Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T21:19:32.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of between-person differences and within-person changes in symptoms of anxiety and depression on older age cognitive performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 October 2017

E. J. Laukka*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences, and Society (NVS), Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet and Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
D. Dykiert
Affiliation:
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
M. Allerhand
Affiliation:
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
J. M. Starr
Affiliation:
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK Geriatric Medicine Unit, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
I. J. Deary
Affiliation:
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
*
Author for correspondence: E. J. Laukka, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Anxiety and depression are both important correlates of cognitive function. However, longitudinal studies investigating how they covary with cognition within the same individual are scarce. We aimed to simultaneously estimate associations of between-person differences and within-person variability in anxiety and depression with cognitive performance in a sample of non-demented older people.

Methods

Participants in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 study, a population-based narrow-age sample (mean age at wave 1 = 79 years, n = 535), were examined on five occasions across 13 years. Anxiety and depression were measured with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) and cognitive performance was assessed with tests of reasoning, logical memory, and letter fluency. Data were analyzed using two-level linear mixed-effects models with within-person centering.

Results

Divergent patterns were observed for anxiety and depression. For anxiety, between-person differences were more influential; people who scored higher on HADS anxiety relative to other same-aged individuals demonstrated poorer cognitive performance on average. For depression, on the other hand, time-varying within-person differences were more important; scoring higher than usual on HADS depression was associated with poorer cognitive performance relative to the average level for that participant. Adjusting for gender, childhood mental ability, emotional stability, and disease burden attenuated these associations.

Conclusions

The results from this study highlight the importance of addressing both between- and within-person effects of negative mood and suggest that anxiety and depression affect cognitive function in different ways. The current findings have implications for assessment and treatment of older age cognitive deficits.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

General cognitive ability is a stable trait with high intraindividual correlations across the life span. Although some aspects of cognitive function generally decline with age, there are large individual differences in older age cognitive performance. This variability can be due to differences that were present during most of life (Deary et al. Reference Deary, Pattie and Starr2013; Deary & Brett, Reference Deary and Brett2015), or to differences in the rate of age-related decline (de Frias et al. Reference de Frias, Lövdén, Lindenberger and Nilsson2007; Small et al. Reference Small, Dixon and McArdle2011). Furthermore, cognitive performance may vary within the same individual, from one occasion to another, for reasons other than age. These fluctuations may be partly due to random factors; however, previous research has shown that they can be explained and thus systematically investigated (Nesselroade & Salthouse, Reference Nesselroade and Salthouse2004; Sliwinski et al. Reference Sliwinski, Smyth, Hofer and Stawski2006). Several factors have been associated with this intraindividual variability, such as motivation, well-being, stress, and positive and negative affect (Sliwinski et al. Reference Sliwinski, Smyth, Hofer and Stawski2006; Brose et al. Reference Brose, Schmiedek, Lövdén, Molenaar and Lindenberger2010, Reference Brose, Schmiedek, Lövdén and Lindenberger2012, Reference Brose, Lövdén and Schmiedek2014; Allerhand et al. Reference Allerhand, Gale and Deary2014). To achieve fuller understanding of older adults’ cognitive performance, both between-person differences and within-person variability need to be taken into account.

Depression, even in its mildest forms, has been associated with lower levels of cognitive function. People with clinical depression (Pantzar et al. Reference Pantzar, Laukka, Atti, Fastbom, Fratiglioni and Bäckman2014), depressive symptoms (Dotson et al. Reference Dotson, Resnick and Zonderman2008), or experimentally induced low mood (Seibert & Ellis, Reference Seibert and Ellis1991) on average perform worse on cognitive tests compared to individuals free of depression. A range of domains, including memory (Dotson et al. Reference Dotson, Resnick and Zonderman2008), perceptual speed (Bielak et al. Reference Bielak, Gerstorf, Kiely, Anstey and Luszcz2011), and verbal fluency (Freiheit et al. Reference Freiheit, Hogan, Eliasziw, Patten, Demchuk, Faris, Anderson, Galbraith, Parboosingh, Ghali, Knudtson and Maxwell2012) are affected, and having more depressive symptoms have been associated with both lower performance and faster cognitive decline in older adults. Studies on the effects of anxiety on older age cognitive performance have produced more mixed results. Cross-sectionally, anxiety has been related to poorer performance, whereas few significant associations with the rate of cognitive decline have been observed (Bierman et al. Reference Bierman, Comijs, Rijmen, Jonker and Beekman2008; Beaudreau & O'Hara, Reference Beaudreau and O'Hara2009; Bunce et al. Reference Bunce, Batterham, Mackinnon and Christensen2012; de Bruijn et al. Reference de Bruijn, Direk, Mirza, Hofman, Koudstaal, Tiemeier and Ikram2014).

Symptoms of anxiety and depression show large degrees of stability across the lifespan, mainly due to genetic influences (Johnson et al. Reference Johnson, McGue, Gaist, Vaupel and Christensen2002; Nivard et al. Reference Nivard, Dolan, Kendler, Kan, Willemsen, van Beijsterveldt, Lindauer, van Beek, Geels, Bartels, Middeldorp and Boomsma2015), but may vary across adulthood due to, for example, environmental factors, positive and negative life events, and protective and risk factors accumulating over time (Rosenström et al. Reference Rosenström, Jokela, Hintsanen, Pulkki-Råback, Hutri-Kähönen and Keltikangas-Järvinen2013). Longitudinal studies offer the possibility of assessing not only between-person differences but also whether two variables are coupled together and covary within the same individual. However, most previous studies have limited their investigations to between-person relationships, and findings from studies investigating correlated change in depressive symptoms and cognition have not been consistent (van den Kommer et al. Reference van den Kommer, Comijs, Aartsen, Huisman, Deeg and Beekman2013; Brailean et al. Reference Brailean, Aartsen, Muniz-Terrera, Prince, Prina, Comijs, Huisman and Beekman2017). Separating between-person and within-person effects provides important complementary information as they may sometimes give different or even opposite patterns of results (Curran & Bauer, Reference Curran and Bauer2011; Thorvaldsson et al. Reference Thorvaldsson, Skoog, Hofer, Börjesson-Hanson, Östling, Sacuiu and Johansson2012).

Here, we aimed to explore the influences of time-invariant between-person differences and time-varying within-person changes in negative affect (anxiety and depression) on cognitive performance in a sample of non-demented older adults. At the between-person level, we estimate the effects of having a higher level of anxiety or depression compared to other individuals of the same age. At the within-person level, we address the effects of an individual deviating from their average level of depression or anxiety. Further, we explore the moderating role of known correlates to cognitive performance, anxiety, and depression on these associations.

Methods

Participants

Data were collected from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 (LBC1921) sample. Recruitment and data collection in this study have been described in detail elsewhere (Deary et al. Reference Deary, Whiteman, Starr, Whalley and Fox2004, Reference Deary, Gow, Pattie and Starr2012). In brief, LBC1921 follows up older people resident in Edinburgh or the Lothians, Scotland, who were born in 1921. At age 11, most participants were tested with a general intelligence test as part of the Scottish Mental Survey of 1932 (Scottish Council for Research in education, 1933). Participants were recruited for a follow-up study in 1999–2001 at a mean age of about 79 years. The 550 participants tested at wave 1 were later invited back for follow-up assessments at a mean age of 83 (wave 2, n = 321), 87 (wave 3, n = 235), 90 (wave 4, n = 129), and 92 (wave 5, n = 59) years. Reasons for attrition were death (n = 208), severe illness (n = 129), refusal (n = 75), loss of contact (n = 16), or other (n = 63). Each assessment involved an interview, cognitive testing, a physical examination, and self-report questionnaires. Ethics permissions were obtained from the Lothian (waves 1–3) and the Scotland A (waves 4 and 5) Research Ethics Committees. Informed consent was collected from all participants and the ethical guidelines from the Declaration of Helsinki were followed.

For the present analyses, we excluded participants who had a self-reported history of dementia or a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE: Folstein et al. Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) score < 24 (n = 13), or missing data on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS: Zigmond & Snaith, Reference Zigmond and Snaith1983, n = 2) at wave 1, resulting in a sample of 535 persons. For the follow-up assessments, we excluded participants with missing data on HADS; participants with a record of dementia history or low MMSE score were excluded for that and all subsequent waves. After these exclusions, follow-up data were available for 302 participants at wave 2, 187 participants at wave 3, 111 participants at wave 4, and 50 participants at wave 5; 233 participants contributed with data only at wave 1, whereas 115 participants had data for two waves, 76 for three, 61 for four, and 50 for all five waves. Mean follow-up time between waves was 4.32, 3.25, 3.46, and 2.01 years, respectively.

Measures

Cognitive abilities

Childhood mental ability was assessed at age 11 with a modified version of the Moray House Test No. 12. This is a test of general intelligence that was validated against the Terman–Merill revision of the Binet scales (Scottish Council for Research in Education, 1933) and scaled to provide IQ-type scores.

At each wave of the LBC1921 study, participants completed a battery of cognitive tests. Here, we report on the tests that were administered at wave 1 and all four subsequent waves.

Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven et al. Reference Raven, Court and Raven1977) was used to assess non-verbal reasoning ability. For this 60-item test, participants were asked to choose the correct item from a set of alternatives, to complete an incomplete pattern. The score used was number of correctly completed patterns within the 20 min time limit.

The Logical Memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1987) measures verbal declarative memory. Participants had two short stories read out loud to them. Immediately after each reading, and again after a minimum of 30 min delay, they were asked to tell the interviewer as much as they could remember from each story. The score used was total number of correctly recalled memory elements from immediate and delayed recall for the two stories combined.

Three Letter fluency (Lezak, Reference Lezak2004) tasks were included as measures of verbal fluency ability. Here, participants were asked to generate as many words as possible beginning with the letters C, F, and L, with a time limit of 1 min for each letter. The score used was total number of words generated across the three tasks.

Symptoms of anxiety and depression

HADS was developed by Zigmond & Snaith (Reference Zigmond and Snaith1983) to identify cases of anxiety and depression among patients in non-psychiatric hospital clinics. This self-assessment questionnaire includes 14 items, seven for anxiety and seven for depression, rated on a scale from 0 to 3. Participants are instructed to underline the alternative that best reflects how they have been feeling in the past week. Anxiety and depression are generally considered separately and maximum score on each subscale is 21. A score of 0–7 is considered normal; 8–10 is considered to be an indication of mild anxiety/depression; whereas scores ⩾11 indicate anxiety/depression. Both HADS subscales show good internal consistency and concurrent validity (Bjelland et al. Reference Bjelland, Dahl, Haug and Neckelmann2002). In LBC1921, HADS was administered at waves 1, 3, 4, and 5.

Emotional stability

Personality was measured at wave 1 using the International Personality Item Pool Big-Five 50-item inventory (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg1992). This scale has 10 items for each of the Big-Five personality factors and has previously been validated in LBC1921 (Gow et al. Reference Gow, Whiteman, Pattie and Deary2005). Emotional stability represents the same trait as neuroticism but is named and scored from the opposite end of the continuum, with higher scores representing more emotional stability.

Disease burden

A medical history, including diagnoses of cancer, cerebrovascular, and cardiovascular diseases, was taken as a part of a standardized interview at wave 1. Disease burden was summarized as number of diseases.

Statistical analyses

We used two-level linear mixed-effects models with within-person centering (Hoffman & Stawski, Reference Hoffman and Stawski2009; Allerhand et al. Reference Allerhand, Gale and Deary2014), which allow the separation of time-varying within-person effects from time-invariant between-person effects of anxiety and depression upon cognitive function.

Initially, six mixed-effects models with random intercept were fitted: models of reasoning, memory, and fluency each predicted by either anxiety or depression scores. These basic models were unadjusted except for age (centered on 79 years, being the mean age at wave 1). In each model, the time-varying predictor (anxiety or depression) was replaced by two independent variables derived from it: a within-person centered (WP) variable and a person mean (PM) variable. The WP variable was entered as a time-varying (level-1) variable. The PM variable was centered on its mean, and entered as a time-invariant (level-2) variable in the equation for the random intercept (the PM main effect). The models included their cross-level interaction. The models were fitted by R function lmer (package lme4). Residuals were assessed graphically for normality and judged to be acceptable. Following this, four covariates were added to each model: gender, age 11 IQ, emotional stability, and number of diseases. These were all centered on their means, except gender which was coded such that 0 reflects male and 1 reflects female gender. The covariates were first added individually to assess the effect of each by itself. The final, fully adjusted models included the main effects of the within- and between-person centered anxiety or depression variables and their interactions with each other and with age, and the main effects of the four covariates and their interactions with the within- and between-person centered variables.

Results

Characteristics of the study sample according to wave are shown in Table 1. Levels of anxiety and depression in this sample were generally low. Of the 121 individuals (23%) who were above the frequently used cut off for anxiety (⩾8) at wave 1, 92 (76%) had mild anxiety and 29 (24%) moderate-to-severe anxiety. Of the 34 persons (6%) above the cut off for depression (⩾8), 29 (85%) had mild and only five (15%) moderate-to-severe depression. At waves 1 and 3, participants tended to score higher on anxiety than on depression (p < 0.05), whereas these scores were not significantly different at waves 4–5. This could be due to higher dropout rates among persons scoring high on anxiety at wave 1 or increasing depression scores with increasing age. For characteristics of those who stayed in the study for the entire follow-up (i.e. completers; n = 50), see online Supplementary Table S1. Completers had significantly fewer diseases and better cognitive performance at wave 1 compared to non-completers (p < 0.05).

Table 1. Sample characteristics according to wave

s.d., standard deviation; HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.

a Number of participants with data on Raven's were 528 at wave 1, 298 at wave 2, 183 at wave 3, 105 at wave 4, and 46 at wave 5.

b Number of participants with data on Logical memory were 535 at wave 1, 301 at wave 2, 187 at wave 3, 111 at wave 4, and 50 at wave 5.

c Number of participants with data on Letter fluency were 532 at wave 1, 301 at wave 2, 187 at wave 3, 111 at wave 4, and 50 at wave 5.

Figure 1 presents an overview of HADS scores across waves for the total sample and for completers. HADS anxiety scores assessed at wave 1 were highly correlated with anxiety scores at subsequent waves (total sample: wave 3: 0.57, wave 4: 0.69, wave 5: 0.70; completers: wave 3: 0.61, wave 4: 0.68, wave 5: 0.70, p < 0.001). HADS depression scores at wave 1 showed somewhat weaker correlations with subsequent depression scores (total sample: wave 3: 0.43, wave 4: 0.39, wave 5: 0.53; completers: wave 3: 0.67, wave 4: 0.50, wave 5: 0.53, p < 0.001). Again, this might be partly due to increasing depression scores with each wave, but it could also indicate greater variability for the depression scores. Formally comparing the correlations, however, they only differed significantly for wave 4 (p < 0.01, total sample).

Fig. 1. Levels of anxiety and depression across waves for the total sample (a; n = 535) and for individuals who stayed in the study for the entire follow-up period (b; n = 50).

We calculated the within-person mean and the within-person standard deviation (s.d.) across all four waves for HADS anxiety (mean = 5.10, s.d. = 1.61) and HADS depression (mean = 3.70, s.d. = 1.46). Correlations among the covariates and within-person mean and s.d. for anxiety and depression are shown in Table 2. Having a higher score on HADS anxiety was related to lower age 11 IQ, less emotional stability, and a larger number of diseases; women tended to score higher on HADS anxiety. HADS depression was negatively associated with emotional stability. There was a positive association between anxiety and depression, so that persons scoring higher on HADS anxiety tended to score higher on HADS depression, and vice versa. Higher variability in anxiety was related to less emotional stability; similarly, higher variability in depression was related to less emotional stability and having more diseases. Furthermore, higher levels (mean) of anxiety or depression were related to more variability (s.d.).

Table 2. Correlations among covariates, mean anxiety and depression, and variability in anxiety and depression across waves

s.d., standard deviation; HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.

a Associations with gender were analyzed using point-biserial correlation.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

The results from the main analyses are shown in Table 3. Here we report the associations between the outcome (Raven's, Logical memory, or Letter fluency) and the focal predictor (anxiety or depression), represented by the WP and PM variables, their interactions with each other, and their interactions with age. The WP effect reflects the difference from a participant's usual cognitive score per within-person s.d. increase in anxiety/depression relative to the average level for that participant. In other words, it tells us how much within-person changes in mood affect the person's cognitive performance. The PM effect, on the other hand, is a between-person effect. It reflects the difference in cognitive score between people whose mean level of anxiety/depression differs by 1 s.d.

Table 3. Associations between HADS anxiety and depression scores and performance on cognitive tests (fixed effects)

HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.

Estimates are in standard deviation (s.d.) units, with standard errors in brackets. WP is the effect of the average person deviating 1 s.d. unit from their own within-person mean score on the focal predictor (anxiety or depression). PM is the effect of the average individual scoring 1 s.d. unit higher on the person-mean variable of the focal predictor (anxiety or depression). Estimates labeled a x b are interactions. The fully adjusted model controls for gender, age11 IQ, emotional stability, and number of diseases. All WP x covariate and PM x covariate interactions were included in the models, but are not included in the table. Significance was based on standard errors.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Different patterns were observed for anxiety and depression. Specifically, for anxiety, PM effects appeared to be more important, whereas for depression, it was mainly the WP effect that was linked to cognitive outcomes. The cross-level interaction between WP and PM was non-significant in all models except one, suggesting that the effect of within-person changes in anxiety/depression was independent of the person's level of anxiety/depression, and vice versa.

Let us consider PM (i.e. between-person) effects first. For anxiety, people who scored higher on HADS on average had poorer cognitive performance. After accounting for age and the WP effect, scoring 1 s.d. higher on HADS anxiety compared to the population mean was associated with 0.21 s.d. lower performance on Raven's, 0.09 s.d. lower performance on Logical memory and 0.10 s.d. lower performance on Letter fluency. The association was statistically significant for Raven's (p < 0.001) and Letter fluency (p < 0.05), whereas it was marginally significant for Logical memory (p = 0.05, basic model). Controlling for the covariates weakened these associations, and in the fully adjusted models, they were no longer significant. For a full report on all models, including the effect of each covariate, see online Supplementary Tables S2–S4. PM effects of depression were not significant for any of the cognitive outcomes in basic models.

Turning now to the WP (i.e. within-person) effects, having a higher than usual depression score on a testing occasion, was associated with poorer cognitive performance on that occasion. After adjusting for age and PM effects, the WP effect was significant for Logical memory (p < 0.05) and Letter fluency (p < 0.01), and at a non-significant trend level for Raven's (p = 0.07, basic model). For Logical memory, the association was no longer significant after controlling for covariates, whereas the association remained significant in the fully adjusted model for Letter fluency. For this task, there was also a significant effect of between-person difference in depression symptoms, suggesting that PM and WP effects of depression both gave independent contributions to performance on Letter fluency. The between-person effect only appeared after adding gender as covariate. In the fully adjusted model, a person scoring 1 s.d. higher than the group mean on HADS depression was performing on average 0.18 s.d. poorer on Letter fluency. Furthermore, scoring 1 s.d. higher on HADS depression relative to that individual's usual level was associated with 0.23 s.d. lower performance on Letter fluency relative to that person's mean level. No WP anxiety effects were significant for any of the cognitive outcomes.

For every year increase in age, participants showed significant decline on Raven's and Letter fluency, but not on Logical memory. There were only a few cases in which the interactions between age and the WP and PM variables were significant, suggesting that these effects were largely age-invariant. In all cases where an interaction was observed, the interaction suggested that the negative WP or PM effect weakened with age.

Effects of single covariates are shown in online Supplementary Tables S2–S4. Females showed poorer performance on Raven's, age 11 IQ was positively associated with all cognitive outcomes, emotional stability was positively associated with all cognitive outcomes in the models for depression, and number of diseases was negatively associated with performance on Logical memory and Letter fluency. There were some significant interactions between the WP and PM variables and the covariates. Scoring higher on emotional stability was associated with a larger negative WP effect in the depression model for Logical memory, although this interaction effect was not significant in the fully adjusted model. Having more diseases at wave 1 was associated with a smaller negative PM effect in the depression model for Letter fluency. In contrast, higher disease burden was associated with a smaller positive WP effect in the anxiety model and a larger negative WP effect in the depression model for Raven's.

Discussion

The results of this study show that both anxiety and depression are negatively associated with cognitive performance, which is in agreement with previous research. However, a novel finding is that separating the between-person and within-person influences on cognitive function resulted in different patterns of results for anxiety and depression; whereas between-person differences showed to be more important for anxiety, within-person changes appeared more influential for depression.

The observed pattern for anxiety confirms the results of previous studies on the cross-sectional influence of anxiety on cognition (Beaudreau & O'Hara, Reference Beaudreau and O'Hara2009; Bunce et al. Reference Bunce, Batterham, Mackinnon and Christensen2012). In comparison, within-person variability in anxiety seems to have a smaller effect. This suggests that, after controlling for between-person effects, within-person variability in anxiety is not an important factor for older age cognitive performance. It should be noted, however, that the variability in anxiety was relatively modest; for people who stayed in the study, anxiety levels remained rather stable across waves (r = 0.61–0.70). As increases were relatively small and likely to be below the clinical threshold in most cases, they might not have been large enough to affect cognitive performance. Furthermore, anxiety might even be beneficial in a testing situation, up to a certain point. Results from a previous study on older adults are in support of such a threshold effect; mild anxiety symptoms were associated with better cognitive performance and it was only with increasing levels that a negative effect appeared (Bierman et al. Reference Bierman, Comijs, Jonker and Beekman2005).

Anxiety levels have been found to be more stable across time within elderly people compared to levels of depression (Wetherell et al. Reference Wetherell, Gatz and Pedersen2001). Possibly, this reflects that anxiety is closely related to underlying personality traits. Associations between anxiety and neuroticism are typically high (Watson & Clark, Reference Watson and Clark1984), as confirmed by the present study. The genetic influence on the variance in anxiety increases from age 60 at the expense of environmental influences (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Gatz, Pedersen and Prescott2016), thus further supporting the view of older age anxiety as a more trait-like characteristic. In addition to being more stable, older individuals may be less affected by changes in anxiety compared to young adults. In a study comparing two age groups, older adults both reported experiencing fewer stressors and that the stressors had less impact on their daily routines (Brose et al. Reference Brose, Scheibe and Schmiedek2013).

The largest effect of anxiety on cognition was observed for Raven's, a test of reasoning ability. This is consistent with Eysenck's processing efficiency theory (Eysenck & Calvo, Reference Eysenck and Calvo1992), stating that anxiety interferes with cognitive tasks by reducing available resources for working memory. Therefore, tasks relying on the central executive of working memory, for example, problem solving and reasoning tasks, are hypothesized to be mostly affected by anxiety. Older adults might be particularly vulnerable to such effects as their working memory capacity is already limited.

More anxiety symptoms were observed in individuals with lower age 11 IQ, females, and persons with lower emotional stability (Watson & Clark, Reference Watson and Clark1984; Johnson et al. Reference Johnson, Corley, Starr and Deary2011; Lee et al. Reference Lee, Gatz, Pedersen and Prescott2016). Covarying all of these resulted in non-significant effects of anxiety for Raven's. Childhood mental ability showed the strongest association to cognition (Deary et al. Reference Deary, Pattie and Starr2013) and was also related to anxiety; controlling for this variable resulted in non-significant associations between anxiety and Letter fluency. For Logical memory, effects of anxiety were only borderline significant (p = 0.05) but followed the same trend as for the other cognitive tasks, with between-person effects being most important.

In contrast to the results for anxiety, the results for depression suggest that within-person variability is more important for cognition in the elderly population compared to differences in mean level. That depression is negatively related to cognitive performance is in agreement with the bulk of research in this area (Dotson et al. Reference Dotson, Resnick and Zonderman2008; Köhler et al. Reference Köhler, Thomas, Barnett and O'Brien2010). With regard to longitudinal associations, a large population-based study reported that the course of cognitive functioning was not significantly associated with the course of depressive symptoms (van den Kommer et al. Reference van den Kommer, Comijs, Aartsen, Huisman, Deeg and Beekman2013), whereas a recent study observed that steeper decline in processing speed correlated with a steeper increase in depressive symptoms (Brailean et al. Reference Brailean, Aartsen, Muniz-Terrera, Prince, Prina, Comijs, Huisman and Beekman2017). In a sample of older adults with major depressive disorder, having more depressive symptoms than usual was associated with worse than average global cognitive function (Dzierzewski et al. Reference Dzierzewski, Potter, Jones, Rostant, Ayotte, Yang, Sachs, Feldman and Steffens2015). Here, we provide evidence that depressive symptoms covary with cognitive performance, especially letter fluency, in the general elderly population.

When interpreting these findings, one should bear in mind that very few participants had clinical depression. To have a current diagnosis of depression, or having experienced several reoccurring depressive episodes, is still likely to have an impact on cognitive performance at the between-person level. We observed associations at the between- as well as the within-person level between for Letter fluency. This is consistent with previous findings that tasks depending on frontal lobe functioning is affected already in mild depression, and may also persist in remission (Köhler et al. Reference Köhler, Thomas, Barnett and O'Brien2010; Pantzar et al. Reference Pantzar, Laukka, Atti, Fastbom, Fratiglioni and Bäckman2014).

An important finding of this study is that even rather modest intraindividual change in depression exerts significant influence on cognitive performance in the general population. Thus, our results extend the findings from a previous study targeting a population of depressed individuals (Dzierzewski et al. Reference Dzierzewski, Potter, Jones, Rostant, Ayotte, Yang, Sachs, Feldman and Steffens2015). The observed effects may reflect changes due to environmental factors, or feeling more happy or sad on a particular day. However, they may also reflect the common trend of increasing levels of depressive symptoms after age 60 (Johnson et al. Reference Johnson, McGue, Gaist, Vaupel and Christensen2002; Chui et al. Reference Chui, Gerstorf, Hoppmann and Luszcz2015); mean levels of depression increased with increasing age. The combined pattern from this study suggests that variability in depressive symptoms is more important for cognitive functioning than within-person changes in anxiety. This corroborates previous findings that depressive symptoms show a linear association with cognition, where more symptoms are associated with worse cognition, whereas anxiety levels need to exceed a certain threshold before exerting a negative influence on cognitive performance (Bierman et al. Reference Bierman, Comijs, Jonker and Beekman2005).

The strongest effects for depression were observed for Letter fluency. This is in agreement with that the most consistent associations between depression and cognition have been observed for tasks dependent on speed and executive functioning (Köhler et al. Reference Köhler, Thomas, Barnett and O'Brien2010; Pantzar et al. Reference Pantzar, Laukka, Atti, Fastbom, Fratiglioni and Bäckman2014); Letter fluency requires both fast processing and active generation of responses. Logical memory also covaried with number of depressive symptoms. However, controlling for age 11 IQ, emotional stability, or number of diseases resulted in non-significant associations. Results were somewhat weaker for Raven's (p = 0.07) and largely disappeared after controlling for gender. Cross-level interactions between WP and PM were not significant, meaning that within-person associations with cognition were not different for individuals with high or low levels of depression.

There are several potential explanations for the association between depression and cognitive performance. Feeling more depressed than usual may place higher demands on available resources (Seibert & Ellis, Reference Seibert and Ellis1991) and negative affect has been shown to covary inversely with motivation (Brose et al. Reference Brose, Schmiedek, Lövdén and Lindenberger2012). These factors may contribute to the intraindividual coupling of depression and cognition. In addition, clinical depression may have more long-term effects on cognition by causing structural and functional brain changes (Duman et al. Reference Duman, Heninger and Nestler1997). However, the direction of potential causal relationships is not clear and it might also be that a third factor, for example, disease burden or impending dementia, accounts for the association. Depressive symptoms may be both risk factors or early markers of dementia (Byers & Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011), and it is possible that subclinical neurodegenerative or vascular changes underlie both the depressive symptoms and cognitive declines observed in this study.

Strengths and limitations

A strength of the present study is that we were able to assess both between- and within-person effects of both anxiety and depression within the same individuals. Additional strengths are that we could investigate these effects in a large population-based sample with information on performance in several cognitive domains as well as important confounding variables. The fact that all individuals were of nearly the same age at wave 1 and that we controlled for longitudinal age effects in the analyses make it possible to disregard the effects of age in this study, which would have been a major possible confounder.

The study also has limitations; one is that we do not have information on levels of depression and anxiety between assessments. Given the relatively long time intervals between testing occasions, our findings are likely to inform on general trends spanning longer time periods rather than the effects of day-to-day fluctuation in mood. It is also probable that there was some selective attrition throughout the study, restricting the range for anxiety and depression (see Results section, first paragraph). Our findings are correlational and thus do not inform on the direction of the association or any potential causal relationships.

Implications

Symptoms of anxiety and depression are common in old age. This study confirms that they are both important correlates of cognition but also shows that they affect performance in different ways. For anxiety, mean levels were more important, strengthening the view of anxiety as a more trait-like factor in relation to cognitive performance. For depression, deviating from one's mean level was more important, illustrating that even rather small changes in depression may affect cognitive performance. The results have implications for the interpretation of cognitive test scores, as using scores based on only one occasion may give a somewhat misleading picture of that individual's actual cognitive abilities. The fluctuating nature of depression and its impact on cognitive performance may explain some of the inconsistencies in previous research concerning depressive symptoms and cognitive decline (Han et al. Reference Han, McCusker, Abrahamowicz, Cole and Capek2006; Dzierzewski et al. Reference Dzierzewski, Potter, Jones, Rostant, Ayotte, Yang, Sachs, Feldman and Steffens2015). From a treatment perspective, the results from this study also show that when levels of depression go down, cognitive performance improves. This suggests that reducing levels of depressive symptoms, even outside the clinical range, may lead to improved cognitive performance.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002896.

Acknowledgements

The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 Study data collection was supported by grants from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC; 15/SAG09977), a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award to I.J.D., the Scottish Government's Chief Scientist Office (CZB/4/505 and ETM/55), and the Medical Research Council (MRC) and BBSRC via funding for the Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology (which supports I.J.D.), part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (G0700704/84698 and MR/K026992/1). E.J.L. was supported by a grant from the Swedish Research Council and D.D. was funded by Age UK. The authors thank Alison Pattie, Alan Gow, Janie Corley, Ross Henderson, Catherine Murray, Caroline Brett, and the nurses at the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility, Western General Hospital, Edinburgh, for testing the participants.

References

Allerhand, M, Gale, CR, Deary, IJ (2014) The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and positive wellbeing in older people: a prospective study using the English Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychology and Aging 29, 306318.Google Scholar
Beaudreau, SA, O'Hara, R (2009) The association of anxiety and depressive symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults. Psychology and Aging 24, 507512.Google Scholar
Bielak, A, Gerstorf, D, Kiely, KM, Anstey, KJ, Luszcz, M (2011) Depressive symptoms predict decline in perceptual speed in older adulthood. Psychology and Aging 26, 576583.Google Scholar
Bierman, EJ, Comijs, HC, Jonker, C, Beekman, AT (2005) Effects of anxiety versus depression on cognition in later life. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 13, 686693.Google Scholar
Bierman, EJ, Comijs, HC, Rijmen, F, Jonker, C, Beekman, AT (2008) Anxiety symptoms and cognitive performance in later life: results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Aging and Mental Health 12, 517523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjelland, I, Dahl, AA, Haug, TT, Neckelmann, D (2002) The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: an updated review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 52, 6677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brailean, A, Aartsen, MJ, Muniz-Terrera, G, Prince, M, Prina, AM, Comijs, HC, Huisman, M, Beekman, A (2017) Longitudinal associations between late-life depression dimensions and cognitive functioning: a cross-domain latent growth curve analysis. Psychological Medicine 47, 690702.Google Scholar
Brose, A, Lövdén, M, Schmiedek, F (2014) Daily fluctuations in positive affect positively co-vary with working memory performance. Emotion 14, 16.Google Scholar
Brose, A, Scheibe, S, Schmiedek, F (2013) Life context make a difference: emotional stability in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging 28, 148159.Google Scholar
Brose, A, Schmiedek, F, Lövdén, M, Lindenberger, U (2012) Daily variability in working memory is coupled with negative affect: the role of attention and motivation. Emotion 12, 605617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brose, A, Schmiedek, F, Lövdén, M, Molenaar, PCM, Lindenberger, U (2010) Adult age differences in covariation of motivation and working memory performance: contrasting between-person and within-person findings. Research in Human Development 7, 6178.Google Scholar
Bunce, D, Batterham, PJ, Mackinnon, AJ, Christensen, H (2012) Depression, anxiety, and cognition in community-dwelling adults aged 70 years and over. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 16621666.Google Scholar
Byers, AL, Yaffe, K (2011) Depression and risk of developing dementia. Nature Reviews. Neurology 7, 323331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chui, H, Gerstorf, D, Hoppmann, CA, Luszcz, MA (2015) Trajectories of depressive symptoms in old age: integrating age-, pathology-, and mortality-related changes. Psychology and Aging 30, 940951.Google Scholar
Curran, PJ, Bauer, DJ (2011) The disaggregation of within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review of Psychology 62, 583619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, IJ, Brett, CE (2015) Predicting and retrodicting intelligence between childhood and old age in the 6-day sample of the Scottish Mental Survey 1947. Intelligence 50, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, IJ, Gow, AJ, Pattie, A, Starr, JM (2012) Cohort profile: the Lothian Birth Cohorts of 1921 and 1936. International Journal of Epidemiology 41, 15761584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, IJ, Pattie, A, Starr, JM (2013) The stability of intelligence from age 11 to 90 years: the Lothian Birth Cohort of 1921. Psychological Science 24, 23612368.Google Scholar
Deary, IJ, Whiteman, MC, Starr, JM, Whalley, LJ, Fox, HC (2004) The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish Mental Surveys of 1932 and 1947. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86, 130147.Google Scholar
de Bruijn, RF, Direk, N, Mirza, SS, Hofman, A, Koudstaal, PJ, Tiemeier, H, Ikram, A (2014) Anxiety is not associated with the risk of dementia or cognitive decline: the Rotterdam Study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22, 13821390.Google Scholar
de Frias, CM, Lövdén, M, Lindenberger, U, Nilsson, LG (2007) Revisiting the dedifferentiation hypothesis with longitudinal multi-cohort data. Intelligence 35, 381392.Google Scholar
Dotson, VM, Resnick, SM, Zonderman, AB (2008) Differential association of concurrent, baseline, and average depressive symptoms with cognitive decline in older adults. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16, 318330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duman, RS, Heninger, GR, Nestler, EJ (1997) A molecular and cellular theory of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 597606.Google Scholar
Dzierzewski, JM, Potter, GG, Jones, RN, Rostant, OS, Ayotte, B, Yang, FM, Sachs, BC, Feldman, BJ, Steffens, DC (2015) Cognitive functioning throughout the treatment history of clinical late-life depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 30, 10761084.Google Scholar
Eysenck, MW, Calvo, MG (1992) Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion 6, 409434.Google Scholar
Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE, McHugh, PR (1975) Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12, 189198.Google Scholar
Freiheit, EA, Hogan, DB, Eliasziw, M, Patten, SB, Demchuk, AM, Faris, P, Anderson, T, Galbraith, D, Parboosingh, JS, Ghali, WA, Knudtson, M, Maxwell, CJ (2012) A dynamic view of depressive symptoms and neurocognitive change among patients with coronary heart disease. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 244255.Google Scholar
Goldberg, L (1992) The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment 4, 2642.Google Scholar
Gow, AJ, Whiteman, MC, Pattie, A, Deary, IJ (2005) Goldberg's IPIP Big-Five factor markers: internal consistency and concurrent validation in Scotland. Personality and Individual Differences 39, 317329.Google Scholar
Han, L, McCusker, J, Abrahamowicz, M, Cole, M, Capek, R (2006) The temporal relationship between depression symptoms and cognitive functioning in older medical patients – prospective or concurrent? Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences 61A, 13191323.Google Scholar
Hoffman, L, Stawski, RS (2009) Persons as contexts: evaluating between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analysis. Research in Human Development 6, 97120.Google Scholar
Johnson, W, Corley, J, Starr, JM, Deary, IJ (2011) Psychological and physical health at age 70 in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936: links with early life IQ, SES, and current cognitive function and neighborhood environment. Health Psychology 30, 111.Google Scholar
Johnson, W, McGue, M, Gaist, D, Vaupel, JW, Christensen, K (2002) Frequency and heritability of depression symptomatology in the second half of life: evidence from Danish twins over 45. Psychological Medicine 32, 11751185.Google Scholar
Köhler, S, Thomas, AJ, Barnett, NA, O'Brien, JT (2010) The pattern and course of cognitive impairment in late-life depression. Psychological Medicine 40, 591602.Google Scholar
Lee, LO, Gatz, M, Pedersen, NL, Prescott, CA (2016) Anxiety trajectories in the second half of life: genetic and environmental contributions over age. Psychology and Aging 31, 101113.Google Scholar
Lezak, M (2004) Neuropsychological Testing. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Nesselroade, JR, Salthouse, TA (2004) Methodological and theoretical implications of intraindividual variability in perceptual-motor performance. Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological and Social Sciences 59B, 4955.Google Scholar
Nivard, MG, Dolan, CV, Kendler, KS, Kan, KJ, Willemsen, G, van Beijsterveldt, CE, Lindauer, RJ, van Beek, JH, Geels, LM, Bartels, M, Middeldorp, CM, Boomsma, DI (2015) Stability in symptoms of anxiety and depression as a function of genotype and environment: a longitudinal twin study from ages 3 to 63 years. Psychological Medicine 45, 10391049.Google Scholar
Pantzar, A, Laukka, EJ, Atti, AR, Fastbom, J, Fratiglioni, L, Bäckman, L (2014) Cognitive deficits in unipolar old-age depression: a population-based study. Psychological Medicine 44, 937947.Google Scholar
Raven, JC, Court, JH, Raven, J (1977) Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. HK Lewis: London, UK.Google Scholar
Rosenström, T, Jokela, M, Hintsanen, M, Pulkki-Råback, L, Hutri-Kähönen, N, Keltikangas-Järvinen, L (2013) Longitudinal course of depressive symptoms in adulthood: linear stochastic differential equation modeling. Psychological Medicine 43, 933944.Google Scholar
Scottish Council for Research in Education (1933) The Intelligence of Scottish Children: A National Survey of an Age-group. University of London Press: London, UK.Google Scholar
Seibert, PE, Ellis, HC (1991) Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance. Memory & Cognition 19, 507513.Google Scholar
Sliwinski, MJ, Smyth, JM, Hofer, SM, Stawski, RS (2006) Intraindividual coupling of daily stress and cognition. Psychology and Aging 21, 545557.Google Scholar
Small, BJ, Dixon, RA, McArdle, JJ (2011) Tracking cognition-health changes from 55 to 95 years of age. The Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 66B, i153i161.Google Scholar
Thorvaldsson, V, Skoog, I, Hofer, SM, Börjesson-Hanson, A, Östling, S, Sacuiu, S, Johansson, B (2012) Nonlinear blood pressure effects on cognition in old age: separating between-person and within-person associations. Psychology and Aging 27, 375383.Google Scholar
van den Kommer, TN, Comijs, HC, Aartsen, MJ, Huisman, M, Deeg, DJ, Beekman, AT (2013) Depression and cognition: How do they interrelate in old age? American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21, 398410.Google Scholar
Watson, D, Clark, LA (1984) Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin 96, 465490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1987) Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised. Psychological Corporation: New York, NY.Google Scholar
Wetherell, JL, Gatz, M, Pedersen, NL (2001) A longitudinal analysis of anxiety and depressive symptoms. Psychology and Aging 16, 187195.Google Scholar
Zigmond, AS, Snaith, RP (1983) The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia 67, 361370.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics according to wave

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Levels of anxiety and depression across waves for the total sample (a; n = 535) and for individuals who stayed in the study for the entire follow-up period (b; n = 50).

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations among covariates, mean anxiety and depression, and variability in anxiety and depression across waves

Figure 3

Table 3. Associations between HADS anxiety and depression scores and performance on cognitive tests (fixed effects)

Supplementary material: File

Laukka et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Laukka et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15 KB
Supplementary material: File

Laukka et al. supplementary material

Tables S2-S4

Download Laukka et al. supplementary material(File)
File 27.7 KB