Introduction
Mental imagery refers to cognitive representations accompanying the ‘experience of sensory information without a direct external stimulus’ (Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes and Kosslyn2015; p. 590). Although the visual domain is predominant, mental images can also involve other sensory modalities, including auditory, olfactory, gustatory elements, and bodily sensations (Iyadurai et al., Reference Iyadurai, Visser, Lau-Zhu, Porcheret, Horsch, Holmes and James2019). These visual representations are usually recalled from memory (Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes and Kosslyn2015); yet, not all mental imagery is voluntary. Unlike voluntary or spontaneous recall, such instances of unintentional retrieval do not arise from deliberate effort or search, and are typically unwanted and negative in nature (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Gregory, Lipton and Burgess2010). These involuntary intrusions are often recurrent and vivid perceptual retrieval of life-threatening events which spontaneously intrude into consciousness, triggered by both situational and internal cues (Çili and Stopa, Reference Çili and Stopa2015; Hackmann et al., Reference Hackmann, Clark and McManus2000). Intrusive mental imagery is distressing and feels uncontrollable, and may act as an ‘emotional amplifier’ (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Geddes, Colom and Goodwin2008) as well as eliciting dysfunctional cognitive and behavioural responses that contribute to the onset and maintenance of symptoms in several mental health disorders (Hales et al., Reference Hales, Blackwell, Di Simplicio, lyadurai, Young, Holmes, Brown and Clark2014).
The presence of involuntary images in the wide range of psychopathology has been well-established (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Watkins, Mansell and Shafran2004); it is a hallmark of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Brewin and Holmes, Reference Brewin and Holmes2003), and represents a common feature of many anxiety disorders (Hirsch and Holmes, Reference Hirsch and Holmes2007). Considered a transdiagnostic process, the content and the type of such intrusions, however, vary according to the core concerns of each disorder (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Gregory, Lipton and Burgess2010). For example, in health anxiety, images usually depict physical catastrophes centred on themes of death and serious disease, whilst reflecting negative beliefs about the self and the consequences of illness and death (Wells and Hackmann, Reference Wells and Hackmann1993). Such images can prompt people to engage in avoidance, rumination, distraction and reassurance-seeking behaviours, which can in turn increase anxiety and pre-occupation with one’s health, leading to a vicious cycle (Muse et al., Reference Muse, McManus, Hackmann, Williams and Williams2010).
Because of the growing recognition of the pivotal role of dysfunctional images across many psychological conditions, greater prominence has been given to the development of imagery-based interventions in cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), adopting a multitude of strategies to help patients discern and challenge their distorted thoughts (Blackwell, Reference Blackwell2021; Fenn and Byrne, Reference Fenn and Byrne2013). By probing various aspects of theories and models underpinning CBT approaches, mental imagery can provide a valuable experimental tool to assist patients’ recovery, facilitating cognitive, affective, and behavioural changes (Saulsman et al., Reference Saulsman, Ji and McEvoy2019).
One area where imagery may be important is chronic pain. Chronic pain is a major public health care problem, a significant source of suffering for 100 million people in Europe (Breivik et al., Reference Breivik, Collett, Ventafridda, Cohen and Gallacher2006), affecting 35–51% of the adult population in the UK (Fayaz et al., Reference Fayaz, Croft, Langford, Donaldson and Jones2016). Accompanied by strong mental distress and functional disability (Dueñas et al., Reference Dueñas, Ojeda, Salazar, Mico and Failde2016), chronic pain has a negative implication on the quality of life of its sufferers, making them prone to multiple mental health conditions (e.g. anxiety disorders, depression; McWilliams et al., Reference McWilliams, Cox and Enns2003). An international survey by Graham et al. (Reference Graham, Brown, Vincent and Horne2020), assessing a large sample size (n = 785), demonstrated that 52% of the women with endometriosis-associated pain reported experiencing pain imagery. The authors go on to suggest therapeutic implications for pain management, such as targeting imagery within current evidence-based interventions such as CBT.
Psychological treatments for chronic pain are recommended as part of a multi-modal treatment approach (Dale and Stacey, Reference Dale and Stacey2016). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT; Vowles et al., Reference Vowles, Witkiewitz, Sowden and Ashworth2014) and eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR; de Roos et al., Reference de Roos, Veenstra, de Jongh, den Hollander-Gijsman, van der Wee, Zitman and van Rood2010; Grant and Threlfo, Reference Grant and Threlfo2002) are two more recent interventions developed for use in chronic pain, while CBT has been the dominant psychological treatment since the 1970s (Grant and Threlfo, Reference Grant and Threlfo2002).
Cognitive behavioural therapy is a widely accepted and effective psychosocial approach for pain, integrated into interdisciplinary pain rehabilitation programs to induce functional remission and reduce medication use and healthcare utilisation (Gilliam et al., Reference Gilliam, Schumann, Cunningham, Evans, Luedtke, Morrison, Sperry and Vowles2021). While it is important to continue delivering CBT for pain due to its positive impact on patient outcomes, a systematic review by Williams et al. (Reference Williams, Fisher, Hearn and Eccleston2020) found that CBT has only modest benefits for pain management, and these benefits are not sustainable over the long term. Furthermore, the lack of clarity regarding pain treatment quality indicators in research (e.g. treatment content, and duration; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Morley, Eccleston and Williams2005) makes it unclear which aspects should be included in interventions, additionally limiting the understanding of its effectiveness and how this is achieved (Zuhury et al., Reference Zuhury, Mason-Robbie and Walklet2021).
To date, few studies investigate the influence intrusive imagery might have on people with chronic pain. Evidence suggests that the effects of imagery on chronic pain are similar to those observed in other mental health conditions. However, little is known about the consequences of mental images on individuals’ emotions, cognitions, and behaviours during pain.
Initial research by Berna et al. (Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011) focused on examining the characteristics of intrusive mental imagery in a sample of individuals with chronic pelvic pain using a qualitative approach. Following their findings, the authors proposed a CBT-based framework to explain the potential role of imagery in chronic pain. In the study, images were described as carrying negative connotations, subsequently leading participants to engage in avoidance behaviours. Yet, determining whether these images directly affected cognition or merely reflected existing pain experiences proved to be challenging. By adopting semi-structured interviews, a similar study by Philips (Reference Philips2011) found that intrusive mental imagery exacerbated negative emotions (i.e. anxiety, sadness, and anger) in participants with both acute and chronic pain. However, this study did not delve deeper into the understanding of the cognitions, emotions and behaviours brought about by such imagery, nor did it explore the relationship between the potency of the images and the beliefs elicited by them. Recent research (Gillanders et al., Reference Gillanders, Potter and Morris2012; Gosden et al., Reference Gosden, Morris, Ferreira, Grady and Gillanders2014) employed postal surveys for data collection instead. This has resulted in participants using descriptive metaphors for their pain instead of conveying actual images they experienced (Gillanders et al., Reference Gillanders, Potter and Morris2012). With studies not determining participants’ thoughts and behaviours triggered by intrusive imagery, findings were therefore limited to the presence of imagery and their general characteristics.
With only two studies (Berna et al., Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011; Philips, Reference Philips2011) exploring such cognitions and behaviours on a sample of participants with chronic pain and intrusive imagery, the extent of this relationship remains unclear. Research specifically exploring the effects of intrusive mental imagery may therefore help elucidate critical treatment components to provide more effective interventions for managing the pain (Jensen, Reference Jensen2011). However, without the detailed information provided by a qualitative approach, it might be difficult to ascertain the impact of mental imagery on those with chronic pain. Hence, the current research attempts to bridge this gap by shedding light on participants’ lived experiences of pain-related visual images, providing further insight into how intrusive imagery affects their day-to-day lives, and offering a potential future target to enhance cognitive behavioural therapy for chronic pain.
Aims and objectives
The current study aims to gain a deeper understanding of mental imagery in chronic pain by (1) exploring the nature and characteristics of intrusive imagery in those with chronic pain, (2) investigating the characteristics and personal meaning that participants attribute to pain and pain-related visual images, and (3) examining the perceived impact on emotions, cognition, and behaviour.
Method
Design
A qualitative design with an inductive approach to data analysis was used. Individual interviews were conducted online, with a semi-structured approach which allowed an extensive description of participants’ pain-related mental imagery and the effect on their pain experience. The qualitative research was underpinned by a critical-realist ontology and followed a contextualist epistemological position (Braun and Clarke, Reference Braun and Clarke2006), assuming the existence of multiple realities and the impact of factors such as participants’ social context and experience with the imagery on the impact of imagery itself.
Participants and recruitment
Participation was limited to those (age 18+) with a diagnosis of chronic pain. This was self-reported, in that participants indicated a diagnosis of chronic pain had been given by a medical professional, however this was not formally confirmed. Participants were required to confirm that their pain had lasted for a minimum duration of 3 months and affected different locations of the body (in accordance with the ICD-11). In addition, experiences of intrusive mental images pertaining to their pain must be present. Individuals not fluent in English and with a history of psychotic illness were excluded due to the possibility of distinct perceptions of imagery from those discussed in the current study.
Snowball sampling was adopted to maximise recruitment during the planned sampling period of April to October 2022. The study was advertised online across social media platforms (e.g. Twitter, Facebook). As an incentive, participants were eligible to enter a randomised prize draw for one £50 Amazon eGift voucher upon completion of the study.
Measures
Demographic questionnaire
The demographic questionnaire gathered basic personal and clinically relevant descriptive information of the sample. Particularly, pain-related data included the first diagnosis of pain, primary pain location, pain duration, and average pain levels.
Visual analog scale
The visual analog scale (VAS) is a pain rating scale used to obtain a rough understanding of varying degrees of pain experienced by participants. The VAS has been extensively adopted in patients with chronic pain (Hjermstad et al., Reference Hjermstad, Fayers, Haugen, Caraceni, Hanks, Loge, Fainsinger, Aass and Kaasa2011), with evidence supporting its reliability and validity (Bijur et al., Reference Bijur, Silver and Gallagher2001; Gallagher et al., Reference Gallagher, Bijur, Latimer and Silver2002; Gift, Reference Gift1989).
Short Health Anxiety Inventory
The Short Health Anxiety Inventory (SHAI; Salkovskis et al., Reference Salkovskis, Rimes, Warwick and Clark2002) is a 14-item self-rated questionnaire assessing health anxiety. It is a reliable and valid measure that has shown high internal consistency (Abramowitz et al., Reference Abramowitz, Deacon and Valentiner2007) alongside high reliability across a range of clinical and non-clinical samples (Alberts et al., Reference Alberts, Hadjistavropoulos, Jones and Sharpe2013).
Given the high prevalence of health anxiety in patients with chronic pain (Rode et al., Reference Rode, Salkovskis, Dowd and Hanna2006), the SHAI was employed to characterise subjects and to illustrate their levels of health anxiety using a clinical cut-off score of ≥18 (Salkovskis et al., Reference Salkovskis, Rimes, Warwick and Clark2002). However, the measure was not included as one of the main outcomes of the study.
Semi-structured interviews
To fit the experience-type research question, an interview schedule was prepared before conducting the interview, yet without strictly adhering to it to provide responsiveness to participants.
The interview guide (Table 1) was adapted from past research exploring intrusive imagery (Berna et al., Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011; Muse et al., Reference Muse, McManus, Hackmann, Williams and Williams2010; Philips, Reference Philips2011) and covered several areas. From the clarification of intrusive imagery to the definition of an index image, the aim was to thoroughly explore the characteristics of the most distressing and significant visual representation, assessing the clarity, viewpoint, and triggers of the chosen one. Participants answered all the other questions in relation to the index image only, additionally investigating cognitions regarding the imagery and emotional and behavioural responses resulting from experiencing such image during pain.
Procedure
Those willing to participate accessed the study link provided on the advert. Individuals were directed to the Qualtrics platform, where they were fully informed of the nature and purpose of the study through an information sheet. Participants were also provided with a consent form to be filled in before completing the online demographic survey and SHAI. Eligible participants were then contacted via email to arrange the interview session, and further oral consent was sought prior to proceeding with it. At the end of the interview, participants were given a debrief form, containing useful sources of support.
Each semi-structured interview was conducted using Microsoft Teams by the first author (R.D.N.), and audio-recorded using encrypted devices. All interviews lasted approximately 30–45 minutes, were transcribed verbatim and anonymised for subsequent analysis. Complying with the Data Protection Act 2018, all interviews were stored securely and separately from personally identifiable data.
Analysis
Results from interviews were analysed by performing thematic analysis, using the six-recursive-phases guide described by Braun and Clarke (Reference Braun and Clarke2006) while adopting an inductive, data-driven approach.
A minimum of 10 participants was set for this study. As per guidelines for thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke, Reference Braun and Clarke2006), a sample of between six and ten participants has been recommended for small projects, which may be particularly relevant to novel projects where relevance is not yet established. The sample size choice was also informed by previous similar study samples size, such as the interview study of Berna et al. (Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011) which included 10 women, who were interviewed about their chronic pelvic pain experiences of mental imagery. Therefore, a sample of 10 participants was deemed sufficient for the qualitative analysis, despite unsuccessful efforts to increase the sample further.
All transcripts were double-coded by the research team (R.D.N., J.D.) to improve robustness of the analysis. The analytic process (Table 2) was shared by the authors through an iterative process of discussion and reflection, where minor discrepancies were debated and codes were revised accordingly. When high agreement was reached and consensus was achieved for the final coding, codes were categorised to then advance with the generation of themes and sub-themes.
Results
Characteristics of 10 interviewees are shown in Table 3. The sample consisted of eight females (median age = 29.5, IQR = 16) and two males (median age = 63.5, IQR = 15), with mixed diagnoses of chronic pain and pain affecting different areas of the body. However, one participant reported an unconfirmed diagnosis that was consistent with the clinical presentation and diagnostic criteria of chronic musculoskeletal pain.
Participants described a median pain duration of 10 years (IQR = 19) and a median average pain level of 6 (IQR = 4). Participants’ SHAI scores (median = 24.5, IQR = 11) are also illustrated in Table 3. Eight participants had a SHAI score above the clinical cut-off (≥18), indicating the high prevalence of health anxiety among the sample. Two participants did not cross the threshold.
Most participants (n = 7) reported experiencing images on a daily basis, whereas one individual reported weekly occurrences. Two interviewees were unable to specify the frequency of their imagery but mentioned its occurrence being linked to describing pain to others and during intense periods of pain, respectively.
Main themes
Three key themes and six sub-themes were derived from the thematic analysis, aligned with the study aims (Table 4). Themes are presented with quotations; participants are identified by pseudonym, age and gender.
‘I start to create images in my head’: pain-related mental images
Table 5 describes the characteristics of participants’ most significant imagery, including description, triggers, frequency, valence and associated meaning, emotion and behaviour. Results will be reported in line with the aims set out within the Method section.
Imagery characteristics
Participants reported intrusive, clear and vivid images rich in details, with some of them containing different sensory modalities (e.g. physical sensations: ‘Heaviness on my chest’) other than the visual component. The frequency of images varied among the sample, depending on the pain levels, especially when the pain was at its worst, up to five times a day. All images but one resembled descriptive metaphors of the pain. One participant’s imagery consisted of a future catastrophic worry linked to the causative accident that led to pain problems. In this case only, the respondent found it difficult to articulate the image, accompanied by emotional response due to the content of the depiction. Two participants also identified memories of a past traumatic experience (i.e. car accident, sexual assault) to be related to the index image. For all three respondents, the imagery sparked intrusive memories of the adverse event. Two of them additionally reported experiencing the image from an observer perspective, accompanied by a strong sense of reality: ‘And you are just playing a really [unclear] video game where your body is attempting not to die from pain’ (Jen, 25, F).
Triggers of the imagery
Participants did not experience intrusive mental imagery relating to the experience of pain prior to the onset of chronic pain. As pain was the identified root cause of participants’ difficulties, such uncontrollable images intruded into consciousness mainly because of the ‘excruciating pain’, with imagery so intrusive, they appear even during the night-time in the form of dreams. One participant also voiced concerns regarding the side-effects of medications (i.e. opioids), having a ‘big impact on the imagery’, and pointed out the Covid-19 pandemic as a negative influence on the image associated with pain: ‘Would this leave me more vulnerable?’ (Patty, 36, F).
Metaphors for pain
Language for pain
Participants described pain using violent terms, such as ‘stabbing’ and ‘impaling’, when reporting their accounts of pain experience. In such narratives, participants conceptualised the ‘unnatural’ pain sensation to both the self and those unfamiliar with their condition by employing the imagery as a metaphor for pain. As an alternative method of communication, participants better articulated the severity of their pain and their feelings towards it: ‘I’m still pretty adamant that there aren’t really any words that you can use to make somebody understand what [the pain] is like when it’s really bad’ (Louise, 31, F).
Meaning behind the image
In this sense-making process, participants struggled to expand insight into their lives with chronic pain. For some participants, the images themselves were simply a visual representation of the pain. For others, the imagery gave meaning to their experience, revolving around the self and the catastrophic projection of an impending future, as pain was interpreted as a symptom of something life-threatening to come:
‘I feel like a failure, failure as a mum … It represents my desperation to make sure I am there for her [the daughter], to be a good mum to her. And my worst fear is not being able to do that.’ (Patty, 36, F)
‘With the pain comes the image’: a companion to pain
This theme captures the presence of the images as an accompaniment to pain experience. Particularly, it focuses on the imagery as a means to represent participants’ loss of control over pain, and the role of mental imagery in exacerbating their condition.
‘It’s part of me now’: acceptance of pain
The imagery depicted a separate entity other than the self; the pain. Pain was seen as an external factor out of participants’ control, who felt incredibly disconnected from it. However, what appeared to distinguish them was their attitude towards the uncontrollability of pain. For the majority of participants, the imagery felt like a reflection of their struggle to manage pain. One participant described his pain as ‘burning flames’ so intense that they ‘overrule everything’ (Arthur, 71, M), while others projected their pain onto external agents represented with weapons assaulting the self. Many participants also used pain-related words as ‘defeating’, suggesting strong feelings of a diminished sense of control, agency and identity over the experience of pain. The perceived lack of control over the experience of pain was evident as some participants reported their inability to cope with pain by passively accepting it:
‘And then you become resigned and you go through all the stages of grief with the fact that you’re in pain every day and then you get back to work.’ (Anna, 28, F)
On the contrary, one participant reported dealing with pain by actively engaging at mind levels to achieve distraction, because ‘if I accept it, then it’s all over’:
‘I’ve got two brains. I’ve got my brain and I’ve got my pain brain … They’re just constantly in a battle with each other all the time. I can’t let it win … So, rather than pain brain winning and being miserable, I might have a bit of pain brain and try and be productive at the same time.’ (Louise, 31, F)
‘It makes it more real’: the facilitator of pain
The imagery had a negative valence for the majority of participants. In most cases, such ‘disturbing’ images exacerbated negative thoughts and emotions connected to the pain, increasing anxiety, blame, frustration, and anger, and further leading to rumination of the situation:
‘You just wish your mind wouldn’t go in certain places … And it kind of reminds you “You’re in pain, you’re in pain”.’ (Alice, 26, F)
One participant reported that the image itself had ‘more of an emotional impact’ (Patty, 36, F) rather than the pain, experiencing tightening of the chest due to the intrusiveness and content of the imagery. In contrast, for Megan, the image was a source of coping towards her pain, representative of an imaginary sense of relief to be pain-free:
‘That’s when I start punching them [the legs]… I just, like, bang them up and down on the bed thinking that’d get rid of it, but it doesn’t.’ (Megan, 61, F)
According to three respondents, however, the pain was the primary concern. The imagery did not prompt any emotional and behavioural responses: ‘I know I’m not gonna basically get a knife and put it into my stomach’ (Marie, 27, F).
Attempting to avoid and suppress anything related to the unpleasant experience of either the pain or the imagery itself, some similarities were then observed in the use of coping strategies across the sample. Participants employed safety and avoidance behaviours, including behavioural restrictions (e.g. limiting physical movements), bodily monitoring and reassurance-seeking (e.g. googling symptoms), and distractions (e.g. watching television).
This was different for Milly, who adopted Graded Motor Imagery, meditation techniques, and virtual reality to aid her recovery. She suggests that imagery itself can be a ‘powerful’ tool for reframing distressing images, thereby altering their content and facilitating improvement:
‘So, the tools, the awareness of the good things that are happening … The little steps to the recovery, recognising them, and knowing that … I am on the road to recovery and life is all good again.’ (Milly, 49, F)
Discussion
The study investigated the experience of intrusive mental imagery in the daily life of individuals with chronic pain, offering further insight into its impact on their emotions, cognitions, and behaviours components.
Findings reflect that for those in the current study, mental imagery can be an intrusive, vivid and uncontrollable cognitive accompaniment to participants’ pain experience. Findings identified three major themes: (1) ‘I start to create images in my head’: pain-related mental images; (2) metaphors for pain; and (3) ‘With the pain comes the image’: a companion to pain.
There is an overlap between pain-related visual images described here and earlier investigations of mental imagery in chronic pain sufferers. In the current study, participants described imagery-based language that fell into four of the thematic categories classified by Gosden et al. (Reference Gosden, Morris, Ferreira, Grady and Gillanders2014). Descriptions of the images illustrated an anatomical representation of damage, depicted pain as a localised object and as an external entity assaulting the self, and were related to sensory qualities of pain. One interviewee reported a projection about the future in the form of a catastrophic worry, a theme evident in the study by Philips (Reference Philips2011). Some pain-related imagery reported by participants may also be considered to have parallels with ‘flashback’ and ‘flash-forward’ images identified in PTSD and depression (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Gregory, Lipton and Burgess2010; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Crane, Fennell and Williams2007b). Although the event was not present in the imagery, the intrusive images were linked to past adverse experiences and led to intrusive memories, a cornerstone of PTSD (Brewin and Holmes, Reference Brewin and Holmes2003; Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004). In particular, the future-oriented imagery additionally resembled those centred on themes of future death in depressed patients found by Crane et al. (Reference Crane, Shah, Barnhofer and Holmes2012) and Holmes et al. (Reference Holmes, Crane, Fennell and Williams2007b). This may indicate that cognitions in chronic pain, such as mental imagery, may be correlated with those reported in PTSD and depression. Further research should investigate the association between chronic pain, PTSD, and depression.
All participants in this study defined their pain as a complex situation. As found in previous work (Gosden et al., Reference Gosden, Morris, Ferreira, Grady and Gillanders2014), the imagery was a valuable means to better conceptualise the experience of living with chronic pain by elaborating its meaning through sensory aspects and object-based representations. Participants outlined their difficulties to describe their pain to others, with the imagery serving as a metaphor to express their condition, a tool of communication and bridge to understanding. Similar results were found in an earlier study (Warren, Reference Warren2019) aiming to improve communication between patients and healthcare professionals through the use of visual representations, indicating the implication that pain metaphors may have in the diagnosis and management for pain. The communication of pain through imagery has also garnered strong interest among artists; Padfield explored the value of visual images in clinician–patient interactions and the expression of pain, aiming to investigate whether and how photographic images of pain, co-created with patients, could assist them in conveying their pain to clinicians and enhance mutual understanding (Padfield, Reference Padfield2003). The conceptualisation of pain has also been extensively studied by Moseley et al. (Reference Moseley, Butler, Beames and Giles2012), who outline strategies to enrich or ‘power up’ those useful pain metaphors to help reflect and reconceptualise pain according to a biology of pain that underpins a biopsychosocial approach (Moseley, Reference Moseley2007). In therapy, metaphorical visualisations have frequently been employed in ACT to facilitate therapeutic change. In this context, the use of metaphors aims to assist individuals in coping with chronic pain by conjuring images of their unhealthy ways of thinking to better comprehend their experiences of pain (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Liu, McCracken, Li and Ren2023).
The perceived sense of control and agency was a common theme among the sample. The imagery was representative of participants’ loss of autonomy and ability to maintain their identities in the experience of pain, depicted as an external entity attacking one’s own life and self. The majority of participants felt indeed defeated by their pain, and reported passive reactions to episodes of persistent and disabling pain rather than active coping strategies to manage it. These results may be understood according to the cognitive concept of mental defeat, previously studied in research in PTSD and depression (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Dunmore, Jaycox, Meadows and Foa1998; Gilbert and Allan, Reference Gilbert and Allan1998). In chronic pain literature, Tang et al. (Reference Tang, Salkovskis and Hanna2007) defined mental defeat as a type of catastrophising, which implies negative self-beliefs about one’s ability to effectively cope with pain. Due to the uncontrollability of pain, patients perceive a loss of autonomy that triggers such a negative view of the self in relation to pain, increasing the sense of mental defeat that then hinders people’s engagement in pain-related coping strategies; a vicious cycle. Further quantitative work on mental defeat and chronic pain has confirmed this hypothesis (Tang et al., Reference Tang, Goodchild, Hester and Salkovskis2010, Reference Tang, Shum, Leung, Chen and Salkovskis2013), and a later study illustrated that mental defeat was strongly associated with negative appraisals of the images (Philips and Samson, Reference Philips and Samson2012).
Consistent with previous research (Berna et al., Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011; Philips, Reference Philips2011), for some participants the index image represented maladaptive self-schemas (e.g. ‘I am a failure’) alongside threatening and depressing appraisals of the future, while heightening negative emotions and dysfunctional behavioural responses. These results are in line with the hypothetical model of imagery and emotion applied to chronic pain proposed by Berna et al. (Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011). The model is based on a CBT framework, whereby the distressing image can amplify pain experience. In the present study, pain was identified as the primary trigger to their imagery. Many participants described these images as enhancing the intensity of their emotions, consistent with the idea of imagery as an ‘emotional amplifier’ (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Geddes, Colom and Goodwin2008). In such cases, the imagery was often associated with negative emotions and cognitions, which led to avoidance of activities. This in turn worsened the experience of pain and increased the new occurrence of the visual image. Notably, despite its predominantly negative valence, the imagery tended to have comforting effects in one participant. Similar depictions have been categorised as coping imagery in Berna et al. (Reference Berna, Vincent, Moore, Tracey, Goodwin and Holmes2011, Reference Berna, Tracey and Holmes2012). However, far from being positive, the image entailed self-harming behaviour that closely resembled thoughts of suicidal ideation in depression, as seen in Holmes et al. (Reference Holmes, Blackwell, Burnett Heyes, Renner and Raes2016). This may suggest that the outcomes associated with pain-related visual imagery may be akin to those found in mental health conditions. Conversely, for one participant, the imagery represented an actual tool to aid pain recovery, suggesting reframing the negative image into a benign visualisation by means of imagery-based strategies. This approach resembled the imagery rescripting technique adopted in CBT (Blackwell, Reference Blackwell2021; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Arntz and Smucker2007a), aiming to enhance access to positive mental images and encourage more positive mental processes in individuals dealing with chronic pain. Similarly in EDMR protocols for chronic pain management, where the use of an ‘antidote’ imagery becomes an important tool to reduce emotional distress and alleviate chronic pain (Grant and Threlfo, Reference Grant and Threlfo2002).
Strengths and limitations
This exploratory study extends work in the under-explored area of intrusive mental imagery in chronic pain. The use of semi-structured interviews was a valuable source of enquiry to gain an in-depth understanding of participants’ experiences of pain and pain-related images compared with postal survey methods employed in previous studies (Gillanders et al., Reference Gillanders, Potter and Morris2012; Gosden et al., Reference Gosden, Morris, Ferreira, Grady and Gillanders2014). However, the assessment of appraisal using a time-limited interview design proved difficult for some participants who considered the presence and meaning of the index image for the first time. These preliminary results indicate that imagery in chronic pain is worthy of further investigation, particularly as a potential target in cognitive behavioural approaches; however, future research will need to develop more reliable and effective approaches to clarify image appraisals. Difficulties in recruitment may be linked to a lack of understanding of imagery by participants, despite a definition of pain-related visual images provided in the participant information sheet; this may be the first hurdle to overcome in future work. Further factors contributing to our challenges in recruitment might stem from individuals’ difficulty in identifying themselves as ‘imagers’. The struggle could arise from the perceived lack of vividness and clarity usually associated with such experiences. Additionally, some individuals may be deterred from discussing their experiences for an extended period, due to the unpleasant nature of the images.
Participants were recruited using a purposive approach to sampling based on a diagnosis of chronic pain and the sample consisted of a heterogeneous group with mixed diagnoses. While there were clear common themes across the sample, further work may seek to examine whether there are differences or similarities between homogenous pain groups (e.g. headache, lower back pain) perhaps through replication or quantitative approaches. The small sample size does limit transferability; however, as an exploratory study in this field, and with findings consistent with existing research, this is a firm foundation for further development.
Clinical implications
Currently available treatments for pain are only mildly effective, and recent guidelines from the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2021) recommend CBT within a collaborative model of care to improve chronic pain management. These findings have implications for the delivery and adaptation of CBT for chronic pain, which may seek to take account of and target imagery within interventions. Imagery interventions have a strong evidence base (Saulsman et al., Reference Saulsman, Ji and McEvoy2019), and the findings from this study support the relevance of imagery in cognitive, affective and behavioural processes in chronic pain. Identifying meanings of common images associated with the pain may help to provide a gateway to underlying beliefs, which can be targeted to reduce the potency of the image (Hales et al. Reference Hales, Blackwell, Di Simplicio, lyadurai, Young, Holmes, Brown and Clark2014). Imagery-based treatment techniques, such as imagery rescripting (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Arntz and Smucker2007a), can then facilitate cognitive restructuring, aiming at maladaptive core belief modification. Such imagery-based intervention may be useful to alter the content of negative images, which can be reappraised in a less distressing manner, facilitating cognitive and affective changes (Blackwell, Reference Blackwell2019).
Conclusions
The present study offers further evidence of the impact that pain-related mental imagery may have on individuals suffering from chronic pain. Such uncontrollable, intrusive and vivid images may fit a classic CBT model, as the imagery can serve as an emotional amplifier of the pain experience. Future research is needed to develop better assessment and research-informed interventions targeting such distressing images for pain management.
Key practice points
-
(1) Intrusive mental imagery can be a cognitive accompaniment to participants’ pain experience.
-
(2) Pain-related mental images can have an impact on the emotions, cognitions, and behaviours of those with chronic pain.
-
(3) It might be beneficial to address and target mental imagery in CBT assessment and treatment.
Data availability statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, J.D., upon reasonable request. The data are not publicly available due to them containing information that could compromise research participant privacy and consent.
Acknowledgements
Thanks are extended to all the participants who took part in this study, and to Feargus Mason.
Author contributions
Rita De Nicola: Data curation-Lead, Formal analysis-Lead, Investigation-Lead, Methodology-Lead, Resources-Lead, Writing – original draft-Lead, Writing – review & editing-Equal; Simon E. Blackwell: Writing – review & editing-Supporting; Edward Hirata: Formal analysis-Supporting, Investigation-Supporting, Methodology-Supporting, Resources-Supporting, Writing – review & editing-Supporting; Jo Daniels: Conceptualization-Lead, Project administration-Lead, Supervision-Lead, Writing – review & editing-Supporting.
Financial support
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
Competing interests
The authors declare none.
Ethical standards
Ethical approval was granted by the Department of Psychology, Research Ethics Committee at the University of Bath (reference number: 21-116). Any necessary informed consent to participate and for the results to be published has been obtained. The authors have abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS.
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.