Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:29:54.288Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Examining associations, moderators and mediators between childhood maltreatment, social functioning, and social cognition in psychotic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2023

Natalia E. Fares-Otero*
Affiliation:
Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Hospital Clínic Barcelona, Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences University of Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain Fundació Clínic per a la Recerca Biomèdica (FCRB), Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Network Centre for Biomedical Research in Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Health Institute Carlos III (ISCIII), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
Luis Alameda
Affiliation:
Service of General Psychiatry, Treatment and Early Intervention in Psychosis Program, University Hospital (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College of London, London, UK Department of Psychiatry, CIBERSAM, Institute of Biomedicine of Sevilla (IBIS), University Hospital Virgen del Rocio, University of Seville, Seville, Spain
Monique C. Pfaltz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Social Work, Mid Sweden University, Östersund, Sweden
Anabel Martinez-Aran
Affiliation:
Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Hospital Clínic Barcelona, Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences University of Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain Fundació Clínic per a la Recerca Biomèdica (FCRB), Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Network Centre for Biomedical Research in Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Health Institute Carlos III (ISCIII), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
Ingo Schäfer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
Eduard Vieta*
Affiliation:
Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Hospital Clínic Barcelona, Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences University of Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain Fundació Clínic per a la Recerca Biomèdica (FCRB), Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Network Centre for Biomedical Research in Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Health Institute Carlos III (ISCIII), Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
*
Corresponding authors: Natalia E. Fares-Otero; Email: [email protected] Eduard Vieta; Email: [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Natalia E. Fares-Otero; Email: [email protected] Eduard Vieta; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Childhood maltreatment (CM) has been related to social functioning and social cognition impairment in people with psychotic disorders (PD); however, evidence across different CM subtypes and social domains remains less clear. We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to quantify associations between CM, overall and its different subtypes (physical/emotional/sexual abuse, physical/emotional neglect), and domains of social functioning and social cognition in adults with PD. We also examined moderators and mediators of these associations. A PRISMA-compliant systematic search was performed on 24 November 2022 (PROSPERO CRD42020175244). Fifty-three studies (N = 13 635 individuals with PD) were included in qualitative synthesis, of which 51 studies (N = 13 260) with 125 effects sizes were pooled in meta-analyses. We found that CM was negatively associated with global social functioning and interpersonal relations, and positively associated with aggressive behaviour, but unrelated to independent living or occupational functioning. There was no meta-analytic evidence of associations between CM and social cognition. Meta-regression analyses did not identify any consistent moderation pattern. Narrative synthesis identified sex and timing of CM as potential moderators, and depressive symptoms and maladaptive personality traits as possible mediators between CM and social outcomes. Associations were of small magnitude and limited number of studies assessing CM subtypes and social cognition are available. Nevertheless, adults with PD are at risk of social functioning problems after CM exposure, an effect observed across multiple CM subtypes, social domains, diagnoses and illness stages. Maltreated adults with PD may thus benefit from trauma-related and psychosocial interventions targeting social relationships and functioning.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Psychotic disorders (PD), comprising schizophrenia spectrum and affective psychoses, are among the leading causes of disability (Navarro-Mateu et al., Reference Navarro-Mateu, Alonso, Lim, Saha, Aguilar-Gaxiola and Al-Hamzawi2017) and a public health concern worldwide (Anderson, Reference Anderson2019). Impairments of both social functioning (i.e. the ability to fulfil expected roles at work, social activities, and social relations with partners and family) (Long, Stansfeld, Davies, Crellin, & Moncrieff, Reference Long, Stansfeld, Davies, Crellin and Moncrieff2022) and social cognition (i.e. the ability to decode the intentions and behaviours of others) (Green, Reference Green2016) are core features of PD and are thought to underlie severe functional disabilities (de Winter et al., Reference de Winter, Couwenbergh, van Weeghel, Hasson-Ohayon, Vermeulen, Mulder and Veling2021; Vita et al., Reference Vita, Gaebel, Mucci, Sachs, Erfurth, Barlati and Galderisi2022). About two-thirds of individuals with PD are unable to fulfil basic social roles as spouse, parent, or worker. Possibly related to a lack of early interventions (Birchwood, McGorry, & Jackson, Reference Birchwood, McGorry and Jackson1997; McGorry, Reference McGorry2015), these social problems can remain remarkably stable in the years after the first episode of psychosis (FEP) (Velthorst et al., Reference Velthorst, Fett, Reichenberg, Perlman, van Os, Bromet and Kotov2017), also when psychotic symptoms are in remission (Bellack et al., Reference Bellack, Green, Cook, Fenton, Harvey, Heaton and Wykes2007). Accordingly, identifying factors that potentially hinder social functions is a major aim in recovery-oriented treatment and research (Albert, Uddin, & Nordentoft, Reference Albert, Uddin and Nordentoft2018; Javed & Charles, Reference Javed and Charles2018; Yamada et al., Reference Yamada, Inagawa, Sueyoshi, Sugawara, Ueda, Omachi and Sumiyoshi2019).

Childhood maltreatment (CM), i.e. physical, emotional or sexual abuse, as well as physical and/or emotional neglect, including witnessing domestic violence and bullying occurring before age 18 years (Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2013), is one of the most serious environmental risk factors for the development of physical or mental illness (Gilbert et al., Reference Gilbert, Widom, Browne, Fergusson, Webb and Janson2009; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Bellis, Hardcastle, Sethi, Butchart, Mikton and Dunne2017), including PD (Morgan & Fisher, Reference Morgan and Fisher2007; Varese et al., Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer and Bentall2012). Prevalence can vary across populations, but some reports show rates as high as 85% in schizophrenia spectrum disorders and 77% in affective psychoses (Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Andreassen, Aas, Røssberg, Mork, Steen and Lorentzen2013). At least one subtype of CM is reported by around half of individuals with FEP (Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Del Cacho, Butjosa, Serra Arumí, Esteban Santjusto, Abella and Usall2022), and schizophrenia (Morgan & Fisher, Reference Morgan and Fisher2007).

CM is thought to play a key role in the aetiology and course of PD (Varese et al., Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer and Bentall2012). CM is further related to neurobiological and clinical characteristics (McCrory, De Brito, & Viding, Reference McCrory, De Brito and Viding2011; Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2013) that may lead to difficulties of individuals with PD to engage with and navigate the social world (McCrory, Foulkes, & Viding, Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022). At a neurobiological level, the diathesis-stress or vulnerability-stress model (Read, Fosse, Moskowitz, & Perry, Reference Read, Fosse, Moskowitz and Perry2014; van Winkel, Stefanis, & Myin-Germeys, Reference van Winkel, Stefanis and Myin-Germeys2008; Vargas, Conley, & Mittal, Reference Vargas, Conley and Mittal2020) posits that experiencing highly stressful or traumatic events, such as CM, may impact on later expression of PD by increasing stress sensitivity to later adversity (Lardinois, Lataster, Mengelers, Van Os, & Myin-Germeys, Reference Lardinois, Lataster, Mengelers, Van Os and Myin-Germeys2011; Lataster, Myin-Germeys, Lieb, Wittchen, & van Os, Reference Lataster, Myin-Germeys, Lieb, Wittchen and van Os2012). It may further have long-lasting effects on the neurobiological processes required to manage the multifaceted roles that are undertaken as part of daily functioning. CM constitutes a stressor that can occur at sensitive periods of development (Schaefer, Cheng, & Dunn, Reference Schaefer, Cheng and Dunn2022), affecting the regular functioning of brain areas involved in the response to stress (e.g. the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis) (Teicher, Samson, Anderson, & Ohashi, Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016). These brain alterations may lead to impaired emotion regulation skills and maladaptive coping strategies (Lincoln, Marin, & Jaya, Reference Lincoln, Marin and Jaya2017), which in turn can lead to poor social functioning in those with PD, as manifested in various areas of their daily life such as occupational functioning (Hjelseng et al., Reference Hjelseng, Vaskinn, Ueland, Lunding, Reponen, Steen and Aas2020; Stain et al., Reference Stain, Brønnick, Hegelstad, Joa, Johannessen, Langeveld and Larsen2014) and interpersonal relations (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021), including a reduction in the quality and quantity of relationships (McCrory, Ogle, Gerin, & Viding, Reference McCrory, Ogle, Gerin and Viding2019; McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022). Neurobiological alterations might also contribute to social cognition difficulties (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Dazzan, Mondelli, Melle, Murray and Pariante2014; Rokita, Dauvermann, & Donohoe, Reference Rokita, Dauvermann and Donohoe2018). For instance, CM has been associated with altered (facial) emotion recognition and processing (Pfaltz et al., Reference Pfaltz, Passardi, Auschra, Fares-Otero, Schnyder and Peyk2019; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Holleran, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holland, Costello and Donohoe2020) and poorer or altered understanding of people's beliefs (theory of mind) (Dorn et al., Reference Dorn, Struck, Bitsch, Falkenberg, Kircher, Rief and Mehl2021; Pang et al., Reference Pang, Zhao, Li, Li, Yu, Zhang and Wang2022), all of which might contribute to diminished social involvement in those with PD.

Moreover, a heightened emotional reactivity to daily stressors seems robustly related to the severity of psychotic experiences and negative affect (Paetzold et al., Reference Paetzold, Myin-Germeys, Schick, Nelson, Velthorst, Schirmbeck and Reininghaus2021; Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Kempton, Valmaggia, Craig, Garety, Onyejiaka and Morgan2016; van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Lecei, Myin-Germeys, Collip, Viechtbauer, Jacobs and van Winkel2018). CM relates to depressive symptoms and suicide attempts, and the occurrence, severity and persistence of both hallucinations and delusions, as well as negative symptoms (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Christy, Rodriguez, Salazar de Pablo, Thrush, Shen and Murray2021). All these domains of symptoms might be related to diminished social involvement in individuals with PD during early (Stain et al., Reference Stain, Brønnick, Hegelstad, Joa, Johannessen, Langeveld and Larsen2014) and active illness phases, as well as during remission (Hjelseng et al., Reference Hjelseng, Vaskinn, Ueland, Lunding, Reponen, Steen and Aas2020; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, King, Veru, Schalinski, Vracotas, Abadi and Joober2021). In fact, differential effects of CM on clinical outcome may not be apparent at PD onset, but only become evident through poor symptomatic remission and global social functioning over time (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Andreassen, Aminoff, Færden, Romm, Nesvåg and Melle2016; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, King, Veru, Schalinski, Vracotas, Abadi and Joober2021).

Despite the well-established link between CM and PD (Schäfer & Fisher, Reference Schäfer and Fisher2011; Stanton, Denietolis, Goodwin, & Dvir, Reference Stanton, Denietolis, Goodwin and Dvir2020) across specific subtypes of CM (Ajnakina et al., Reference Ajnakina, Trotta, Forti, Stilo, Kolliakou, Gardner-Sood and Fisher2018) and symptoms dimensions (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Christy, Rodriguez, Salazar de Pablo, Thrush, Shen and Murray2021), and increasing recognition that social functions are closely related to adverse experiences in childhood in adults with PD (Turner et al., Reference Turner, Harvey, Hayes, Castle, Galletly, Sweeney and Spittal2020), meta-analytic research assessing the magnitude and consistency of associations between different subtypes of CM and domains of social functioning and social cognition in PD is lacking. Lately, the research about PD and CM has generated wide interest in researchers. One prior meta-analysis has quantitatively examined associations between broadly defined and specific types of childhood adversities and functional outcomes in PD. This study found small negative associations of CM with global social functioning and no association with occupational functioning. This study, however, focused on global aspects of functional outcomes, as well as on the social and occupational domains independently (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022). Furthermore, the nature of the association between overall, broadly defined CM, and specific subtypes, across global and specific domains of social functioning and social cognition has not been appraised. Examination of whether there are differences between diagnoses (non-affective v. affective psychoses) in how CM relates to social outcomes in different illness stages (FEP v. chronic PD) (Breitborde, Srihari, & Woods, Reference Breitborde, Srihari and Woods2009) is also warranted, given fundamental differences in how these disorders present (Chen, Liu, Liu, Zhang, & Wu, Reference Chen, Liu, Liu, Zhang and Wu2021; de Winter et al., Reference de Winter, Couwenbergh, van Weeghel, Hasson-Ohayon, Vermeulen, Mulder and Veling2021; Torrent et al., Reference Torrent, Reinares, Martinez-Arán, Cabrera, Amoretti, Corripio and group2018).

Moreover, some factors are thought to moderate between CM and social outcomes (e.g. age at the time of exposure) (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Ferrari, Baumann, Gholam-Rezaee, Do and Conus2015, Reference Alameda, Golay, Baumann, Ferrari, Do and Conus2016) in PD. In addition, knowledge on possible mediators (depressive symptoms) (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Rodriguez, Carr, Aas, Trotta, Marino and Murray2020) of proposed association between CM and both impairments in social functioning and social cognition could help to understand underlying mechanisms to design interventions that might be more effective for those with PD and CM. To date the possible mediators and moderators in the association between CM and social functioning and social cognition in PD have never been reviewed and synthesised. The respective synthesis would improve our understanding of whether CM relates to social functioning and social cognition and might provide targets to develop preventive strategies and effective interventions to improve social outcomes in people with PD and CM histories.

Therefore, the first aim of our systematic review and meta-analysis was to provide an estimate on the magnitude and consistency of associations between CM (overall and its subtypes) and global and different domains of social functioning and social cognition in adults with PD. The second aim was to examine and narratively summarise moderators and mediators of these associations. We hypothesised that CM would be related to poorer social functioning and social cognition in individuals with PD.

Methods

This Study protocol was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42020175244) and published elsewhere before completion of the study (Fares-Otero, Pfaltz, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Schäfer, & Trautmann, Reference Fares-Otero, Pfaltz, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Schäfer and Trautmann2021). This review follows the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA 2020) guideline (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow and Moher2021) (see ST1 and ST2 in the supplement), the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) (Stroup et al., Reference Stroup, Berlin, Morton, Olkin, Williamson, Rennie and Thacker2000) (see ST3 in the supplement), and the Enhancing the Quality and Transparency of Health Research (EQUATOR) (Altman, Simera, Hoey, Moher, & Schulz, Reference Altman, Simera, Hoey, Moher and Schulz2008) reporting guidelines. For a comprehensive glossary of terms used in this work, see SA1 in the supplement.

Search strategy and selection criteria

A systematic literature search using multiple Medical Subject Headings and keywords related to: (1) ‘psychosis’; (2) ‘childhood maltreatment’; (3) ‘social functioning’ OR ‘social cognition’ using the Boolean operator ‘AND’ (see the search strategy and terms appended in SA2 in the supplement) was conducted in PubMed (Medline), PsycINFO, Embase, Web of Science (Core Collection), and PILOTS, initially searched for inception from 1990 until 25 June 2021, and updated twice, on 4 March 2022, and on 24 November 2022. The following filters were used: human samples, written in English, German, and Spanish, and removal of duplicates. To identify additional eligible articles, the reference lists of the included articles and relevant studies already included in the previously identified meta-analysis (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022) were cross referenced manually.

Titles and abstracts of articles were independently screened by three reviewers (NEF-O, L-MN, SW) (89.15% agreement); discrepancies were resolved through discussion with an independent reviewer (ST). After excluding irrelevant articles, full-texts were independently assessed for eligibility by three reviewers (NEF-O, L-MN, SW) (88.90% agreement); full-text discrepancies were screened by an independent reviewer (ST) and resolved through consensus. The software Zotero was used to manage citations and remove duplicates. The software Rayyan QCRI (https://rayyan.qcri.org/) was used to manage citations, remove duplicates, and screening in the search updates. Because of high agreement during first screening, NEF-O independently conducted the search updates; discrepancies were resolved through discussion with an independent reviewer (ST).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

According to the PICO framework, studies were included if they: (1) (P) were conducted in individuals with PD spectrum, including non-affective PD (schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder) and affective PD (bipolar disorder, major depression with psychotic features) based on ICD (World Health Organization, 1993) and DSM (DSM-5 Diagnostic Classification, 2013) criteria (see manual codes of PD diagnoses in ST4 in the supplement); (2) (I) assessed the presence of CM defined as physical/emotional/sexual abuse and/or physical/emotional neglect, including domestic violence and bullying, occurring before age 18 (Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2013) and measured as overall (total) or specific CM subtypes (3) (C) compared individuals with and without CM within the same sample population of individuals with PD; (4) (O) evaluated social functioning or social cognition with validated instruments (see details in section 2.3); (5) quantitatively examined and reported associations between CM (exposure variable) and social functioning or social cognition (outcome variable) or data that allowed correlations to be calculated, or provided these data on request (see the definition and operationalisation of exposure and outcome variables in SA3 in the supplement); (6) were original research articles published in a peer-reviewed journal.

Studies were excluded if they: (1) were reviews, clinical case studies, abstracts, conference proceedings, study protocols, letters to the editor not reporting original data, theoretical pieces, or grey literature; (2) only recruited children or adolescents; (3) only investigated animals; (4) involved interventions and/or assessed treatment outcomes not providing baseline data.

Study outcomes

After study selection, we categorised the study outcomes into six separate domains of social functioning and four separate domains of social cognition. The selection of outcome domains was based on outcomes examined in the included studies, and categorisations used in previous meta-analyses in the field (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022; de Winter et al., Reference de Winter, Couwenbergh, van Weeghel, Hasson-Ohayon, Vermeulen, Mulder and Veling2021; Fares-Otero et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023).

Social functioning

(1) Global social functioning: overall functioning in a social setting or role in any social domain (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Andreassen, Aminoff, Færden, Romm, Nesvåg and Melle2016; de Winter et al., Reference de Winter, Couwenbergh, van Weeghel, Hasson-Ohayon, Vermeulen, Mulder and Veling2021); (2) Independent living: independent functioning (Monfort-Escrig & Pena-Garijo, Reference Monfort-Escrig and Pena-Garijo2021), autonomy, and financial management (Shah et al., Reference Shah, Mackinnon, Galletly, Carr, McGrath, Stain and Morgan2014); (3) Occupational functioning: vocational functioning, involvement into (competitive) employment/work (Lindgren et al., Reference Lindgren, Mäntylä, Rikandi, Torniainen-Holm, Morales-Muñoz, Kieseppä and Suvisaari2017); (4) Interpersonal relations: social relationships and community functioning; (5) Aggressive behaviour: social violent behaviour, including hostility and criminality (Bosqui et al., Reference Bosqui, Shannon, Tiernan, Beattie, Ferguson and Mulholland2014); and (6) Psychosocial problems: Axis IV psychosocial and environmental problems (Ramsay, Flanagan, Gantt, Broussard, & Compton, Reference Ramsay, Flanagan, Gantt, Broussard and Compton2011).

Social cognition

(1) Theory of mind: ability to reason about mental states and understand intentions, dispositions, emotions, and beliefs of both oneself and others or mentalising (Brüne, Reference Brüne2005; Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson and Mulholland2018); (2) Emotion processing: ability to manage emotions, and to identify, recognise, understand (facial) emotions of others (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Kauppi, Brandt, Tesli, Kaufmann, Steen and Melle2017); (3) Attributional style/bias: the way in which individuals infer the causes of particular social events (Chalker et al., Reference Chalker, Parrish, Cano, Kelsven, Moore, Granholm and Depp2022; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019); and (4) Empathy: ability to comprehend and share the emotions of others (Bonfils, Lysaker, Minor, & Salyers, Reference Bonfils, Lysaker, Minor and Salyers2017).

Appendix SA3 in the supplement provides a complete definition and operationalisation of each outcome domain, and ST5 provides a complete overview of assessments of each outcome domain.

Data extraction and quality assessment

Data from eligible studies were extracted and tracked in Microsoft Excel by three independent reviewers (NEF-O, L-MN, SW) using a structured coding form; discrepancies were resolved through consensus with an additional reviewer (ST) to ensure high quality of data extraction.

Descriptive variables extracted included first author and publication year, country/region, sample size, mean age (with standard deviation), percentage of males in the sample, study design, type of diagnosis in the sample, type and instrument for diagnosis (and criteria), duration (in years) of the illness, CM instrument used and type of CM exposure reported (overall CM and/or subtypes), social functioning or social cognition instrument/measure, results on the association between CM and social functioning or social cognition (including p value, effect size and descriptive summary), confounders, moderators, and mediators investigated in the included studies (if reported).

Correlation coefficients (r) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were extracted as measures of effect size. If not reported, information was transformed from available statistics (e.g. mean and standard deviations between groups comparisons, unstandardised regression coefficients, and standardised β coefficients, and odds ratios), as per procedures used in previous meta-analyses (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Christy, Rodriguez, Salazar de Pablo, Thrush, Shen and Murray2021; Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022; Fares-Otero et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023), using established formulas (Practical Meta-Analysis, 2022). Corresponding authors were contacted by email to retrieve additional information if necessary. Studies that reported either an overall (total) continuous score of CM, or binary category (high/low exposure), and/or a score for the CM subtypes (subscales) were included into one or more of the meta-analyses conducted. In the case where no overall CM effect was reported, only the effects of specific subtypes of CM were extracted to be included in meta-analyses. For longitudinal studies, data indicating associations at baseline were extracted (see a detailed description of the extracted variables in SA4 in the supplement).

The quality and risk of bias assessment was independently assessed by two independent reviewers (NEF-O, ST) using an adapted version of the Newcastle Ottawa Scale (NOS) (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Wells, Shea, Shea, O'Connell, Peterson and Petersen2014) for non-randomised (cross-sectional and longitudinal) studies which contains additional items to assess sample size, confounders, and statistical tests, recommended by Cochrane Handbook (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Altman, Gøtzsche, Jüni, Moher, Oxman and Sterne2011) (see SA5, ST5 and ST6 in the supplement).

Statistical analysis

All quantitative analyses were performed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis v4.0 (CMA, version 4 -meta-analysis.com) (Borenstein, Reference Borenstein, Egger, Higgins and Smith2022a). A PRISMA-compliant systematic review (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow and Moher2021) and random-effect meta-analyses (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2011) were conducted applying the DerSimonian-Laird estimator (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2022), when a minimum of five studies were available (Jackson & Turner, Reference Jackson and Turner2017). If the number of available effect sizes did not allow random effects meta-analysis, study findings were summarised and appraised qualitatively.

We conducted separate meta-analyses with random-effect estimates to quantify associations between each CM subtype or overall CM and social functioning (global, independent living, occupational functioning, interpersonal relations, aggressive behaviour) or social cognition (theory of mind and emotion processing) domain. For studies conducting separate analyses for men and women (Penney, Pruessner, Malla, Joober, & Lepage, Reference Penney, Pruessner, Malla, Joober and Lepage2022), physical and verbal aggression (Spidel, Lecomte, Greaves, Sahlstrom, & Yuille, Reference Spidel, Lecomte, Greaves, Sahlstrom and Yuille2010), independence competence and performance (Monfort-Escrig & Pena-Garijo, Reference Monfort-Escrig and Pena-Garijo2021), and disorganised attachment styles (Aydin et al., Reference Aydin, Balikci, Tas, Aydin, Danaci, Bruene and Lysaker2016; Hodann-Caudevilla, García, & Julián, Reference Hodann-Caudevilla, García and Julián2021), results were pooled using correction estimates (Olkin & Pratt, Reference Olkin and Pratt1958) before inclusion to meta-analyses.

For those studies not reporting correlation coefficients, the ‘Practical Meta-Analysis Effect Size Calculator’ (Practical Meta-Analysis, 2022) was used to convert the reported statistics. Pearson correlation coefficients (effect sizes) were Fisher's Z transformed and back transformed after pooling. Thus, all pooled effects are reported as correlation coefficients. A small number of effects (1.9%) were reported as null findings without sufficient information to calculate effect sizes. These effects were not excluded to avoid upward bias of effect estimation. Instead, they were set to zero, resulting in rather conservative pooled effect size estimations (Albajes-Eizagirre, Solanes, & Radua, Reference Albajes-Eizagirre, Solanes and Radua2019).

Analyses for heterogeneity were performed using Cochran's Q-test and I 2 statistics with significant heterogeneity being indicated by I 2 ⩾ 50% (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, Reference Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman2003) [25, 50, and 75% defining thresholds for low, moderate, and high heterogeneity (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2022)]. Alongside the 95% CI and the mean pooled effect provided, the prediction intervals, to estimate to which extent effect sizes vary across studies (Borenstein, Reference Borenstein2022b), were displayed as part of the forest plots (marked in red).

The forest plots were explored, and one-study-removed sensitivity analyses were conducted to determine whether a particular study or a set of studies were contributing to the potential heterogeneity (Borenstein, Reference Borenstein, Egger, Higgins and Smith2022a).

To further examine potential factors explaining heterogeneity, a series of random-effect meta-regressions (López-López, Van den Noortgate, Tanner-Smith, Wilson, & Lipsey, Reference López-López, Van den Noortgate, Tanner-Smith, Wilson and Lipsey2017) were conducted on pre-selected variables: mean sample age, percentage of male individuals, non-affective v. affective psychosis samples, FEP (illness duration <2 years) v. chronic PD samples, diagnostic instrument (structured interview v. clinical judgment), use of Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) v. any other instrument to assess CM, use of self-report v. clinician judgment to assess social functioning, use of behavioural data v. any other instrument to assess social cognition, and study quality (NOS rating) as per procedures used in previous meta-analyses (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022; de Winter et al., Reference de Winter, Couwenbergh, van Weeghel, Hasson-Ohayon, Vermeulen, Mulder and Veling2021; Fares-Otero et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023). Because of the limited number of included studies in some analyses (n < 10) (Borenstein, Reference Borenstein, Egger, Higgins and Smith2022a; Higgins & Thompson, Reference Higgins and Thompson2004), meta-regression analyses should be considered exploratory. Other evidence of confounders (section 3.2., Table 1) and effect moderators and mediators examined in the included studies (section 3.7. Fig. 3) on associations between CM and social outcomes was narratively synthesised (Popay et al., Reference Popay, Roberts, Sowden, Petticrew, Arai, Rodgers and Duffy2006).

Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of included studies

AoM, Awareness of the Mind of the Other; BBTS, The Brief Betrayal Trauma Survey; BD, Bipolar Disorder; BDHI, Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory; BES, The Basic Empathy Scale; BLERT, Bell Lysaker Emotional Recognition Task; CAMIR-R, from French; Cartes-Modeles Individuels de Relations (Short form); CAQ, Childhood Abuse Questionnaire; CASH, The Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History; CECA-Q, Childhood Experiences of Care and Abuse Questionnaire; CEVQ, Childhood Experiences of Violence Questionnaire; CSTQ, Childhood Sexual Trauma Questionnaire; CT, Childhood Trauma; CTQ (-SF), Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (-Short Form); DFAR, The degraded facial affect recognition task; DIP, Diagnostic Interview for Psychosis; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; DV, Domestic Violence; EA, Emotional Abuse; ECR-R, Experience in Close Relationships Revised; EN, Emotional Neglect; ERT, Emotion Recognition Task; ETISR-SF, The Early Trauma Inventory Self Report-Short Form; ExpTra-S, Screening of Early Traumatic Experiences in Patients with Severe Mental Illness; FAST, Functioning Assessment Short Test; FEEST, Facial Expressions of Emotion Stimuli and Tests; FEP, First Episode Psychosis; GAF (-F), Global Assessment of functioning (Function subscale); GAS, Global Assessment Scale; HCR-20, The Historical Clinical Risk Management-20; ICD-10, International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th revision; INQ, Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire; IPII, The Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview; ISMI, Internalised Stigma – Social Withdrawal; IQ, Intelligence quotient; LHA-A, Lifetime History of Aggression Scale-Aggression Subscale; MACE, Maltreatment and Abuse Chronology of Exposure Scale; MAS-A, Metacognition Assessment Scale-Abbreviated; MASC, Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition; MCCB, MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery; MDD, Major Depressive Disorder; MINI, Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview; MOAS, Modified Overt Aggression Scale; MSCEIT, Mayer Salovery Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test; NS, Not specified, OPCRIT, Operational Criteria Checklist for Psychotic Illness and Affective Illness (v.4.0.: checklist to generate DSM-IV diagnoses for PD); PA, Psychical Abuse; PAM, Psychosis Attachment Measure; PD, Psychotic Disorder; PERE, from Spanish; Prueba de Reconocimiento de Emociones or Emotion recognition Task; PN, Physical Neglect; PSPS, Personal and Social Performance Scale; PsyQol, Psychological Quality of Life; QoL, Quality of Life; QSF, Questionnaire of Social Functioning; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task; SA, Sexual Abuse; SASS, Social Adaptation Task-Multiple Choice; SANS, Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms; SAT-MC, Social Attribution Task-Multiple Choice; SCAN, Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry; SCID-I, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders – Axis I; SECT, Social Emotional Cognition Task; SFS, Social Functioning Scale; SCZ, Schizophrenia; SF-36; Short Form-36 Health-related QoL-Psychological Subscore; SOFAS, Social and Occupational Functioning; SQoL, Social Quality of Life; TAA, Trauma Assessment for Adults; TASIT, The Awareness of Social Inference Test; TEC, Traumatic Experience Checklist; ToM, Theory of Mind; WHOQOL_BREF, World Health Organization Quality of Life.

To examine publication bias, funnel plots were visually inspected, investigating possible outliers or studies going in the opposite direction of all the others, and the intercept Egger's test was used to numerically explore the risk of publication bias (namely Egger's test p value <0.05) (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Altman, Gøtzsche, Jüni, Moher, Oxman and Sterne2011; Lin & Chu, Reference Lin and Chu2018). Where indications for publication biases were found, corrected effect sizes using the Duval and Tweedie's trim-and-fill method were additionally reported to correct for significant publication bias (Duval & Tweedie, Reference Duval and Tweedie2000).

Statistical significance was evaluated two-sided at the 5% threshold (two tailed). Interpretation of correlations coefficients was based on predefined cut-offs as follows: r values between 0 and 0.3 indicate small, values between 0.3 and 0.7 indicate moderate, and values above 0.7 indicate strong associations (Ratner, Reference Ratner2009).

Results

Study inclusion and characteristics

Of 5350 eligible studies, 283 were full text screened, and 53 were included in the qualitative synthesis, of which 51 studies were included in the quantitative synthesis, contributing to 125 effect sizes pooled in meta-analyses (see the process of study selection in detail in Fig. 1, the full list of included studies in SA6, and excluded studies with reasons in SA7 in the supplement).

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flowchart outlining the study selection process.

The total sample of the included studies compromised 13 635 individuals with PD (sample size range 25–1825), of which 9429 (69.2%) were male. The mean age was 33.9 (s.d. = 7.7; range = 22–48) years. Of the 53 included studies, 14 (26.4%) studies included samples with non-affective PD, and 10 (19.2%) studies included samples with FEP.

Sample sizes of the 51 included studies included in the meta-analyses ranged from 25 to 1825, comprising a total of 13 260 individuals with PD, of which 9236 (69.7%) were male. The mean age was 34.02 (s.d. = 7.44; range = 22–48) years. Fourteen (27.5%) of the samples of the 51 included studies fulfilled criteria for non-affective PD, and 9 (18%) studies included samples with FEP.

A structured clinical interview was used in 32 (60.4%) of the included studies for the assessment of PD. The SCID-Structured Clinical Interview for DSM (First & Gibbon, Reference First and Gibbon2004) was the most frequently used diagnostic instrument. It was used in 17 (32.7%) studies, followed by the OPCRIT electronic system (Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Newman, Gray, Gunasinghe, Broadbent, Brittain and McGuffin2011) in 5 (9.4%) studies. Ten (18.9%) studies used an unstructured clinical interview based on DSM, while five (9.4%) studies used ICD criteria, and six (11.3%) studies used a clinical judgment (non-specified criteria).

Fifty (94.3%) of the 53 included studies were cross-sectional. The CTQ, including shortened (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003) or translated versions, was the most used instrument to measure CM in 31 (58.5%) studies, and the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA.Q) (Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran, & Jacobs, Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs2005) was used in three (5.7%) studies. Four (7.6%) studies reported CM results from a clinical interview.

Overall CM was the most frequently assessed variable, being examined in 34 (62.75%) of the included studies, while 28 (52.8%) studies examined only CM subtypes, and eight (15.1%) studies examined both overall CM and all subtypes. Twenty-five (47.2%) studies examined physical abuse, 27 (50.9%) studies examined sexual abuse, 18 (34.0%) studies examined emotional abuse, 18 (34.0%) studies examined emotional neglect, and 17 (32.1%) studies examined physical neglect.

Of note, five studies investigated types of maltreatment that could not be pooled in meta-analysis (n < 5 and/or k < 5) (Jackson & Turner, Reference Jackson and Turner2017) such as aggregated scores for abuse and neglect (Brañas, Lahera, Barrigón, Canal-Rivero, & Ruiz-Veguilla, Reference Brañas, Lahera, Barrigón, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022; Kilian et al., Reference Kilian, Asmal, Chiliza, Olivier, Phahladira, Scheffler and Emsley2018; Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019), separation from parents and domestic violence (Oakley, Harris, Fahy, Murphy, & Picchioni, Reference Oakley, Harris, Fahy, Murphy and Picchioni2016), and parental harsh discipline (Ramsay et al., Reference Ramsay, Flanagan, Gantt, Broussard and Compton2011). Among these studies, a negative association between neglect (but not abuse) and emotion processing [r = −0.45 (CI −0.64 to −0.21), p < 0.001] was found in individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Kilian et al., Reference Kilian, Asmal, Chiliza, Olivier, Phahladira, Scheffler and Emsley2018). Yet no association between abuse and theory of mind was found in individuals with non-affective PD (Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019). While others found no association between abuse and theory of mind or emotion recognition of different emotions except for better recognition of fearful faces (r = 0.32 (CI 0.05–0.54)] in FEP (Brañas et al., Reference Brañas, Lahera, Barrigón, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022). A positive association between childhood exposure to domestic violence [r = 0.54 (CI 0.32 to −0.71), p = 0.001] and separation from parents [r = 0.34 (CI 0.08–0.56), p = 0.015] but not child abuse and propensity to violent behaviour was found in adults with schizophrenia (Oakley et al., Reference Oakley, Harris, Fahy, Murphy and Picchioni2016). Finally, a positive association between parental harsh discipline and psychosocial problems was found [r = 0.28 (CI 0.03–0.50)] in people with FEP (Ramsay et al., Reference Ramsay, Flanagan, Gantt, Broussard and Compton2011).

Of the 53 included studies, 34 (70.8%) examined social functioning, of which 21 (61.8%) used self-report questionnaires (v. clinician judgment). Nineteen studies examined social cognition, of which ten (52.6%) used behavioural data (v. any other instrument). Across studies, five social functioning and four social cognition domains were examined, of which four domains of social functioning and two domains of social cognition had sufficient data for meta-analysis.

Of the 51 included studies in the meta-analyses, 33 (62.3%) examined social functioning. Global social functioning was most frequently examined in a total of 21 (39.6%) studies. In terms of social functioning domains, eight (15.1%) studies examined independent living, 13 (24.5%) studies examined occupational functioning, 14 (26.4%) studies examined interpersonal relations, and 11 (20.8%) studies examined aggressive behaviour. No studies examined associations between CM subtypes and independent living or occupational functioning, or interpersonal relations (except for sexual abuse) in PD. No studies examined the association between CM subtypes (except for physical and sexual abuse) and aggressive behaviour. One above-mentioned study concerning a positive association between parental harsh discipline and psychosocial problems in FEP (Ramsay et al., Reference Ramsay, Flanagan, Gantt, Broussard and Compton2011) could not be meta-analysed.

In terms of social cognition domains, a total of 19 (34.0%) studies were examined, of which ten (18.9%) studies examined theory of mind, and 11 (20.8%) examined emotion processing. No studies examined the relationship between CM subtypes and emotion processing or (except for sexual abuse) theory of mind. Two studies concerning associations of CM with empathy (Cui et al., Reference Cui, Kim, Lee, Kim, Yu, Lee and Chung2019; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019), and two studies concerning associations of CM with attributional style/bias (Chalker et al., Reference Chalker, Parrish, Cano, Kelsven, Moore, Granholm and Depp2022; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019) could not be meta-analysed. Among these studies, no association between overall CM and empathy was found in FEP (Cui et al., Reference Cui, Kim, Lee, Kim, Yu, Lee and Chung2019). Although a negative association between emotional neglect and empathy (cognitive trait) [r = −0.47 (95% CI 0.72 to −0.11)] was found in individuals with schizophrenia, no significant correlation was observed after controlling for gender, age, duration of illness, and medication (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019). Furthermore, the same study (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019) found no association between CM and attributional style, while others found that only emotional abuse was associated with more negative and hostile social attributional biases in PD (Chalker et al., Reference Chalker, Parrish, Cano, Kelsven, Moore, Granholm and Depp2022).

Twenty-nine studies controlled for confounders in their analysis, and several adjusted for sex (Brañas et al., Reference Brañas, Lahera, Barrigón, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022; Quide et al., Reference Quide, Cohen-Woods, O'Reilly, Carr, Elzinga and Green2018; Sweeney, Air, Zannettino, & Galletly, Reference Sweeney, Air, Zannettino and Galletly2015) or gender (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019; Monfort-Escrig & Pena-Garijo, Reference Monfort-Escrig and Pena-Garijo2021; van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Bak, de Graaf, Ten Have, van Dorsselaer and van Winkel2016). A wide range of confounders were considered. These included family income and socioeconomic status (Turner et al., Reference Turner, Harvey, Hayes, Castle, Galletly, Sweeney and Spittal2020), residence (city v. rural area), parental styles (Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holleran, Holland, Costello and Donohoe2021), attachment dimensions (Hjelseng et al., Reference Hjelseng, Vaskinn, Ueland, Lunding, Reponen, Steen and Aas2020), and first-degree relative mental illness (Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019). Also, child premorbid social, cognitive (Hodann-Caudevilla et al., Reference Hodann-Caudevilla, García and Julián2021), and academic functioning (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Andreassen, Aminoff, Færden, Romm, Nesvåg and Melle2016), IQ (Vaskinn, Melle, Aas, & Berg, Reference Vaskinn, Melle, Aas and Berg2021), educational level (yeas of education) (Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus, & Rockstroh, Reference Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus and Rockstroh2018) as well as gender (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019; Monfort-Escrig & Pena-Garijo, Reference Monfort-Escrig and Pena-Garijo2021; van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Bak, de Graaf, Ten Have, van Dorsselaer and van Winkel2016), sex (Brañas et al., Reference Brañas, Lahera, Barrigón, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022; Quide et al., Reference Quide, Cohen-Woods, O'Reilly, Carr, Elzinga and Green2018; Sweeney et al., Reference Sweeney, Air, Zannettino and Galletly2015), ethnicity (Rosenberg, Lu, Mueser, Jankowski, & Cournos, Reference Rosenberg, Lu, Mueser, Jankowski and Cournos2007), age at psychosis onset (Penney et al., Reference Penney, Pruessner, Malla, Joober and Lepage2022), duration of illness (Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019), severity of positive symptoms (Lysaker, Wright, Clements, & Plascak-Hallberg, Reference Lysaker, Wright, Clements and Plascak-Hallberg2002), type of PD diagnosis (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Kauppi, Brandt, Tesli, Kaufmann, Steen and Melle2017), psychopathy, lifetime substance use disorders (Oakley et al., Reference Oakley, Harris, Fahy, Murphy and Picchioni2016), cannabis use (Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019) and antipsychotic medication (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019) were considered.

The included studies were published between 2001 and 2022 and were conducted in Europe (n = 30), North America (n = 10), Asia (n = 3), Australia (n = 6), Turkey (n = 2), Brazil (n = 1), and South Africa (n = 1) (see a detailed description of demographic and clinical characteristics of the included studies in Table 1).

Study quality assessment

The mean quality rating (range between 0 and 8) of the included studies was 5.28 (s.d. = 1.09), range 4–8. Overall, 14 (26.4%) studies were rated as ‘poor’ (NOS score = 4), 20 (37.7%) studies were rated as ‘fair’ (NOS score = 5), 11 (20.8%) studies were rated as ‘good’ (NOS score = 6), and 8 (15.1%) studies received a rating considered as ‘high’ (NOS score >6). Of those rated as ‘high’, six (11.3%) studies (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Andreassen, Aminoff, Færden, Romm, Nesvåg and Melle2016; Andrianarisoa et al., Reference Andrianarisoa, Boyer, Godin, Brunel, Bulzacka and Aouizerate2017; Bosqui et al., Reference Bosqui, Shannon, Tiernan, Beattie, Ferguson and Mulholland2014; Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Liu, Zhang, Tang and Wang2015; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Lu, Mueser, Jankowski and Cournos2007; Turner et al., Reference Turner, Harvey, Hayes, Castle, Galletly, Sweeney and Spittal2020) examined social functioning, and two (3.8%) studies (Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019; Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019) examined social cognition (see further details of the study quality assessment in ST6 and ST7 in the supplement).

The representativeness of samples was mixed, and most of the included studies did not report either on non-response or a priori power analyses or otherwise justified their sample sizes. More than half of the included studies (n = 29) controlled for confounders in their design or analysis, and several adjusted for sex (Brañas et al., Reference Brañas, Lahera, Barrigón, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022; Quide et al., Reference Quide, Cohen-Woods, O'Reilly, Carr, Elzinga and Green2018; Sweeney et al., Reference Sweeney, Air, Zannettino and Galletly2015) or gender (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019; Monfort-Escrig & Pena-Garijo, Reference Monfort-Escrig and Pena-Garijo2021; van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Bak, de Graaf, Ten Have, van Dorsselaer and van Winkel2016) (see section 3.1. and Table 1). Many studies did not fully report results from statistical tests, e.g. omitting named effect estimates, p values, or measures of precision if appropriate (such as standard errors or confidence intervals).

Meta-analyses of associations between childhood maltreatment and social functioning

Overall childhood maltreatment

Overall CM was negatively associated with global social functioning [n = 19, k = 19, r = −0.104 (95% CI −0.142 to −0.066), p < 0.001], as well as interpersonal relations [n = 9, k = 9, r = −0.114 (95% CI −0.180 to −0.046), p = 001], and positively associated with aggressive behaviour [n = 6, k = 6, r = 0.181 (CI 0.140–0.222), p < 0.001] (see Table 2 and forest plots in Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Forest plots investigating associations between overall childhood maltreatment and social functioning: (1) Global social functioning, (2) Independent living, (3) Occupational functioning, (4) Interpersonal relations, and (5) Aggressive behaviour in individuals with psychotic disorders.

Table 2. Meta-analyses of associations between childhood maltreatment and social outcomes in individuals with psychotic disorders

Note: *Two effect sizes from two different populations in the same study were meta-analysed.

Childhood maltreatment subtypes

All subtypes of CM were negatively associated with global social functioning: physical abuse: [n = 7, k = 7, r = −0.123 (95% CI −0.216 to −0.027), p < 0.001]; emotional abuse: [n = 6, k = 6, r = −0.138 (95% CI −0.226 to −0.047), p = 0.003]; sexual abuse: [n = 9, k = 9, r = −0.087 (95% CI −0.216 to −0.027), p = 0.012]; physical neglect: [n = 6, k = 6, r = −0.241 (95% CI −0.349 to −0.127), p < 0.001]; emotional neglect: [n = 6, k = 6, r = −0.226 (95% CI −0.323 to −0.125), p < 0.001].

Physical abuse [n = 6, k = 6, r = 0.230 (95% CI 0.119–0.334), p < 0.001], and sexual abuse [n = 5, k = 5, r = 0.126 (95% CI 0.042–0.208), p = 0.003] were positively associated with aggressive behaviour. Sexual abuse was also negatively associated with interpersonal relations [n = 7, k = 7, r = −0.102 (95% CI −0.189 to −0.013), p = 0.024] (see Table 2 and forest plots in SF1 in the supplement).

Meta-analyses of associations between childhood maltreatment and social cognition

No significant associations were found of associations between Overall CM (n = 6, k = 6, r = −0.003) and sexual abuse (n = 6, k = 6, r = 0.021, p = 0.679) and theory of mind. In addition, no significant association was found between overall CM and emotion processing (n = 6, k = 6, r = −0.105, p = 0.076) (see Table 2 and forest plots in SF1b and SF1d in the supplement).

Heterogeneity, meta-regression, and sensitivity analyses

Heterogeneity

Meta-analyses showed zero to low heterogeneity in results for most associations with a few exceptions: Associations between overall CM and independent living (n = 8, k = 8, I 2 = 87%, p < 0.001), interpersonal relations (n = 9, k = 9, I 2 = 52%, p = 0.036) and theory of mind (n = 6, k = 6, I 2 = 60%, p = 0.003), and between physical abuse and aggressive behaviour (n = 6, k = 6, I 2 = 54%, p < 0.001) showed moderate-high heterogeneity (see Table 2).

Meta-regressions

Results of meta-regressions for the association between overall and subtypes of CM and social outcomes are provided in ST8 in the supplement. Associations were largely independent from sample age, sex (% male), non-affective v. affective psychosis samples, FEP v. chronic PD samples, structured interview v. unstructured clinical judgment for PD diagnosis, CTQ v. any other instrument to assess CM, self-report v. clinical judgment to assess social functioning, behavioural data v. any other instrument to assess social cognition, and study quality (NOS rating) with a few exceptions.

Social Functioning: The association between physical neglect and global social functioning [n = 6, k = 6, β = −0.013, 95% CI (−0.021 to 0.002), p = 0.025] was weaker in males (v. females). The association between emotional neglect and global social functioning [n = 6, k = 6, β = −0.415, 95% CI (−0.826 to −0.004), p = 0.048] decreased with using self-report (v. clinical judgment). The association between Overall CM and independent living decreased with study quality (NOS rating) [n = 8, k = 8, β = −0.132, 95% CI (−0.023 to −0.038), p = 0.006]. The association between overall CM and interpersonal relations (n = 9, k = 9) was stronger in non-affective (v. affective) PD samples [β = 0.135, 95% CI (0.050–0.221), p = 0.002], and decreased with using CTQ v. any other instrument to assess CM [β = −0.138, CI 95% (−0.216 to −0.061), p = 0.001]. Finally, the association between physical abuse and aggressive behaviour (n = 6, k = 6) was stronger in males [β = 0.074, 95% CI (0.011–0.014), p = 0.021] and in non-affective PD samples [β = 0.245, 95% CI (0.094–0.396), p = 0.015], and increased with using self-report [β = 0.243, 95% CI (0.028–0.457), p = 0.027].

Social Cognition: The association between overall CM and theory of mind increased with using CTQ [n = 6, k = 6, β = 0.291, 95% CI (0.046–0.536), p = 0.020] and with study quality [n = 6, k = 6, β = 0.093, 95% CI (0.004–0.183), p = 0.042]. The association between overall CM and emotion processing increased with increasing age [n = 6, k = 6, β = 0.012, 95% CI (−0.001–0.024), p = 0.032].

Of note, as a general rule, estimates of heterogeneity based on n < 10 are not likely to be reliable (Borenstein, Reference Borenstein, Egger, Higgins and Smith2022a; Higgins & Thompson, Reference Higgins and Thompson2004).

Sensitivity analysis

Results of sensitivity analyses for the association between overall and subtypes of CM and social functioning and social cognition domains are provided in SF2 in the supplement. One-study-removed analysis did not change the patterns of most results with a few exceptions.

Social functioning: For the association between overall CM and independent living, the removal of Spence et al. [r = −0.109 (95% CI −0.213 to −0.003), p = 0.043] and Trauelsen et al. [r = −0.115 (95% CI −0.217 to −0.010, p = 0.032] led to a negative association, which was not observed with the inclusion of these studies (Spence et al., Reference Spence, Mulholland, Lynch, McHugh, Dempster and Shannon2006; Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019). For the association between sexual abuse and interpersonal relations, the removal of Akbey, Yildiz, and Gündüz (Reference Akbey, Yildiz and Gündüz2019) [r = −0.080 (95% CI −0.177 to 0.019, p = 0.115] led to a non-significant association.

Social cognition: For the association between overall CM and emotion processing, the removal of Quide et al. [r = −0.131 (95% CI −0.253 to −0.005, p = 0.042] and Pena-Garijo et al. [r = −0.131 (95% CI −0.232 to −0.028, p = 0.013] led to a negative association which was not observed with the inclusion of these studies (Pena-Garijo & Monfort-Escrig, Reference Pena-Garijo and Monfort-Escrig2021; Quide et al., Reference Quide, Cohen-Woods, O'Reilly, Carr, Elzinga and Green2018).

Assessment of publication bias

For associations between overall CM and independent living there was indication for publication bias (Egger's p = 0.004; 4 hypothetically missing studies identified), and the trim-and-fill adjustment method revealed a higher and significant corrected random effect estimate [r = −0.211, 95% CI (−0.315 to −0.103)]. For the association between physical abuse and aggressive behaviour (Egger's p = 0.002; 3 hypothetically missing studies identified), the trim-and-fill adjustment method revealed a lower (still significant) corrected random effect estimate [r = 0.142, 95% CI (0.031–0.251)] (see Table 2 and the funnel plots in SF3 in the supplement).

Narrative synthesis of moderators and mediators

Twenty-one of the included studies investigated effect moderation and eight studies investigated effect mediation between CM and social outcomes (see a summary of reported moderators and mediators in the included studies in Fig. 3).

Figure 3. Summary of the evidence on moderators and mediators between childhood maltreatment and social outcomes in psychotic disorders. Note. The figure summarises the findings of our narrative synthesis on effect moderators and mediators examined in the included studies. Moderators examined in the included studies are represented by circles/ovals (brick orange in online version). Mediators examined in the included studies are represented by rectangles (green in online version). The colour and thickness of the lines represent the robustness of the evidence, i.e., a stronger colour and thicker line representing major evidence (n ≥ 5). Lighter colour and thinner lines represent emerging evidence (n = 1). Dotted line and grey font indicate where evidence is lacking, and more research is needed.

Moderators

The most often investigated moderator was sex or gender (n = 6), with four studies finding a stronger association between CM exposure and impaired social functioning (Hjelseng et al., Reference Hjelseng, Vaskinn, Ueland, Lunding, Reponen, Steen and Aas2020; Lindgren et al., Reference Lindgren, Mäntylä, Rikandi, Torniainen-Holm, Morales-Muñoz, Kieseppä and Suvisaari2017) or social cognition (Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019; Penney et al., Reference Penney, Pruessner, Malla, Joober and Lepage2022) in male than in female participants. Yet, Garcia et al., found poor social cognition in males and females but impaired social functioning only in women with FEP (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Montalvo, Creus, Cabezas, Solé, Algora and Labad2016). Kincaid et al. (Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson and Mulholland2018) found poorer theory of mind performance in males than females with schizophrenia.

There were also two studies supporting a moderating role of timing of CM exposure and emotional neglect, with CM during early childhood (0–6 years) specifically predicting theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia (Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson and Mulholland2018), and neglect experienced at 11–12 years specifically predicting social cognition impairment (Schalinski et al., Reference Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus and Rockstroh2018).

There is consistent evidence for a dose-response-relation (cumulative effect) for severity (n = 6) and number of CM experiences (n = 5) being linked to more pronounced social functioning or social cognition impairments in PD (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Kauppi, Brandt, Tesli, Kaufmann, Steen and Melle2017; Li et al., Reference Li, Li, Liu, Zhang, Tang and Wang2015; Lindgren et al., Reference Lindgren, Mäntylä, Rikandi, Torniainen-Holm, Morales-Muñoz, Kieseppä and Suvisaari2017; Penney et al., Reference Penney, Pruessner, Malla, Joober and Lepage2022; Schalinski et al., Reference Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus and Rockstroh2018) across all illness stages.

There are mixed results on the moderating effects of different types of CM (Bosqui et al., Reference Bosqui, Shannon, Tiernan, Beattie, Ferguson and Mulholland2014; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Montalvo, Creus, Cabezas, Solé, Algora and Labad2016), with seven studies finding both physical and emotional neglect being the strongest predictors of diminished global social functioning (Gil et al., Reference Gil, Gama, de Jesus, Lobato, Zimmer and Belmonte-de-Abreu2009; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019), interpersonal relations (by anxious attachment) (Aydin et al., Reference Aydin, Balikci, Tas, Aydin, Danaci, Bruene and Lysaker2016), as well as impaired emotion processing (Kilian et al., Reference Kilian, Asmal, Chiliza, Olivier, Phahladira, Scheffler and Emsley2018; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holleran, Holland, Costello and Donohoe2021), empathy (cognitive trait) (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kwon, Min, Kim, Jin and Lee2019) and (affective) theory of mind (Vaskinn et al., Reference Vaskinn, Melle, Aas and Berg2021) in non-affective PD. There is also evidence (n = 3) on physical and sexual abuse being the strongest predictors of impaired interpersonal relations (Trotta et al., Reference Trotta, Murray, David, Kolliakou, O'Connor, Di Forti and Fisher2016) and aggressive behaviour in schizophrenia (Bosqui et al., Reference Bosqui, Shannon, Tiernan, Beattie, Ferguson and Mulholland2014; Hachtel et al., Reference Hachtel, Fullam, Malone, Murphy, Huber and Carroll2020).

Finally, there is little evidence (n = 1) for moderating effects of neurocognitive functions, with poorer executive function and physical abuse predicting aggressive behaviour in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Wright, Clements and Plascak-Hallberg2002).

Of note, none of the included studies examined the potential moderating role of the duration of illness or diagnosis type (e.g. affective v. non affective psychosis).

Mediators

There was some evidence (n = 3) for a mediation role of depressive symptoms between CM and impaired global social functioning in schizophrenia (Andrianarisoa et al., Reference Andrianarisoa, Boyer, Godin, Brunel, Bulzacka and Aouizerate2017), and occupational functioning in FEP (Ortega et al., Reference Ortega, Montalvo, Solé, Creus, Cabezas, Gutiérrez-Zotes and Labad2020), as well as emotion processing in PD (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Kauppi, Brandt, Tesli, Kaufmann, Steen and Melle2017). There is also evidence (n = 2) that maladaptive personality traits (Boyette et al., Reference Boyette, van Dam, Meijer, Velthorst, Cahn, de Haan and Myin-Germeys2014; Lopez-Mongay et al., Reference Lopez-Mongay, Ahuir, Crosas, Blas Navarro, Antonio Monreal, Obiols and Palao2021) may mediate between CM and social functioning and relations.

There is emerging evidence (from one study in each mediator), through the duration of untreated psychosis and poor premorbid functioning (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Andreassen, Aminoff, Færden, Romm, Nesvåg and Melle2016) in the association between CM and social outcomes. There is also evidence that conduct disorder may mediate between cumulative childhood adversities and adult propensity to aggressive behaviour (Oakley et al., Reference Oakley, Harris, Fahy, Murphy and Picchioni2016). Finally, Weijers et al., found that in those with non-affective PD, mentalising impairment mediates the relationship between CM and clinical outcomes (e.g. severity of negative and positive symptoms) but not between CM and social (dys)function (Weijers et al., Reference Weijers, Fonagy, Eurelings-Bontekoe, Termorshuizen, Viechtbauer and Selten2018).

Discussion

This systematic review and meta-analysis investigated associations between overall and different subtypes of CM and different domains of social functioning and social cognition in adults with PD. Across the identified studies, we found an association between CM and impaired social functioning in PD. This finding is in line with the vast literature on clinical (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Christy, Rodriguez, Salazar de Pablo, Thrush, Shen and Murray2021), psychological, neurobiological (Bramon & Murray, Reference Bramon and Murray2001; Lim, Radua, & Rubia, Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Read, Perry, Moskowitz, & Connolly, Reference Read, Perry, Moskowitz and Connolly2001; Read et al., Reference Read, Fosse, Moskowitz and Perry2014; Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016) and neurocognitive (McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022) alterations associated with CM that are likely to impact social functioning (Pfaltz et al., Reference Pfaltz, Halligan, Haim-Nachum, Sopp, Åhs, Bachem and Seedat2022). This finding is also in line with our initial hypothesis and with the only previous meta-analysis on the topic (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022). The associations were overall small (with weak effects), and findings differed essentially in consistency depending on the social domain considered, suggesting differential and specific effects. However, against our initial hypotheses and prior evidence suggesting a link between CM and social cognition (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holleran, Holland, Costello and Donohoe2021), the results of our meta-analysis do not support, with the limited data existing at this stage, an association between CM and social cognition domains in individuals with PD.

In our study, the most consistent associations across overall and CM subtypes were found for the impaired interpersonal relations and aggressive behaviour in PD. This is in line with findings of a recent meta-analysis in affective disorders (Fares-Otero et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023), which may reflect a transdiagnostic effect of CM – particularly regarding difficulties in interpersonal behaviour and interactions. These difficulties might reflect early attachment-related problems, maladaptive internalised schemas (Messman-Moore & Coates, Reference Messman-Moore and Coates2007), and heightened sensitivity to interpersonal stress, which may have implications for problematic interpersonal adaptation, poor pro-social coping (e.g. overcompensation, avoidance, or surrender), help-seeking, and social withdrawal.

Furthermore, even though the risk of violence perpetration increases in individuals with a history of CM (Fitton, Yu, & Fazel, Reference Fitton, Yu and Fazel2020), our results should not be interpreted as generalised problems in prosocial behaviour or even as antisocial tendencies in individuals with PD and CM. In fact, the incidence of hostile or aggressive behaviour in PD is rather low (Faay et al., Reference Faay, van Os, van Amelsvoort, Bartels-Velthuis, Bruggeman and van Os2020; Fusar-Poli, Sunkel, & Patel, Reference Fusar-Poli, Sunkel and Patel2022). Further (longitudinal) research on associations between all CM subtypes and social interactions, considering comorbid personality traits, impulsivity, substance use, and environmental factors in PD is needed.

Our findings on the association between CM and poor social functioning replicate earlier work (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022) by showing that CM exposure relates to impairments in global measure of social functioning but not to occupational functioning. Of note, in our study, the finding on the negative association between overall CM and global social functioning in PD can be considered more accurate (than the previous meta-analysis) (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022) because our inclusion criteria was stricter as we only examined baseline data, without any intervention involved, and only in adults with PD.

Whether social functioning impairment precedes PD, or vice versa remains unclear. Recent evidence (McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022) indicates that whilst social problems are likely to arise where a history of CM is present, they might also put the child at greater risk of further negative social experiences and interactions, such as greater maltreatment (e.g. bullying) later in adolescence, and limit future opportunities for social learning and support throughout the lifespan. Therefore, whether associations between CM and social functioning and interactions in PD may in fact be bidirectional should be examined in future prospective studies.

We also replicate previous findings supporting that physical (Gil et al., Reference Gil, Gama, de Jesus, Lobato, Zimmer and Belmonte-de-Abreu2009) and emotional neglect is associated with higher impairment in social functioning in PD (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022; Sideli et al., Reference Sideli, Schimmenti, La Barbera, La Cascia, Ferraro and Aas2022) than other CM subtypes. We found associations between sexual abuse and global social functioning, which is a novel finding maybe due to additional (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Andreassen, Aminoff, Færden, Romm, Nesvåg and Melle2016; Akbey et al., Reference Akbey, Yildiz and Gündüz2019; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Montalvo, Creus, Cabezas, Solé, Algora and Labad2016; Gil et al., Reference Gil, Gama, de Jesus, Lobato, Zimmer and Belmonte-de-Abreu2009) and newly (Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Del Cacho, Butjosa, Serra Arumí, Esteban Santjusto, Abella and Usall2022) included studies leading to bigger sample sizes to examine this association (that was not significant in the previous meta-analysis) (Christy et al., Reference Christy, Cavero, Navajeeva, Murray-O'Shea, Rodriguez, Aas and Alameda2022). The fact that findings generally replicated across subtypes of CM raises further questions about the underlying mechanisms that are altered by these diverse adverse childhood experiences, with likely broad consequences for social outcomes. Understanding these mechanisms could provide new intervention targets for individuals with PD and a history of CM.

We explored independent living, but did not find an association between CM and this important domain in people with PD (Ang, Rekhi, & Lee, Reference Ang, Rekhi and Lee2021). Altogether, it seems that CM exposure relates to social functioning impairment globally and to impaired specific domains, but not to independent living or occupational functioning. Of note, only overall CM was examined, and independent living was mainly based on living status while occupational functioning on employment status measures in the included studies. More studies are needed assessing associations between all CM subtypes and financial issues, and education or academic functioning in PD.

The suggested association between CM and social cognition in previous reviews (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann and Donohoe2018) was not confirmed in our study using a quantitative approach. Nonetheless, evidence in this area is based on a limited number of studies, with only two social cognition domains having sufficient data for meta-analysis. Further studies are needed on less explored domains such as attributional style/bias and empathy, and on not yet explored domains such as social perception or knowledge. While overall, there was no meta-analytic evidence for a relationship between CM and social cognition, some of the research summarised in our narrative review suggests that for specific subpopulations, there might in fact exist such a relationship. For instance, a relationship between CM and impaired social cognition has been observed (Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis and van Winkel2019; Penney et al., Reference Penney, Pruessner, Malla, Joober and Lepage2022) that maybe stronger in males with PD (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Montalvo, Creus, Cabezas, Solé, Algora and Labad2016) and in certain development periods (Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson and Mulholland2018), or even found to be positive in FEP (Pena-Garijo & Monfort-Escrig, Reference Pena-Garijo and Monfort-Escrig2021). Differences in assessment instruments may explain the mixed results as studies using the same social cognition instruments (Hinting Task), but not the same trauma instruments (CTQ, CAMIR, and CECA-Q) found differing results. Future attempts to understand the socio-cognitive underpinnings of associations between CM and wider social functions in PD are critically needed.

There is evidence of a relationship between social functioning and social cognition in PD (Couture, Penn, & Roberts, Reference Couture, Penn and Roberts2006). Indeed, social cognition refers to the mental operations underlying social interactions (Green, Reference Green2016; Green, Horan, & Lee, Reference Green, Horan and Lee2015). There is also evidence supporting the link between adversity and poorer social functioning, and between social cognition and impairment in social functional outcomes in PD, especially in chronic stages (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021). Yet, in a recent systematic review of longitudinal studies on the relationship between cognition and social functioning in FEP, findings regarding social cognition were not unanimous (Montaner-Ferrer, Gadea, & Sanjuán, Reference Montaner-Ferrer, Gadea and Sanjuán2023). Taken together, there is still a gap in the literature regarding the role of social cognition in the association between CM (and its subtypes) and domains of social functioning, with a particular focus on social interactions, at different stages of PD.

In meta-regression analyses, we found some evidence for associations that may be stronger in non-affective samples – between overall CM and interpersonal relations, and between physical abuse and aggressive behaviour in males. There was some evidence of an association that may be weaker in males between physical neglect and global social functioning. However, findings stem from <10 studies precluding substantial conclusions. In line with previous work (Fares-Otero et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023) there was very little evidence for other moderation effects, and no consistent pattern. Future studies on moderating factors between CM (across all subtypes) and social functioning in PD are needed.

As a main finding, the number of relevant studies on associations between CM subtypes and social functioning, and social cognition was small. Given the major importance of CM for the course of PD (Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin, & Varese, Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012; Schäfer & Fisher, Reference Schäfer and Fisher2011), and that CM is related to various characteristics associated with social impairment (McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022; Pfaltz et al., Reference Pfaltz, Halligan, Haim-Nachum, Sopp, Åhs, Bachem and Seedat2022), our analysis shows that CM is understudied regarding social features in PD. CM is still less likely to be recognised in PD than other mental disorders (Read, Sampson, & Critchley, Reference Read, Sampson and Critchley2016). Clinicians themselves report that they are less likely to ask patients about CM histories if they are diagnosed with PD (Neill & Read, Reference Neill and Read2022; Read, Harper, Tucker, & Kennedy, Reference Read, Harper, Tucker and Kennedy2018; Read et al., Reference Read, Sampson and Critchley2016). However, 32 studies of the 53 included in this work were published within the last five years, which is in line with the growing interest and empirical findings regarding the importance of CM in PD (Kaufman & Torbey, Reference Kaufman and Torbey2019; Teicher, Gordon, & Nemeroff, Reference Teicher, Gordon and Nemeroff2022). This underlines the importance of further investigations (see SA8 in the supplement) of the relationship between CM and PD also regarding social outcomes.

Clinical Implications

The results of our meta-analysis suggest that it would be beneficial to systematically assess CM in routine care as a standard practice in (mental) health settings (Neill & Read, Reference Neill and Read2022). Clinicians should ask about all types of CM experiences (Read, Hammersley, & Rudegeair, Reference Read, Hammersley and Rudegeair2007; Read et al., Reference Read, Harper, Tucker and Kennedy2018), implement meaningful measures for its detection and provide effective service responses (Campodonico, Varese, & Berry, Reference Campodonico, Varese and Berry2022). Extra-training on CM and its social consequences for (mental) health professionals supporting those with PD is indicated. In addition to trauma-focused therapy (van den Berg et al., Reference van den Berg, de Bont, van der Vleugel, de Roos, de Jongh, van Minnen and van der Gaag2018), our findings suggest that individuals with PD and different CM subtypes might benefit from additional treatment components, that target social circumstances (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Steare, Dedat, Pilling, McCrone, Knapp and Lloyd-Evans2022) and interactions (Faay & Sommer, Reference Faay and Sommer2021; Flechsenhar, Kanske, Krach, Korn, & Bertsch, Reference Flechsenhar, Kanske, Krach, Korn and Bertsch2022) and social (aggressive) behaviour. Interventions to counteract negative social anticipations might also be beneficial. Such approaches might be further supported by corrective, positive relationship experiences, including therapeutic engagement (Spidel, Lecomte, Kealy, & Daigneault, Reference Spidel, Lecomte, Kealy and Daigneault2018) and communication (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, John, Dooley, Healey, Cushing, Kingdon and Priebe2016). Improving social attitudes, building trust and positive beliefs about self and others (Fowler, Hodgekins, & French, Reference Fowler, Hodgekins and French2019), and reducing feelings of guilt and/or shame (Sekowski et al., Reference Sekowski, Gambin, Cudo, Wozniak-Prus, Penner, Fonagy and Sharp2020) might be valuable strategies to improve resilience in individuals with PD and CM at early illness stages (Arango et al., Reference Arango, Buitelaar, Correll, Díaz-Caneja, Figueira, Fleischhacker and Vitiello2022; Vieta & Berk, Reference Vieta and Berk2022). Psychoeducation on both PD diagnosis and the consequences of CM might also prove helpful for these individuals.

As suggested in our narrative review, depressive symptoms and maladaptive personality traits might be mediators in the pathway between CM and social functioning (Andrianarisoa et al., Reference Andrianarisoa, Boyer, Godin, Brunel, Bulzacka and Aouizerate2017; Ortega et al., Reference Ortega, Montalvo, Solé, Creus, Cabezas, Gutiérrez-Zotes and Labad2020) which is in line with previous research (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Golay, Baumann, Progin, Mebdouhi, Elowe and Conus2017; Kampling et al., Reference Kampling, Kruse, Lampe, Nolte, Hettich, Brähler and Riedl2022) and a model on the affective pathway to psychosis (Alameda, Conus, Ramain, Solida, & Golay, Reference Alameda, Conus, Ramain, Solida and Golay2022; Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Rodriguez, Carr, Aas, Trotta, Marino and Murray2020), suggesting that treatment of sub-diagnostic levels of depressive symptoms and psychotherapy targeting personality functioning (Kampling et al., Reference Kampling, Kruse, Lampe, Nolte, Hettich, Brähler and Riedl2022) (and therapeutic relationship) (Picken, Berry, Tarrier, & Barrowclough, Reference Picken, Berry, Tarrier and Barrowclough2010) could help to improve psychotic symptoms, as well as social outcomes.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this study include the rigorous methodology with the systematic search, study selection, and data extraction all performed by independent researchers, the inclusion of studies published in English, German, and Spanish, the evaluation of the quality of individual studies, and other key practices for meta-analysis.

On the other hand, some limitations must be considered when interpreting the presented findings. First, the number of studies available for some meta-analyses was small, as were the sample sizes of many studies, meaning that some analyses may not have been sufficiently powered for detecting small effects, and the capacity to examine heterogeneity and moderators was limited (Jackson & Turner, Reference Jackson and Turner2017). Even with the DerSimonian-Laird estimator (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2022), extra caution is needed for conclusions, particularly if the number of studies in a model is small (Jackson & Turner, Reference Jackson and Turner2017). However, we followed the Cochrane recommendations (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2022) and the number of studies included in meta-analyses was constrained by the limited number of studies that examined CM and social functioning and social cognition in PD. Second, the effect sizes found in our analyses were generally weak. We need to consider that some of the significant results found in this review maybe dependent on sample sizes bias and affected by potential confounding variables not addressed by the included studies (e.g. duration of untreated PD). Third, it was impossible to account for all the possible variations across instruments utilised, although most studies assessed social outcomes with robust tools. Fourth, most of the identified studies were cross-sectional. Recent evidence (McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022) indicates a potential bidirectional association between CM and social impairment, so whether associations between CM and social outcomes in PD may be bidirectional should be prospectively examined. Fifth, all the included studies used retrospective assessments of CM, which has been criticised (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004). However, empirical studies show that retrospective self-reports on the presence of CM are sufficiently reliable, and provide strong support for their validity (Baldwin, Reuben, Newbury, & Danese, Reference Baldwin, Reuben, Newbury and Danese2019; Newbury et al., Reference Newbury, Arseneault, Moffitt, Caspi, Danese, Baldwin and Fisher2018). Sixth, we did not focus on associations between CM and social outcomes in healthy samples. It would be interesting in future research to replicate our findings in those without PD and explore whether association effects are similar in consistency and magnitude across social domains or whether associations are specific to individuals with PD. Finally, the exclusion of grey literature may lead to less heterogeneity in study quality but can also cause relevant findings to be missed. Nonetheless, the methodological quality of all included studies was assessed to examine the degree to which study design, conduct and analyses minimised potential errors and bias.

Conclusion

This meta-analysis informs being exposed to CM (abuse and neglect) is related to impaired social relations and functioning in individuals with PD. These social impairments suggest intervention targets and make the development of a trauma and social working model of importance for maltreated adults with PD. However, published studies on the relationship between CM (and its subtypes) and social functioning, and in particular social cognition in PD are scarce, and further longitudinal studies in non-affective psychosis and in FEP are needed. The potential role of moderation and mediation factors (e.g. illness duration, type of diagnosis) in the relationship between CM and social outcomes warrants further investigation. Nevertheless, it seems critical to consider CM with view to the clinical assessment, diagnosis, and interventions for PD. Further research should identify mechanisms through which CM contributes to worsen social functioning to provide a better basis for identifying individuals with specific needs to provide direction for prevention and to inform early tailored interventions targeting not only the reduction of psychopathology, but also enhancing social interactions (Flechsenhar et al., Reference Flechsenhar, Kanske, Krach, Korn and Bertsch2022) (e.g. guiding them to establish healthy relationships, reconfiguring behaviour patterns), and functioning (Morse et al., Reference Morse, Sandhu, Mulligan, Tierney, Polley, Giurca and Husk2022) (e.g. helping them to develop social roles and skills). Addressing depressive symptoms and psychotherapy targeting personality seems also important although more research is needed to test whether such treatments can specifically improve social outcomes in individuals with PD suffering from the consequences of CM.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001678.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from NEF-O upon reasonable request. NEF-O has full access to all data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analyses.

Acknowledgements

We thank Jose Manuel Estrada Lorenzo, who was involved in the literature search (strategy design, databases, and full-text retrieval), Lena-Marie Neuhaus and Svetlana Wiese who were involved in the literature assessment and data extraction, and Sebastian Trautmann who was involved in the quality assessment and previous version of the manuscript.

Authors’ contributions

Conceptualisation: NEF-O, LA. Methodology: NEF-O, LA. Data collection: NEF-O. Data curation: NEF-O. Writing – original draft: NEF-O. Writing – reviewing & editing: NEF-O, LA, MCP, AM-A, IS, EV. Formal analysis: NEF-O, LA. Software: NEF-O. Validation: NEF-O. Visualisation: NEF-O. Investigation: NEF-O. Supervision: LA, EV. Project administration: NEF-O. Resources, Funding acquisition: NEF-O, EV. All authors were involved in the interpretation of the data and approved the final version of the submitted manuscript.

Financial support

NEF-O thanks the support of the German Academic Exchange Agency DAAD (91629413), the European Union Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (EU.3.1.3. Treating and managing disease: Grant No 945151), CIBER -Consorcio Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red- (CIBERSAM), ISCIII, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación and Unión Europea – European Regional Development Fund. EV thanks the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (PI15/00283; PI18/00805; PI21/00787) integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación y el Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER); CIBERSAM; the Comissionat per a Universitats i Recerca del DIUE de la Generalitat de Catalunya to the Bipolar Disorders Group (2021 SGR 1358) and project SLT006/17/00357, from PERIS 2016–2020 (Departament de Salut), CERCA Programme/Generalitat de Catalunya. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report.

Competing interest

EV has received grants and served as consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following entities: AB-Biotics, AbbVie, Angelini, Biogen, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Celon Pharma, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, Ferrer, Gedeon Richter, GH Research, Glaxo-Smith Kline, Janssen, Lundbeck, Novartis, Orion Corporation, Organon, Otsuka, Sage, Sanofi-Aventis, Sunovion, Takeda, and Viatris, outside the submitted work. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

Note. References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis (see the full list of the included studies in SA6 in the supplement).Google Scholar
*Aas, M., Andreassen, O. A., Aminoff, S. R., Færden, A., Romm, K. L., Nesvåg, R., … Melle, I. (2016). A history of childhood trauma is associated with slower improvement rates: Findings from a one-year follow-up study of patients with a first-episode psychosis. BMC Psychiatry, 16, 126126. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-0827-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Aas, M., Dazzan, P., Mondelli, V., Melle, I., Murray, R. M., & Pariante, C. M. (2014). A systematic review of cognitive function in first-episode psychosis, including a discussion on childhood trauma, stress, and inflammation. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 182182. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aas, M., Kauppi, K., Brandt, C. L., Tesli, M., Kaufmann, T., Steen, N. E., … Melle, I. (2017). Childhood trauma is associated with increased brain responses to emotionally negative as compared with positive faces in patients with psychotic disorders. Psychological Medicine, 47(4), 669679. PubMed (27834153). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716002762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ajnakina, O., Trotta, A., Forti, M. D., Stilo, S. A., Kolliakou, A., Gardner-Sood, P., … Fisher, H. L. (2018). Different types of childhood adversity and 5-year outcomes in a longitudinal cohort of first-episode psychosis patients. Psychiatry Research, 269, 199206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Akbey, Z. Y., Yildiz, M., & Gündüz, N. (2019). Is there any association between childhood traumatic experiences, dissociation and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenic patients? Psychiatry Investigation, 16(5), 346354. https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2019.02.10.2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alameda, L., Christy, A., Rodriguez, V., Salazar de Pablo, G., Thrush, M., Shen, Y., … Murray, R. M. (2021). Association between specific childhood adversities and symptom dimensions in people with psychosis: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 47(4), 975985. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, L., Conus, P., Ramain, J., Solida, A., & Golay, P. (2022). Evidence of mediation of severity of anxiety and depressive symptoms between abuse and positive symptoms of psychosis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 150, 353359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.11.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, L., Ferrari, C., Baumann, P. S., Gholam-Rezaee, M., Do, K. Q., & Conus, P. (2015). Childhood sexual and physical abuse: Age at exposure modulates impact on functional outcome in early psychosis patients. Psychological Medicine, 45(13), 27272736. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715000690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, L., Golay, P., Baumann, P. S., Ferrari, C., Do, K. Q., & Conus, P. (2016). Age at the time of exposure to trauma modulates the psychopathological profile in patients with early psychosis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(5), e6128. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.15m09947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, L., Golay, P., Baumann, P. S., Progin, P., Mebdouhi, N., Elowe, J., … Conus, P. (2017). Mild depressive symptoms mediate the impact of childhood trauma on long-term functional outcome in early psychosis patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(5), 10271035. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, L., Rodriguez, V., Carr, E., Aas, M., Trotta, G., Marino, P., … Murray, R. M. (2020). A systematic review on mediators between adversity and psychosis: Potential targets for treatment. Psychological Medicine, 50(12), 19661976. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albajes-Eizagirre, A., Solanes, A., & Radua, J. (2019). Meta-analysis of non-statistically significant unreported effects. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 28(12), 37413754. https://doi.org/10.1177/0962280218811349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, N., Uddin, J., & Nordentoft, M. (2018). Social functioning in patients with first-episode psychosis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(1), 34. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30475-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G., Simera, I., Hoey, J., Moher, D., & Schulz, K. (2008). EQUATOR: Reporting guidelines for health research. The Lancet, 371(9619), 11491150. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60505-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, K. K. (2019). Towards a public health approach to psychotic disorders. The Lancet Public Health, 4(5), e212e213. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30054-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Andrianarisoa, M., Boyer, L., Godin, O., Brunel, L., Bulzacka, E., & Aouizerate, B., … FACE-SCZ Group. (2017). Childhood trauma, depression and negative symptoms are independently associated with impaired quality of life in schizophrenia. Results from the national FACE-SZ cohort. Schizophrenia Research, 185, 173181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.12.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ang, M. S., Rekhi, G., & Lee, J. (2021). Associations of living arrangements with symptoms and functioning in schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry, 21(1), 497. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03488-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arango, C., Buitelaar, J. K., Correll, C. U., Díaz-Caneja, C. M., Figueira, M. L., Fleischhacker, W. W., … Vitiello, B. (2022). The transition from adolescence to adulthood in patients with schizophrenia: Challenges, opportunities and recommendations. European Neuropsychopharmacology: The Journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 59, 4555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2022.04.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Aydin, O., Balikci, K., Tas, C., Aydin, P. U., Danaci, A. E., Bruene, M., & Lysaker, P. H. (2016). The developmental origins of metacognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 245, 1521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. R., Reuben, A., Newbury, J. B., & Danese, A. (2019). Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 584593. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, P., Steare, T., Dedat, Z., Pilling, S., McCrone, P., Knapp, M., … Lloyd-Evans, B. (2022). Interventions to improve social circumstances of people with mental health conditions: A rapid evidence synthesis. BMC Psychiatry, 22(1), 302. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03864-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellack, A. S., Green, M. F., Cook, J. A., Fenton, W., Harvey, P. D., Heaton, R. K., … Wykes, T. (2007). Assessment of community functioning in people with schizophrenia and other severe mental illnesses: A white paper based on an NIMH-sponsored workshop. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 805822. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentall, R. P., Wickham, S., Shevlin, M., & Varese, F. (2012). Do specific early-life adversities lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 the adult psychiatric morbidity survey. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 734740. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., … Zule, W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169190. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, A., Bernazzani, O., Moran, P. M., & Jacobs, C. (2005). The childhood experience of care and abuse questionnaire (CECA.Q): Validation in a community series. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(Pt 4), 563581. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X35344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birchwood, M., McGorry, P., & Jackson, H. (1997). Early intervention in schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 170(1), 25. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.170.1.2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonfils, K. A., Lysaker, P. H., Minor, K. S., & Salyers, M. P. (2017). Empathy in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the interpersonal reactivity Index. Psychiatry Research, 249, 293303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M. (2022a). Comprehensive meta-analysis software. In Egger, M., Higgins, J. P. T., & Smith, G. D. (Eds.), Systematic reviews in health research (pp. 535548). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119099369.ch27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borenstein, M. (2022b). In a meta-analysis, the I-squared statistic does not tell us how much the effect size varies. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 152, 281284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2022.10.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, H. R. (2011). Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Google Scholar
*Bosqui, T. J., Shannon, C., Tiernan, B., Beattie, N., Ferguson, J., & Mulholland, C. (2014). Childhood trauma and the risk of violence in adulthood in a population with a psychotic illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 54(1), 121125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.03.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Boyette, L.-L., van Dam, D., Meijer, C., Velthorst, E., Cahn, W., de Haan, L., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2014). Personality compensates for impaired quality of life and social functioning in patients with psychotic disorders who experienced traumatic events. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(6), 13561365. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramon, E., & Murray, R. M. (2001). A plausible model of schizophrenia must incorporate psychological and social, as well as neuro developmental, risk factors. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 3(4), 243256. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2001.3.4/ebramon.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Brañas, A., Lahera, G., Barrigón, M. L., Canal-Rivero, M., & Ruiz-Veguilla, M. (2022). Effects of childhood trauma on facial recognition of fear in psychosis. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental (English Edition), 15(1), 2937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpsmen.2022.01.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breitborde, N. J. K., Srihari, V. H., & Woods, S. W. (2009). Review of the operational definition for first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 3(4), 259265. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7893.2009.00148.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brüne, M. (2005). “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 2142. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbi002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campodonico, C., Varese, F., & Berry, K. (2022). Trauma and psychosis: A qualitative study exploring the perspectives of people with psychosis on the influence of traumatic experiences on psychotic symptoms and quality of life. BMC Psychiatry, 22(1), 213. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03808-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Chalker, S. A., Parrish, E. M., Cano, M., Kelsven, S., Moore, R. C., Granholm, E., … Depp, C. A. (2022). Childhood trauma associations with the interpersonal psychological theory of suicide and social cognitive biases in psychotic disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 210(6), 432438. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000001462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S., Liu, Y., Liu, D., Zhang, G., & Wu, X. (2021). The difference of social cognitive and neurocognitive performance between patients with schizophrenia at different stages and influencing factors. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 24, 100195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2021.100195.Google ScholarPubMed
Christy, A., Cavero, D., Navajeeva, S., Murray-O'Shea, R., Rodriguez, V., Aas, M., … Alameda, L. (2022). Association between childhood adversity and functional outcomes in people with psychosis: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(2), sbac105. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac105.Google Scholar
Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., & Roberts, D. L. (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32 (Suppl 1), S44S63. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Cui, Y., Kim, S.-W., Lee, B. J., Kim, J. J., Yu, J.-C., Lee, K. Y., … Chung, Y.-C. (2019). Negative schema and rumination as mediators of the relationship between childhood trauma and recent suicidal ideation in patients with early psychosis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 80(3), 17m12088. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.17m12088.Google ScholarPubMed
de Winter, L., Couwenbergh, C., van Weeghel, J., Hasson-Ohayon, I., Vermeulen, J. M., Mulder, C. L., … Veling, W. (2021). Changes in social functioning over the course of psychotic disorders-A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 239, 5582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2021.11.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorn, L. M.-L., Struck, N., Bitsch, F., Falkenberg, I., Kircher, T., Rief, W., & Mehl, S. (2021). The relationship between different aspects of theory of mind and symptom clusters in psychotic disorders: Deconstructing theory of mind into cognitive, affective, and hyper theory of mind. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 607154. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.607154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DSM-5 Diagnostic Classification. (2013). In DSM Library. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Association. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.x00DiagnosticClassification.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelstad, K. N., Rund, B. R., Lau, B., Vaskinn, A., & Torgalsbøen, A. K. (2019). Increased prevalence of psychopathy and childhood trauma in homicide offenders with schizophrenia compared to nonviolent individuals with schizophrenia. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 73(8), 501508. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039488.2019.1656777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faay, M. D. M., & Sommer, I. E. (2021). Risk and prevention of aggression in patients with psychotic disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 178(3), 218220. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.21010035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Faay, M. D. M., van Os, J., van Amelsvoort, T., Bartels-Velthuis, A. A., Bruggeman, R., … van Os, J. (2020). Aggressive behavior, hostility, and associated care needs in patients with psychotic disorders: A 6-year follow-up study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00934.10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fares-Otero, N. E., De Prisco, M., Oliva, V., Radua, J., Halligan, S. L., Vieta, E., & Martinez-Aran, A. (2023). Association between childhood maltreatment and social functioning in individuals with affective disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, n/a(n/a). https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13557.Google Scholar
Fares-Otero, N. E., Pfaltz, M. C., Rodriguez-Jimenez, R., Schäfer, I., & Trautmann, S. (2021). Childhood maltreatment and social functioning in psychotic disorders: A systematic review protocol. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 12(1), 1943872. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2021.1943872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., & Gibbon, M. (2004). The structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I) and the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II disorders (SCID-II). Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, Vol. 2: Personality assessment (pp. 134143). Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Fitton, L., Yu, R., & Fazel, S. (2020). Childhood maltreatment and violent outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 21(4), 754768. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838018795269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flechsenhar, A., Kanske, P., Krach, S., Korn, C., & Bertsch, K. (2022). The (un)learning of social functions and its significance for mental health. Clinical Psychology Review, 98, 102204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, D., Hodgekins, J., & French, P. (2019). Social recovery therapy in improving activity and social outcomes in early psychosis: Current evidence and longer term outcomes. Schizophrenia Research, 203, 99104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.10.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Sunkel, C., & Patel, V. (2022). Associating violence with schizophrenia – risks and biases. JAMA Psychiatry 79(7), 738739. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Garcia, M., Montalvo, I., Creus, M., Cabezas, Á, Solé, M., Algora, M. J., … Labad, J. (2016). Sex differences in the effect of childhood trauma on the clinical expression of early psychosis. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 68, 8696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.04.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Gil, A., Gama, C. S., de Jesus, D. R., Lobato, M. I., Zimmer, M., & Belmonte-de-Abreu, P. (2009). The association of child abuse and neglect with adult disability in schizophrenia and the prominent role of physical neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33(9), 618624. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.02.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, R., Widom, C. S., Browne, K., Fergusson, D., Webb, E., & Janson, S. (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. Lancet (London, England), 373(9657), 6881. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61706-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F. (2016). Impact of cognitive and social cognitive impairment on functional outcomes in patients with schizophrenia. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77 (Suppl 2), 811. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.14074su1c.02.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Horan, W. P., & Lee, J. (2015). Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(10), 620631. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haahr, U. H., Larsen, T. K., Simonsen, E., Rund, B. R., Joa, I., Rossberg, J. I., … Melle, I. (2018). Relation between premorbid adjustment, duration of untreated psychosis and close interpersonal trauma in first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 12(3), 316323. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Hachtel, H., Fullam, R., Malone, A., Murphy, B. P., Huber, C., & Carroll, A. (2020). Victimization, violence and facial affect recognition in a community sample of first-episode psychosis patients. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 14(3), 283292. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardt, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45(2), 260273.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., Altman, D. G., Gøtzsche, P. C., Jüni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., … Sterne, J. A. C. (2011). The Cochrane collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ, 343, d5928. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d5928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., Welch, V. A. (eds). (2022). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.3 (updated February 2022). Cochrane.Google Scholar
Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2004). Controlling the risk of spurious findings from meta-regression. Statistics in Medicine, 23(11), 16631682. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557560. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Hjelseng, I. V., Vaskinn, A., Ueland, T., Lunding, S. H., Reponen, E. J., Steen, N. E., … Aas, M. (2020). Childhood trauma is associated with poorer social functioning in severe mental disorders both during an active illness phase and in remission. Schizophrenia Research 243, 241246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.03.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Hodann-Caudevilla, R. M., García, J. J. M., & Julián, F. A. B. (2021). Childhood trauma and personal recovery in schizophrenia: Mediating role of experiential avoidance and insecure attachment. Clinical Schizophrenia and Related Psychoses, 15(2). https://doi.org/10.3371/CSRP.RMGJ.070421.Google Scholar
Hughes, K., Bellis, M. A., Hardcastle, K. A., Sethi, D., Butchart, A., Mikton, C., … Dunne, M. P. (2017). The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet. Public Health, 2(8), e356e366. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, D., & Turner, R. (2017). Power analysis for random-effects meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(3), 290302. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Javed, A., & Charles, A. (2018). The importance of social cognition in improving functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00157.10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kampling, H., Kruse, J., Lampe, A., Nolte, T., Hettich, N., Brähler, E., … Riedl, D. (2022). Epistemic trust and personality functioning mediate the association between adverse childhood experiences and posttraumatic stress disorder and complex posttraumatic stress disorder in adulthood. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.919191.10.3389/fpsyt.2022.919191CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., & Torbey, S. (2019). Child maltreatment and psychosis. Neurobiology of Disease, 131, 104378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2019.01.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Kilian, S., Asmal, L., Chiliza, B., Olivier, M. R., Phahladira, L., Scheffler, F., … Emsley, R. (2018). Childhood adversity and cognitive function in schizophrenia spectrum disorders and healthy controls: Evidence for an association between neglect and social cognition. Psychological Medicine, 48(13), 21862193. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Kim, Y., Kwon, A., Min, D., Kim, S., Jin, M. J., & Lee, S.-H. (2019). Neurophysiological and psychological predictors of social functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Investigation, 16(10), 718727. https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2019.07.28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Kincaid, D., Shannon, C., Boyd, A., Hanna, D., McNeill, O., Anderson, R., … Mulholland, C. (2018). An investigation of associations between experience of childhood trauma and political violence and theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 270, 293297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lardinois, M., Lataster, T., Mengelers, R., Van Os, J., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2011). Childhood trauma and increased stress sensitivity in psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123(1), 2835. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01594.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, S., Andreassen, O. A., Aas, M., Røssberg, J. I., Mork, E., Steen, N. E., … Lorentzen, S. (2013). High prevalence of childhood trauma in patients with schizophrenia spectrum and affective disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(2), 123127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.06.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lataster, J., Myin-Germeys, I., Lieb, R., Wittchen, H.-U., & van Os, J. (2012). Adversity and psychosis: A 10-year prospective study investigating synergism between early and recent adversity in psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(5), 388399. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01805.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecomte, T., Giguère, C. É., Cloutier, B., Potvin, S., & Signature Consortium. (2020). Comorbidity profiles of psychotic patients in emergency psychiatry. Journal of Dual Diagnosis, 16(2), 260270. https://doi.org/10.1080/15504263.2020.1713425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Li, X.-B., Li, Q.-Y., Liu, J.-T., Zhang, L., Tang, Y.-L., & Wang, C.-Y. (2015). Childhood trauma associates with clinical features of schizophrenia in a sample of Chinese inpatients. Psychiatry Research, 228(3), 702707. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, L., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2014). Gray matter abnormalities in childhood maltreatment: A voxel-wise meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(8), 854863. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, L., & Chu, H. (2018). Quantifying publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 74(3), 785794. https://doi.org/10.1111/biom.12817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, T. M., Marin, N., & Jaya, E. S. (2017). Childhood trauma and psychotic experiences in a general population sample: A prospective study on the mediating role of emotion regulation. European Psychiatry, 42, 111119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.12.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Lindgren, M., Mäntylä, T., Rikandi, E., Torniainen-Holm, M., Morales-Muñoz, I., Kieseppä, T., … Suvisaari, J. (2017). Childhood adversities and clinical symptomatology in first-episode psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 258, 374381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, M., Stansfeld, J. L., Davies, N., Crellin, N. E., & Moncrieff, J. (2022). A systematic review of social functioning outcome measures in schizophrenia with a focus on suitability for intervention research. Schizophrenia Research, 241, 275291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2022.02.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-López, J. A., Van den Noortgate, W., Tanner-Smith, E. E., Wilson, S. J., & Lipsey, M. W. (2017). Assessing meta-regression methods for examining moderator relationships with dependent effect sizes: A Monte Carlo simulation. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(4), 435450. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Lopez-Mongay, D., Ahuir, M., Crosas, J. M., Blas Navarro, J., Antonio Monreal, J., Obiols, J. E., & Palao, D. (2021). The effect of child sexual abuse on social functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(7), NP3480NP3494. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260518779074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lysaker, P. H., Gumley, A., Brüne, M., Vanheule, S., Buck, K. D., & Dimaggio, G. (2011). Deficits in the ability to recognize one’s own affects and those of others: associations with neurocognition, symptoms and sexual trauma among persons with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 11831192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lysaker, P. H., Meyer, P. S., Evans, J. D., Clements, C. A., & Marks, K. A. (2001). Childhood sexual trauma and psychosocial functioning in adults with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services (Washington, DC), 52(11), 14851488. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.52.11.1485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Lysaker, P. H., Wright, D. E., Clements, C. A., & Plascak-Hallberg, C. D. (2002). Neurocognitive and psychosocial correlates of hostility among persons in a post-acute phase of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43(4), 319324. PubMed (12107869). https://doi.org/10.1053/comp.2002.33493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Mansueto, G., Schruers, K., Cosci, F., van Os, J., Alizadeh, B. Z., Bartels-Velthuis, A. A., … van Winkel, R. (2019). Childhood adversities and psychotic symptoms: The potential mediating or moderating role of neurocognition and social cognition. Schizophrenia Research, 206, 183193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.11.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, R., John, P., Dooley, J., Healey, P., Cushing, A., Kingdon, D., … Priebe, S. (2016). Training to enhance psychiatrist communication with patients with psychosis (TEMPO): Cluster randomised controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 209(6), 517524. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.115.179499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E., De Brito, S. A., & Viding, E. (2011). The impact of childhood maltreatment: A review of neurobiological and genetic factors. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E., Foulkes, L., & Viding, E. (2022). Social thinning and stress generation after childhood maltreatment: A neurocognitive social transactional model of psychiatric vulnerability. The Lancet Psychiatry, 9(10), 828837. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(22)00202-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E., Ogle, J. R., Gerin, M. I., & Viding, E. (2019). Neurocognitive adaptation and mental health vulnerability following maltreatment: The role of social functioning. Child Maltreatment, 24(4), 1077559519830524. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559519830524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D. (2015). Early intervention in psychosis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 203(5), 310318. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messman-Moore, T. L., & Coates, A. A. (2007). The impact of childhood psychological abuse on adult interpersonal conflict: The role of early maladaptive schemas and patterns of interpersonal behavior. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 7(2), 7592. https://doi.org/10.1300/J135v07n02_05.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Monfort-Escrig, C., & Pena-Garijo, J. (2021). Attachment dimensions predict social functioning in persons with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, regardless of symptom severity. Actas Espanolas de Psiquiatria, 49(6), 269281.Google ScholarPubMed
Montaner-Ferrer, M. J., Gadea, M., & Sanjuán, J. (2023). Cognition and social functioning in first episode psychosis: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 14. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1055012.10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1055012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, C., & Fisher, H. (2007). Environment and schizophrenia: Environmental factors in schizophrenia: Childhood trauma – A critical review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(1), 310. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morse, D. F., Sandhu, S., Mulligan, K., Tierney, S., Polley, M., Giurca, B. C., … Husk, K. (2022). Global developments in social prescribing. BMJ Global Health, 7(5), e008524. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjgh-2022-008524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarro-Mateu, F., Alonso, J., Lim, C. C. W., Saha, S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S.,, Al-Hamzawi, A., … WHO World Mental Health Survey Collaborators. (2017). The association between psychotic experiences and disability: Results from the WHO world mental health surveys. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 136(1), 7484. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neill, C., & Read, J. (2022). Adequacy of inquiry about, documentation of, and treatment of trauma and adversities: A study of mental health professionals in England. Community Mental Health Journal, 58(6), 10761087. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-021-00916-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newbury, J. B., Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Danese, A., Baldwin, J. R., & Fisher, H. L. (2018). Measuring childhood maltreatment to predict early-adult psychopathology: Comparison of prospective informant-reports and retrospective self-reports. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 96, 5764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.09.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakley, C., Harris, S., Fahy, T., Murphy, D., & Picchioni, M. (2016). Childhood adversity and conduct disorder: A developmental pathway to violence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 172(1), 5459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.01.047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olkin, I., & Pratt, J. W. (1958). Unbiased estimation of certain correlation coefficients. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29(1), 201211. https://doi.org/10.1214/aoms/1177706717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Ortega, L., Montalvo, I., Solé, M., Creus, M., Cabezas, Á, Gutiérrez-Zotes, A., … Labad, J. (2020). Relationship between childhood trauma and social adaptation in a sample of young people attending an early intervention service for psychosis. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental (English Edition), 13(3), 131139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpsm.2020.05.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paetzold, I., Myin-Germeys, I., Schick, A., Nelson, B., Velthorst, E., Schirmbeck, F., … Reininghaus, U. (2021). Stress reactivity as a putative mechanism linking childhood trauma with clinical outcomes in individuals at ultra-high-risk for psychosis: Findings from the EU-GEI high risk study. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 30, e40. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796021000251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., … Moher, D. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 372, n71. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71.Google ScholarPubMed
Pang, Y., Zhao, S., Li, Z., Li, N., Yu, J., Zhang, R., … Wang, J. (2022). Enduring effect of abuse: Childhood maltreatment links to altered theory of mind network among adults. Human Brain Mapping, 43(7), 22762288. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Pena-Garijo, J., & Monfort-Escrig, C. (2021). The centrality of secure attachment within an interacting network of symptoms, cognition, and attachment dimensions in persons with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: A preliminary study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 135, 6067.10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Penney, D., Pruessner, M., Malla, A. K., Joober, R., & Lepage, M. (2022). Severe childhood trauma and emotion recognition in males and females with first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 17(2), 149158. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.13299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfaltz, M. C., Halligan, S. L., Haim-Nachum, S., Sopp, M. R., Åhs, F., Bachem, R., … Seedat, S. (2022). Social functioning in individuals affected by childhood maltreatment: Establishing a research agenda to inform interventions. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 91(4), 238251. https://doi.org/10.1159/000523667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfaltz, M. C., Passardi, S., Auschra, B., Fares-Otero, N. E., Schnyder, U., & Peyk, P. (2019). Are you angry at me? Negative interpretations of neutral facial expressions are linked to child maltreatment but not to posttraumatic stress disorder. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1), 1682929. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1682929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picken, A. L., Berry, K., Tarrier, N., & Barrowclough, C. (2010). Traumatic events, posttraumatic stress disorder, attachment style, and working alliance in a sample of people with psychosis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198(10), 775778. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181f4b163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popay, J., Roberts, H., Sowden, A., Petticrew, M., Arai, L., Rodgers, M., … Duffy, S. (2006). Guidance on the conduct of narrative synthesis in systematic reviews: A product from the ESRC methods programme. UK: Lancaster University. https://doi.org/10.13140/2.1.1018.4643.Google Scholar
Practical Meta-Analysis. (2022). Retrieved 17 March 2022, from SAGE Publications Inc website. https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/practical-meta-analysis/book11092.Google Scholar
Pruessner, M., King, S., Veru, F., Schalinski, I., Vracotas, N., Abadi, S., … Joober, R. (2021). Impact of childhood trauma on positive and negative symptom remission in first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 231, 8289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2021.02.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Quide, Y., Cohen-Woods, S., O'Reilly, N., Carr, V. J., Elzinga, B. M., & Green, M. J. (2018). Schizotypal personality traits and social cognition are associated with childhood trauma exposure. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(4), 397419. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quide, Y., Ong, X. H., Mohnke, S., Schnell, K., Walter, H., Carr, V. J., & Green, M. J. (2017). Childhood trauma-related alterations in brain function during a Theory-of-Mind task in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 189, 162168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.02.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Ramsay, C. E., Flanagan, P., Gantt, S., Broussard, B., & Compton, M. T. (2011). Clinical correlates of maltreatment and traumatic experiences in childhood and adolescence among predominantly African American, socially disadvantaged, hospitalized, first-episode psychosis patients. Psychiatry Research, 188(3), 343349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratner, B. (2009). The correlation coefficient: Its values range between + 1/−1, or do they? Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing, 17(2), 139142. https://doi.org/10.1057/jt.2009.5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, J., Fosse, R., Moskowitz, A., & Perry, B. (2014). The traumagenic neurodevelopmental model of psychosis revisited. Neuropsychiatry, 4(1), 6579. https://doi.org/10.2217/NPY.13.89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, J., Hammersley, P., & Rudegeair, T. (2007). Why, when and how to ask about childhood abuse. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 13(2), 101110. https://doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.106.002840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, J., Harper, D., Tucker, I., & Kennedy, A. (2018). Do adult mental health services identify child abuse and neglect? A systematic review. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 27(1), 719. https://doi.org/10.1111/inm.12369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, J., Perry, B. D., Moskowitz, A., & Connolly, J. (2001). The contribution of early traumatic events to schizophrenia in some patients: A traumagenic neurodevelopmental model. Psychiatry, 64(4), 319345. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.64.4.319.18602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, J., Sampson, M., & Critchley, C. (2016). Are mental health services getting better at responding to abuse, assault and neglect? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 134(4), 287294. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reininghaus, U., Kempton, M. J., Valmaggia, L., Craig, T. K. J., Garety, P., Onyejiaka, A., … Morgan, C. (2016). Stress sensitivity, aberrant salience, and threat anticipation in early psychosis: An experience sampling study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(3), 712722. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, V., Aas, M., Vorontsova, N., Trotta, G., Gadelrab, R., Rooprai, N. K., & Alameda, L. (2021). Exploring the interplay between adversity, neurocognition, social cognition, and functional outcome in people with psychosis: A narrative review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.596949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rokita, K. I., Dauvermann, M. R., & Donohoe, G. (2018). Early life experiences and social cognition in major psychiatric disorders: A systematic review. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 53, 123133. PubMed (30144982). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.06.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Rokita, K. I., Dauvermann, M. R., Mothersill, D., Holleran, L., Holland, J., Costello, L., … Donohoe, G. (2021). Childhood trauma, parental bonding, and social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy adults. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 77(1), 241253. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rokita, K. I., Holleran, L., Dauvermann, M. R., Mothersill, D., Holland, J., Costello, L., … Donohoe, G. (2020). Childhood trauma, brain structure and emotion recognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy participants. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(12), 13251339. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Rosenberg, S. D., Lu, W., Mueser, K. T., Jankowski, M. K., & Cournos, F. (2007). Correlates of adverse childhood events among adults with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatric Services, 58(2), 245253. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.58.2.245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rucker, J., Newman, S., Gray, J., Gunasinghe, C., Broadbent, M., Brittain, P., … McGuffin, P. (2011). OPCRIT + : An electronic system for psychiatric diagnosis and data collection in clinical and research settings. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 199(2), 151155. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.110.082925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, J. D., Cheng, T. W., & Dunn, E. C. (2022). Sensitive periods in development and risk for psychiatric disorders and related endpoints: A systematic review of child maltreatment findings. The Lancet Psychiatry, 9(12), 978991. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(22)00362-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schäfer, I., & Fisher, H. L. (2011). Childhood trauma and psychosis – what is the evidence? Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 13(3), 360365.10.31887/DCNS.2011.13.2/ischaeferCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Schalinski, I., Teicher, M. H., Carolus, A. M., & Rockstroh, B. (2018). Defining the impact of childhood adversities on cognitive deficits in psychosis: An exploratory analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 192, 351356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.05.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekowski, M., Gambin, M., Cudo, A., Wozniak-Prus, M., Penner, F., Fonagy, P., & Sharp, C. (2020). The relations between childhood maltreatment, shame, guilt, depression and suicidal ideation in inpatient adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 276, 667677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Shah, S., Mackinnon, A., Galletly, C., Carr, V., McGrath, J. J., Stain, H. J., … Morgan, V. A. (2014). Prevalence and impact of childhood abuse in people with a psychotic illness. Data from the second Australian national survey of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 159(1), 2026. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.07.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sideli, L., Schimmenti, A., La Barbera, D., La Cascia, C., Ferraro, L., Aas, M., … EU-GEI WP2 Group. (2022). Childhood maltreatment, educational attainment, and IQ: Findings from a multicentric case–control study of first-episode psychosis (EU-GEI). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(3), 575589. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Spence, W., Mulholland, C., Lynch, G., McHugh, S., Dempster, M., & Shannon, C. (2006). Rates of childhood trauma in a sample of patients with schizophrenia as compared with a sample of patients with non-psychotic psychiatric diagnoses. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 7(3), 722. https://doi.org/10.1300/J229v07n03_02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Spidel, A., Lecomte, T., Greaves, C., Sahlstrom, K., & Yuille, J. C. (2010). Early psychosis and aggression: Predictors and prevalence of violent behaviour amongst individuals with early onset psychosis. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 33(3), 171176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2010.03.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spidel, A., Lecomte, T., Kealy, D., & Daigneault, I. (2018). Acceptance and commitment therapy for psychosis and trauma: Improvement in psychiatric symptoms, emotion regulation, and treatment compliance following a brief group intervention. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 91(2), 248261. PubMed (28976056). https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stain, H. J., Brønnick, K., Hegelstad, W. T. V., Joa, I., Johannessen, J. O., Langeveld, J., … Larsen, T. K. (2014). Impact of interpersonal trauma on the social functioning of adults with first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(6), 14911498. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanton, K. J., Denietolis, B., Goodwin, B. J., & Dvir, Y. (2020). Childhood trauma and psychosis: An updated review. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29(1), 115129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2019.08.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroup, D. F., Berlin, J. A., Morton, S. C., Olkin, I., Williamson, G. D., Rennie, D., … Thacker, S. B. (2000). Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: A proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA, 283(15), 20082012. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.283.15.2008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., Van Dorn, R. A., Elbogen, E. B., Wagner, H. R., Rosenheck, R. A., … Lieberman, J. A. (2006). A national study of violent behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(5), 490499. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Sweeney, S., Air, T., Zannettino, L., & Galletly, C. (2015). Gender differences in the physical and psychological manifestation of childhood trauma and/or adversity in people with psychosis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 17681768. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Gordon, J. B., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2022). Recognizing the importance of childhood maltreatment as a critical factor in psychiatric diagnoses, treatment, research, prevention, and education. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(3), 13311338. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01367-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2013). Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology: A case for ecophenotypic variants as clinically and neurobiologically distinct subtypes. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(10), 11141133. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(10), 652666. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrent, C., Reinares, M., Martinez-Arán, A., Cabrera, B., Amoretti, S., Corripio, I., … group, P. E. P. S. (2018). Affective versus non-affective first episode psychoses: A longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 238, 297304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.06.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trauelsen, A. M., Bendall, S., Jansen, J. E., Nielsen, H-G. L., Pedersen, M. B., Trier, C. H., … Simonsen, E. (2016). Childhood adversities: Social support, premorbid functioning and social outcome in first-episode psychosis and a matched case-control group. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatryy, 50(8), 770782. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867415625814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
*Trauelsen, A. M., Gumley, A., Jansen, J. E., Pedersen, M. B., Nielsen, H.-G. L., Haahr, U. H., & Simonsen, E. (2019). Does childhood trauma predict poorer metacognitive abilities in people with first-episode psychosis? Psychiatry Research, 273, 163170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.01.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Trotta, A., Murray, R. M., David, A. S., Kolliakou, A., O'Connor, J., Di Forti, M., … Fisher, H. L. (2016). Impact of different childhood adversities on 1-year outcomes of psychotic disorder in the genetics and psychosis study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(2), 464475. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, S., Harvey, C., Hayes, L., et al. (2019). Childhood adversity and clinical and psychosocial outcomes in psychosis. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, e78e78. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796019000684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Turner, S., Harvey, C., Hayes, L., Castle, D., Galletly, C., Sweeney, S., … Spittal, M. J. (2020). Childhood adversity and clinical and psychosocial outcomes in psychosis. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796019000684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Berg, D., de Bont, P. A. J. M., van der Vleugel, B. M., de Roos, C., de Jongh, A., van Minnen, A., & van der Gaag, M. (2018). Long-term outcomes of trauma-focused treatment in psychosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 212(3), 180182. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2017.30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*van Nierop, M., Bak, M., de Graaf, R., Ten Have, M., van Dorsselaer, S., Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) Investigators, & van Winkel, R. (2016). The functional and clinical relevance of childhood trauma-related admixture of affective, anxious and psychosis symptoms. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 133(2), 91101. PubMed (25961128). https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Nierop, M., Lecei, A., Myin-Germeys, I., Collip, D., Viechtbauer, W., Jacobs, N., … van Winkel, R. (2018). Stress reactivity links childhood trauma exposure to an admixture of depressive, anxiety, and psychosis symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 260, 451457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Winkel, R., Stefanis, N. C., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2008). Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interaction. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(6), 10951105. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varese, F., Smeets, F., Drukker, M., Lieverse, R., Lataster, T., Viechtbauer, W., … Bentall, R. P. (2012). Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: A meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 661671. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargas, T., Conley, R. E., & Mittal, V. A. (2020). Chronic stress, structural exposures and neurobiological mechanisms: A stimulation, discrepancy and deprivation model of psychosis. International Review of Neurobiology, 152, 4169. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.irn.2019.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Vaskinn, A., Melle, I., Aas, M., & Berg, A. O. (2021). Sexual abuse and physical neglect in childhood are associated with affective theory of mind in adults with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research-Cognition, 23, 100189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2020.100189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velthorst, E., Fett, A.-K. J., Reichenberg, A., Perlman, G., van Os, J., Bromet, E. J., & Kotov, R. (2017). The 20-year longitudinal trajectories of social functioning in individuals with psychotic disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(11), 10751085. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieta, E., & Berk, M. (2022). Early intervention comes late. European Neuropsychopharmacology: The Journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 59, 13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2022.02.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Vila-Badia, R., Del Cacho, N., Butjosa, A., Serra Arumí, C., Esteban Santjusto, M., Abella, M., … Usall, J. (2022). Prevalence and types of childhood trauma in first episode psychosis patients. Relation with clinical onset variables. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 146, 102108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.12.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vita, A., Gaebel, W., Mucci, A., Sachs, G., Erfurth, A., Barlati, S., … Galderisi, S. (2022). European Psychiatric association guidance on assessment of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. European Psychiatry, 65(1), e58. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2022.2316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
*Weijers, J., Fonagy, P., Eurelings-Bontekoe, E., Termorshuizen, F., Viechtbauer, W., & Selten, J. P. (2018). Mentalizing impairment as a mediator between reported childhood abuse and outcome in nonaffective psychotic disorder. Psychiatry Research, 259, 463469, PubMed (29145104). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G., Wells, G., Shea, B., Shea, B., O'Connell, D., Peterson, J., … Petersen, J. (2014). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analyses. Retrieved from https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/The-Newcastle-Ottawa-Scale-(NOS)-for-Assessing-the-Wells-Wells/c293fb316b6176154c3fdbb8340a107d9c8c82bf.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. CIM-10/ICD-10 : Classification Internationale Des Maladies. Dixième Révision. Chapitre V(F) : Troubles Mentaux et Troubles Du Comportement : Critères Diagnostiques Pour La Recherche. WHO IRIS. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/37108.Google Scholar
Yamada, Y., Inagawa, T., Sueyoshi, K., Sugawara, N., Ueda, N., Omachi, Y., … Sumiyoshi, T. (2019). Social cognition deficits as a target of early intervention for psychoses: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of included studies

Figure 1

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flowchart outlining the study selection process.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Forest plots investigating associations between overall childhood maltreatment and social functioning: (1) Global social functioning, (2) Independent living, (3) Occupational functioning, (4) Interpersonal relations, and (5) Aggressive behaviour in individuals with psychotic disorders.

Figure 3

Table 2. Meta-analyses of associations between childhood maltreatment and social outcomes in individuals with psychotic disorders

Figure 4

Figure 3. Summary of the evidence on moderators and mediators between childhood maltreatment and social outcomes in psychotic disorders. Note. The figure summarises the findings of our narrative synthesis on effect moderators and mediators examined in the included studies. Moderators examined in the included studies are represented by circles/ovals (brick orange in online version). Mediators examined in the included studies are represented by rectangles (green in online version). The colour and thickness of the lines represent the robustness of the evidence, i.e., a stronger colour and thicker line representing major evidence (n ≥ 5). Lighter colour and thinner lines represent emerging evidence (n = 1). Dotted line and grey font indicate where evidence is lacking, and more research is needed.

Supplementary material: File

Fares-Otero et al. supplementary material

Fares-Otero et al. supplementary material

Download Fares-Otero et al. supplementary material(File)
File 12.4 MB