Introduction
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is characterized by intrusive obsessions and repetitive compulsions. The lifetime prevalence is estimated to be 1–3% (Ruscio et al., Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler2010). Despite its prevalence and its impact on life quality, the pathophysiology of OCD is not well understood. Further, despite accumulating evidence for heritability of OCD and associated traits (Pauls et al., Reference Pauls, Abramovitch, Rauch and Geller2014), reliable evidence for the involvement of specific genes has not emerged (Pauls et al., Reference Pauls, Abramovitch, Rauch and Geller2014; International Obsessive Compulsive Disorder Foundation Genetics and Studies, 2017). OCD is etiologically complex, with multiple genetic, epigenetic, and environmental factors as well as their interactions contributing to its development (Pauls et al., Reference Pauls, Abramovitch, Rauch and Geller2014). Phenotypic heterogeneity, overlap in symptoms, and high comorbidity between psychiatric disorders (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010; Ruscio et al., Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler2010) further complicate research and it remains a major scientific challenge to unravel the etiology of complex disorders such as OCD. The Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative and the endophenotype concept are hoped to help overcoming these problems (Gottesman and Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010; Miller and Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013). RDoC takes a transdiagnostic perspective and conceptualizes psychopathology with a stronger grounding in neuroscience and in relation to dysfunctions in fundamental dimensions of behavior and neurobiology (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010). Endophenotypes are heritable biological or psychological traits that represent simpler signs to etiological underpinnings and are assumed to be closer to genes and disease mechanisms (Gottesman and Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010; Miller and Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013). Both concepts consistently emphasize a grounding in neuroscience, a focus on disease mechanisms, and transdiagnostic processes (Miller and Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013).
Neural error-signals are a promising research target within both concepts, and alterations in error-processing are suggested to play a role in several mental disorders. The error-related negativity (ERN) is a well-validated and established electrophysiological marker of error-processing. The ERN is a negative deflection in the event-related brain potential peaking over fronto-central electrodes following an error and has been associated with activity in the midcingulate cortex predominantly the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Debener et al., Reference Debener, Ullsperger, Siegel, Fiehler, von Cramon and Engel2005; Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Grutzmann et al., Reference Grutzmann, Endrass, Kaufmann, Allen, Eichele and Kathmann2016).Footnote †Footnote 1 The ERN has been repeatedly found to be increased in OCD patients (Endrass and Ullsperger, Reference Endrass and Ullsperger2014; Riesel, Reference Riesel2019) and fulfills key criteria for an endophenotype (Gottesman and Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003; Miller and Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013), including heritability (Anokhin et al., Reference Anokhin, Golosheykin and Heath2008), and a robust association with the disorder (Endrass and Ullsperger, Reference Endrass and Ullsperger2014; Riesel, Reference Riesel2019). Furthermore, increased ERN amplitudes persist after symptom reduction over the course of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT, Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Franklin, Foa and Simons2008; Huyser et al., Reference Huyser, Veltman, Wolters, de Haan and Boer2011; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Auerbach and Kathmann2015) and are also observed in unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients in the absence of symptoms (Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Kaufmann and Kathmann2011; Carrasco et al., Reference Carrasco, Harbin, Nienhuis, Fitzgerald, Gehring and Hanna2013). Altogether, these results highlight that increased ERN amplitudes seem not to be the consequence or a correlate of OCD symptoms but to rather represent a promising endophenotype reflecting vulnerability for the disorder.
However, increased ERN amplitudes are not specific to OCD. Hyperactive neural error-signals in the brain have been linked to worry (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan and Yeung2013), repetitive behavior (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013), checking (Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Kotov and Proudfit2015b), and anxiety proneness (Cavanagh and Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2014), symptoms that represent core features of OCD but are shared with other disorders. In the same vein, increased ERN amplitudes are not specific to OCD but have also been observed in generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), social anxiety, health anxiety, and less consistently in depression (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Endrass et al., Reference Endrass, Riesel, Kathmann and Buhlmann2014; Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Dieterich and Riesel2015a; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Goldhahn and Kathmann2017). Moreover, increased amplitudes prospectively predict the development of anxiety symptoms (Lahat et al., Reference Lahat, Lamm, Chronis-Tuscano, Pine, Henderson and Fox2014; Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Walker, Degnan, Henderson, Pine, McDermott and Fox2014; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Hajcak, Torpey-Newman, Kujawa and Klein2015; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Nelson, Perlman, Klein and Kotov2018). In contrast, reduced ERN amplitudes have been found in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder (Minzenberg et al., Reference Minzenberg, Gomes, Yoon, Swaab and Carter2014), substance use disorder (SUD) and, inconsistently, in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Luijten et al., Reference Luijten, Machielsen, Veltman, Hester, de Haan and Franken2014; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017). In schizophrenia and SUD, results from a treatment study and high-risk populations indicate that error-processing deficits precede illness onset (Simmonite et al., Reference Simmonite, Bates, Groom, Jackson, Hollis and Liddle2012; Euser et al., Reference Euser, Evans, Greaves-Lord, Huizink and Franken2013; Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017). Against this background, error-related brain activity qualifies as a promising transdiagnostic endophenotype with both reduction and enhancement in neural error-signals each indicating vulnerability for different types of psychopathology. To further strengthen this notion, studies examining unaffected first-degree relatives are of crucial importance given that family history is a major risk-factor for the development of psychopathology. The current study aims to replicate the finding that OCD patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives show increased ERN amplitudes. Moreover, we investigate whether alterations in neural error-signals reflect vulnerability across diagnoses by examining unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with anxiety disorders, depression, and SUD. We hypothesize that individuals with familial risk for anxiety show increased ERN amplitudes, whereas those at risk for SUD show reduced amplitudes.
Methods
Participants
One hundred and seventeen patients with OCD, 50 unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with OCD (parents: n = 34, siblings: n = 13, offspring: n = 3), and 130 healthy comparison participants without a family history of OCD participated in the study (see Table 1). All participants were examined by trained clinical psychologists using the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV (SCID, First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996) to assess past and present psychiatric disorders. For all participants family history information of psychopathology for their first-degree relatives was collected using the Family History Screen (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Adams, Wolk, Verdeli and Olfson2000), a structured interview that assesses all major DSM-IV diagnoses. Of the 130 healthy comparison participants, 76 were free of any family history for psychopathology (i.e. none of their first-degree relatives fulfilled criteria for a major DSM diagnosis), whereas the remaining reported a family history of depression (unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with depression, n = 28), anxiety disorders (unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with anxiety disorders, n = 27), and/or SUD (unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with SUD, n = 27)Footnote 2. All participants were between 18 and 65 years of age, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and reported no history of neurological disease or head trauma. Details about the subject flow and dropout reasons are presented in the supplementary information (online Supplementary Fig. 1).
s.d., standard deviation; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; IQ, intelligence quotient; SUD, substance use disorders; OC-TCDQ, Obsessive-Compulsive Trait Core Dimensions Questionnaire.
a State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory and OC-TCDQ scores were missing for six OCD patients.
b Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale, Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale and age of onset were only applicable in patients.
c For performance data, age was included as a covariate to account for group differences. Further, performance data of six participants were missing due to technical problems (n = 2 OCD patients, n = 2 relatives, and n = 2 healthy comparison participants).
OCD patients were recruited via the outpatient clinic at Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany, and fulfilled criteria for OCD as verified with the SCID. Exclusion criteria for patients were: a current or lifetime diagnosis of psychotic, SUD, or bipolar disorders as well as neuroleptic medication in the past 4 weeks and/or benzodiazepines use in the past 2 weeks. Fifty OCD patients were currently taking psychotropic medication (serotonin reuptake inhibitors: n = 38, serotonin–norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors: n = 6, tricyclic antidepressants: n = 6). The majority of patients had one to three comorbid axis-I disorders: major depression (n = 27 remitted, n = 31 current episode), dysthymia (n = 10), panic disorder (n = 3), agoraphobia (n = 2), social phobia (n = 10), specific phobia (n = 10), GAD (n = 5), unspecific somatoform disorder (n = 6), pain disorder (n = 1), hypochondria (n = 1), body dysmorphic disorder (n = 1), anorexia nervosa (n = 1), binge eating disorder (n = 2), tic disorder (n = 6), excoriation disorder (n = 1).
First-degree relatives of patients with OCD were recruited via OCD patients that were diagnosed and treated at the outpatient clinic at Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin. OCD patients gave written informed consent for contacting them. For all first-degree relatives of OCD patients the diagnosis of their affected relative was verified in a face-to-face SCID. OCD relatives were only included if they themselves were free of past or present OCD. Additional exclusion criteria applied to unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients were: lifetime diagnosis of psychotic, bipolar, or SUD, and psychotropic medication in the past 4 weeks.
Healthy comparison participants were recruited via public advertisements and were matched for age, gender, and education level to OCD patients. Exclusion criteria for comparison participants were as follows: psychoactive medication in the past 3 months; any current or past axis-I disorder; and a family history of OCD. The Family History Screen (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Adams, Wolk, Verdeli and Olfson2000) indicated that 28 healthy participants had first-degree relatives with depression, 27 first-degree relatives with anxiety disorders, and/or 27 reported to have first-degree relatives with SUD. Thus, the effects of a family history for depression, SUD (alcohol and substance use), and anxiety disorders (GAD, specific phobia, social phobia, agoraphobia, panic disorder) were examined. The number of cases did not allow to examine familial risk for other disorder such as schizophrenia, or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or to separately analyze different anxiety disorders with sufficient power2. Note that family history information for unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with SUD, anxiety, or depression was not verified by a direct interview with the family members but was derived via the Family History Screen (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Adams, Wolk, Verdeli and Olfson2000).
All participants gave written informed consent after receiving written and verbal information about the study, and were monetarily compensated for their time. The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the revised Declaration of Helsinki, as confirmed by the ethics committee of Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin.
Measures
All participants completed the Beck-Depression-Inventory II (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996), the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (Foa et al., Reference Foa, Huppert, Leiberg, Langner, Kichic, Hajcak and Salkovskis2002), and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, Spielberger et al., Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983) to assess self-reported severity of depressive, obsessive-compulsive, and anxiety symptoms, respectively. The Obsessive-Compulsive Trait Core Dimensions Questionnaire (Ecker and Gonner, Reference Ecker and Gonner2008) was used to assess harm avoidance and sense of incompleteness in the participants. The Wortschatztest (Schmidt and Metzler, Reference Schmidt and Metzler1992) was applied to measure verbal intelligence. In addition, for patients only, severity of obsessive-compulsive and depressive symptoms was rated by trained clinicians using the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Price, Rasmussen, Mazure, Fleischmann, Hill, Heninger and Charney1989) and the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (Montgomery and Asberg, Reference Montgomery and Asberg1979).
Task
An arrow-version of the flanker task was administered using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, California). On each trial, five vertically aligned arrows were presented and participants were instructed to indicate the direction of the central arrow. The stimuli were presented for 100 ms, followed by a 1000 ms response interval, followed by an inter-trial interval that varied randomly between 200 and 1200 ms. Half of the trials were congruent and half were incongruent. Eight blocks with 60 trials and at the beginning 20 practice trials were presented. After each block, participants were reminded to respond both quickly and accurately. The duration of the experiment was about 25 min.
Electroencephalographic recording and analyses
The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from 61 Ag/AgCl-electrodes using an equidistant electrode montage (EASYCAP GmbH, Herrsching-Breitbrunn, Germany). External electrodes were placed below the eyes, below T1 (ground), and on the nasion. Channels were referenced to Cz during recording and impedances were below 5 kΩ. The EEG was sampled at a rate of 1000 Hz and a resolution of 0.1 µV. EEG data were processed offline using BrainVision Analyzer 2.1 (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany). The following filters were applied: a 50 Hz notch filter, a 30 Hz low-pass filter, and a 0.01 Hz high-pass filter. To correct for eye movements and eye blinks, an ocular correction independent component analysis was conducted. Data were re-referenced to average reference and response-locked segments were computed from 400 ms before and 1000 ms after response execution (baseline-correction −100 to 0 ms). Artifacts were excluded based on the following criteria: amplitude changes exceeding 50 µV between consecutive data points, voltage differences of more than 200 µV within a 200 ms interval, and voltage changes of less than 0.50 µV within a 100 ms interval. Only participants with at least six artifact-free error trials were analyzed to ensure a reliable quantification of ERN (Olvet and Hajcak, Reference Olvet and Hajcak2009). ERN and CRN were quantified as the difference between the most negative peak occurring in a 150 ms post-response epoch and the immediately preceding positive peak at electrode FCz where error-related brain activity was maximal (Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Kaufmann and Kathmann2011). In addition, we also calculated ΔERN (ERN minus CRN) and quantified the ERN and CRN as the mean amplitude centered around the most negative peak occurring in a 150 ms epoch following the response at electrode FCz (peak ± 20 ms). Note that the pattern of results was replicated across the different ERN quantifications and related results are reported in online Supplementary information (SI 2). Results for the CRN are presented in the online Supplementary information (SI 1).
Statistical analyses
One-way analyses of covariance were used to examine differences in ERN, symptom severity, and error rate between OCD patients, OCD relatives and healthy participants. Repeated-measurement analyses of covariance were used to analyze response times including group (OCD patients, OCD relatives, and healthy comparison participants) as between-subjects and response type (correct, error) as within-subjects factors. Age was included as a covariate in these analyses to control for age differences between groups. Additional analyses for the OCD group included medication (with n = 50, without medication n = 66) and comorbidity (with n = 86, without comorbidity n = 31) as between-subjects factors. We used hierarchical regressions to examine whether family history explained variance in ERN in unaffected comparison participants. Results for the CRN and the whole sample are presented in the online Supplementary information (SI 1 and SI 3). In a first step, the effects of age, depressive, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms were controlled. In a second step, family history information for SUD, depression, and anxiety was entered. In a third step, interactions between familial risk statuses were included. Regression and correlational analyses were performed to determine the relation between ERN and symptoms (online Supplementary information SI 4). Heritability estimates for error-related brain activity were computed using Sequential Oligogenic Linkage Analysis Routines (SOLAR) and are presented in the online Supplementary information (SI 5). All statistical tests were two-tailed with α = 0.05. Post-hoc comparisons were corrected using the Bonferroni procedure. Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS (Version 21.0, Chicago, USA).
Results
Demographic and behavioral data
Table 1 shows demographic, clinical, behavioral measures, and the statistics for the comparison between groups. Patients with OCD and healthy comparison participants did not differ in age (t 245 = 0.14, p = 0.89). OCD relatives were significantly older compared to patients (t 165 = 6.82, p < 0.001) and healthy comparison participants (t 178 = 6.71, p < 0.001). Groups did not differ in gender and verbal intelligence quotient (IQ). OCD patients scored higher on symptom measures than OCD relatives (depressive symptoms: t 165 = 9.78, p < 0.001; obsessive-compulsive symptoms: t 165 = 11.87, p < 0.001; anxiety symptoms: t 159 = 12.00, p < 0.001; harm avoidance: t 159 = 13.16, p < 0.001; sense of incompleteness: t 159 = 11.98, p < 0.001) and healthy comparison participants (depressive symptoms: t 245 = 16.47, p < 0.001; obsessive-compulsive symptoms: t 245 = 20.85, p < 0.001; anxiety symptoms: t 239 = 18.12, p < 0.001; harm avoidance: t 239 = 21.48, p < 0.001; sense of incompleteness: t 239 = 19.10, p < 0.001). OCD relatives did not differ from healthy comparison participants in depressive (t 178 = 1.52, p = 0.13), anxiety symptoms (t 178 = 1.11, p = 0.27), harm avoidance (t 178 = 1.74, p = 0.08) and sense of incompleteness (t 178 = 1.21, p = 0.23). However, unaffected first-degree relatives reported slightly more obsessive-compulsive symptoms (t 178 = 2.51, p = 0.01).
Groups did not differ in error rates. Correct responses were slower compared to errors (F 1,287 = 28.31, p < 0.001, η 2P = 0.09). A main effect of the covariate age (F 1,287 = 52.47, p < 0.001, η 2P = 0.16) reflected that increasing age was associated with slower responses (r = 0.49, p < 0.001). No main effect of group was observed after accounting for age differences. Robust post-error slowing (difference in reaction time between post-error and the associated pre-error trial) did not differ between groups.
Error-related negativity
Figure 1 displays ERN for OCD patients, unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients and unaffected comparison participants. Results for the CRN are presented in the online Supplementary information (SI 1). A significant main effect of group (F 2,293 = 4.39, p = 0.005, η 2P = 0.04, d = 0.39) was observed. Both OCD patients and unaffected relatives of OCD patients showed enhanced ERN amplitudes compared to unaffected comparison participants [OCD patients v. healthy comparison participants: mean difference = 1.39, s.e. = 0.59, p = 0.05, 95% confidence interval (CI) (0.04–2.80); OCD relatives v. healthy comparison participants: mean difference = 2.49, s.e. = 0.83, p = 0.009, 95% CI (0.48–4.50)]. OCD patients and OCD relatives did not differ in ERN [mean difference = 0.19, s.e. = 0.81, p = 0.81, 95% CI (−1.40 to 1.79)]. A significant main effect of the covariate age (F 1,293 = 7.85, p = 0.005, η 2P = 0.03) was observed. Age and ERN showed a positive correlation (r = 0.12; p = 0.05) with increasing age being associated with more positive (i.e. smaller) amplitudes. For patients, neither a main effect for medication (F 1,114 = 0.02, p = 0.89, η 2P = 0.00), or comorbidity (F 1,114 = 0.44, p = 0.51, η 2P = 0.004) nor significant interactions involving these variables were observed (p values >0.33). When analyzing associations with symptoms across the whole sample, only harm avoidance showed a correlation with ERN (r = −0.13, p = 0.03). Higher harm avoidant motivation was associated with more negative, i.e. larger amplitudes. No other associations with symptoms or verbal IQ in the whole sample as well as symptom dimensions and clinical features in OCD patients were observed (see online Supplementary information SI 4).
Using hierarchical regressions, we examined whether family history information explained variance in ERN in healthy comparison participants after adjusting for age, gender and symptom severity (Table 2, Fig. 2). Familial risk for SUD and anxiety (i.e. having a first-degree relative with this disorder) significantly predicted ERN magnitude in healthy participants. Unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with SUD (i.e. healthy participants at familial risk for SUD) reported lower ERN amplitudes compared to those without familial risk for SUD [t 128 = 2.49, p = 0.01, d = 0.54, mean difference = 2.40, s.e. = 0.96, 95% CI (0.49–4.30)]. Further, increased ERN amplitudes were observed in unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with anxiety [i.e. healthy participants with a family history for anxiety; t 128 = 2.83, p = 0.005, d = 0.61, mean difference = 2.70, s.e. = 0.96, 95% CI (0.81–4.59)]. Unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with anxiety disorder (M = −10.37, s.d. = 5.66) did not differ in ERN from OCD patients (M = −9.62, s.d. = 4.58) and OCD relatives (M = −9.82, s.d. = 5.22; F 2,190 = 0.72, p = 0.49, η 2P = 0.008). Entering interactions between risk statuses does not significantly enhance the model fit (change in F = 0.75) and no significant interactions were observed.
DV, dependent variable; SUD, substance use disorders.
Significant values are printed in bold.
Discussion
Neural error-signals were examined as an endophenotype for OCD by comparing it between patients with OCD, unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients and healthy participants without a family history for OCD. Furthermore, we evaluated whether neural error-signals may represent a transdiagnostic endophenotype by analyzing unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with SUD, depression, and anxiety disorders. Three main findings were observed: first, both patients with OCD and unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients showed increased ERN amplitudes compared to healthy comparison participants. Second, the increase in ERN magnitude was not specific to OCD as unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with anxiety disorders were also characterized by increased ERN amplitudes compared to healthy comparison participants. Third, unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with SUD showed reduced ERN amplitudes compared to healthy comparison participants, OCD patients, and relatives of individuals with OCD or anxiety. Overall, these results support the notion that neural error-signals represent a potential endophenotype not only for OCD but across different psychiatric disorders.
A useful endophenotype is a robust, state-independent quantitative deviation that is found in patients and unaffected family members. It is heritable, easy and reliable to assess and informative about the pathophysiology of the disorder (Gottesman and Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003; Miller and Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013). Enhanced neural error-signals in OCD fulfill most of these criteria. The ERN has been shown to have good psychometric properties including validity and reliability (Olvet and Hajcak, Reference Olvet and Hajcak2009; Weinberg and Hajcak, Reference Weinberg and Hajcak2011; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Weinberg, Endrass, Meyer and Hajcak2013). Increased neural error-signals are robustly associated with OCD (Endrass and Ullsperger, Reference Endrass and Ullsperger2014; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017) and can also be found in unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients (Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Kaufmann and Kathmann2011; Carrasco et al., Reference Carrasco, Harbin, Nienhuis, Fitzgerald, Gehring and Hanna2013). Further, elevated ERN amplitudes in OCD persist after symptom reduction due to CBT (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Franklin, Foa and Simons2008; Huyser et al., Reference Huyser, Veltman, Wolters, de Haan and Boer2011; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Auerbach and Kathmann2015). A twin study suggested a heritability of about 50% for ERN (Anokhin et al., Reference Anokhin, Golosheykin and Heath2008) and we derived comparable heritability estimates ranging between 22% and 48% (online Supplementary information SI 5). Altogether, these results highlight that increased neural error-signals represent a promising endophenotype for OCD.
The current study indicates that increased ERN amplitudes are also found in healthy participants at familial risk for anxiety (i.e. unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with anxiety disorders). This is consistent with studies reporting increased ERN amplitudes for several anxiety disorders including GAD, social anxiety, and health anxiety (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Goldhahn and Kathmann2017). These disorders are frequently comorbid and share symptoms that have been linked to elevated neural error signals such as repetitive behavior (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013), worry (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan and Yeung2013), and anxiety-proneness (Cavanagh and Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2014) pointing to shared pathophysiological mechanisms. The observed increase in ERN in unaffected relatives with a family history for anxiety disorders or OCD compared to healthy controls provides important evidence for the assumption that increased neural error-signals represent a transdiagnostic endophenotype indicating a shared vulnerability for OCD and anxiety. This is further supported by results indicating the state-independence of increased ERN amplitudes not only in OCD (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Franklin, Foa and Simons2008; Huyser et al., Reference Huyser, Veltman, Wolters, de Haan and Boer2011; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Auerbach and Kathmann2015) but also in social anxiety disorder (Kujawa et al., Reference Kujawa, Weinberg, Bunford, Fitzgerald, Hanna, Monk, Kennedy, Klumpp, Hajcak and Phan2016). Finally, increased ERN amplitudes prospectively predict the development of anxiety symptoms (Lahat et al., Reference Lahat, Lamm, Chronis-Tuscano, Pine, Henderson and Fox2014; Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Walker, Degnan, Henderson, Pine, McDermott and Fox2014; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Hajcak, Torpey-Newman, Kujawa and Klein2015; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Nelson, Perlman, Klein and Kotov2018) highlighting the predictive validity of neural error signals.
The current study did not find support for altered ERN amplitudes in unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with depression (i.e. healthy individuals at familial risk for depression). Results regarding increased ERN amplitudes in depression are inconsistent (Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017), pointing toward increases (e.g. Chiu and Deldin, Reference Chiu and Deldin2007), as well as decreases (e.g. Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Liu and Shankman2016), or no differences (e.g. Schoenberg, Reference Schoenberg2014). Overall, neural error-signals in depression seem to be more affected by symptom state and severity (Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, De Bruijn, Maas, Vancoillie, Hulstijn and Sabbe2009), as well as subtype (Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Liu and Shankman2016). An increase was reported for mild to moderate depression, but not in severely depressed patients with melancholic or anhedonic features that even showed a blunted response to errors (Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, De Bruijn, Maas, Vancoillie, Hulstijn and Sabbe2009; Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Liu and Shankman2016). Further, alterations in error-monitoring disappear after symptom reduction (Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, De Bruijn, Maas, Vancoillie, Hulstijn and Sabbe2009). Together with our results showing normal error-monitoring in individuals at familial risk for depression this suggests that ERN alterations seem not to be an endophenotypic trait marker of depression. Observed alterations may rather reflect a state marker or a result from overlap/comorbidity with relevant transdiagnostic phenotypes. This supports some specificity and suggests that increased neural error-signals may distinguish vulnerability for OCD and anxiety from depression.
Finally, unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with SUD showed reduced error-related brain activity compared to healthy participants without a family history for SUD. This supports that a blunted neural response to errors may not only be found in patients with SUD (Luijten et al., Reference Luijten, Machielsen, Veltman, Hester, de Haan and Franken2014; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017) but can also be seen in individuals at increased risk for SUD. This confirms a previous study in adolescents at high-risk for SUD that were characterized by diminished neural error-signals (Euser et al., Reference Euser, Evans, Greaves-Lord, Huizink and Franken2013). Further, the suitability of reduced error-related brain activity as an endophenotype for SUD is supported by longitudinal studies showing that it predicts relapse (Luo et al., Reference Luo, Zhang, Hu, Bednarski, Erdman, Farr, Hong, Sinha, Mazure and Li2013; Marhe et al., Reference Marhe, van de Wetering and Franken2013) and the initiation of tobacco use (Anokhin and Golosheykin, Reference Anokhin and Golosheykin2015). Besides SUD, several studies suggest that reduced ERN amplitudes are also found in schizophrenia (Martin et al., Reference Martin, McCleery, Moore, Wynn, Green and Horan2018), bipolar disorder (Minzenberg et al., Reference Minzenberg, Gomes, Yoon, Swaab and Carter2014), and inconsistently in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Luijten et al., Reference Luijten, Machielsen, Veltman, Hester, de Haan and Franken2014; Gillan et al., Reference Gillan, Fineberg and Robbins2017). This suggests that a blunted neural response to errors is common to different disorders and may reflect a shared pathophysiological mechanism.
In summary, the present results support the utility of neural error-signals as a dimensional, transdiagnostic endophenotype with an enhancement in ERN reflecting vulnerability for OCD and anxiety, and a reduction indicating risk for SUD. OCD and anxiety disorders are frequently comorbid (Ruscio et al., Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler2010) and share prominent clinical features such as heightened harm avoidance (Kampman et al., Reference Kampman, Viikki, Jarventausta and Leinonen2014), worry (Yook et al., Reference Yook, Kim, Suh and Lee2010; Dar et al., Reference Dar and Iqbal2015), increased intolerance of uncertainty (Tolin et al., Reference Tolin, Abramowitz, Brigidi and Foa2003; Holaway et al., Reference Holaway, Heimberg and Coles2006), as well as reassurance seeking and checking behavior (Kobori and Salkovskis, Reference Kobori and Salkovskis2013; Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Kotov and Proudfit2015b; Halldorsson and Salkovskis, Reference Halldorsson and Salkovskis2017). These symptoms have also been related to hyperactive neural error signals that are observed across OCD and anxiety disorders (e.g. Moser et al., Reference Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan and Yeung2013; Cavanagh and Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2014; Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Kotov and Proudfit2015b). Collectively, similarities in symptoms, neural substrate, and comorbidity point to a partly shared etiology involving alterations in error-monitoring. More specifically, differences in neural error signals that are generated by the ACC (Debener et al., Reference Debener, Ullsperger, Siegel, Fiehler, von Cramon and Engel2005; Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013; Grutzmann et al., Reference Grutzmann, Endrass, Kaufmann, Allen, Eichele and Kathmann2016), are assumed to trigger adaptations in cognition, affect, and control implementation to avoid future negative outcomes (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011; Cavanagh and Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2014). In this regard, hyperactive neural error-signals (i.e. increased sensitivity to errors) in OCD and anxiety may reflect over-controlled responses and an error- and harm-avoidant response style. Moreover, elevated neural error-signals are not only found in patients with these disorders but seem to indicate vulnerability for OCD and anxiety that persists independent of symptom status and are present in individuals at risk. On the other hand, reduced neural error-signals may indicate under-controlled behavior (i.e. reduced responsiveness to errors/negative consequences and deficits in adaptive control) and seem to be related to risk for substance use. Overall, our results add to numerous findings pointing to common etiological factors shared between different psychiatric disorders which encompass structural abnormalities in ACC and disruptions in cognitive control (Goodkind et al., Reference Goodkind, Eickhoff, Oathes, Jiang, Chang, Jones-Hagata, Ortega, Zaiko, Roach, Korgaonkar, Grieve, Galatzer-Levy, Fox and Etkin2015; McTeague et al., Reference McTeague, Huemer, Carreon, Jiang, Eickhoff and Etkin2017). Error-monitoring dysfunctions fit nicely into this picture since they have been linked to cognitive control (Cavanagh and Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2014) and are assumed to depend on activity in the ACC (Debener et al., Reference Debener, Ullsperger, Siegel, Fiehler, von Cramon and Engel2005; Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013). The ACC is implicated in the processing and integration of negative affect, cognitive conflict, and pain, as well as the implementation of adaptive control (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011) and its function seems to be critically involved in mental health.
Some limitations have to be noted. Groups differ in age, however, results are corrected for age differences. Some patients were medicated and some had current comorbid disorders. But in line with previous reports, our findings were not affected by medication or comorbidity in patients (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Liu, Gehring, Lister, Yin, Zhang, Fitzgerald, Himle, Abelson and Taylor2010; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Auerbach and Kathmann2015). Family history methods have high specificity (above 0.9), but rather low sensitivity and false negative diagnoses are common (Rougemont-Buecking et al., Reference Rougemont-Buecking, Rothen, Jeanpretre, Lustenberger, Vandeleur, Ferrero and Preisig2008; Vandeleur et al., Reference Vandeleur, Rothen, Jeanpretre, Lustenberger, Gamma, Ayer, Ferrero, Fleischmann, Besson, Sisbane and Preisig2008; Vandeleur et al., Reference Vandeleur, Rothen, Lustenberger, Glaus, Castelao and Preisig2015). This may have led to an underestimation of family history for depression, SUD, and anxiety. Further, in the current study diagnostic confidence differs between unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD patients and unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with disorders other than OCD. All unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with OCD were recruited via patients of the outpatient clinic for OCD at Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, where they were carefully diagnosed using the SCID. Thus, the OCD diagnosis of the affected first-degree relative was verified by a direct SCID. Family history information for disorders other than OCD was assessed with a family history interview (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Adams, Wolk, Verdeli and Olfson2000). Due to restrictions by data protection regulations, diagnostic information was not verified with a direct interview of the affected family member. Further, prevalence rates were too small to separately analyze the effects of specific anxiety disorders. Finally, the endophenotype concept is not without criticism (Kendler and Neale, Reference Kendler and Neale2010; Miller and Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013; Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, Malone and Vrieze2017). The success of the endophenotype strategy to identify predisposing genes has been demonstrated in non-psychiatric disorders, such as cardiac syndromes (Keating et al., Reference Keating, Atkinson, Dunn, Timothy, Vincent and Leppert1991; Vincent et al., Reference Vincent, Timothy, Leppert and Keating1992; Keating and Sanguinetti, Reference Keating and Sanguinetti2001), but remains limited for psychiatric disorders (Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, Malone and Vrieze2017). The genetic basis of endophenotypes is complex (Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, Malone and Vrieze2017). For the ERN results regarding its genetic foundation are inconsistent, rely on rather small samples, and replications are needed (Manoach and Agam, Reference Manoach and Agam2013). Furthermore, as Kendler and Neale (Reference Kendler and Neale2010) have noted, endophenotypes can either causally mediate between genes and the clinical phenotype, or be risk-indicators that share genes with the phenotype. Nevertheless, even complex endophenotypes are presumably simpler than the complex disorder. Besides pointing out genetic influences, endophenotypes can help to illuminate disorder mechanisms and familial environmental risk factors (Kendler and Neale, Reference Kendler and Neale2010) and potentially provide targets for interventions. Further, endophenotypes and especially profiles of different potential endophenotypes have the power to differentiate disorders that overlap symptomatically and in pathophysiological mechanisms, as has been shown for psychotic disorders including schizophrenia, bipolar, and schizoaffective disorder (e.g. Benes, Reference Benes2007; Braff and Tamminga, Reference Braff and Tamminga2017; Ivleva et al., Reference Ivleva, Clementz, Dutcher, Arnold, Jeon-Slaughter, Aslan, Witte, Poudyal, Lu, Meda, Pearlson, Sweeney, Keshavan and Tamminga2017). Thus, they may inform new ways of categorizing disorders (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010) and can foster individualized and precision medicine (Insel and Cuthbert, Reference Insel and Cuthbert2015). Finally, the clinical relevance of neural error-signals as a risk indicator or endophenotype is further supported by studies demonstrating the predictive validity for the onset of disorders or symptoms (Lahat et al., Reference Lahat, Lamm, Chronis-Tuscano, Pine, Henderson and Fox2014; Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Walker, Degnan, Henderson, Pine, McDermott and Fox2014; Anokhin and Golosheykin, Reference Anokhin and Golosheykin2015; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Hajcak, Torpey-Newman, Kujawa and Klein2015, Reference Meyer, Nelson, Perlman, Klein and Kotov2018; Kessel et al., Reference Kessel, Meyer, Hajcak, Dougherty, Torpey-Newman, Carlson and Klein2016), as well as treatment outcome (Luo et al., Reference Luo, Zhang, Hu, Bednarski, Erdman, Farr, Hong, Sinha, Mazure and Li2013; Marhe et al., Reference Marhe, van de Wetering and Franken2013) across different mental disorders.
The current study is the first to examine error-monitoring in unaffected individuals with familial risk for several disorders simultaneously. A diagnosis of OCD, familial risk for OCD or anxiety disorders were associated with increased error-related brain activity compared to healthy control participants without a family history for psychopathology. In contrast, familial risk for SUD was associated with a reduced neural response to errors compared to the healthy control group as well as patients with OCD and relatives with a family history of OCD or anxiety. The results provide important evidence for variations in neural error-signals being a promising transdiagnostic endophenotype reflecting vulnerability for the development of OCD, anxiety, or SUD. Further, they add to results suggesting that structural ACC alterations and deficits in cognitive control represent a shared neurobiological substrate across mental disorders.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000199.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by the German Research Foundation, with grants awarded to Norbert Kathmann (Grant KA815/6-1) and Michael Wagner (Grant WA731/10-1). The funding source had no further influence on study conduction and publication. The authors thank Thomas Pinkpank and Rainer Kniesche for technical support. We thank Ulrike Bunzenthal, Sarah Dreßel, Alexandra Günther, Marvin Groh, Anna Unger-Nübel, and Verena Wüllhorst for their help in data acquisition. We thank Dr Eva Kischkel and Dr Benedikt Reuter for their assistance in patient recruitment. Further, we are grateful for the help of Prof. Sebastian Market in performing the heritability analyses.
Conflict of interest
All authors assure to have no competing financial interests and conflict of interest regarding the presented work.