials in which infants were looking away from both pictures or shifting (i.e., a rapid change of gaze) from one to the other were not included in these analyses (Fernald et al., Reference Fernald, Pinto, Swingley, Weinbergy and McRoberts1998, Reference Fernald, Perfors and Marchman2006). The entire looking behavior since target word onset was captured, though the accuracy before 300 ms was not included since shifts to the target occurring in this window had presumably been initiated before the onset of the noun (Haith et al., Reference Haith, Wentworth, Canfield, Rovee-Collier and Lipsitt1993). Following the rationale in Bion et al. (Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013), we also adopted a longer window (3300ms, which encompasses the entire trial duration) because on the majority of trials the visual stimuli included one or two novel objects, which elicited more shifting back and forth between target and distractor than do sequences of trials on which only familiar objects are presented. Following their approach (Bion et al., Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013, p. 43), when the infant was gazing at a picture at the beginning of the speech stimulus during the trials, their precision was measured by calculating the duration of their fixation on the intended object, divided by their fixation on both the intended object and the distractor, between 300 to 3300 ms after the target word was presented.

Mean accuracy was then computed for each participant on each trial type as the mean proportion of time looking to the target divided by the mean proportion of time looking to the target or to the distracter. Though our primary interest lies in the results from the Retention trials, which are the most challenging, we also analyzed the Training trials to ensure that learning has occurred.

Data preparation

Four participants were excluded from the analysis because they did not report either maternal education or maternal occupations. In the rest of 104 participants, 13 parents (12.5%) had not reported the CDI vocabulary, and 26 participants (25%) had missing data in ME retention trials. Little’s MCAR test indicated that the data were missed completely at random, χ2 (8, N = 76) = 3.583, p =. 694. Expectation Maximization (EM) was used to deal with the missing data. In the full data with EM, Shapiro–Wilks tests indicated that the distribution of productive vocabulary was normally distributed, W =. 982, p >. 05, while the ME performance was not normally distributed, W =. 959, p <. 01. Four outliers on ME retention trials were then removed from the dataset (skewness & kurtosis, out of the range from -2 to +2; George & Mallery, Reference George and Mallery2010), and 100 participants were included in the final analytical sample. The demographic information of the analytic sample was summarized in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic Information for the Analytic Sample (N = 100)

Results

Descriptive analysis

All analyses were conducted in SPSS 25.0 (IBM Corp, 2017). The reported productive vocabulary size ranged from 10 to 645, and the mean of the sample’s productive vocabulary size was 279.47, SD = 146.59. The mean accuracy of the sample’s novel word learning performance was. 53, SD =. 11. Table 2 provides descriptive statistics for the three maternal employment groups in terms of child age, maternal education, productive vocabulary, and word learning ability. There was no significant difference among the at home, part-time, and full-time working groups apropos of child age, productive vocabulary, and novel word learning performance. However, mothers with a higher education level tended to work more hours a week, F (2, 97) = 5.12, p =. 008, η2 =. 10. Productive vocabulary and word learning performance were closely connected, r =. 403, p <. 001. This is expected and consistent with previous studies (e.g., Bion et al., Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013). Maternal education was not significantly associated with productive vocabulary, r =. 053, p =. 604, but significantly associated with word learning performance, r =. 207, p =. 039. When examining the correlations between maternal education and novel word learning across maternal employment subgroups, results showed that only at home mothers’ education, but not full-time or part-time working mothers’ education, was related to infants’ novel word learning performance, r =. 598, p <. 001.

Table 2. Means (and SDs) of Child Age, Maternal Education, Productive Vocabulary and Novel Word Learning Performance in Three Maternal Employment Groups

Note. * p <.05, ** p <.01, *** p <.001.

Interaction of maternal factors and productive vocabulary

To examine the association between the interaction of maternal education and work status and children’s productive vocabulary at 24-months, a moderation model controlling for child age was conducted using Model 1 in PROCESS Macro in SPSS (Hayes, Reference Hayes2018). Maternal education was mean centered to reduce multicollinearity. We used the mcw option to tell PROCESS that moderator Maternal Work Status is a multicategorical variable, so it automatically dummy coded the moderator and created interaction terms (Hayes, Reference Hayes2018).

The at home group was set as the reference group; W1 represents the part-time working group, and W2 represents the full-time working group. The model was not significant, accounting for only 3% of the variance in productive vocabulary, $ {R}^2 $ =. 03, F (5, 94) =. 60, p =. 70. The model summary is presented in Table 3. Maternal education and maternal work status were not significant predictors of infant’s productive vocabulary, $ {b}_1 $ = 20.85, t (96) = 1.52, p =. 13; $ {b}_2 $ = 394.48, t (96) = 1.29, p =. 20; $ {b}_3 $ = 270.98, t (96) =. 97, p =. 33. Both interaction terms were not significant, $ {b}_4 $ = -24.83, t (96) = -1.31, p =. 19; $ {b}_5 $ = -19.07, t (96) = -1.11, p =. 27. Moreover, the test of highest order unconditional interactions indicated that adding the interaction terms did not significantly result in incremental variance explained in 24-month-olds’ productive vocabulary, Δ $ {R}^2 $ =. 02, ΔF (2, 94) =. 95, p =. 39. It suggests that there is no evidence of a difference in two-year-olds’ productive vocabulary size as a function of either factors alone or together.

Table 3. Coefficients Results for the Moderation Model with Productive Vocabulary

Note. Dependent Variable: Productive Vocabulary Size. Maternal Ed = Maternal Education; CI = confidence interval. Dummy coding was used on maternal working status, with at home group as reference, W1 = part-time working group; W2 = full-time working group.

Interaction of maternal factors and novel word learning

To ensure that infants were able to reliably identify familiar object-label mappings in this task, we first tested their performance on recognition trials. As one group, a one-sample t-test showed that participants performed above chance (.50) on the recognition trials, M =. 75, SD =.12, t(99) = 20.60, p <. 001, Cohen’s d = 2.08. Consistent with previous studies using the same paradigm (e.g., Bion et al., Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013; Fernald et al., Reference Fernald, Perfors and Marchman2006), infants looked more at the familiar object when they heard its label in this task. Similarly, infants performed above chance level on the training trials, M =. 55, SD =. 12, t(99) = 4.26, p <. 001, Cohen’s d =. 42; and retention trials, M =. 53, SD =. 11, t(99) = 2.55, p =. 006, Cohen’s d =. 27. Since our primary focus was to investigate how infants’ ability to use ME to learn novel word-object mappings may differ due to maternal factors, we analyzed their performance on the retention trials as well as the recognition and training trials by maternal employment subgroups. For these a priori one-sample t-tests, we treated infants’ looking behavior as the dependent variable and compared it to the chance performance (.50).

All three maternal employment subgroups showed above chance level performance on the recognition trials: at home group, M =. 77, SD =. 11, t(40) = 15.80, p <. 001, Cohen’s d = 2.45; part-time working group, M =. 72, SD =. 15, t(28) = 7.68, p <. 001, Cohen’s d = 1.47; full-time working group, M =. 77, SD =. 11, t(29) = 13.97, p <. 001, Cohen’s d = 2.45. Similarly, the three subgroups performed better than chance on the training trials: at home group, M =. 56, SD =. 08, t(40) = 4.46, p <. 001, Cohen’s d =. 75; part-time working group, M =. 55, SD =. 15, t(28) = 1.77, p =. 043, Cohen’s d =. 33; full-time working group, M =. 54, SD =. 13, t(29) = 1.74, p =. 046, Cohen’s d =. 31. These patterns suggest that infants across different maternal employment status were able to look longer at familiar objects when they heard the familiar labels and looked more at the unfamiliar objects when they heard a novel label. For the retention trials, only the part-time working group significantly performed above chance, M =. 55, SD =. 09, t(28) = 2.75, p =. 005, Cohen’s d =. 56. Infants with at home mothers and full-time working mothers performed at the chance level in the retention trials: at home group, M =. 51, SD =. 11, t(40) =. 85, p =. 20, Cohen’s d =. 09; full-time working mothers, M =. 53, SD =. 13, t(29) = 1.20, p =. 12, Cohen’s d =. 23. A plot illustrating the three maternal employment subgroups’ looking behavior in the retention trials is included in Appendix B.

Next, we conducted a similar moderation model through PROCESS Macro with novel word learning performance as a dependent variable. Since productive vocabulary size was associated with novel word learning performance, infants’ productive vocabulary size was entered as a covariate here. The model results are summarized in Table 4. Overall, the model was significant, accounting for 28.6% of the variance in novel word learning performance, $ {R}^2 $ =. 286, F (6, 93) = 6.21, p <. 001. Maternal education was a significant predictor of infant’s novel word learning performance, $ {b}_1 $ =. 03, t (95) = 3.55, p <. 001. Similarly, part-time work status in mothers was associated to marginally better novel word learning performance than at home group, $ {b}_2 $ =. 39, t (95) = 1.95, p =. 054; and full-time work status was significantly related to stronger novel word learning in infants compared to at home group, $ {b}_3 $ =. 59, t (95) = 3.25, p =. 002. The interaction term W1*Maternal Education was not significant, $ {b}_4 $ = -.02, t (95) = -1.90, p =. 06, suggesting that the slopes relating maternal education to novel word learning in the at home and part-time working groups did not significantly differ from each other. However, the interaction term W2* Maternal Education was significant, $ {b}_5 $ = -.04, t (95) = -3.24, p =. 002, indicating that the relation between maternal education and novel word learning for the at home and full-time working groups significantly differed from each other. Moreover, the test of highest order unconditional interactions indicated that adding the interaction terms significantly resulted in an incremental variance explained in 24-month-olds’ novel word learning performance, Δ $ {R}^2 $ =. 08, ΔF (2, 93) = 5.26, p =.007.

Table 4. Coefficients Results for the Moderation Model with Novel Word Learning (Retention Trials)

Note. Dependent Variable: Novel word learning performance (in Mutual Exclusivity task). Maternal Ed = maternal education; Productive Voc = productive vocabulary; CI = confidence interval. Dummy coding was used on maternal working status, with at home group as reference, W1= part-time working group; W2 = full-time working group.

As shown in Figure 2, maternal work status moderated the association between maternal education and novel word learning in 24-month-olds. Having a higher level of education had the greatest positive association with infant’s novel word learning for the at home group, compared to the other two working groups. The partial correlations between maternal education and novel word learning performance after controlling for productive vocabulary size across three maternal working groups indicated that the positive relation between novel word learning performance and number of years of education completed by mother was strongest for the at home group (rpartial =. 56, p <.001), but the associations were not significant for the part-time (rpartial =.27, p =.173) and full-time working groups (rpartial = -.09, p =.628).

Figure 2. A visual representation of the conditional relationship between years of maternal education (SES) and novel word learning ability (in a Mutual Exclusivity task) as a function of maternal work status.

To further determine if the divergent findings on productive vocabulary and novel word learning were due to the online versus offline measures, we also conducted a moderation analysis on the recognition trials in ME task, which represents an online measure for infants’ ability to recognize familiar words. The model was not significant, accounting for only 5.8% of the variance in recognition trial performance, $ {R}^2 $ =. 058, F (5, 94) = 1.16, p =. 336. As shown in Table 5, both maternal education and employment status were not significant predictors of infant’s recognition accuracy on familiar word-object mappings. In addition, the interaction terms W1*Maternal Education and W2* Maternal Education were also not significant, suggesting that the interaction of maternal education and work status did not significantly influence 24-month-olds’ familiar word recognition ability.

Table 5. Coefficients Results for the Moderation Model with ME Recognition Trials

Note. Dependent Variable: Recognition trials performance (in Mutual Exclusivity task). Maternal Ed = maternal education; CI = confidence interval. Dummy coding was used on maternal working status, with at home group as reference, W1= part-time working group; W2 = full-time working group.

Discussion

The current study investigated how different components of maternal background (maternal education and work status) relate to two-year-olds’ language development, specifically productive vocabulary size and novel word learning ability. Consistent with our hypothesis, while we did not find evidence for differences on infants’ vocabulary size as a function of maternal education or work status, these factors and their interaction were associated with infants’ ability to learn new words. Mother’s education attainment holds a strong positive relationship with infant’s novel word learning ability when mothers stay at home, while this relationship was largely weakened in infants of part-time and full-time working mothers.

Perhaps surprisingly, in our sample, no evidence was found associating infants’ productive vocabulary size with either maternal education level or maternal work status. This is partly inconsistent with previous studies that found a positive correlation between maternal education and children’s vocabulary development (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Miyazaki and Bell2022; Friend et al., Reference Friend, Lopez, De Anda, Abreu-Mendoza and Arias-Trejo2022; Hoff, Reference Hoff2003). Additionally, using longitudinal data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD), Magnuson et al. (Reference Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean and Huston2009) found that increases in maternal education between infants’ 24th to 36th months were associated with infants’ productive and receptive language skills at 36 months. It has also been posited that children of mothers with less educational background could face challenges in language development as these children may experience a lower degree of maternal responsiveness and less maternal language input (Hoff, Reference Hoff2013; Lonigan et al., Reference Lonigan, Farver, Nakamoto and Eppe2013). However, when we considered both maternal education and maternal work status in our study, no evidence for a difference in 24-month-old infants’ concurrent productive vocabulary size was found.

Instead, we found that there was a significant interaction of maternal education and work status on infants’ novel word learning ability, which is measured by the lab-based ME task. By using the more challenging online task, we examined infants’ ability beyond recognizing familiar object-label mappings. One of the possible explanations for the inconsistency is that infants’ productive vocabulary size measured by the MCDIs may not capture certain variations in language abilities related to maternal factors due to its offline nature. As discussed previously, the validity of a parental report depends on parental characteristics, such as how much time the parent spends with the infant, and how much attention they pay to the infant’s language development (Laing & Bergelson, Reference Laing and Bergelson2019). This possibility, however, is proven unlikely to be the main factor contributing to the null result since the recognition trials in the ME task also did not show outcome differences as a function of maternal factors (shown in Table 5). The fact that both an offline task (i.e., MCDI) and an online task (i.e., recognition trials in the ME task) yielded similar findings, and that the differences in language abilities related to maternal factors were exclusively detected in cognitively challenging retention trials suggests that task difficulty may be a more possible explanation. The ME retention trials utilized in this experiment to assess infants’ novel word learning ability involved encoding, retaining, and retrieving the novel word-object mappings, which has been found to result in significant variability among infants of this age group (Bion et al., Reference Bion, Borovsky and Fernald2013).

This is in line with previous research on task difficulty and individual differences. Task difficulty has been found to play a crucial role in revealing individual differences in cognitive abilities (Dodonova & Dodonov, Reference Dodonova and Dodonov2013; Lohman, Reference Lohman and Sternberg2000; Robinson, Reference Robinson2001). For instance, research has demonstrated that high-ability individuals and low-ability individuals tend to show a greater difference in accuracy rates in complex tasks that require higher cognitive demand (Dodonova & Dodonov, Reference Dodonova and Dodonov2013). It is therefore plausible to suggest that tasks with increased difficulty levels can provide valuable insights into understanding individual differences in language development during infancy as well. As such, our findings indicate that this real-time language measure with a high level of task difficulty may be able to detect more nuanced variations in infant’s language development relating to maternal factors.

Extending from previous research, our findings revealed that for two-year-olds whose mothers stayed at home, the ability to learn novel words was positively associated with maternal education, while the association between maternal education and novel word learning was not evident in infants with part-time and full-time working moms – when mothers were working either part-time or full-time, there was no salient connection between maternal education and infant’s language learning performance in the challenging task. This finding goes against the “rich get richer” framework of language development, though the source of differences (or the lack thereof) in infants’ novel word learning ability remains to be identified. One possible explanation is that working mothers may have to seek nonmaternal care for their child when they are not available due to work. Nonmaternal care, such as child care centers, or other family member’s care, may be an equalizer for infants’ language development across families with varying levels of maternal education when mothers are working, which merits further exploration in future studies. Another potential factor to consider is the possibility of shared genetic propensity between mothers and infants, which could be linked to their language learning skills. It is plausible that genetic factors play a role in shaping the language abilities of both mothers and infants (e.g., Dale et al., Reference Dale, Tosto, Hayiou-Thomas and Plomin2015; Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin1977). Future studies investigating maternal factors’ impact on infant language learning should consider the shared genetic propensity.

Ample research demonstrated that parent education is a positive factor associated with the diversity and sophistication of vocabulary used by parents when interacting with infants (Dailey & Bergelson, Reference Dailey and Bergelson2022; Rowe, Reference Rowe2012; Rowe & Snow, Reference Rowe and Snow2012), and that the variability of language input is strongly associated with infants’ language development (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Graham, Prime, Jenkins and Madigan2021; Pancsofar & Vernon-Feagans, Reference Pancsofar and Vernon-Feagans2006). It is also important to note that even in SES-homogeneous samples, there is large variability in the language input parents provided (Hirsh-Pasek et al., Reference Hirsh-Pasek, Adamson, Bakeman, Owen, Golinkoff, Pace, Yust and Suma2015; Rowe, Reference Rowe2018). This suggests that there are additional factors, beyond parental education or SES, that contribute to the quantity and quality of language input provided to infants, which in turn relate to infant’s language skills. Maternal work status could be one of such contributing factors. While there is limited research directly examining the link between different work statuses in mothers and their language input toward infants, studies have shown that working mothers are more likely to utilize center-based child care services (Brooks-Gunn et al., Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010). Moreover, children whose mothers were employed and utilized non-maternal care resources may benefit from increased language input, which ultimately leads to enhanced language abilities (Milne et al., Reference Milne, Myers, Rosenthal and Ginsburg1986; Yoshikawa, Reference Yoshikawa1999).

Implications, limitations, and future directions

This study holds important practical implications for families concerning infants’ language development, especially those who are economically disadvantaged. Mothers who work in infants’ early years often experience feelings of guilt about leaving their child while they are at work, and worry that limited maternal care during infancy could lead to negative child outcomes (Westervelt, Reference Westervelt2018). However, our findings provide evidence that infants who have working mothers with limited education attainment may experience benefits in their language development. Mothers going back to work often leads to higher income, which increases the likelihood of having access to more learning resources at home and being able to afford high-quality nonmaternal care for the child (Nobel, Reference Nobel2015). Access to high-quality nonmaternal care may provide children with diverse and enriching language environments, which could compensate for limited home language environments. More enriched language environments may thus facilitate infant’s language development.

While the study has important implications, it is necessary to acknowledge its limitations as an initial exploration of the interaction between maternal employment and education, and its correlations with infant’s language skills. Firstly, while we have discussed the mechanisms that could potentially explain the associations between maternal factors and infants’ language abilities, the study did not include measures to capture variations in maternal language input, the amount of time spent by mothers with their children, or other environmental factors within the family that may be associated with maternal work status or education. Similarly, the study did not assess the specific types of care or the quality of care that the participating families utilized. This makes it difficult to draw conclusions about the effect of child care attendance on infant language development based solely on maternal employment status. Future studies should incorporate measures to capture and evaluate childcare-related variables to pinpoint the compensatory effect that nonmaternal care may bring. In addition, future studies may collect information about shared genetic propensities between mothers and infants and investigate how the shared genetic propensities could account for the relations between maternal factors and infant’s language learning abilities.

Another limitation of our study is that we did not screen or control for maternal mental health, such as depression and anxiety symptoms. These factors have been shown to have a significant impact on the home language environment and language outcomes of infants (Brookman et al., Reference Brookman, Kalashnikova, Conti, Xu Rattanasone, Grant, Demuth and Burnham2020). Future studies should consider maternal mental health, especially during the postnatal period, when examining the association between maternal factors and infant language learning. Additionally, it is important to note that maternal work status was only reported when the infants were around 24-month-old, and we do not have information on when mothers started to work postpartum. Previous studies have suggested that the timing of maternal work status can have varying effects on child developmental outcomes (Baum, Reference Baum2003; Brooks-Gunn et al., Reference Brooks-Gunn, Han and Waldfogel2010). Specifically, mothers who return to work in the first year after childbirth may lead to more negative outcomes for their children compared to those who return to work at a later stage (Baum, Reference Baum2003; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Waldfogel, Brooks-Gunn and Han2005). Therefore, it would be valuable for future studies to examine the history of maternal employment and investigate the concurrent and longitudinal effects of timing differences of maternal employment on infant’s novel word learning.

Conclusion

The present study provides valuable insight into the associations between interactive maternal factors (maternal education and work status) and infants’ language abilities, as assessed by both offline and online language measures. Our research findings suggest that maternal backgrounds may be associated with differences in infants’ language development, which can be better captured using more demanding real-time language tasks. This highlights the potential of utilizing high-demand tasks beyond the widely used MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MCDIs) even for infants as young as 24 months old. Additionally, this study shares practical implications with mothers in the labor force.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000011.

Acknowledgements

We thank all the members of the Infant Studies Lab at the University of Notre Dame for their contribution to this work. We are grateful to the families who participated in this study. We also thank Wenqian Robertson and Alya Minhas for their contribution on an earlier version. This research was supported by funds from NSF BCS – 1352443.

Competing interest

The authors declare none.

References

Anderson, N. J., Graham, S. A., Prime, H., Jenkins, J. M., & Madigan, S. (2021). Linking quality and quantity of parental linguistic input to child language skills: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 92(2), 484501. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, C. L. (2003). Does early maternal employment harm child development? An analysis of the potential benefits of leave taking. Journal of Labor Economics, 21(2), 409448. https://doi.org/10.1086/345563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (2006). Early child care and early child development: Major findings of the NICHD study of early child care. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3(1), 95110. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620600557755CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, L., Brooks-Gunn, J., Paxson, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2008). First-year maternal employment and child outcomes: Differences across racial and ethnic groups. Children and Youth Services Review, 30(4), 365387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2007.10.010Google Scholar
Berry, D., Blair, C., Willoughby, M., Garrett-Peters, P., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Mills-Koonce, W. R. (2016). Household chaos and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development in early childhood: Does childcare play a buffering role? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 34(1), 115127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.09.003Google Scholar
Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18,24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126(1), 3953. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008Google Scholar
Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5 (9/10), 341345.Google Scholar
Booth, C. L., Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Vandell, D. L., McCartney, K., & Owen, M. T. (2002). Child-care usage and mother-infant “Quality Time.” Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(1), 1626. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00016.xGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., McAdoo, H. P., & García Coll, C. (2001). The home environments of children in the United States part I: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development, 72(6), 18441867. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382Google Scholar
Brilli, Y., Del Boca, D., & Pronzato, C. D. (2013). Does child care availability play a role in maternal employment and children’s development? Evidence from Italy. Review of Economics of the Household, 14(1), 2751. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-013-9227-4Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Evans, G. W. (2000). Developmental science in the 21st century: Emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Social Development, 9(1), 115125. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00114Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 9931028). John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Brookman, R., Kalashnikova, M., Conti, J., Xu Rattanasone, N., Grant, K. A., Demuth, K., & Burnham, D. (2020). Depression and anxiety in the postnatal period: An examination ofinfants’ home language environment, vocalizations, and expressive language abilities. Child Development, 91(6), e1211e1230. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13421Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W. J., & Waldfogel, J. (2002). Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life: The NICHD study of early child care. ChildDevelopment, 73(4), 10521072. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00457Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W. J., & Waldfogel, J. (2010). First-year maternal employment and child development in the first seven years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 75(2), 79. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2010.00562.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, M., Miyazaki, Y., & Bell, M. A. (2022). Infant attention and maternal education are associated with childhood receptive vocabulary development. Developmental Psychology, 58(7), 12071220. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Côté, S. M., Boivin, M., Nagin, D. S., Japel, C., Xu, Q., Zoccolillo, M., Junger, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). The role of maternal education and nonmaternal care services in the prevention of children’s physical aggression problems. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(11), 13051312. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1305Google Scholar
Dailey, S., & Bergelson, E. (2022). Language input to infants of different socioeconomic statuses: A quantitative meta-analysis. Developmental Science, 25(3), e13192. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13192Google Scholar
Dale, P. S., Tosto, M. G., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Plomin, R. (2015). Why does parental language input style predict child language development? A twin study of gene–environment correlation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 57(5),106117. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0021992415000593Google Scholar
Davies, C., Hendry, A., Gibson, S. P., Gliga, T., McGillion, M., & Gonzalez-Gomez, N. (2021). Early childhood education and care (ECEC) during COVID-19 boosts growth in language and executive function. Infant and Child Development, 30(4), e2241. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2241Google Scholar
Dodonova, Y. A., & Dodonov, Y. S. (2013). Faster on easy items, more accurate on difficult ones: Cognitive ability and performance on a task of varying difficulty. Intelligence, 41(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. J., & Noble, K. G. (2005). Socioeconomic influences on brain development: A preliminary study. In Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Keele, S. W. (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner (pp. 189208.) American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2006). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories user’s guide and technical manual (2nd Ed). Brooks Publishing.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234248. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 209221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 98116. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Pinto, J. P., Swingley, D., Weinbergy, A., & McRoberts, G. W. (1998). Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science, 9 ( 3), 228231. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00044Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernández, E. M., & Clahsen, H. (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97135). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.44.06ferCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friend, M., Lopez, O., De Anda, S., Abreu-Mendoza, R. A., & Arias-Trejo, N. (2022). Maternal education revisited: Vocabulary growth in English and Spanish from 16 to 30 months of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 66(2), 101685. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101685Google Scholar
George, D., & Mallery, P. (2010). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple guide and reference 17.0 update (10th Edition). Pearson.Google Scholar
Gregg, P., Washbrook, E., & ALSPAC Study Team. (2003). The effects of early maternal employment on child development in the UK. University of Bristol CMPO Discussion Paper, 03, 070.Google Scholar
Haith, M. M., Wentworth, N., & Canfield, R. (1993). The formation of expectations in early infancy. In Rovee-Collier, C. & Lipsitt, L. (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 8, pp. 251297). Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Heinrich, C. J. (2014). Parents’ employment and children’s wellbeing. The Future of Children, 24(1), 121146. https://doi.org/10.1353/foc.2014.0000Google Scholar
Hill, J. L., Waldfogel, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Han, W. J. (2005). Maternal employment and child development: a fresh look using newer methods. Developmental Psychology, 41(6), 833. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.833CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., Yust, P. K., & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological science, 26(7), 10711083. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 13681378. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49, 414. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027238Google Scholar
Hoffman, L. W. (1974). Effects of maternal employment on the child: A review of the research. Developmental Psychology, 10(2), 204228. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035981Google Scholar
Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention in 24-month-old infants. Infancy, 13(2), 128157. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000701795598Google Scholar
Hsin, A., & Felfe, C. (2014). When does time matter? Maternal employment, children’s time with parents, and child development. Demography, 51(5), 18671894. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-014-0334-5Google Scholar
Huang, W., Weinert, S., von Maurice, J., & Attig, M. (2022). Specific parenting behaviors link maternal education to toddlers’ language and social competence. Journal of Family Psychology, 36(6), 9981009. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000950Google Scholar
IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Kaufman, D. (2004). Constructivist issues in language learning and teaching. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 24, 303319. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190504000121Google Scholar
Laing, C., & Bergelson, E. (2019). Mothers’ work status and 17‐month‐olds’ productive vocabulary. Infancy, 24(1), 101109. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12265Google Scholar
Laughlin, L. (2013). Who’s minding the kids? Child care arrangements: Spring 2011. Current Population Reports, 70135. U.S. Census Bureau. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/prod/2013pubs/p70-135.pdf.Google Scholar
Letourneau, N. L., Duffett-Leger, L., Levac, L., Watson, B., & Young-Morris, C. (2013). Socioeconomic status and child development: A meta-analysis. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 21(3), 211224. https://doi.org/10.1177/1063426611421007Google Scholar
Linver, M. R., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Kohen, D. E. (2002). Family processes as pathways from income to young children’s development. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 719734. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.719Google Scholar
Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence (pp. 285340). (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Farver, J. M., Nakamoto, J., & Eppe, S. (2013). Developmental trajectories of preschool early literacy skills: A comparison of language-minority and monolingual-english children. Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 19431957. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031408Google Scholar
Magnuson, K. A., Sexton, H. R., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Huston, A. C. (2009). Increases in maternal education and young children’s language skills. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 55(3), 319350. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masarik, A. S., & Conger, R. D. (2017). Stress and child development: A review of the Family Stress Model. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13(2), 8590. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mashburn, A. J., Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B. K., Downer, J. T., Barbarin, O. A., Bryant, D., Burchinal, M., Early, D. M., & Howes, C. (2008). Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children’s development of academic, language and social skills. Child Development, 79(3), 732749. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01154.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMurray, B., Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2012). Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review, 119(4), 831877. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029872Google Scholar
Milne, A. M., Myers, D. E., Rosenthal, A. S., & Ginsburg, A. (1986). Single parents, working mothers, and the educational achievement of school children. Sociology of Education, 59(3), 125139. https://doi.org/10.2307/2112335Google Scholar
Newland, R. P., Crnic, K. A., Cox, M. J., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2013). The family model stress and maternal psychological symptoms: Mediated pathways from economic hardship to parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(1), 96105. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031112Google Scholar
Nobel, C. (2015, May 15). Kids benefits from having a working mom. Harvard Business School Working Knowledge. https://hbswk.hbs.edu/item/kids-benefit-from-having-a-working-momGoogle Scholar
Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Socioeconomic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21(3), 349368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.007Google Scholar
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: Contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 571587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.003Google Scholar
Pappas, S., Ginsburg, H. P., & Jiang, M. (2003). SES differences in young children’s metacognition in the context of mathematical problem solving. Cognitive Development, 18(3), 431450. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00043-1Google Scholar
Piot, L., Havron, N., & Cristia, A. (2022). Socioeconomic status correlates with measures of Language Environment Analysis (LENA) system: a meta-analysis. Journal of Child Language, 49(5), 10371051. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000921000441Google Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84(2), 309322. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.84.2.309Google Scholar
Pungello, E. P., Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., Mills-Koonce, R., & Reznick, J. S. (2009). The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013917Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Alley, A. (2001). Validation of the Language Development Survey (LDS): A parent report tool for identifying language delay in toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(2), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/035)Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2001). Task complexity, task difficulty, and task production: Exploring interactions in a componential framework. Applied Linguistics, 22(1), 2757. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/22.1.27Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 17621774. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2018). Understanding socioeconomic differences in parents’ speech to children. Child Development Perspectives, 12(2), 122127. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12271Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., & Snow, C. E. (2012). Analyzing input quality along three dimensions: interactive, linguistic, and conceptual. Journal of Child Language, 47(1), 521. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000655Google Scholar
Segura, D. A. (2016). Working at motherhood: Chicana and Mexican immigrant mothers and employment. In Glenn, E. N., Chang, G., & Forcey, L. R. (Eds.), Mothering: Ideology, experience, and agency (pp. 211233). Routledge.Google Scholar
Sperry, D. E., Sperry, L. L., & Miller, P. J. (2019). Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development, 90(4), 13031318. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13072Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In Bavin, E., (Ed), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 6987). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511576164.005Google Scholar
Tracey, D. H., & Young, J. W. (2002). Mothers’ helping behaviors during children’s at-home oral-reading practice: Effects of children’s reading ability, children’s gender, and mothers’ educational level. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 729737. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.4.729Google Scholar
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2021). Women in the labor force: A databook. Retrieved from https://www.bls.gov/opub/reports/womens-databook/2020/home.htm.Google Scholar
Vandell, D. L., & Ramanan, J. (1992). Effects of early and recent maternal employment on children from low-income families. Child Development, 63(4), 938949. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01673.xGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Emergence of syntax: Commonalities and differences across children. Developmental Science, 11(1), 8497. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00656.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldfogel, J., Han, W. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). The effects of early maternal employment on child cognitive development. Demography, 39(2), 369392. https://doi.org/10.2307/3088344Google Scholar
Watamura, S. E., Phillips, D. A., Morrissey, T. W., McCartney, K., & Bub, K. (2011). Double jeopardy: Poorer social-emotional outcomes for children in the NICHD SECCYD experiencing home and child-care environments that confer risk. Child Development, 82(1), 4865. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01540.xGoogle Scholar
Westervelt, A. (2018). Forget “having it all”: How America messed up motherhood-and how to fix it. Seal Press.Google Scholar
Yoshikawa, H. (1999). Welfare dynamics, support services, mothers’ earnings, and child cognitive development: Implications for contemporary welfare reform. Child Development, 70(3), 779801. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00056Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Three types of trials in the Mutual Exclusivity (ME) experiment.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic Information for the Analytic Sample (N = 100)

Figure 2

Table 2. Means (and SDs) of Child Age, Maternal Education, Productive Vocabulary and Novel Word Learning Performance in Three Maternal Employment Groups

Figure 3

Table 3. Coefficients Results for the Moderation Model with Productive Vocabulary

Figure 4

Table 4. Coefficients Results for the Moderation Model with Novel Word Learning (Retention Trials)

Figure 5

Figure 2. A visual representation of the conditional relationship between years of maternal education (SES) and novel word learning ability (in a Mutual Exclusivity task) as a function of maternal work status.

Figure 6

Table 5. Coefficients Results for the Moderation Model with ME Recognition Trials

Supplementary material: File

Huang and Wang supplementary material

Huang and Wang supplementary material
Download Huang and Wang supplementary material(File)
File 72.4 KB