The prevalence of feline obesity has rapidly increased over recent decades. One of the most recent studies has suggested that approximately 35 % of adult cats in the USA were considered either overweight or obese(Reference Lund, Armstrong and Kirk1). Obesity prevalence is influenced by numerous factors. Activity is one of the factors influenced by both individual (e.g. neuter) and environmental variables (e.g. accommodations, owner behaviours and tendencies). Decreased physical activity has been identified as a major risk factor for the development of obesity and feline diabetes(Reference Slingerland, Fazilova and Plantinga2). However, strategies to increase physical activity to reduce obesity incidence have been lacking. Increasing feeding frequency has been suggested as a contributing factor to manage body weight (BW)(Reference German, Martin, Pibot, Biourge and Elliott3). It was our hypothesis that by increasing feeding frequency, without altering the amount fed per day, more common feelings of hunger and the consequent search for food would alter activity pattern and increase overall activity level. The effect of feeding frequency on physical activity level has not been quantitatively measured in cats. Moreover, it is not clear how increased feeding frequency may affect 24 h activity patterns. The use of activity monitors allowed us to continuously monitor activity during several days in the dark and light periods without human interference, which has been demonstrated to be an accurate method of measuring physical activity in cats(Reference Belsito, Vester and Keel4, Reference Vester, Liu and Keel5). Therefore, the objective of the present study was to evaluate the effect of feeding frequency on physical activity in adult cats.
Materials and methods
Animals and diets
A total of twelve healthy adult, neutered, male domestic shorthair cats (approximately 3 years old; 4·74 (sem0·16) kg BW; 4·5–5·5 body condition score (BCS) on a 9-point scale) were used in this experiment. Cats were group-housed in the same room (26·67 m2 × 3 m) most of the day but were individually housed 4 h per day in stainless-steel cages (0·61 m × 0·61 m × 0·61 m) in the animal facility of the Edward R. Madigan Laboratory at the University of Illinois (Urbana, IL, USA). The room was environmentally controlled (20°C) with a 16 h light–8 h dark cycle. The 16 h light period lasted from 06.00 to 22.00 hours each day. Cats were allowed access to various toys and scratching poles for behavioural enrichment and to encourage play.
Cats were fed the same dry kibble diet at amounts to maintain BW and BCS throughout the study. Before the present study, cats were fed commercial or experimental diets once daily in the morning (08.00–09.00 hours). The experimental diet fed in the present study was based on maize gluten meal, brewer's rice, wheat flour and poultry by-product meal, and was formulated to contain moderate concentrations of carbohydrate, fat and protein (approximately 33 % of energy from each). The diet was formulated to meet all nutrient recommendations for adult cats as provided by the Association of American Feed Control Officials (Oxford, IN, USA)(6). Water was available ad libitum throughout the experiment. All animal procedures were approved by the University of Illinois Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee before experimentation.
Experimental design
A cross-over study design consisting of 32 d (two 16 d periods) was used. Voluntary activity levels were measured from the morning of the 8th day to the morning of the 15th day. In the first period, cats (six animals per treatment) were fed either two meals or four meals daily and vice versa for the second period. Half of the daily intake was offered to two-meal-fed cats at 08.00 and 20.00 hours. At 08.00, 12.00, 16.00 and 20.00 hours, one-quarter of the daily intake was offered to four-meal-fed cats, respectively. After the cross-over study, all twelve cats were changed back to their normal feeding pattern, during which all cats were fed once daily at 08.00 hours. Activity measurements were also performed during this time as in the previous two periods. Cats were maintained on a strict housing schedule, being individually housed in cages for access to diet from 08.00 to 09.00, 12.00 to 13.00, 16.00 to 17.00, and 20.00 to 21.00 hours each day. For the other 20 h, cats were group-housed in the room to allow for voluntary physical activity without human interference.
Physical activity level assessment
Voluntary activity levels were evaluated using Actical activity monitors (Mini Mitter, Bend, OR, USA), which were worn about the neck for seven consecutive days at the middle of each period as performed previously in research cats in our laboratory(Reference Belsito, Vester and Keel4, Reference Vester, Liu and Keel5). The monitors contain omnidirectional sensors capable of accurately incorporating both intensity and duration of movement. The Actical software analysed the data compiled by the monitor and converted it into arbitrary numbers referred to as ‘activity counts’. Activity levels were expressed as activity counts per epoch (epoch length 15 s). Values represent the mean epoch activity count over 7 d, during the selected hours (light hours, dark hours and average throughout the entire day) without human interference. Monitors were calibrated by Actical. To control for variability, cats wore the same monitor for each period of the study.
Statistical analyses
Data were analysed using the Mixed procedure of Statistical Analysis Systems 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Feeding frequency and time were tested as the main fixed effects. Random effects of cat, sequence and period were included in the model. Results are presented as means with their standard errors. A probability of P < 0·05 was considered significant.
Results
There were no differences (P>0·05) in daily average activity, activity during the light period or activity during the dark period between two-meal-fed cats and four-meal-fed cats (Table 1). Daily activity levels of all twelve cats peaked during meal times, which were all during the light period. The daily average activity level for one-meal-fed cats (15·57 (sem 2·03), activity counts/epoch (15 s)) was numerically (22·5 %) lower than cats on the multiple-meal treatments, two-meal-fed treatment (20·04 (sem 2·19), activity counts/epoch (15 s)) or four-meal-fed treatment (20·14 (sem 2·15), activity counts/epoch (15 s)). Because the once daily feeding was not included in the cross-over design, it is, however, not appropriate to analyse these differences statistically. As shown in Fig. 1, the daily activity pattern of one-meal-fed cats differed from multiple-meal-fed cats. Although activity during the dark period and 08.00 hours meal were similar for all cats, peaks of activity for one-meal-fed cats tended to occur during group-housing immediately following an hour in their individual cage rather than peaks during meal times for multiple-meal-fed cats.
Data were compiled by activity collars by Actical software. Activity levels were defined as arbitrary units referred to ‘activity counts’. Activity is expressed as activity counts per epoch (epoch length 15 s).
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to quantitatively evaluate the physical activity level of adult cats with varied feeding frequency. All cats were fed the same dry kibble diet, consistent food intakes to maintain BW and BCS, and were kept under the same daily schedule (in and out of cages) to eliminate the effects of factors other than feeding frequency. The activity monitors have been demonstrated to be an accurate method of measuring physical activity in cats(Reference Belsito, Vester and Keel4, Reference Vester, Liu and Keel5). The results showed that the daily average activity level for two-meal-fed cats was not different from four-meal-fed cats. The feeding frequency had no effects on this cross-over design. It is possible that the interactions between the cats from two different treatments attenuated the effects of feeding frequencies on the activity levels. Cats in the twice-daily-feeding group may have increased activity associated with the cats in the four times-daily-feeding group. Although it appears to be large and contains many toys, scratching posts, etc., the size of the animal room may also be a limiting factor in such a study.
Before the present study, cats were fed a commercial diet once daily at 08.00 hours. We used the same method and same housing schedule to measure the physical activity levels of all twelve cats when fed one meal daily. Due to the limitation of the study design, we feel it is not appropriate to perform statistical analysis on once daily feeding. However, it is interesting to note that the daily average activity level and the ratio of light–dark activities for one-meal-fed cats were numerically lower than the multiple-meal treatments. Slingerland et al. (Reference Slingerland, Fazilova and Plantinga2) reported that physical inactivity and indoor confinement was significantly correlated with the development of type 2 diabetes mellitus in cats. In human subjects, reducing levels of physical activity can markedly affect energy balance. A sedentary routine does not induce decreased food intake but leads to a significantly positive energy balance, most of which has been stored as fat(Reference Stubbs, Hughes and Johnstone7). Research focusing on the influence of eating patterns on BW in male subjects suggested that eating frequency was negatively correlated with BW and body fatness(Reference Drummond, Crombie and Cursiter8, Reference Ruidavets, Bongard and Bataille9). It is unknown whether eating frequency affects BW by altering physical activity levels, hormone levels, metabolism or other unknown factors associated with energy homeostasis.
In the present study, one-meal and multiple-meal treatments resulted in different daily activity patterns. By contrast, two-meal and four-meal treatments resulted in very similar daily patterns. Activity levels peaked immediately before and during meal times or at times they anticipated a meal. The daily activity level of one-meal treatment peaked at the meal time (08.00 hours) and immediately after time in individual housing (14.00, 18.00 and 22.00 hours). This change of daily activity pattern may be related to the anticipation of a meal for cats, which may be influenced by cats' memories, surrounding cats, feeding behaviours of humans, or changes in appetite-regulating hormones or metabolism of a meal in cats. A recent mouse study has demonstrated that ghrelin was an important hormone coordinating behavioural activity rhythms with the timing of food availability. It acts in the hypothalamus to stimulate locomotor activity in anticipation of scheduled meals(Reference Blum, Patterson and Khazall10). Ghrelin was not measured in the present study, but would be of interest in future studies.
In conclusion, there was no difference between two-time and four-time feeding patterns. However, multiple-meal feeding patterns may increase the activity of cats compared with one-meal feeding. Furthermore, study is needed to clarify the effects of feeding frequency on physical activity and the potential mechanisms involved.
Acknowledgements
The present study was supported by the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture, Hatch project no. ILLU-538-396. All authors state that there are no conflicts of interest. P. D. conducted the experimental work and wrote the manuscript. R. W. G. took part in the evaluation of the results. K. S. S. designed the experiment.