Introduction: oceans as inhibitors or enablers of dispersal?
To what extent is there spatial and temporal patterning in the spread of our genus around the planet, and what environmental and behavioural factors specify this patterning? The prevailing model of Pleistocene dispersals of Homo holds that this process was essentially terrestrial, with oceans and seas inhibiting and directing the movement of hominins out of Africa (e.g. Mellars Reference Mellars2006; Dennell & Petraglia Reference Dennell and Petraglia2012; Gamble Reference Gamble2013), although some scholars propose short-range maritime hops at both the Strait of Gibraltar and Bab-el-Mandeb (Lambeck et al. Reference Lambeck, Purcell, Flemming, Vita-Finzi, Alsharekh and Bailey2011; Rolland Reference Rolland2013). The relatively recent discovery of stone tools with apparently Lower and Middle Palaeolithic characteristics on islands in the eastern Mediterranean and in Island Southeast Asia (ISEA) has, however, been used by some scholars to challenge this terrestrial model.
The most extensive—if still contentious—data come from Flores in the Lesser Sundas, where sites in the So'a Basin provide the earliest dates for hominin presence on the island. The chipped stone assemblage from Wolo Sege dates to 1.02 Mya (Brumm et al. Reference Brumm, Jensen, van den Bergh, Morwood, Kurniawan, Aziz and Storey2010), while a mandible fragment and isolated teeth from Mata Menge, assigned to H. floresiensis on a morphological basis, and found in association with non-hominin taxa as well as chipped stone artefacts, are dated to 0.7 Mya (Brumm et al. Reference Brumm, van den Bergh, Storey, Kurniawan, Alloway, Setiwan, Setiyabudi, Grün, Moore, Yurnaldi, Puspaningrum, Wibowo, Insani, Sutisna, Westgate, Pearce, Duval, Meijer, Aziz, Sutikna, van der Kars, Flude and Morwood2016; van den Bergh et al. Reference van den Bergh, Kaifu, Kurniawan, Kono, Brumm, Setiyabudi, Aziz and Morwood2016a), much earlier than the controversial (e.g. Henneberg et al. Reference Henneberg, Eckhardt, Chavanaves and Hsü2014), if now re-dated (Sutikna et al. Reference Sutikna, Tocheri, Morwood, Saptomo, Jatmiko, Due Awe, Wasisto, Westaway, Aubert, Li, Zhao, Storey, Alloway, Morley, Meijer, van den Bergh, Grün, Dosseto, Brumm, Jungers and Roberts2016), fossil evidence provided by specimen LB1. Examples of islands devoid of fossil data, yet with purported pre-Upper Palaeolithic artefacts, include Sulawesi, to the north of the Lesser Sundas across the Flores Sea (van den Bergh et al. Reference van den Bergh, Li, Brumm, Grün, Yurnaldi, Moore, Kurniawan, Setiawan, Aziz, Roberts, Suyono, Storey, Setiyabudi and Morwood2016b), and a number of Mediterranean islands, most notably the Greek islands of Crete, Gavdos, Naxos and potentially Melos and Kephalonia (Strasser et al. Reference Strasser, Panagopoulou, Runnels, Murray, Thompson, Karkanas, McCoy and Wegmann2010; Carter et al. Reference Carter, Contreras, Doyle, Mihailović, Moutsiou and Skarpelis2014; Runnels Reference Runnels2014a). The stone tools reported from the Greek islands have Lower and Middle Palaeolithic morpho-typological characteristics, and some have associated radiometric dates that suggest that they are late Middle Pleistocene in age, if not earlier (Strasser et al. Reference Strasser, Runnels, Wegmann, Panagopoulou, McCoy, DiGregorio, Karkanas and Thompson2011; Runnels et al. Reference Runnels, DiGregorio, Wegmann, Gallen, Panagopoulou and Strasser2014). These islands have probably remained insular throughout glacially driven Quaternary sea-level oscillations (van der Geer et al. Reference van der Geer, Lyras, Van den Hoek Ostend, de Vos and Drinia2014; Sakellariou & Galanidou Reference Sakellariou, Galanidou, Harff, Bailey and Lüth2015; but cf. Lykousis Reference Lykousis2009), and thus these data have been interpreted as indicative of a hitherto unexpected capacity among archaic hominins for intentional maritime dispersal as early as the mid-Lower Palaeolithic (Simmons Reference Simmons2012, Reference Broodbank2014; Runnels Reference Runnels2014a).
If this revisionist maritime model is supported by future research, it could radically destabilise our current understanding of the structure of hominin dispersals. Other scholars, however, have hesitated to accept either the Palaeolithic credentials of the lithic artefacts, or the likelihood of intentional maritime dispersal potentially implied by them (e.g. Broodbank Reference Broodbank2013: 82–108, Reference Broodbank2014). We ourselves have disagreed substantively on this issue; yet despite one of us (CR) being a major proponent of the revisionist maritime model (e.g. Runnels Reference Runnels2014a & b; Runnels et al. Reference Runnels, DiGregorio, Wegmann, Gallen, Panagopoulou and Strasser2014), and the other (TPL) a serial critic of it (e.g. Leppard Reference Leppard2014, Reference Leppard2015a & b), we find that we are nevertheless in full agreement on two major issues connected with the debate. First, this topic is of profound importance for the archaeology of hominin dispersal, with broader implications for cognitive evolution in Homo. If we have fundamentally misunderstood environmental constraints on, and the spatial structure of, hominin dispersal, then serious aspersions would be cast both on models of tempo and gradient of the evolution of behavioural plasticity, and on our ability to recognise the presence versus absence of Palaeolithic occupation (or necessitate providing a revised account of why many islands do seem to lack such occupation). Second, as a function of this recognition, we agree that the utmost clarity—of expression, of definition of the problem at hand and of articulation of question-led research, the absence of which can only further cloud already murky waters—needs to be brought to a well-defined body of data. This is the goal of our present contribution: to frame how we perceive the problem in terms that are as precise as possible; and, based on this, to outline how we may better bring theory, method and data to bear on this issue so that we might move towards understanding whether archaeologists have accurately grasped—or not—the broad outline of how, and in what terms, hominins spread around the planet.
Clarifying the debate
We suggest that a number of trends can be identified that have prevented this debate from moving forward, not least different terminologies being employed for different ends (e.g. the nuanced but significant distinction between Broodbank (Reference Broodbank2014: 269) and Runnels (Reference Broodbank2014b: 274) regarding definitions of voyaging and seafaring). We do not dwell here on the spectrum of ecodynamic and behavioural processes implied by the commonly used term ‘colonisation’, which has a wide range of possible definitions, mostly referring to permanent, year-round settlement, and which is most appropriate for the activities of modern humans in the Holocene (see Cherry Reference Cherry1981; Dawson Reference Dawson2014: 42–68). For human dispersals in the Pleistocene, very different terms from colonisation may be apt, and it is sufficient to note that detailed specificity regarding the type and degree of activity under discussion can only improve clarity, especially when underdetermined phraseologies have very different meanings for scholars working at opposite ends of the Pleistocene (and into the Holocene). We also note that the tacit reliance on varying standards of evidence when evaluating claims for early island occupation—as well as differences between implicitly deductive and inductive approaches to the material—has also clouded the picture; do we require deeply stratified sites with suites of absolute dates, or are morpho-typologically Palaeolithic surface assemblages adequate? We disagree on the answer (and explore the issue in greater detail below); but we agree completely that it is necessary to be explicit about which position is being endorsed and the degree of scepticism with which the dataset is being approached.
A more substantial obstacle to progress in this discussion is the potential to conflate three interrelated but discrete research problems. The first problem involves the possibility of maritime dispersals by archaic species of Homo, particularly H. erectus sensu lato and heidelbergensis (as well as neanderthalensis and anatomical moderns from around 300–200 kya) during the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic (Bednarik Reference Bednarik2003; Dennell et al. Reference Dennell, Louys, O'Regan and Wilkinson2014). The second problem relates to the extent and form of seagoing and colonising behaviour among behavioural moderns practising hunting-gathering-fishing following their arrival in ISEA and the circum-Mediterranean, and yet prior to the advent of agropastoral lifestyles (Broodbank Reference Broodbank2006; Phoca-Cosmetatou & Rabett Reference Phoca-Cosmetatou, Rabett, Boyle, Rabett and Hunt2014). A third and final problem involves patterning in Early–Mid Holocene seagoing and colonising behaviour by behavioural Moderns practising various forms of agropastoralism (Cherry Reference Cherry1981, Reference Cherry1990; Broodbank Reference Broodbank2006). We argue that the explicit recognition of these research problems is essential if we are to escape conflation of these individual issues and the terms in which they are defined. The substantial behavioural, cognitive and social differences between, for example, Lower and Upper Palaeolithic contexts suggests that apparent patterning might derive from very different structuring factors. Despite accumulating evidence for sporadic activity, the general reluctance on the part of behavioural moderns in the aftermath of the Last Glacial Maximum (LGM) to exploit island environments is potentially a function of the general unattractiveness of islands to hunter-gatherer-fisher populations, perhaps relating to the intrinsic trophic limitations of comparatively small habitat fragments (Brose et al. Reference Brose, Ostling, Harrison and Martinez2004). That is, we might understand this apparent lack of interest in small islands to be a function of insular energetic limitations relating to viable range and group size, rather than an outcome of strictly biogeographic barriers to dispersal; terminal Upper Palaeolithic communities were, after all, demonstrably capable of reaching Mediterranean islands (Broodbank Reference Broodbank2006). By contrast, the absence of, for example, Australopith populations from the insular Mediterranean (or, indeed, from anywhere outside sub-Saharan Africa) is more readily comprehensible as an outcome of biogeographic and behavioural constraints.
In short, the evidence for seagoing and island colonisation by behavioural moderns during the initial Holocene is probably irrelevant in the context of understanding potential trans-maritime dispersal in species separated by evolutionary time and genetic distance, just as research on possible Neanderthal island exploitation in the Ionian Sea is unlikely to be useful in considering why Early Neolithic farmers were reluctant to settle the Balearics permanently. These problems differ in their assumptions about which hominin species were involved, the cognitive architecture and social organisation of these species, palaeoenvironmental conditions and palaeogeographic reconstructions. New data or models pertaining to one problem do not necessarily pertain to another. It is this first issue—the possibility of deliberate maritime dispersal as a hominin behaviour with a deep antiquity—that poses the greatest challenge to the received Palaeolithic consensus; the ability of behavioural moderns to traverse substantial water gaps, and their general, surprising recalcitrance in doing so prior to the Neolithic, is a topic for another venue.
Advancing the debate
Having clarified what it is we wish to know more about, how might we move the discussion forward? We identify intrinsic limitations in the data, and highlight possible pathways towards mitigating the effects of these limitations. In so doing, we tentatively sketch an agenda for more rigorously evaluating claims for maritime hominin dispersal in the Pleistocene.
Intrinsic limitations in the data: the Robinson Crusoe problem
From the perspective of research design and data recovery, developing answers to the question of Early and Middle Pleistocene sea-crossing dispersals is impeded by a methodological issue: the ‘Robinson Crusoe’ problem. Small-scale or short-term hominin activity on islands—‘Robinson Crusoe events’—during the Palaeolithic will be inconspicuous archaeologically because of stochastic effects having a bearing on the taphonomy and recovery of small datasets. Other factors being equal, the greater the temporal and spatial extent (and material complexity) of a behavioural phenomenon, the greater should be its archaeological visibility; the frequency of Robinson Crusoe events (isolated crossings, whether intentional or passive (Dennell et al. Reference Dennell, Louys, O'Regan and Wilkinson2014), followed by extinction or even potentially departure) is, then, likely to be inverse to their archaeological visibility (Leppard Reference Leppard2015a). While current research designs—specifically traditional site excavations and diachronic regional pedestrian surveys—are effective in identifying large-scale and long-term, materially rich behavioural phenomena, they are less suited to identifying small-scale and/or short-term maritime dispersal and island occupation events. As there is reason to suppose (in terms of probable behavioural and organisational scenarios) that any potential maritime dispersal during the Pleistocene would closely resemble a Robinson Crusoe event, it is probable that the suggestive yet patchy evidence that we currently possess represents the richest dataset possible. An immediate practical lesson is that waiting for a deluge of high-quality, overlapping, insular Pleistocene data is unlikely to prove a fruitful avenue of research; we must necessarily deal with the data that we have, rather than indefinitely postpone their evaluation.
The problem of sparse yet potentially very significant data relating to hominin dispersal across or into less biogeographically optimal zones is, of course, not restricted to island environments. Recent claims have been made—to provide two examples—for pre-LGM high- (as opposed to mid-) latitude presence of moderns during early–middle Marine Isotope Stage 3 (Pitulko et al. Reference Pitulko, Tikhonov, Pavlova, Nikolskiy, Kuper and Polozov2016), and for precociously early extra-African chipped stone industries and associated types of behaviour (Malassé et al. Reference Malassé, Moigne, Singh, Calligaro, Karir, Gaillard, Kaur, Bhardwaj, Pal, Abdessadok, Sao, Gargani, Tudryn and Sanz2016). The implications for models of cognitive, technological and social change in Homo are extensive; yet how are we to evaluate claims that rest on small evidential foundations? One conceptual solution involves dealing explicitly with long-term trajectories of success or failure. Bar-Yosef (in press) emphasises that we are methodologically ill-equipped both to identify comparatively rapid settlement failure, and to distinguish the successful, long-term establishment of settlement from repeated short-term failures. This is because sporadic recovery and capricious taphonomic process may conspire to give periods of established, long-term settlement the appearance, archaeologically, of repeated cycles of colonisation and failure. Successful establishment of Homo in an environment that we might consider comparatively challenging in terms of the complexity of behaviour and degree of encephalisation of the species in question is probably likely to have greater palaeoanthropological significance overall than unsuccessful establishment deriving from environmental demands (broadly conceived) exceeding the degree of adaptive plasticity in the respective species.
This distinction can be illuminated by the temporally punctuated Flores data. Is the separation of bodies of both fossil and artefact evidence by substantial temporal lacunae (Brumm et al. Reference Brumm, Jensen, van den Bergh, Morwood, Kurniawan, Aziz and Storey2010; Gómez-Robles Reference Gómez-Robles2016; Sutikna et al. Reference Sutikna, Tocheri, Morwood, Saptomo, Jatmiko, Due Awe, Wasisto, Westaway, Aubert, Li, Zhao, Storey, Alloway, Morley, Meijer, van den Bergh, Grün, Dosseto, Brumm, Jungers and Roberts2016) indicative of repeated colonisation followed by localised extinction, or of long-term and successful settlement that is archaeologically inconspicuous because of the small numbers involved and the vagaries of observation in a heterogeneous sampling universe? The answer is not clear (although, in the Flores instance, the probable time required for a taxon to undergo island dwarfing as part of an allopatric speciation process may suggest the latter (e.g. Bromham & Cardillo Reference Bromham and Cardillo2007)), but Bar-Yosef's point has implications for how we conceptualise potential pre-Holocene maritime dispersal events. The ecodynamic intersection between insular environments and an archaic hominin modus vivendi may mean that any maritime dispersal event resulted in unsuccessful settlement in the long term, rather than resulting in a continued presence (the usual connotation of the term colonisation) that we are failing to identify archaeologically.
Recognising that inherently patchy data may derive from—and more importantly further clarify—the type of post-dispersal establishment processes with which we are dealing (i.e. long-term success versus reiterated failure) does little to resolve the question of the nature of initial dispersal. Useful models do, however, exist that may serve to develop our understanding of how, if at all, archaic hominins traversed water gaps.
Modes of dispersal and structural homologies on the Eurasian fringe
Future research must be contextualised within the wider debate in Pleistocene archaeology and palaeoanthropology. We note, in particular, the development within the scholarship on ISEA archaeology of more nuanced models of dispersal. Dennell et al. (Reference Dennell, Louys, O'Regan and Wilkinson2014) have explored the possibility that the presence of a species of hominin on a given landmass—they focus on Wallacea, but the concept can be generalised—may not necessarily be indicative of choices reflecting strategic dispersal goals. Turning to the literature on modes of dispersal in non-human taxa (e.g. de Quieroz Reference de Quieroz2014), one of us (Leppard Reference Leppard2015a) has speculated whether passive, rather than active or strategic, dispersal events could account in principle for dispersal of archaic hominins across water gaps. Recognising that non-human taxa cross biogeographic barriers in a probabilistic fashion, we can ask whether this mode of dispersal has any explanatory power in accounting for hominin dispersal; if it has this potential, how should we balance invoking passive versus active/strategic dispersal in accounting for purported maritime crossings?
Thinking along these eco-geographical lines, it is worth highlighting conceivably relevant contextual similarities between the Mediterranean and ISEA. Despite evident environmental variability, there are structural homologies: both contexts are mid- to low-latitude, comparatively shallow seas situated—mostly—on continental shelves. Glacial-insterstadial oscillations have therefore affected their palaeogeography substantially; in both instances, cyclically exposing and inundating large swathes of potentially productive habitats. Both ISEA and the Mediterranean comprise groups of large and ecologically robust islands that have remained insular throughout these eustatic oscillations, islands that are nonetheless proximate to the Eurasian mainland and its evidence for the long-term persistence of archaic hominin metapopulations (Broadfield et al. Reference Broadfield, Holloway, Mowbray, Silvers, Yuan and Márquez2001; Carbonell et al. Reference Carbonell, Bermúdez de Castro, Parés, Pérez-González, Cuenca-Bescós, Ollé, Mosquera, Huguet, van der Made, Rosas, Sala, Vallverdú, García, Granger, Martinón-Torres, Rodríguez, Stock, Vergès, Allué, Burjachs, Cáceres, Canals, Benito, Díez, Lozano, Mateos, Navazo, Rodríguez, Rosell and Arsuaga2008). These conditions facilitated maritime dispersal in other large-bodied mammals, and may have encouraged the intentional short-distance dispersal of Neanderthals in the Mediterranean (Broodbank Reference Broodbank2006, Reference Broodbank2014), as well as passive dispersal of H. erectus sensu lato in ISEA and potentially the Mediterranean (Dennell et al. Reference Dennell, Louys, O'Regan and Wilkinson2014; Leppard Reference Leppard2014). The lack of evidence for archaic hominins from, for example, Madagascar or the North Atlantic volcanic islands—structurally unsuitable contexts for successful passive dispersal—further hints at the applicability of biogeography and dispersal studies of non-human taxa for interpreting this patterning.
Finally, in terms of developing robust bodies of theory for interrogating suggestive data, we highlight the interest amongst scholars who deal with maritime dispersal in moderns in using palaeoenvironmental data to refine models of colonisation (Burney Reference Burney1997; Rick et al. Reference Rick, Kirch, Erlandson and Fitzpatrick2013). The essential working assumption is that human colonisation should be accompanied by various types of environmental reorganisation (both direct, such as fire-setting and exploitation of endemic biota; and indirect, such as disruptions in nutrient-cycling deriving from trophic reorganisation in disturbed ecologies), which should be visible in the palaeoenvironmental record. There are reasons to suppose that the ecological impacts of agropastoral modern humans may be substantially greater than, or differ from, parallel types of impact in more archaic species. Even so, a productive avenue into assessing the probable nature and scale of any pre-modern maritime dispersal events may involve modelling probable types of hominin palaeoenvironmental impact (Phoca-Cosmetatou & Rabett Reference Phoca-Cosmetatou, Rabett, Boyle, Rabett and Hunt2014), particularly in terms of predation pressure affecting relative frequencies of taxa and subsequent assessment of the degree of correlation between the model and relevant datasets. In essence, the arrival of hominins in a previously insulated ecosystem should be visible as a series of processes in proxy records other than the archaeological.
Investigation and demonstration of maritime hominin dispersals in the Pleistocene
While we continue to disagree about the degree and type of evidence required to demonstrate maritime dispersals during the Pleistocene, we agree completely on probable pathways of productive investigation. Our goal is not to prescribe best practice, but to highlight strategies that are likely to generate data that help to clarify rather than obfuscate potential conclusions.
We suggested above that modes of fieldwork that are common in the Mediterranean (less so in ISEA) will be intrinsically inefficient in identifying the type of phenomenon in which we are interested. Accordingly, it is hard to be sure whether the continued absence of evidence that might be interpreted as Lower–Middle Palaeolithic on some Mediterranean islands reflects Pleistocene behaviour or is an artefact of research. This is exacerbated by the lack of detailed palaeoenvironmental and palaeogeographical data for many areas, inhibiting the construction of robust chronostratigraphies. Testing hypotheses about archaic hominin maritime dispersals via archaeological data necessitates research designs involving ‘targeted’ pedestrian surveys, such as those used successfully in Greece (Runnels et al. Reference Runnels, Panagopoulou, Murray, Tsartsidou, Mullen and Tourloukis2005; Strasser et al. Reference Strasser, Panagopoulou, Runnels, Murray, Thompson, Karkanas, McCoy and Wegmann2010), and the investigation of niches likely to have been a focus of any hominin activity. Targeted surveys focus attention on geological and pedological contexts (identified after suitable palaeoenvironmental and palaeogeographic reconstructions) that have a high probability of preserving remains of the cultural phase of interest. Such surveys are indispensable—a point not always recognised—for identifying Palaeolithic remains that are not ‘sites’ in the usual sense (i.e. not deeply stratified), but more often clusters of stone tools, forming surface signatures on geologic outcrops or in lag deposits on palaeosurfaces, which represent brief bursts of activity amongst non-sedentary populations (Runnels Reference Runnels2014a).
Clearly, the more direct archaeological evidence we have, and the better-dated this evidence, the more convincing will be associated claims for Pleistocene maritime dispersal. Accepting, however, that artefact clusters on lag deposits are, in many cases, likely to represent best-case scenarios in terms of strictly archaeological data, palaeoenvironmental records (i.e. fossil, sub-fossil, palynological) offer useful means of assessing ecosystem disturbance likely to accompany the arrival of Homo in a previously insulated context. We stress that a series of behaviourally plastic invasive species in the higher echelons of trophic systems colonising habitats hitherto free of such species should drive direct but also indirect ecosystemic change, and that this should be visible to a degree in environmental proxies. Not only more obvious examples of such proxies (such as direct evidence for predation or scavenging as evidenced by butchery marks or peri-mortem trauma), but also less obvious: asymmetric disturbance in herbivorous taxa driving dynamism in pollen deposition, for example, or behavioural adaptations in prey species reflected in stable isotope records.
Imaginative modelling of hominin environmental impacts will provide additional means, beyond contentious archaeological data, to evaluate claims for Pleistocene dispersal and colonisation. In general, maximising the qualitative and quantitative richness of data, driven by question-led fieldwork and complementary palaeoenvironmental studies (alongside the appropriate use of analogical argumentation, e.g. Cherry & Leppard Reference Cherry and Leppard2015; Howitt-Marshall & Runnels Reference Howitt-Marshall and Runnels2016), represents the best chance for advancing our understanding of hominin dispersal across biogeographic barriers.
Discussion
Did seas and oceans inhibit the spread of our genus around the planet, as has generally been assumed? Or did they instead enable the dispersal of hominins during the Early and Middle Pleistocene? There is much at stake here. The answer will have major implications for our understanding of the spatial and temporal structure of early human dispersal. If large expanses of open water inhibited archaic hominin movement, we can continue to rely upon the still dominant terrestrial model in understanding the peopling of Eurasia, and rule out contact of archaic hominins with insular Quaternary landmasses such as Australia/Sahul, Oceania and beyond to the Americas (recognising that, Pitulko et al. (Reference Pitulko, Tikhonov, Pavlova, Nikolskiy, Kuper and Polozov2016) notwithstanding, Beringia remained too far north to constitute an effective land bridge until late in the Pleistocene). Conversely, if it is demonstrated that open sea-crossings enabled archaic hominin dispersals between landmasses, this would revolutionise our understanding of the human diaspora across the globe.
The implications are not limited to dispersal. Intrinsic to our current understanding of the structure of the spread of our genus in spatial and temporal terms is an assumption that this structure in part derives from the ability, incrementally increasing through evolutionary time, of Homo to overcome certain types of environmental restriction: that is, of more complex neurocognitive architecture allowing increasingly flexible behavioural responses to otherwise hostile environments (e.g. Davidson & Noble Reference Davidson and Noble1992; Davidson Reference Davidson2010, Reference Davidson2013; Leppard Reference Leppard2015b; Howitt-Marshall & Runnels Reference Howitt-Marshall and Runnels2016). If, in fact, there is no reason to suppose that the capacity to access and exploit challenging niches is related to cognitive evolution, and archaic Homo was undertaking long-distance dispersal in the Early and Middle Pleistocene, then discussions of ‘behavioural modernity’ will require substantial revision. In this sense, many archaeologists—working in diverse research contexts—are stakeholders in this debate. All the more reason for urging caution, clarity of expression and intellectual openness as we pursue the research agenda sketched here.
Acknowledgements
We are extremely grateful to the two reviewers for their considered and illuminating comments on an earlier draft of this paper. We are particularly thankful to Robin Dennell for his detailed and insightful reaction to that draft, and we acknowledge his assistance in clarifying and framing the second half of the manuscript.