Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:10:35.392Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Amir A. Khan*
Affiliation:
Mental Health and Behavioral Sciences Service, Providence VA Medical Center, Providence, Rhode Island
Kristen C. Jacobson
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, USA
Charles O. Gardner
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, USA
Carol A. Prescott
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, USA
Kenneth S. Kendler
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, USA
*
Amir A. Khan, MD, Mental Health and Behavioral Sciences Service, Providence VA Medical Center, 830 Chalkstone Ave., Providence, RI 02908, USA. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

We know little about the degree to which comorbidity, so commonly seen among psychiatric disorders, arises from variation in normal personality.

Aims

To study the degree to which variation in normal personality accounts for the comorbidity of eight common psychiatric and substance use disorders.

Method

Internalising disorders (major depression, generalised anxiety and panic disorders, phobias), externalising disorders (alcohol and drug dependence, anti-social personality and conduct disorders) and personality dimensions of neuroticism, extraversion and novelty seeking were assessed in 7588 participants from a population-based twin registry. The proportion of comorbidity explained by each personality dimension was calculated using structural equation modelling.

Results

Neuroticism accounted for the highest proportion of comorbidity within internalising disorders (20–45%) and between internalising and externalising disorders (19–88%). Variation in neuroticism and novelty seeking each accounted for a modest proportion (10–12% and 7–14%, respectively) of the comorbidity within externalising disorders. Extraversion contributed negligibly

Conclusions

High neuroticism appears to be a broad vulnerability factor for comorbid psychiatric disorders. Novelty seeking is modestly important for comorbid externalising disorders.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

High comorbidity among psychiatric disorders is consistently reported (Reference Kessler, McGonagle and ZhaoKessler et al, 1994; Reference Merikangas, Angst and EatonMerikangas et al, 1996). Among many proposed explanations, one possibility is that personality mediates part of this comorbidity (Reference Jardine, Martin and HendersonJardine et al, 1984; Reference Clark, Watson and MinekaClark et al, 1994; Reference Battaglia, Przybeck and BellodiBattaglia et al, 1996; Reference Bienvenu, Brown and SamuelsBienvenu et al, 2001; Reference Krueger and MarkonKrueger & Markon, 2001). This study examines the association of variation in personality traits of neuroticism, extraversion and novelty seeking and the comorbidity among eight disorders: major depression, generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), panic disorder, any phobia, alcohol dependence, drug dependence, antisocial personality disorder and conduct disorder. This study not only attempts to replicate previous work using a large epidemiological sample, including more comprehensive diagnostic categories and different statistical methodology, but also attempts to quantify the proportion of comorbidity among psychiatric disorders explained by individual personality dimensions.

METHOD

Participants

Our sample derives from two related projects utilising the population-based Virginia Twin Registry, which was formed from a systematic review of all birth certificates in the Commonwealth of Virginia and now constitutes part of the Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry. The female-female (FF) twin pairs used in this study come from birth years 1934-1974. Twin pairs became eligible to participate if both members had responded previously to a mailed questionnaire, the response rate to which was 64%. Eighty-eight per cent of our sample were first interviewed face to face in 1987-1989 (wave 1) and subsequently have participated in up to three additional telephone interviews (waves 2-4).

The male-male and male-female (MM/MF) twin pairs, covering the birth years 1940-1974, were ascertained in a separate study beginning in 1993. We interviewed 72% of the eligible sample, usually by telephone, in our wave 1 study. This sample was followed up in a second wave of face-to-face interviews (1994-1998) that were completed with 79.4% of eligible participants.

We examine here the results of combined data from the MM/MF and FF samples, based on the second and fourth wave of interviews, respectively, because these were the most recent waves in which we had measured both personality and psychiatric diagnoses. Our sample consisted of 7588 individual twins, with 4240 males (55.9%) and 3348 females (44.1%). All participants were Caucasian, ranging in age from 20 to 58 years (mean=36.8, s.d.=8.9) at the time of the interview. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to assessment.

Measures

Psychiatric disorders

The outcome measures of interest, as outlined in the introduction, were lifetime diagnoses of common psychiatric disorders. In order to facilitate the discussion, we will use the concepts of internalising (propensity to express distress inwards, including major depression, GAD, panic disorder, any phobia) and externalising (propensity to express distress outwards, including alcohol and drug dependence, antisocial personality disorder, conduct disorder) disorders as described by Krueger et al (Reference KruegerKrueger, 1999; Reference Krueger and MarkonKrueger & Markon, 2001). With the exception of ‘any phobia’, all disorders were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (Reference Spitzer and WilliamsSpitzer & Williams, 1985). Diagnostic algorithms for GAD, panic disorder and alcohol dependence were modified to reflect DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), whereas major depression, drug dependence, antisocial personality disorder and conduct disorder were based on DSM-III-R criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) owing to the lack of items corresponding to DSM-IV criteria. The drug dependence diagnosis included dependence on marijuana, cocaine, opiates, hallucinogens, stimulants, sedatives or other drugs. Phobias were assessed with an adaptation of the phobic disorders section of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule, version III-A (Reference Robins and HelzerRobins & Helzer, 1985), and the diagnosis of ‘any phobia’ included agoraphobia, social, situational, animal, blood and miscellaneous phobias. The diagnostic algorithm for phobias has been described in detail previously (Reference Kendler, Myers and PrescottKendler et al, 2002).

Interviewers were carefully trained and supervised, and had at least a master's degree in a mental health-related field or a bachelor's degree in such a field and two years of clinical experience. Diagnoses for conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder were based on self-report questionnaires; all other diagnoses were assessed using personal interview. Inter-rater reliability for diagnosis (based on a subsample of FF twins) was high (e.g. for major depression, mean (s.d.), κ=0.96 (0.04)), and test-retest reliability (based on an average interval of 4.5 weeks, range 2-8 weeks, between base and reliability interview) was also acceptable for most diagnoses (range=0.23-0.74, average κ=0.52). Finally, the comorbidity of antisocial personality disorder and conduct disorder was not examined because the diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder requires the onset of conduct disorder before age 15 years. Table 1 describes the prevalence of psychiatric disorders in our sample.

Table 1 Prevalence of psychiatric disorders by gender

Disorder Males (n/sample N)1 Females (n/sample N)1
Major depression 29.5% 41.0%
(1252/4240) (1374/3348)
Generalised anxiety disorder 1.7% 2.8%
(71/4226) (95/3340)
Panic disorder 1.1% 3.2%
(48/4214) (108/3393)
Any phobia 22.0% 31.7%
(929/4215) (1055/3329)
Alcohol dependence 25.9% 9.8%
(1092/4213) (326/3332)
Any drug dependence 7.3% 4.3%
(309/4240) (144/3325)
Antisocial personality disorder 3.9% 0.4%
(154/3947) (12/2776)
Conduct disorder 10.1% 1.7%
(402/3962) (48/2781)

Personality

Neuroticism and extraversion, as conceptualised by Eysenck (Reference Eysenck and EysenckEysenck & Eysenck, 1975; Reference Hirschfeld, Klerman and ClaytonHirschfeld et al, 1983), have been identified cross-culturally as major personality traits by nearly all subsequent investigators (Reference PervinPervin, 1990). Neuroticism reflects emotional instability, vulnerability to stress and anxiety proneness, whereas extraversion measures sociability and liveliness. Novelty seeking, another personality dimension, measures exploratory excitability, impulsiveness, extravagance and regimentation (Reference Cloninger, Przybeck and SvrakicCloninger et al, 1991). Personality measures of neuroticism and extraversion were obtained by self-report questionnaire in the MM/MF sample and were part of the telephone interview in the FF sample. Novelty seeking was assessed by self-report questionnaire only, in both samples. Neuroticism and extraversion were assessed with 12 and 8 items, respectively, from the shortened version of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire - Revised (EPQ-R; Reference Eyesenck, Eysenck and BarrettEysenck et al, 1985; Reference Heath, Neale and KesslerHeath et al, 1992). Novelty seeking was evaluated by 18 items from the abbreviated 54-item version of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) of Cloninger (Reference Cloninger, Przybeck and SvrakicCloninger et al, 1991; Reference Heath, Cloninger and MartinHeath et al, 1994). For statistical analyses we used composite personality measures derived from individual items for each dimension, respectively.

Missing data

Valid data on all three personality measures and all eight psychiatric disorders were available for the vast majority (85.6%; n=6499) of the sample. Missing data for major depression, GAD, any phobia and alcohol and drug dependence were minimal (<0.6%). Rates of missing data for conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder were somewhat higher (approximately 7-16%) because these diagnoses were assessed using a separate self-report questionnaire. Rates of missing data for the three personality measures were 2-16%, also due primarily to lower response rates for the self-report questionnaire. Preliminary analyses revealed no significant differences in mean levels of personality or psychiatric diagnosis due to missing data on other variables (results available from the authors upon request).

Statistical analysis

We performed logistic regression analyses to estimate the association of each personality dimension with each psychiatric disorder. Correction for the correlated structure of our twin data was done using generalised estimating equations (Reference Liang and ZegerLiang & Zeger, 1986) as implemented in the Statistical Analysis System (SAS) procedure GEN-MOD. Multiple logistic regression analyses were performed with all three personality measures as independent variables. Age, zygosity and gender were used as covariates. Scores for all personality measures were standardised to a mean of 0 and a variance of 1 to facilitate the direct comparison of their effects on the disorder of interest. Odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals and their statistical significance are reported. An odds ratio of >1 represents the increase in risk of disorder associated with each standard deviation (s.d.) increase in the score of the personality dimension. An odds ratio of <1 represents the decrease in risk associated with each s.d. increase in personality dimension score.

In order to calculate the proportion of comorbidity attributed to variation in normal personality, we conducted structural equation modelling analyses using the software program Mx (Reference Neale, Boker and XieNeale et al, 1999). As depicted in Fig. 1, the model we used allowed us to calculate the total covariance (i.e. comorbidity) between the disorders of interest. This covariance was broken down into the covariance attributed to personality and the residual covariance, which represents any remaining comorbidity after removing the covariation attributable to personality. Covariance due to personality comprised both direct and indirect effects. Direct effects are the direct effects of each personality measure on each of the two disorders. In path analyses, the contribution of personality to comorbidity can be assessed by multiplying the direct effects of a given personality variable on each of the two disorders. Indirect effects are effects of personality on disorder and comorbidity that occur through correlated personality dimensions. Because the overall correlation across personality measures was low to moderate (between neuroticism and extraversion=-0.19, neuroticism and novelty seeking=0.04, extraversion and novelty seeking=0.34) indirect effects of personality are ignored when calculating the contribution to covariance of each individual personality dimension (although they are included as a separate category; see Table 3 below).

Fig. 1 Structural equation model to calculate the total and partial covariance between the personality measures of neuroticism (N), extraversion (E) and novelty seeking (NS) and the disorders of interest (e.g. major depression (disorder 1) and panic disorder (disorder 2)).

Table 2 Association of personality measures and common psychiatric disorders

Personality dimension Major depression Generalised anxiety disorder Panic disorder Any phobia Alcohol dependence Any drug dependence Antisocial personality disorder Conduct disorder
Neuroticism 1.95*** 2.30*** 1.93*** 1.62*** 1.57*** 1.64*** 1.44*** 1.26***
(1.85–2.06) (2.01–2.63) (1.61–2.31) (1.54–1.71) (1.47–1.67) (1.50–1.79) (1.26–1.65) (1.15–1.38)
Extraversion 1.07* 1.21* 0.95 0.98 1.18*** 1.24** 0.92 0.93
(1.01–1.13) (1.01–1.45) (0.77–1.18) (0.92–1.04) (1.09–1.26) (1.08–1.41) (0.77–1.09) (0.83–1.04)
Novelty seeking 1.14*** 0.92 1.10 1.08** 1.37*** 1.58*** 1.83*** 1.39***
(1.09–1.22) (0.76–1.10) (0.88–1.36) (1.03–1.16) (1.28–1.46) (1.42–1.76) (1.57–2.13) (1.26–1.54)

The structural equation models were fitted to the raw data using maximum likelihood estimation, which allowed us to use all valid data, even if some responses or observations for a given individual are missing. Psychiatric disorders were coded as binary (1=present, 0=absent); thus, data were treated as ordinal, and thresholds for each disorder were estimated using z scores that corresponded to the prevalence of the given diagnosis. These thresholds were allowed to vary by gender to accommodate gender differences in the rates of psychiatric disorders. To test for significant gender differences, we constrained the thresholds to be equal for men and women and evaluated the overall fit of the model (using Akaike's information criteria, AIC) compared with the model where thresholds were allowed to vary by gender. Models with the lowest AIC values were considered to be the best-fitting models. We also tested for gender differences in the overall pattern of covariance by constraining the parameter estimates to be the same in males and females, and comparing the pattern of covariance with a model where parameters were allowed to vary by gender. Because Mx currently lacks the capability to analyse continuous and ordinal traits simultaneously, the continuously measured personality traits were divided into categories based on the maximum number of responses possible, and thresholds corresponding to the proportions of individuals in each category were estimated. For example, scores on the neuroticism variable were in the range 0-12. Thus, we used 12 thresholds to estimate the proportion of individuals within each response category.

RESULTS

Logistic regression for the effects of personality on psychiatric disorders

Table 2 shows the odds ratios from the logistic regression analyses for each of the three personality measures. Higher scores on neuroticism significantly increased the risk for all the disorders examined. For each s.d. increase in neuroticism, the highest (130%) risk increase was for GAD and the lowest (26%) for conduct disorder. Extraversion's impact was modest overall, with no consistent pattern across internalising and externalising disorders. Specifically, one s.d. increase in extraversion was associated with a 24% increased risk for drug dependence, with a smaller increase for GAD, alcohol dependence and major depression. Novelty seeking was most strongly associated with externalising disorders (alcohol and drug dependence, antisocial personality disorder, conduct disorder), with increase in risk ranging from 37% to 83%. Inspection of covariates revealed that internalising disorders (major depression, GAD, panic disorder and any phobia) were more prevalent in females whereas externalising disorders (alcohol and drug dependence, antisocial personality disorder, conduct disorder) were more prevalent in males (Table 1). Age was positively associated with internalising disorders (i.e. older subjects reported a higher prevalence of major depression, GAD, panic disorder and any phobia) and was negatively associated with the externalising disorders (i.e. younger subjects had higher rates of alcohol and drug dependence, antisocial personality disorder and conduct disorder). Zygosity was not associated with any of the psychiatric disorders.

We also tested for interactions between gender and each of our three personality measures for each of the disorders. Out of 24 possible interactions (8 disorders×3 interactions), only the interaction between gender and neuroticism for alcohol dependence was significant (β=0.06, s.e.=0.02, Wald χ2=5.22, P<0.05). In this case, the relationship between neuroticism and alcohol dependence was stronger for females than for males. However, it should be noted that this significant interaction may be a stochastic effect. Thus, for the structural equation modelling analyses of personality and comorbidity, males and females were combined into a single sample, although thresholds corresponding to psychiatric disorder were estimated separately for males and females.

Structural equation modelling of personality effects on comorbidity

For ease of interpretation, the results of the structural equation modelling analyses are depicted graphically in Fig. 2. The height of the bar represents the total phenotypic comorbidity of any two given disorders, and the differently shaded segments depict the direct covariance accounted for by each individual personality dimension, as well as any indirect effects, and the residual covariance. For example, the comorbidity (phenotypic correlation) between major depression and GAD is 0.41. Neuroticism accounts for the 0.16 of this comorbidity whereas the remaining comorbidity (0.25) was residual covariance. Extraversion, novelty seeking and indirect effects accounted for negligible (and negative) covariance. In order to facilitate the description, results from these analyses have been presented also as percentages of the total comorbidity (Table 3). Thus, in the case of comorbidity between major depression and GAD, Table 3 shows that 0.41 is total comorbidity. Neuroticism accounts for 39% of this comorbidity, with the remaining comorbidity due primarily to residual covariance (61%).

Fig. 2 Covariance between personality measures and comorbid psychiatric disorders. Bars represent total comorbidity and different segments provide a visual representation of the covariance of different personality measures, as well as residual covariance and indirect covariance. AD, alcohol dependence; AP, any phobia; APD, antisocial personality disorder; CD, conduct disorder; DD, drug dependence; GAD, generalised anxiety disorder; MD, major depression; PAN, panic disorder.

Table 3 Covariance between personality measures and comorbid psychiatric disorders, represented as percentage of total covariance (r tot)

Variable Major depression Generalised anxiety disorder Panic disorder Any phobia Alcohol dependence Drug dependence
r tot N(%) E(%) NS(%) r res(%) IND(%) r tot N(%) E(%) NS(%) r res(%) IND(%) r tot N(%) E(%) NS(%) r res(%) IND(%) r tot N(%) E(%) NS(%) r res(%) IND(%) r tot N(%) E(%) NS(%) r res(%) IND(%) r tot N(%) E(%) NS(%) r res(%) IND(%)
Generalised 0.41 39.0 0.9 -0.8 63.1 -2.2
anxiety
disorder
Panic disorder 0.49 20.9 0 0.1 79.1 0 0.49 21.9 -0.2 -0.2 79.5 -1.0
Any phobia 0.30 39.1 -0.1 0.7 60.2 0.1 0.28 44.9 -0.5 -1.0 58.0 -1.4 0.42 20.0 0 0.1 79.6 0.3
Alcohol 0.34 30.6 0.8 2.8 65.9 0 0.13 88.1 7.4 -7.5 16.2 -4.2 0.27 27.8 -0.4 0.7 72.6 0.7 0.25 33.5 -0.4 3.2 64.1 0
dependence
Any drug 0.40 26.0 0.8 3.0 70.2 0 0.18 61.2 4.9 -6.6 41.7 -1.2 0.21 35.0 -0.5 1.3 65.0 -0.8 0.24 35.0 -0.5 4.2 61.6 0 0.59 12.2 1.1 7.4 78.0 1.3
dependence
Antisocial 0.29 23.9 -0.3 5.8 67.4 3.2 0.19 39.5 -1.4 -10.0 62.2 7.7 0.19 25.5 0.1 1.3 71.0 2.1 0.19 28.9 0.1 7.1 61.6 2.3 0.46 11.4 -0.5 12.1 74.8 2.2 0.52 10.0 -0.4 14.0 74.5 6.0
personality
disorder
Conduct 0.19 26.3 -0.6 5.4 65.4 3.5 0.09 56.9 -4.9 -13.2 48.6 12.6 0.18 19.5 0.2 1.2 77.1 2.0 0.13 30.4 0.3 6.5 59.9 2.9 0.29 12.5 -1.0 12.5 74.0 2.0 0.34 10.7 -0.9 13.6 74.9 1.7
disorder

The overall pattern of results, as shown in Fig. 2 and Table 3, indicates that neuroticism accounts for the highest proportion of comorbidity within internalising disorders (20-45%, arithmetic average=31%) and between internalising and externalising disorders (19-88%, arithmetic average=36.8%). Neuroticism also explained 10-12% of the comorbidity within externalising disorders. Extraversion explained only a very small proportion of the comorbidity (-4.9 to 7.4%). Novelty seeking accounted for a negligible proportion of comorbidity within internalising disorders (-0.8 to 0.7%) and between internalising and externalising disorders (-13.2% to 5.8%); however, novelty seeking did account for 7.4-14% of the comorbidity within externalising disorders. Residual covariance (i.e. due to factors other than personality) accounted for most of the comorbidity, with an arithmetic average of 65%. Negative values in Fig. 2 and Table 3 reflect the effects of low extraversion (introversion) and low novelty seeking on comorbidity, although the majority of these effects are quite small.

Although the models where thresholds for psychiatric disorders were allowed to vary by gender consistently fit the data better than models assuming equal thresholds, there were no significant gender differences in the covariance structure (results available from the authors upon request). Thus, the pattern of comorbidity accounted for by personality was similar in males and females, despite the significant differences in the rates of psychiatric disorders.

DISCUSSION

Neuroticism

Our results suggest that normal personality dimensions of neuroticism not only contributed to individual diagnoses but also accounted for a significant part of the lifetime comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. The most striking finding was that neuroticism, on average, accounted for 26% of the comorbidity among the disorders included in the study (range=12-88%). This finding is consistent with previous research (Reference Clark, Watson and MinekaClark et al, 1994; Reference Sher and TrullSher & Trull, 1994, Reference Krueger and MarkonKrueger & Markon, 2001; Reference Bienvenu, Brown and SamuelsBienvenu et al, 2001) and suggests neuroticism as a potential general underlying vulnerability factor for psychopathology.

Extraversion

Although extraversion was significantly, albeit weakly, associated with four of the eight psychiatric disorders in the logistic regressions, it explained very small proportions of comorbidity. This pattern of weak effects of extraversion on psychiatric disorders and comorbidity is inconsistent with previous research (Reference Sher and TrullSher & Trull, 1994) and probably stems from the restrictive definition of our extraversion scale, which only reflects sociability. Eysenck revised the extraversion scale in the EPQ-R and items that measured impulsivity were largely moved to the psychoticism scale (Reference NyborgNyborg, 1997).

Novelty seeking

High novelty seeking increased the risk for externalising disorders significantly (Table 2) when these disorders were examined individually. Novelty seeking also accounted for the largest proportion of comorbidity between externalising disorders (7-14%, arithmetic average=11.9%). Not surprisingly, novelty seeking was unrelated to the comorbidity within internalising disorders and, for the most part, between internalising and externalising disorders. However, somewhat surprisingly, the contribution of neuroticism to the comorbidity within externalising disorders was comparable with the effects of novelty seeking.

These results further support the existence of broader, underlying dimensions of core psychopathological processes. Neuroticism appears to be a robust underlying dimension not only for the comorbidity within internalising disorders but also between internalising and externalising disorders and within externalising disorders. This leads us to reconsider the issue of psychiatric classification and an age-old question of splitting neurosis (Reference TyrerTyrer, 1985). Our previous research has indicated that the comorbidity between major depression and GAD and, to some extent, between major depression and alcohol dependence largely results from common genetic factors (Kendler et al, Reference Kendler, Neale and Kessler1992, Reference Kendler, Heath and Neale1993a ) with notable gender differences (Reference Prescott, Aggen and KendlerPrescott et al, 2000). In a previous report, we also found that over 50% of the genetic liability for major depression was shared with neuroticism (Reference Kendler, Neale and KesslerKendler et al, 1993b ). Thus, the possibility of common genetic liability between personality and comorbid disorders appears to be a reasonable hypothesis and will be the subject of future investigation.

Limitations

The results of this study should be interpreted in the context of four potential methodological limitations.

First, we used scales of neuroticism and extraversion from the EPQ-R and novelty seeking from the TPQ. Although neuroticism and extraversion represent widely accepted higher dimensions of personality, there is no agreement about the lower-order dimensions among different personality researchers. Moreover, some would argue that these two scales provide an incomplete description of the structure of heritable personality differences (Reference Heath, Cloninger and MartinHeath et al, 1994). How much more of the covariation among disorders would have been explained if we used the complete EPQ-R (neuroticism, extraversion, psychoticism and lie scale) or the complete TPQ (novelty seeking, harm avoidance and reward dependence) is speculative. Similarly, although interrater agreement for diagnosis was high, test-retest reliability for some of the lower-prevalence disorders (i.e. GAD, panic disorder and antisocial personality disorder) was low (0.23-0.42). This lower reliability may have increased the variance due to random errors of measurement, lowering the strength of associations of comorbidity with personality.

Second, the cross-sectional nature of the data made it difficult to establish causality and had a potential to confound state, trait and scar effects. However, the use of lifetime diagnosis provided some assurance that the confounding effects were likely to be minimal.

Third, because of some relatively young individuals in our sample, the risk period for certain psychiatric disorders was not over. As a result, true prevalence may be underestimated in the present sample, with concomitant effects on covariance.

Fourth, the sample was limited to Caucasian individuals so the results may not be generalisable to other ethnic groups.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Comorbidity among psychiatric disorders is a common and consistently reported finding.

  2. The normal personality dimension of neuroticism appears to be a broad vulnerability factor for the comorbid psychiatric disorders.

  3. Novelty seeking is modestly important for the comorbidity between externalising disorders only.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The normal personality dimensions used were from two different scales.

  2. The cross-sectional nature of the data used has a potential to confound state, trait and scar effects.

  3. The sample was limited to Caucasian individuals so the results might not be generalisable to other ethnic groups.

Acknowledgements

Supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants T32/MH-20030, MH-40828, MH/AA/DA-49492, DA-11287 and AA-09095. The authors thank Dr Steve. Aggen for his help in statistical analysis. We also acknowledge the. contribution of the Virginia Twin Registry, now part of the Mid-Atlantic Twin. Registry (MATR), to the ascertainment of subjects for this study. The MATR, directed by Drs L. Corey and L. Eaves, has received support from the National. Institutes of Health, the Carman Trust and the W. M. Keck, John Templeton and. Robert Wood Johnson Foundations.

Footnotes

See editorial, pp. 182-184, this issue.

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

American Psychiatrie Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM–III–R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Battaglia, M., Przybeck, T. R., Bellodi, L., et al (1996) Temperament dimensions explain the comorbidity of psychiatric disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 292298.Google Scholar
Bienvenu, O. J., Brown, C., Samuels, J. F., et al (2001) Normal personality traits and comorbidity among phobic, panic and major depressive disorders. Psychiatry Research, 102, 7385.Google Scholar
Clark, L. A., Watson, D. & Mineka, S. (1994) Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 103116.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R. & Svrakic, D. M. (1991) The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: US normative data. Psychological Reports, 69, 10471057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, S. B. G. & Eysenck, H. J. (1975) Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Junior and Adult). London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Eyesenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J. & Barrett, P. (1985) A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 229.Google Scholar
Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., et al (1992) Evidence for genetic influences on personality from self-reports and informant ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 8596.Google Scholar
Heath, A. C., Cloninger, C. R. & Martin, N. G. (1994) Testing a model for the genetic structure of personality: a comparison of the personality systems of Cloninger and Eysenck. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 762775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschfeld, R. M., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., et al (1983) Assessing personality: effects of the depressive state on trait measurement. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 695699.Google Scholar
Jardine, R., Martin, N. G. & Henderson, A. S. (1984) Genetic covariation between neuroticism and the symptoms of anxiety and depression. Genetic Epidemiology, 1, 89107.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., et al (1992) Major depression and generalized anxiety disorder. Same genes, (partly) different environments? Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 716722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., et al (1993a) Alcoholism and major depression in women. A twin study of the causes of comorbidity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 690698.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S. Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., et al (1993b) A longitudinal twin study of personality and major depression in women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 853862.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Myers, J. & Prescott, C. A. (2002) The etiology of phobias: an evaluation of the stress–diathesis model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 242248.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., et al (1994) Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM–III–R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 819.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F. (1999) The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 921926.Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F. & Markon, K. E. (2001) The higherorder structure of common DSM mental disorders: internalization, externalization, and their connections to personality. Genetic and environmental relationships between normal and abnormal personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 12451259.Google Scholar
Liang, K. Y. & Zeger, S. L. (1986) Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73, 1322.Google Scholar
Merikangas, K. R., Angst, J., Eaton, W., et al (1996) Comorbidity and boundaries of affective disorders with anxiety disorders and substance misuse: results of an international task force. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168 (suppl. 30), 5867.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., et al (1999) Mx: Statistical Modeling (5th edn). Richmond, VA: Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics.Google Scholar
Nyborg, H. (1997) The Scientific Study of Human Nature: Tribute to Hans J. Eysenck at Eighty (lst edn). New York: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Pervin, L. A. (1990) Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Prescott, C. A., Aggen, S. H. & Kendler, K. S. (2000) Sex-specific genetic influences on the comorbidity of alcoholism and major depression in a population-based sample of US twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 803811.Google Scholar
Robins, L. N. & Helzer, J. E. (1985) Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS). Version III–A. St Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J. & Trull, T. J. (1994) Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 92102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R. L. & Williams, J. B. W. (1985) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–III–R (SCID). New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Tyrer, P. (1985) Neurosis divisible? Lancet, i, 685688.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Prevalence of psychiatric disorders by gender

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Structural equation model to calculate the total and partial covariance between the personality measures of neuroticism (N), extraversion (E) and novelty seeking (NS) and the disorders of interest (e.g. major depression (disorder 1) and panic disorder (disorder 2)).

Figure 2

Table 2 Association of personality measures and common psychiatric disorders

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Covariance between personality measures and comorbid psychiatric disorders. Bars represent total comorbidity and different segments provide a visual representation of the covariance of different personality measures, as well as residual covariance and indirect covariance. AD, alcohol dependence; AP, any phobia; APD, antisocial personality disorder; CD, conduct disorder; DD, drug dependence; GAD, generalised anxiety disorder; MD, major depression; PAN, panic disorder.

Figure 4

Table 3 Covariance between personality measures and comorbid psychiatric disorders, represented as percentage of total covariance (rtot)

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.