Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T23:50:37.182Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pragmatics abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 April 2013

COURTENAY FRAZIER NORBURY*
Affiliation:
Royal Holloway, University of London
TRACEY GEMMELL
Affiliation:
Winnebago County Special Education Cooperative
RHEA PAUL
Affiliation:
Sacred Heart University, Connecticut, USA
*
Address for Correspondence: Dr Courtenay Frazier Norbury, Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey, TW20 0EX, United Kingdom. tel: +44(0)1784 276 139; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We aimed to disentangle contributions of socio-pragmatic and structural language deficits to narrative competence by comparing the narratives of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n=25), non-autistic children with language impairments (LI; n=23), and children with typical development (TD; n=27). Groups were matched for age (6½ to 15 years; mean: 10;6) and non-verbal ability; ASD and TD groups were matched on standardized language scores. Despite distinct clinical presentation, children with ASD and LI produced similarly simple narratives that lacked semantic richness and omitted important story elements, when compared to TD peers. Pragmatic errors were common across groups. Within the LI group, pragmatic errors were negatively correlated with story macrostructure scores and with an index of semantic–pragmatic relevance. For the group with ASD, pragmatic errors consisted of comments that, though extraneous, did not detract from the gist of the narrative. These findings underline the importance of both language and socio-pragmatic skill for producing coherent, appropriate narratives.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by a Nuffield Foundation New Career Development Fellowship and an Experimental Psychology Society study visit grant to C. F. Norbury, and grants K24 HD045576 from U.S. National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders and P01-HD03008 from U.S. National Institute of Child Health and Development to R. Paul. The authors are indebted to the children and families who participated in this study.

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV, 4th ed.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2012). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Proposed DSM-IV, 5th ed.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Available at <http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevision/Pages/NeurodevelopmentalDisorders.aspx>..>Google Scholar
Beaumont, R. & Newcombe, P. (2006). Theory of mind and central coherence in adults with high-functiong autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism 10, 365–82.Google Scholar
Berman, R. (2009). Language development in narrative contexts. In Bavin, E. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of child language, 354–75. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The children's communication checklist-2. London: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: phenomimicry or shared etiology? Behavior Genetics 40, 618–29.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-year-olds: distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 52, 156–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreau, D. (2008). Narrative abilities: advances in research and implications for clinical practice. Topics in Language Disorders 28(2), 99114.Google Scholar
Bowyer-Crane, C., Snowling, M. J., Duff, F. J., Fieldsend, E., Carroll, J. M., Miles, J., Götz, K. & Hulme, C. (2008). Improving early language and literacy skills: differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 49, 422–32.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry 18, 121.Google Scholar
Cantwell, D., Baker, L. & Ruttter, M. (1978). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder – analysis of syntax and language function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 19, 351–62.Google Scholar
Capps, L., Losh, M. & Thurber, C. (2000). ‘The frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad’: narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 28(2), 193204.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Loucas, T., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D. & Pickles, A. (2007). Efficacy of three screening instruments in the identification of autistic-spectrum disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry 191, 554–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cicourel, A. (1996). Ecological validity and ‘white room effects’: the interaction of cognitive and cultural models in the pragmatic analysis of elicited narratives from children. Pragmatics & Cognition 44, 221–64.Google Scholar
Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. & van der Lely, H. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38, 2840.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z. & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47(6), 621–28.Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A. & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a convariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 15, 331–43.Google Scholar
Diehl, J., Bennetto, C. & Young, E. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology & Health 34, 87102.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C. & Burley, L. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale, 2nd ed.Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I., de Marchena, A., Schuh, J. & Kelley, E. (2010). Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: a developmental review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5(2), 681–91.Google Scholar
Elliot, C. (1990). Differential Ability Scales. Oxford: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Gabig, C. S. (2008). Verbal working memory and story retelling in school-age children with autism. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 39, 498511.Google Scholar
Garcia-Perez, R., Hobson, P. & Lee, A. (2008). Narrative role-taking in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38, 156–68.Google Scholar
Gemmell, T. (2007). Can conversational deficits in children and young adults with autism be more clearly identified using a semi-structured interview measure versus a standardized pragmatic protocol? Unpublished Master's thesis, Department of Communication Disorders, Southern Connecticut State University, New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
Goldman, S. (2008). Brief report: narratives of personal events in children with autism and developmental language disorders: unshared memories. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38(10), 1982–88.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. (2000). Cultural and linguistic diversity resource guide for speech-language pathology. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Heath, S. (1986). Talking a cross-cultural look at narratives. Topics in Language Disorders 7(1), 8494.Google Scholar
Jolliffe, T. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine 30(5), 1169–87.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R. (1966). Cultural thought patterns in intercultural education. Language Learning 16, 12.Google Scholar
Kelley, E., Naigles, L. & Fein, D. (2010). An in-depth examination of optimal outcome in children with a history of autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 4, 526–38.Google Scholar
Kjelgaard, M. M. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 287308.Google Scholar
Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher- functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 41(7), 831–46.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A. & Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30, 205–23.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Petkova, E., Hus, V., Gan, W., Lu, F., Martin, D. M., … Risi, S. (2012). A multi-site study of the clinical diagnosis of different autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 69(3), 306–13.Google Scholar
Losh, M. & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33(3), 239–51.Google Scholar
Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D. & Baird, G. (2008). Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49(11), 1184–92.Google Scholar
Loveland, K., McEvoy, R., Tunali, B. & Kelley, M. (1990). Narrative story telling in autism and Down's syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 8, 923.Google Scholar
Loveland, K. & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative language in autism and the Theory of Mind hypothesis: a wider perspective. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. et al. (eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism, 247–66. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Manolitsi, M. & Botting, N. (2011). Language abilities in children with autism and language impairment: using narrative as an additional source of clinical information. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 27(1), 3955.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). A boy, a dog and his frog. New York: Dial Books for Young Readers.Google Scholar
Miller, G. & Chapman, J. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 4048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. (2003). Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Madison: University of Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. (2005). The relationship between Theory of Mind and metaphor: evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 23, 383–99.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 38(3), 287313.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Nash, M., Baird, G. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Using a parental checklist to identify diagnostic groups in children with communication impairment: a validation of the Children's Communication Checklist–2. International Journal of Communication Disorders 39, 345–64.Google Scholar
O'Connell, P. (1997). Speech, language, and hearing problems in schools: a guide for students and practitioners. Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publishers.Google Scholar
Pankratz, M., Plante, E., Vance, R. & Insalaco, D. (2007). The diagnostic and predictive validity of the Renfrew Bus Story. Language, Speech, & Hearing Services in Schools 38, 390–99.Google Scholar
Paul, R. (2007). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence, 3rd ed.St Louis: Mosby.Google Scholar
Paul, R., Hernandez, R., Taylor, L. & Johnson, K. (1996). Narrative development in later talkers: early school age. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39, 99107.Google Scholar
Peng, F. (1988). On the acquisition of discourse among autistic children. Language Sciences 10, 193224.Google Scholar
Polyani, L. (1989). Telling the American story: a structural and cultural analysis of conversational storytelling. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reichow, B., Salamak, S., Paul, R., Volkmar, F. & Klin, A. (2008). Pragmatic assessment in autism spectrum disorders: a comparison of a standard measure with parent report. Communication Disorders Quarterly 29, 169–76.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. (1969). The bus story. Oxford: Author.Google Scholar
Semel, E. & Wiig, E. (2006) Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th ed.London, UK: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Sheskin, D. J. (2000) Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statistical procedures, 2nd edn.Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall / CRC.Google Scholar
Stein, N. & Glen, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (ed.), New directions in discourse processing, Vol. 2, 53120. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B. & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: a follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research 41, 407–18.Google Scholar
Tabors, P., Snow, C. & Dickinson, D. (2001). Home and schools together: supporting language and literacy development. In Dickinson, D. & Tabors, P. (eds.), Beginning literacy with language: young children learning at home and school, 313–34. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B(358), 303–14.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: a comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics 16, 241–56.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J. B. (2011). Co-morbidity of autism and SLI: kinds, kin and complexity. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 46(2), 127–37.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, J., Romme, N., Prodi, E., Leemanso, A., Zink, I., Vandewalle, E.Sunaert, S. (2012). Is there a common neuroanatomical substrate of language deficit between autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment? Cerebral Cortex 22(10), 2263–71.Google Scholar
Waterhouse, L. & Fein, D. (1982). Language skills in developmentally disabled children. Brain and Language 15(2), 307–33.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children, 3rd ed.San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children, 4th ed.San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders 41(4), 319–36.Google Scholar