Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T05:31:54.120Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hedonic products for you, utilitarian products for me

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Jingyi Lu*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, No. 3663 North Zhongshan Road, 200062 Shanghai, China
Zhengyan Liu
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University
Zhe Fang
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Consumers make trade-offs when they choose between utilitarian and hedonic products. The former is practical, instrumental, and functional, whereas the latter is sensational and experiential. Prior research shows that people feel more guilt when they contemplate on engaging in hedonic consumption than engaging in utilitarian consumption. The current research investigates the effect of decision targets (i.e., making decisions for oneself or another person) on preferences for utilitarian and hedonic products. Consumers deciding for others were more likely to choose hedonic over utilitarian options than were consumers deciding for themselves. Utilitarian/hedonic was manipulated either through attributes of similar products (Study 1) or through different products (Study 2). Anticipatory guilt accounted for such self–other difference (Study 2). In particular, anticipatory guilt triggered by contemplating hedonic consumption is less for consumers who made choices for others than for those who made choices for themselves. In sum, preferences for utilitarian and hedonic products depend on decision targets.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2016] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Previous research has suggested that consumers face a dilemma when either selecting between necessities and luxuries (Reference Kivetz and SimonsonKivetz & Simonson, 2002a, 2002b) or selecting between highbrow and lowbrow products (Reference Kronrod and DanzigerKronrod & Danziger, 2013). Typically, necessities and highbrow products are distinguished by utilitarian attributes that provide instrumental, functional, or practical utilities. By contrast, luxuries and lowbrow products are distinguished by sensations or experiences from using the products (Reference May and IrmakMay & Irmak, 2014; Reference Voss, Spangenberg and GrohmannVoss, Spangenberg & Grohmann, 2003). For example, consumers will gain knowledge by watching a documentary, whereas they will have fun by watching a comedy. In this case, watching a documentary is utilitarian consumption, whereas watching a comedy is hedonic consumption.

However, preferences for utilitarian and hedonic consumption are unstable. Preference reversals are prevalent even for the same consumer. Research has identified several factors in determining such preference reversals, including the language style of consumer reviews (Reference Kronrod and DanzigerKronrod & Danziger, 2013), attribute quantity (Reference Sela and BergerSela & Berger, 2012), and number of decision stages (Reference Bhargave, Charkravarti and GuhaBhargave, Charkravarti & Guha, 2015).

We consider two headsets as examples: one is utilitarian (i.e., with durable batteries and a traditional design), whereas the other is hedonic (i.e., with less durable batteries and a sleek design). Will you prefer the same headset if you buy for yourself or for another? In this research, we investigate how deciding for oneself or another person (i.e., decision targets) can influence preferences for utilitarian and hedonic consumption.

2 Anticipatory guilt and consumption type

Consumers benefit from both utilitarian and hedonic consumption. Utilitarian products are effective, helpful, functional, necessary, and practical, whereas hedonic products are fun, exciting, delightful, thrilling, and enjoyable (Reference Dhar and WertenbrochDhar & Wertenbroch, 2000; Reference Voss, Spangenberg and GrohmannVoss et al., 2003).

The utilitarian–hedonic distinction is not limited to the product level. This concept also applies to attributes. In this sense, certain products have both utilitarian and hedonic characteristics (Reference ChernevChernev, 2004; Reference Dhar and WertenbrochDhar & Wertenbroch, 2000). For example, a pair of athletic shoes has utilitarian attributes because it provides protection and enhances performance. This item has hedonic attributes as well, that is, wearing brand-name athletic shoes is enjoyable and exciting (Reference Voss, Spangenberg and GrohmannVoss et al., 2003). Therefore, whether a product is perceived to be utilitarian or hedonic is determined by its salient features.

In general, consumers perceive that engaging in hedonic rather than utilitarian consumption is not completely necessary (Reference Dhar and WertenbrochDhar & Wertenbroch, 2000). Utilitarian consumption is considerably linked to necessities, whereas hedonic consumption is substantially linked to luxuries (Reference Kivetz and SimonsonKivetz & Simonson, 2002a, 2002b). For this reason, utilities of utilitarian products are more practical and basic than those of hedonic products. Engaging in or even contemplating hedonic consumption triggers more guilt than engaging in or contemplating utilitarian consumption (Kiveta & Simonson, 2002b; Reference Zemack-Rugar, Rabino, Cavanaugh and FitzsimonsZemack-Rugar, Rabino, Cavanaugh & Fitzsimons, 2016). In sum, anticipatory guilt is more easily induced by hedonic consumption than utilitarian consumption.

Consumers will give up purchasing hedonic products if they feel a strong sense of anticipatory guilt (Reference Zemack-Rugar, Rabino, Cavanaugh and FitzsimonsZemack-Rugar et al., 2016). The stronger the level of anticipatory guilt induced by contemplating hedonic consumption, the less likely consumers will decide to buy hedonic products.

3 Decision targets and anticipatory guilt

In daily life, individuals make purchase decisions not only for themselves but also for others (Reference Baskin, Wakslak, Trope and NovemskyBaskin, Wakslak, Trope & Novemsky, 2014; Reference Steffel and Le BoeufSteffel & Le Boeuf, 2014). Individuals who make choices for others prefer more ideal options (Danziger, Montal & Barkan, 2012), products with higher desirability (Reference Lu, Xie and XuLu, Xie & Xu, 2013), and larger choice sets (Reference PolmanPolman, 2012) than those who make choices for themselves. However, research on the relationship between decision targets and preferences for utilitarian and hedonic consumption is generally limited. Accordingly, we develop our hypothesis regarding these two variables via anticipatory guilt.

People assign different expenditures to different mental accounts (Thaler, 1985, 1999). For example, money for food and entertainment are put into specific accounts. Similarly, products purchased for oneself and another person (usually in the form of gifts) are also put into two accounts (Thaler, 1985, 1999). Money in different accounts would be spent in different ways.

Mental accounting inhibits the impulse to engage in hedonic consumption for the money in the account of paying for oneself (Reference Cheema and SomanCheema & Soman, 2006; Reference KivetzKivetz, 1999). Hence, contemplating hedonic consumption induces anticipatory guilt as aforementioned because hedonic consumption is not essential. However, for the money in the account of paying for another person, contemplating hedonic consumption induces less anticipatory guilt (Reference KivetzKivetz, 1999).

Therefore, preferences for utilitarian and hedonic products shift on the basis of decision targets. We hypothesize that consumers who make choices for others would prefer hedonic products to utilitarian ones more than those who make choices for themselves. Laran (2010) provided initial evidence for our predictions. He found (in Study 5) that supermarket consumers who made choices for themselves were less likely to choose indulgent items than those who made choices for others. However, he failed to find an effect on the first purchase in a laboratory study, although he did find that later items in a sequence of purchases were used to “atone” for earlier indulgence, for self more than for others. Our research substantially analyzes the relationships among decision targets, anticipatory guilt, and hedonic consumption.

4 The present research

The present research tests the effect of decision targets on preferences for utilitarian and hedonic products. Accordingly, our hypotheses are as follows.

H1: Consumers who make decisions for others would be more likely to choose hedonic consumption over utilitarian consumption than would consumers deciding for themselves.

Moreover, hedonic consumption makes people who decide for others feel less guilty than those who decide for themselves. The difference in anticipatory guilt between making decisions for oneself and another person leads to different preferences for utilitarian and hedonic products.

H2: The anticipatory guilt level induced by contemplating on engaging in hedonic consumption would be responsible for the self–other difference in tradeoffs between utilitarian and hedonic consumption.

We conducted two studies to test our hypotheses. In Study 1, we investigated the relationship between decision targets and preferences for utilitarian and hedonic products. The participants, making decisions either for themselves or others, indicated their purchase likelihood for four pairs of products. The manipulation of utilitarian and hedonic characteristics was at an attribute level (Reference ChernevChernev, 2004; Reference Dhar and WertenbrochDhar & Wertenbroch, 2000). In each pair, one option featured dominant utilitarian attributes and the other had dominant hedonic attributes.

In Study 2, we explored the role of anticipatory guilt in which the participants made choices either for themselves or others. A total of 11 pairs of products were provided. The manipulation of utilitarian and hedonic characteristics was at a product level (Reference Kronrod and DanzigerKronrod & Danziger, 2013). Each pair included a utilitarian option (e.g., documentary) and hedonic option (e.g., a comedy). The participants rated their anticipatory guilt and indicated their preferences thereafter.

5 Study 1

Study 1 was designed to test whether consumers who decided for others were more likely to engage in hedonic consumption than those who decided for themselves (H1). The participants were asked to make choices either for themselves or for others. Four pairs of products were shown. In each pair, we provided an option with utilitarian attributes superior to its hedonic attributes. By contrast, the other option featured superior hedonic attributes but inferior utilitarian attributes (Reference Bhargave, Charkravarti and GuhaBhargave et al., 2015; Reference ChernevChernev, 2004; Reference Roy and NgRoy & Ng, 2012; Reference Sela and BergerSela & Berger, 2012; Reference Yeung and WyerYeung & Wyer, 2004). The participants rated how likely they would purchase each option.

5.1 Method

5.1.1 Participants and design

Seventy university students (27 men, 43 women; M age = 20.74 years) participated in Study 1. They were randomly assigned to a condition in a 2 (decision target: self or other) × 2 (dominant attribute: utilitarian or hedonic) mixed design where decision target was a between-participant variable, whereas dominant attribute was a within-participant variable.

5.1.2 Procedure and materials

The cover story was that the research would investigate how people make choices in daily life. The participants in the self condition were asked to write down their surnames, age, and gender. Those in the other condition selected a friend and then wrote down his or her name, age, and gender. This task ensured that the participants in both conditions thought of a specific person.

Thereafter, four pairs of products were shown (Table 1). The participants in the self condition were told to consider making purchase decisions for themselves (e.g., “you are going to buy a headset for you”), whereas those in the other condition were told to consider making purchase decisions for the friends whose names had been written down earlier (e.g., “you are going to buy a headset for your friend whose name has been written down”). For each pair, one option featured dominant utilitarian attributes (i.e., superior in utilitarian attributes but inferior in hedonic attributes) and the other option had dominant hedonic attributes (i.e., superior in hedonic attributes but inferior in utilitarian attributes).Footnote 1 The order of the two options was counterbalanced across the participants. The participants rated their purchase probability (1 = not at all probable, 9 = very probable) for each option.

Table 1: Products in Study 1

Next, the participants indicated the difficulty of this task (“How difficult is the task?” 1 = not at all difficult, 7 = very difficult) and perceived responsibility (“Do you feel responsible for the decision outcome?” 1 = not at all responsible, 9 = very responsible). Thereafter, demographic information, including age, gender, and monthly consumption, was collected. Finally, the participants were thanked, debriefed, and paid 15 RMB (renminbi).

5.2 Results and discussion

The participants in the self and other conditions did not differ from one another in terms of task difficulty, perceived responsibility, and monthly consumption, F(1, 68) = 0.19, p = .662, η p2 < .01, F(1, 68) = 0.26, p = .609, η p2 < .01, and F(1, 68) = 1.07, p = .306, η p2 = .02, respectively (Table 2). Therefore, these variables were excluded from the subsequent analysis.

Table 2: Means (standard deviations) for control and dependent variables

The average purchase probabilities of the four options dominant in utilitarian attributes and four options dominant in hedonic attributes were computed. To test H1, a 2 (decision target) × 2 (dominant attribute) mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the average purchase probability was conducted. Results revealed a main effect for dominant attribute, F(1, 68) = 19.79, p < .001, η p2 = .23, such that the participants were more likely to buy the options dominant in utilitarian attributes (M = 6.28, SD = 1.24) than those dominant in hedonic attributes (M = 5.15, SD = 1.31). The main effect for decision target was insignificant, F(1, 68) = 3.11, p = .082, η p2 = .04.

Crucially, we observed an interaction between decision target and dominant attribute (Table 2), F(1, 68) = 7.91, p = .006, η p2 = .10. The participants who made decisions for others were more likely to purchase the options dominant in hedonic attributes than those who made decisions for themselves, F(1, 68) = 11.05, p= .001, η p2 = .14. However, the purchase probability did not differ in terms of the products dominant in utilitarian attributes between the self and other conditions, F(1, 68) = 1.37, p = .247, η p2 = .02. From another perspective, the participants in the self condition preferred utilitarian options over hedonic options, F(1, 38) = 29.93, p < .001, η p2 = .44. However, no difference was observed in purchase likelihood between the utilitarian and hedonic options in the othercondition, F(1, 30) = 1.19, p = .284, η p2 = .04. Notably, this interaction was crossover and nonremovable according to the classification by Reference Wagenmakers, Krypotps, Criss and IversonWagenmakers, Krypotps, Criss, and Iverson (2012).

In addition, we conducted four 2 (decision target) × 2 (dominant attribute) mixed ANOVAs on purchase probability for the four products, respectively. Generally, the results were consistent across products. The interactions were significant for headset and laptop, F(1, 68) = 5.46, p = .022, η p2 = .07 and F(1, 68) = 7.94, p = .006, η p2 = .11. These two interactions were also nonremovable (Reference Wagenmakers, Krypotps, Criss and IversonWagenmakers et al., 2012). However, the interations were insignificant for foaming cleanser and chocolate, F(1, 68) = 1.92, p = .170, η p2 = .03 and F(1, 68) = 0.03, p = .857, η p2 < .01. Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations in each cell of each product.

The overall results supported H1. Accordingly, consumers who decided for others preferred hedonic consumption over utilitarian consumption compared with consumers who decided for themselves. In the next study, we tested the underlying mechanism of this effect.

6 Study 2

The objective of Study 2 was twofold. First, we sought to replicate the findings in Study 1 to provide further evidence for H1. Second, we investigated why consumers who made decisions for others preferred hedonic products over utilitarian products more than those who made decisions for themselves. We explored the role of anticipatory guilt induced by hedonic products. Hence, we tested H2 by analyzing whether the anticipatory guilt level would be responsible for the results found in Study 1.

The participants were asked to make choices either for themselves or for others. A total of 11 pairs of products were shown. In each pair, one option was utilitarian (e.g., a highbrow movie), whereas the other was hedonic (e.g., a lowbrow movie). The manipulation of utilitarian and hedonic characteristics was at the product level. For most pairs of products, we used the highbrow–lowbrow distinction to reflect utilitarian–hedonic distinctions because both highbrow and utilitarian products serve a prudent purpose, whereas both lowbrow and hedonic products purely offer pleasure instead of other benefits. This is a common practice in this field (Reference Kronrod and DanzigerKronrod & Danziger, 2013; Reference Sela, Berger and LiuSela, Berger & Liu, 2009; Reference Zemack-Rugar, Rabino, Cavanaugh and FitzsimonsZemack-Rugar et al., 2016). The participants rated their level of anticipatory guilt and specified their preferences thereafter.

6.1 Method

6.1.1 Participants and design

Seventy-five undergraduates (27 men, 48 women; M age = 19.89 years) participated in Study 2. They were randomly assigned to make choices either for themselves or for another person.

6.1.2 Procedure and materials

Similar to Study 1, the participants in the self condition wrote down their own surname, age, and gender. Those in the other condition selected a friend and then wrote down his or her surname, age, and gender. Thereafter, 11 pairs of products were provided (Table 3), each with a combination of one utilitarian and one hedonic product.Footnote 2 The participants indicated which choice would make them feel more guilty (1 = Option A, 9 = Option B). Thereafter, they rated their preferences (1 = Option A, 9 = Option B) and made their choices.

Table 3: Products in Study 2

To assess any effect of individual differences in guilt-sensitivity, the participants completed a guilt proneness scale (e.g., “After realizing you have received too much change at a store, you decide to keep it because the salesclerk doesn’t notice. What is the likelihood that you would feel uncomfortable about keeping the money?” 8 items; 1= very unlikely, 7 = very likely) which was used to assess their propensity to experience guilt in daily life (Reference Cohen, Wolf, Panter and InskoCohen, Wolf, Panter & Insko, 2011). Next, demographic information, including age and gender, was collected. Finally, the participants were thanked and debriefed.

6.2 Results and discussion

The participants in the self (M = 6.02, SD = 0.61) and other (M = 5.92, SD = 0.71) conditions did not differ in guilt-sensitivity, F(1, 73) = 0.37, p = .548, η p2 = .01. Hence, this variable was excluded from the following statistical analysis.

A total of 11 ANOVAs on preference ratings for each pair with decision targets as the independent variable were conducted. A higher score indicates a higher preference for hedonic products. Generally, the results were consistent within the 11 pairs of products, showing that the participants who made decisions for others preferred hedonic products more than those who made decisions for themselves (Table 4). We also calculated the average preference ratings for the 11 pairs of products. An ANOVA on the average preference ratings using decision targets as the independent variable revealed a significant effect for decision targets, F(1, 73) = 15.36, p < .001, η p2 = .17. The score was higher in the other condition than in the selfcondition (Table 4). These results supported H1, thereby indicating that the participants deciding for others preferred hedonic products more than those deciding for themselves.

Table 4: Means (standard deviations) and statistical values for anticipatory guilt and preference for hedonic options

In addition, we calculated the percentage of the participants who selected the utilitarian and hedonic options in each pair. The results of 11 chi-square tests were generally consistent, suggesting that making decisions for another person promoted preferences for hedonic options over utilitarian options (Table 5). We also computed the purchase rate of hedonic options among the 11 pairs of products. An ANOVA on the purchase rate of hedonic options using decision targets as the independent variable yielded a significant effect for decision targets, F(1, 73) = 20.91, p < .001, η p2 = .22. This rate was higher in the other condition (M = 66.67%, SD = 15.21%) than in the selfcondition (M = 47.55%, SD = 20.38%). These results supported H1.

Table 5: Purchase percentage of utilitarian and hedonic products

Next, we conducted a total of 11 ANOVAs on the level of anticipatory guilt for each pair of products using decision targets as the independent variable. A higher score indicates a higher level of anticipatory guilt induced by the hedonic products. The results for all the 11 pairs of products were in the predicted direction, revealing that the participants who made choices for others felt less guilt than those who made choices for themselves (Table 4). Furthermore, we calculated the average anticipatory guilt. An ANOVA on the average anticipatory guilt with decision targets as the independent variable revealed a significant effect for decision targets, F(1, 73) = 16.39, p < .001, η p2 = .18. The score was lower in the other condition than in the selfcondition (Table 4).

Finally, we tested a mediation model with the decision target as the independent variable, the average anticipatory guilt induced by hedonic options as a potential mediator, and the average preference for hedonic options as the dependent variable. Anticipatory guilt was influenced by the decision target, β = −.43, p < .001. Additionally, the total effect of the decision target on preference for hedonic options was significant, β = .42, p < .001. However, when both the decision target and anticipatory guilt were included in the regression model to predict preference for hedonic options, the effect of the decision target became insignificant, β = .10, p = .205, while the effect of anticipatory guilt was significant, β = −.73, p < .001 (Figure 1).

Figure 1: Mediation model. Standardized coefficients and their significances are reported. The total effect of the decision target on preference for hedonic options is reported in parentheses and the standardized coefficient when the mediator is included in the model is reported above the arrow. *** denotes p < .001.

The significance of the indirect effect was tested using bootstrapping procedure (Reference Preacher and HayesPreacher & Hayes, 2008). On the basis of 5,000 bootstrap samples, the analysis generated a 95% confidence interval (CI) around the indirect effect with zero falling outside of the confidence interval (95% CI [0.47, 1.32]), indicating that anticipatory guilt induced by hedonic options mediated the effect of the decision target on preference for hedonic options. Specifically, making decisions for another person decreased the level of anticipatory guilt triggered by hedonic consumption, thereby subsequently promoting preferences for hedonic options. These results provided evidence for H2.

In summary, the findings in Study 2 demonstrated that consumers who purchased products for others experienced less anticipatory guilt from hedonic consumption; thus, they preferred hedonic options compared with consumers who purchased for themselves.

7 General discussion

Researchers attempt to identify the factors promoting utilitarian or hedonic consumption. The present study demonstrates an effect of decision targets. Hedonic consumption induces less anticipatory guilt in consumers who make purchase decisions for others than those who make purchase decisions for themselves. Therefore, the former prefer hedonic over utilitarian products, compared with the latter.

7.1 Decision targets and specific emotions

Our study contributes to the research on self–other differences in terms of emotions. For example, people who make decisions for the self experience stronger emotions than people who make decisions for another person (Reference Albrecht, Volz, Sutter and von CramonAlbrecht, Volz, Sutter & von Cramon, 2013). However, these studies focus on general emotion experiences, thereby ignoring self–other differences regarding specific emotions. To our knowledge, the literature on self–other differences among specific emotions is generally limited.

In the context of utilitarian and hedonic consumptions, we clarify the role of guilt, which is a specific emotion beyond emotions distinguished by their valence. Our findings show that the guilt triggered by hedonic consumption is less among consumers who make purchase decisions for others than among consumers who make purchase decisions for themselves. Future research may focus on other specific emotions that are important in certain decision scenarios.

7.2 Decision targets and mental accounting

The current findings are also pertinent to the research on mental accounting. Researchers propose that money is assigned into different mental accounts on the basis of both how it comes (e.g., regular income vs. windfalls; bonus vs. rebate) and goes (e.g., paying for bills vs. paying for leisure) (Reference Epley, Mak and IdsonEpley, Mak & Idson, 2006; Thaler, 1985, 1999). Our research suggests that the money paying for oneself and others are allocated into specific mental accounts. Individuals treat these accounts in different fashions, therefore causing divergent preferences.

Furthermore, recent research reveals that paying by gift cards (vs. cash) shift consumption from utilitarian to hedonic products (Reference Helion and GilovichHelion & Gilovich, 2014). The present study shows that preferences for utilitarian and hedonic options also depend on decision targets. We speculate that people budget less (vs. more) money to hedonic products in the mental account for money paying for themselves (vs. others).

7.3 Other mechanisms for self–other differences in hedonic consumption

Our research tested the role of anticipatory guilt in self–other differences regarding hedonic consumption. However, other mechanisms may also exist. For example, it may be insulting for the participants who made decisions for other people in Study 2 to give someone a lowbrow product.Footnote 3

Another potential reason for our findings is the different goals in self–other decision making. On the one hand, the decisions made for another person are more public than the decisions made for oneself. For this reason, consumers are more concerned with how a certain product would shape oneself when purchasing for others than when purchasing for themselves (Reference Wicklund and GollwitzerWicklund & Gollwitzer, 1981). On the other hand, hedonic products are perceived to express oneself more than utilitarian products (Reference Maimaran and SimonsonMaimaran & Simonson, 2011). Taken together, consumers who purchase for others would prefer hedonic products more than those who purchase for themselves. Future research may investigate the role of self-expression in self–other differences regarding hedonic consumption.

7.4 Practical implications

Effective market segmentation is vital for marketers. Nowadays, new segments are constantly emerging. Our findings support the conclusion that marketers should describe a product using its hedonic features for consumers who make purchase decisions for others (e.g., gift givers). However, they should describe a product by its utilitarian features for consumers who make purchase decisions for themselves.

In addition, the current findings suggest recommendations for marketing hedonic products to consumers who make purchase decisions for themselves. In this case, sellers may encourage customers to share product information with friends via social networks. Thereafter, friends who decide for the customers would have positive attitudes toward hedonic products.

Footnotes

*

This research was financially funded by the programs of National Natural Science Foundation of China (71501073) and China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (2015M570344 and 2016T90344). Zhengyan Liu and Zhe Fang contribute to the paper equally. We extend our sincere gratitude to Jonathan Baron, Tian-Yi Hu, Qingzhou Sun, and the two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments on an earlier version. We also thank Bingjie Li for her help in collecting data.

1 We conducted a pilot study to ensure that our manipulations of utilitarian and hedonic characteristics were valid. Thirty-three participants (15 men, 18 women; M age = 20.00 years) who were provided with the definitions of utilitarian and hedonic characteristics rated the extent to which each attribute in Table 1 (e.g., compatibility, battery life, design, and music indulgence) reflected utilitarian or hedonic characteristics (1 = utilitarian, 7 = hedonic). The manipulation was successful. For each product, the average score of two hedonic attributes (headset, M = 4.18, SD = 1.36; foaming cleanser, M = 4.08, SD = 1.42; laptop, M = 5.89, SD = 1.32; chocolate, M = 5.41, SD = 1.34) was higher than the average score of two utilitarian attributes (headset, M = 2.59, SD = 1.49; foaming cleanser, M = 1.67, SD = 1.06; laptop, M = 2.06, SD = 1.28; chocolate, M = 2.55, SD = 1.43), F(1, 32) = 29.79, p < .001, η p2= .48, F(1, 32) = 57.16, p < .001, η p2= .64, F(1, 32) = 80.06, p < .001, η p2= .71, and F(1, 32) = 58.07, p < .001, η p2= .65, respectively.

2 A pilot study was conducted to ensure our manipulations of utilitarian and hedonic options were valid. Thirty-seven participants (13 men, 24 women; M age = 21.78 years), who were provided with the definitions of utilitarian and hedonic products, rated the extent to which the options in Table 3 (e.g., documentary and comedy) reflected utilitarian or hedonic characteristics (1 = utilitarian, 7 = hedonic). The results showed that the average score of hedonic options (M = 5.51, SD = 0.70) in Table 3 was higher than the average score of utilitarian options (M = 3.04, SD = 0.90), F(1, 36) = 130.73, p < .001, η p2= .78. Thereafter, we conducted 11 within-product comparisons. The results showed that for each pair, the hedonic options scored higher than the utilitarian options, ps < .001, thereby indicating successful manipulations.

3 We thank Jonathan Baron for raising this possibility.

References

Albrecht, K., Volz, K. G., Sutter, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2013). What do I want and when do I want it: Brain correlates of decisions made for self and other. PLoS ONE, 8, e73531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baskin, E., Wakslak, C. J., Trope, Y., & Novemsky, N. (2014). Why feasibility matters more to gift receivers than to givers: A construal-level approach to gift giving. Journal of Consumer Research, 41, 169182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhargave, R., Charkravarti, A., & Guha, A. (2015). Two-stage decisions increase preference for hedonic options. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 130, 123135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheema, A., & Soman, D. (2006). Malleable mental accounting: The effect of flexibility on the justification of attractive spending and consumption decisions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 3344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chernev, A. (2004). Goal–attribute compatibility in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 141150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, T. R., Wolf, S. T., Panter, A. T., & Insko, C. A. (2011). Introducing the GASP scale: A new measure of guilt and shame proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 947966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, S., Montal, R., & Barken, R. (2012). Idealistic advice and pragmatic choice: A psychological distance account. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 11051117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhar, R., & Wertenbroch, K. (2000). Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 37, 6071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epley, N., Mak, D., & Idson, L. C. (2006). Bonus of rebate?: The impact of income framing in spending and saving. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 213227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helion, C., & Gilovich, T. (2014). Gift cards and mental accounting: Green-lighting hedonic spending. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 27, 386393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivetz, R. (1999). Advances in research on mental accounting and reason-based choice. Marketing Letters, 10, 249266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivetz, R., & Simonson, I. (2002a). Earning the right to indulge: Effort as a determinant of customer preferences toward frequency program rewards. Journal of Marketing Research, 39, 155170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivetz, R., & Simonson, I. (2002b). Self-control for the righteous: Toward a theory of precommitment to indulgence. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 199217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kronrod, A., & Danziger, S. (2013). “Wii will rock you!” The use and effect of figurative language in consumer reviews of hedonic and utilitarian consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 40, 726739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laran, J. (2010). Goal management in sequential choices: Consumer choices for others are more indulgent than personal choices. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 304314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lu, J., Xie, X., & Xu, J. (2013). Desirability or feasibility: Self-other decision-making difference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 144155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maimaran, M., & Simonson, I. (2011). Multiple routes to self- versus other- expression in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 755766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, F., & Irmak, C. (2014). Licensing indulgence in the present by distorting memories of past behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 41, 624641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polman, E. (2012). Effects of self-other decision making on regulatory focus and choice overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 980993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, R., & Ng, S. (2012). Regulatory focus and preference reversal between hedonic and utilitarian consumption. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 11, 8188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sela, A., & Berger, J. (2012). How attribute quantity influences option choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 149, 942953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sela, A., Berger, J., & Liu, W. (2009). Variety, vice, and virtue: How assortment size influences option choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 941951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffel, M., & Le Boeuf, R. A. (2014). Overindividuation in gift giving: Shopping for multiple recipients leads givers to choose unique but less preferred gifts. Journal of Consumer Research, 40, 11671180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4, 199214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R. H. (1999). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 183206.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voss, K. E., Spangenberg, E. R., & Grohmann, B. (2003). Measuring the hedonic and utilitarian dimensions of consumer attitude. Journal of Marketing Research, 40, 310320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Krypotps, A.-M., Criss, A. H., & Iverson, G. (2012). On the interpretation of removable interations: A survey of the field 33 years after Loftus. Memory and Cognition, 40, 145160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1981). Symbolic self-completion, attempted influence, and self-deprecation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 89114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, C. W. M. & Wyer, Jr. R. S. (2004). Affect, appraisal, and consumer judgment. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 412424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zemack-Rugar, Y., Rabino, R., Cavanaugh, L. A., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2016). When donating is liberating: The role of product and consumer characteristics in the appeal of cause-related products. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26, 213230.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: Products in Study 1

Figure 1

Table 2: Means (standard deviations) for control and dependent variables

Figure 2

Table 3: Products in Study 2

Figure 3

Table 4: Means (standard deviations) and statistical values for anticipatory guilt and preference for hedonic options

Figure 4

Table 5: Purchase percentage of utilitarian and hedonic products

Figure 5

Figure 1: Mediation model. Standardized coefficients and their significances are reported. The total effect of the decision target on preference for hedonic options is reported in parentheses and the standardized coefficient when the mediator is included in the model is reported above the arrow. *** denotes p < .001.

Supplementary material: File

Lu et al. supplementary material

Lu et al. supplementary material 1
Download Lu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 6.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lu et al. supplementary material

Lu et al. supplementary material 2
Download Lu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 5.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lu et al. supplementary material

Lu et al. supplementary material 3
Download Lu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lu et al. supplementary material

Lu et al. supplementary material 4
Download Lu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 4.7 KB