Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:01:45.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Genome-Wide Association Study of Inattention and Hyperactivity–Impulsivity Measured as Quantitative Traits

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2013

Jane L. Ebejer*
Affiliation:
School of Environmental and Rural Sciences, University of New England, Armidale, New South Wales, Australia Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
David L. Duffy
Affiliation:
Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Julius van der Werf
Affiliation:
School of Environmental and Rural Sciences, University of New England, Armidale, New South Wales, Australia
Margaret J. Wright
Affiliation:
Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Grant Montgomery
Affiliation:
Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Nathan A. Gillespie
Affiliation:
Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Ian B. Hickie
Affiliation:
Brain & Mind Research Institute, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
Nicholas G. Martin
Affiliation:
Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
Sarah E. Medland
Affiliation:
Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
*
Address for correspondence: Jane Ebejer, Genetic Epidemiology Unit, Queensland Institute of Medical Research, 300 Herston Rd, Brisbane QLD 4006, Australia. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) offer the benefit of a hypothesis-free approach to measuring the quantitative effect of genetic variants on affection status. Generally the findings of GWAS relying on ADHD status have been non-significant, but the one study using quantitative measures of symptoms found SLC9A9 and SLC6A1 were associated with inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity. Accordingly, we performed a GWAS using quantitative measures of each ADHD subtype measured with the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD and Normal Behaviour (SWAN) scale in two community-based samples. This scale captures the full range of attention and kinetic behavior; from high levels of attention and appropriate activity to the inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity associated with ADHD within two community-based samples. Our discovery sample comprised 1,851 participants (mean age = 22.8 years [4.8]; 50.6% female), while our replication sample comprised 155 participants (mean age = 26.3 years [3.1]; 68.4% females). Age, sex, age × sex, and age2 were included as covariates and the results from each sample were combined using meta-analysis, then analyzed with a gene-based test to estimate the combined effect of markers within genes. We compare our results with markers that have previously been found to have a strong association with ADHD symptoms. Neither the GWAS nor subsequent meta-analyses yielded genome-wide significant results; the strongest effect was observed at rs2110267 (4.62 × 10−7) for symptoms of hyperactivity–impulsivity. The strongest effect in the gene-based test was for GPR139 on symptoms of inattention (6.40 × 10−5). Replication of this study with larger samples will add to our understanding of the genetic etiology of ADHD.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a behavioral disorder characterized by developmentally inappropriate activity and deficiencies in attention span. ADHD symptoms typically manifest in 4% to 7% of children and continue into adulthood for around 50% of those affected during childhood (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Petty, Evans, Small and Faraone2010; Ebejer et al., Reference Ebejer, Medland, van der Werf, Gondro, Henders, Lynskey and Duffy2012). Heritability estimates of the inattentive and hyperactive–impulsive subtypes range between 70% and 80% (Derks et al., Reference Derks, Hudziak, Dolan, van Beijsterveldt, Verhulst and Boomsma2008; Hudziak et al., Reference Hudziak, Derks, Althoff, Rettew and Boomsma2005; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Scourfield and McGuffin2002), indicating that genetic effects largely account for the variation we see in symptoms. In adulthood, ADHD is associated with comorbidity and ongoing difficulties, for example, substance use, anxiety, mood and disruptive behavior disorders, unemployment, multiple marriages, reduced income, low social support, and poor health (Das et al., Reference Das, Cherbuin, Butterworth, Anstey and Easteal2012; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Adler, Barkley, Biederman, Conners, Demler and Secnik2006). Of most relevance, 6.2% Australian adults have reported impairment in their lives due to ADHD symptoms that do not necessarily meet the diagnostic threshold outlined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) — Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) (Das et al., Reference Das, Cherbuin, Butterworth, Anstey and Easteal2012; Faraone & Biederman, Reference Faraone and Biederman2005; Kooij et al., Reference Kooij, Buitelaar, van den Oord, Furer, Th. Rijnders and Hodiamont2005). Understanding the genetic etiology of symptoms will provide a step toward more directed and appropriate treatment for people affected by symptoms.

Research largely indicates that ADHD symptoms are expressed quantitatively within populations (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood and Waldman1997) and are affected by multiple genetic and environmental factors that provide either risk or protection for developing symptoms (Thapar et al., Reference Thapar, Langley, Asherson and Gill2007). A liability to ADHD becomes evident when enough risk factors accumulate to outweigh the factors protective against symptom expression and is probably the sum of the multi-factorial effects (Gottesman & Shields, Reference Gottesman and Shields1967). The point at which liability becomes pathological is arbitrary and is generally based on DSM-IV or the International Classification of Diseases 2010 (World Health Organization, 1992) criteria, excluding those people with subthreshold ADHD who report that these symptoms cause problems in their lives.

Linkage and association studies of ADHD have mainly focused on specific genetic regions. The results of these studies are mixed (Banaschewski et al., Reference Banaschewski, Becker, Scherag, Franke and Coghill2010), but several genes from the dopamine system are frequently implicated in symptom expression: dopamine transporter DAT1, dopamine receptor DRD4, and dopamine receptor DRD5 (Li et al., Reference Li, Sham, Owen and He2006). Genome-wide association scans provide the benefit of a hypothesis-free approach to measuring the quantitative effect of specific markers across the whole genome. However, to date, meta-analyses of GWAS using case-control methodology have found no significant genetic effects (Mick et al., Reference Mick, McGough, Loo, Doyle, Wozniak, Wilens and Faraone2011; Neale et al., Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Asherson, Franke, Lesch and Holmans2010). The only prior GWAS using symptom count as a quantitative measure of ADHD found markers in the region of SLC6A1 and SLC9A9 that were associated with inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity (Lasky-Su et al., Reference Lasky-Su, Neale, Franke, Anney, Zhou, Maller and Buitelaar2008).

For our purpose, liability is considered to be an underlying continuous distribution of risk on which a diagnosis threshold has been imposed. In the current study we extend the use of a quantitative measure of ADHD symptoms, ranging from high levels of attention and appropriate motor inhibition to the inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity associated with ADHD, to increase the power to detect genes associated with ADHD symptoms at varying degrees of expression. As an additional boost to power, we combine GWAS for two samples into a meta-analysis and run a gene-based test to estimate the independence of marker effects within associated genes. We also compare our findings to a previous genome-wide association scan meta-analysis of ADHD (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Lasky-Su, Anney, Franke, Zhou, Maller and Faraone2008), testing the generalization of quantitative symptoms of ADHD to qualitative diagnosis-based phenotypes.

Methods

Participants

The data used in this study were drawn from two community-based substudies of the Brisbane Longitudinal Twin Study: a twin study of Melanocytic Naevi (MN; for details, see Figure 1 and Wright and Martin, Reference Wright and Martin2004), and the Nineteen-Up study — examining neurobiological changes associated with mental health (NU; additional study details are provided in Gillespie et al., Reference Gillespie, Henders, Davenport, Hermens, Wright, Martin and Hickie2012). From an initial sample of 3,236 participants within the MN study, 1,851 family members from 735 families for whom genotype and phenotype data were available comprised our discovery sample. The sample included 266 monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs and 9 single twins from an MZ pair (251 males and 290 females), 216 same sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs and 26 single twins from the same sex DZ pair (239 males and 219 females), 214 opposite sex DZ twin pairs and 26 single twins from an opposite sex DZ pair (224 males and 230 females), and 398 siblings of twins (189 males and 209 females). The age of these participants ranged from 13 to 40 years (M = 22.8, SD = 4.8).

FIGURE 1 Recruitment process leading to collection of SWAN measured ADHD data used in this study.

The data collected within the NU study provided our replication sample, initially comprising 953 participants. These data were collected in two waves; the first included 374 individuals and the second wave included 711, with 132 people who participated in both waves and 477 who had contributed data for both MN and NU studies. Of the initial replication sample, 155 people from 100 families had phenotype and genotype data available and their age ranged from 19 to 36 years (M = 26.3, SD = 3.1). This group comprised 21 complete and 21 incomplete MZ twin pairs (17 males and 46 females), 18 paired, and 10 unpaired same-sex DZ twin pairs (12 males and 34 females), 8 complete opposite-sex DZ twin pairs and 17 unpaired twins (16 males and 17 females), and 13 siblings of twins (4 males and 9 females). All participants were fully informed of study procedures and they gave consent to participate online. Ethics approval for this study was provided by QIMR Human Research and Ethics Committee (HREC) and Virginia Commonwealth University Institutional Review Board (IRB).

Measures

Data collection for ADHD-related behaviors began in 2010 and were collected from mothers of all twins who had participated in the Brisbane Longitudinal Twin study since 1992. In the MN study the mother-reported data were used, and in the NU study these data were collected directly from the twins (self-reported). Both studies used the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD and Normal-Behavior Scale (SWAN; Swanson et al., Reference Swanson, Schuck, Mann, Carlson, Hartman, Sergeant and McCleary2005) for data collection. Within MN, 1,822 mothers reported ADHD symptoms for the twins and each of their siblings as one component of a 60- to 90-min online questionnaire assessing several health phenotypes: (A) physique, (B) complexion and sun exposure, (C) handedness, (D) to (G) ancestral and family information, (H) migraine headache, (I) asthma and eczema, (J) women's health, and (K) behavior. If the children were older than 20 years, parents were asked what their child's behavior was like in primary school. If the child was 20 or younger, their behavior was rated in relation to their peer group. The remaining 28 parents completed the questionnaire during their child's second clinic visit (at age 14 years). This scale was similarly directed toward the twins and each of their siblings and comprised 30 items: 1 to 18 addressed ADHD symptoms, 19 to 27 were specific to oppositional defiant disorder, and 28 to 30 measured cognitive tempo.

The inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity subscales of the SWAN demonstrate retest reliabilities of 0.72 to 0.90 and a Cronbach's alpha of 0.80 (Arnett et al., Reference Arnett, Pennington, Friend, Willcutt, Byrne, Samuelsson and Olson2013). Each of the 18 SWAN items addresses criterion A of DSM-IV (text revision), but is worded to reflect normal behavior as indicated in Table 1, along with the probability of endorsing a positive symptom by sex. The scale used for both the versions of the questionnaire in the MN study was 7-point scale and coded with high scores reflecting problem behaviors: -3 (far above average), -2 (above average), -1 (slightly above average), 0 (average), 1 (slightly below average), 2 (below average), and 3 (far below average).

TABLE 1 Endorsement Probabilities for Listed SWAN Scale Items Within the Melanocytic Naevi (MN) and Nineteen-Up (NU) Studies

These probabilities were calculated using full samples (MN: 1,572 males and 1,664 females and NU: 392 males and 561 females).

The SWAN data collected for the NU study were self-reported by twins and were also completed as a component of an online questionnaire similarly addressing health phenotypes: (A) general health, (B) relationships, (C) migraine and headaches, (D) SWAN items, (E) hair loss, (F) sleep and wake, (G) activity schedule, (H) personality, (I) romantic preferences, and (J) flexibility loss. This questionnaire also took approximately 60–90 min to complete and respondents could go back and modify their answers or discontinue completion at any time. Within the first wave of the study, twins were asked to report on their behavior between the age of 7 and 10 years ‘in relation to their peer group’. In the second wave, twins rated their current behavior ‘in relation to their current peer group’. The scale of this questionnaire ranged from -2 to 2: -2 (far above average), -1 (above average), 0 (average), 1 (below average), and 2 (far below average) and was coded again, so high scores reflected symptom expression. For questionnaires in both studies, the mean of the first 9 items was used as our measure of inattention, the mean of items 10 to 18 provided our measure of hyperactivity–impulsivity, and the mean of all 18 items provided our measure of combined ADHD symptom expression, as presented in Table 2, with descriptive statistics for the SWAN metrics across studies.

TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics for SWAN Scale Data Collected During the Twin Study of Melanocytic Naevi (MN) and Nineteen-Up (NU) Studies

SWAN scale in MN is coded from -3 to 3 and in NU from -2 to 2. In both studies scores >1 represent positive symptoms. Clinic = data collected within MN from mothers during twin's second clinic visit. Online = data collected within MN from online questionnaire completed by mothers.

Genotyping

In both samples participants were genotyped using the Illumina single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) Human610-Quad platform and 592,392 SNPs were genotyped; 8,447 of these had a call rate < 0.95, and 2,841 had Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) p-value < 10−6. There were 33,347 SNPs with one allele frequency <0.01 or only one observed allele, and these SNPs were removed from the dataset. After cleaning, 529,379 SNPs remained and of these, 274,604 were common to additional waves of data collection as described in Medland et al. (Reference Medland, Nyholt, Painter, McEvoy, McRae, Zhu and Henders2009). These SNPs were imputed to Hapmap II (build 36, release 22) using the Utah residents with ancestry from northern and western Europe references (CEU) with MACH 1.0 (Li & Abecasis, Reference Li and Abecasis2006) and imputation resulted in 2,373,249 SNPs with an rsq > .3.

Statistical Analyses

Genome-wide association studies were conducted to examine genetic associations for the quantitative measures of inattention, hyperactivity–impulsivity, and combined behaviors separately for the MN and NU studies, including sex, current age, sex × age, and age2 as covariates. Within the MN study, the method of form completion (online or clinic) was included as a covariate to correct for any variation across the method of data collection. In the NU study we ran structural equation models using OpenMx (Boker et al., Reference Boker, Neale, Maes, Metah, Kenny, Bates and Spiegel2010) to estimate the similarity of genetic effects across waves of data collection due to the difference in time to which the questions were directed. There was no significant difference in genetic and environmental parameters between waves of data collection, p > .05, h 2 = 0.42 (0.19–0.56), so all data for NU were combined and analyzed with wave as a covariate. Data were screened for ancestry outliers and SNP-based association was conducted across the genome using family-based association analysis in Merlin-offline (Chen & Abecasis, Reference Chen and Abecasis2007) under an additive model. This method models family structure and the additive genetic effect of each genotype onto inattention, hyperactivity–impulsivity, and combined behaviors.

The results of individual GWAS for inattention, hyperactivity–impulsivity, and combined symptoms within each study were combined in a meta-analysis using Meta-Analysis Helper (Metal; Abecasis & Willer, Reference Abecasis and Willer2007). Within Metal, z-scores are calculated from the p-values and direction-of-effect within each sample and weighted according to the inverse of the sample size. This method characterizes the magnitude and direction of additive genetic effects relative to the same reference allele across samples. The results of the meta-analysis were examined for gene-based association using Versatile Gene-based Association Study (VEGAS; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Mcrae, Nyholt, Medland, Wray and Brown2010) to estimate the combined effect of all markers within ±50 kb of 5’ and 3’ untranslated regions of a gene. The significance level required for the gene-based test is p < 2.8 × 10−6 (.05/17,787 autosomal genes). Finally, we compare the top 50 SNPs from a meta-analysis of ADHD case-control studies (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Asherson, Franke, Lesch and Holmans2010) with the effect for the same SNPs within our meta-analysis. The alleles at each of these markers were aligned across studies and samples were entered into an additional meta-analysis using Metal.

Results

We first discuss results for the two separate substudies and three core phenotypes — combined symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity–impulsivity — and present these results in Appendix A. The lambda values within each substudy (quantile–quantile [qq] plots) shown in Figure A1 provide no evidence for population stratification or technical artifact for the data collected during the MN study, but they do indicate low power for data collected within the NU study. Within each sample there were no SNP effects that reached genome-wide significance (p < 5 × 1−8; Figures A2 and A3, Tables A1 and A2). We combined the results of individual analyses to obtain meta-analyses for combined ADHD symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity–impulsivity, and present the SNP effects in Figure 2 and Table 3.

TABLE 3 Top 25 Markers for MN and NU Data Meta-Analysis of Combined Symptoms, Inattention, and Hyperactivity–Impulsivity

FIGURE 2 Manhattan plot for MN and NU data meta-analysis indicating the strongest associations between the 22 autosomes and combined symptoms, inattention, and hyperactive–impulsive behaviors.

No SNPs reached genome-wide significance — the strongest effect was for rs2110267 on chromosome 7, which was positively associated with symptoms of hyperactivity–impulsivity (4.6 × 10−7). The results of the gene-based test (Table 4) for combined symptoms, inattention, and hyperactivity–impulsivity, respectively, showed no genome-wide significance; GPR139 showed the strongest association with ADHD (6.4 × 10−5), specifically for symptoms of inattention.

TABLE 4 Gene-Based Test Results of SNP Effects From Meta-Analysis of MN and NU Combined, Inattentive, and Hyperactive–Impulsive Symptoms

To examine whether our results replicate previous findings from case control analyses, we combined the SNP effects that we found with the 50 best SNPs in a meta-analysis by Neale and colleagues (Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Asherson, Franke, Lesch and Holmans2010) using Metal. We found stronger effects for rs12375086 and rs12673393 than found separately in either of the studies. The Metal results approached genome-wide significance for the effect of rs12375086 on combined symptoms of ADHD (9.9 × 10−8) and hyperactivity–impulsivity (7.4 × 10−8), and we present these in Table 5. This provides evidence for replication of an association between these SNPs and ADHD — this genetic region is illustrated in Appendix B (Figure B1). However, we found no genome-wide significant effects in any of our analyses due to insufficient power, given the relatively small size of our replication sample.

TABLE 5 Top SNPs Identified by Neale et al. (Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Asherson, Franke, Lesch and Holmans2010) and SNP Effects Within This Study Combined Using Metal

The marker effect for Neale et al.'s meta-analysis (Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Asherson, Franke, Lesch and Holmans2010) represents the direction of the z-score associated with each SNP. The combined samples p-value represents the combined SNP effects.

Discussion

In this study we used a novel quantitative measure of ADHD to describe the full spectrum of behavior relating to DSM-IV-defined ADHD symptoms. There was evidence for replication of two SNP effects previously found in a meta-analysis of ADHD case-control GWAS. However, no novel results reached significance. Our qq-plots for data from the NU study appear to be underpowered, indicating fewer p-values at the higher end of the distribution than would be expected by chance. This may be due to reduction in power associated with (1) a reduced sample size, and (2) a reduced effect — the scale used for NU study was 5-point in comparison to the 7-point scale used in the MN study. ADHD symptoms appear to be influenced by many genes of small effect, and as previously found with GWAS, pooling samples to increase sample size and the size of the effects achieved would be the most appropriate way forward. This is supported by the suggested replication of previous results. We could not adequately compare our results with those of Lasky-Su and colleagues (Reference Lasky-Su, Neale, Franke, Anney, Zhou, Maller and Buitelaar2008) as neither direction of effects nor reference alleles were presented in their article, but as described below there was suggested replication.

The methodology we used differs somewhat from that of the previous GWAS of quantitative ADHD symptoms. Lasky-Su and associates (Reference Lasky-Su, Neale, Franke, Anney, Zhou, Maller and Buitelaar2008) used a family-based association test (FBAT), which employs parental genotype and/or the trait distribution in offspring as controls when calculating association between markers and traits. This essentially controls for population stratification and skewness; however, it is unlikely that the variation in our results is due to skewness (see Table 2) or population stratification due to the quality control prior to conducting the GWAS. The second quantitative measure in the study of Lasky-Su et al. represented the sum of positive responses to each ADHD symptom. In contrast, our scale addressed the full spectrum of ADHD-related behaviors, providing more power to detect genetic association and at the same time greater phenotypic variation. This variation in scales might partially account for variation in results.

We were unable to adequately compare our effect sizes with the best SNPs of Lasky-Su and colleagues (Reference Lasky-Su, Neale, Franke, Anney, Zhou, Maller and Buitelaar2008) due to the absence of alleles and directional effects in their article, but there were three SNPs with suggested replication in our study: rs17651978 in association with inattention and ADHD total score (p Lasky-Su = 6.1 × 10−6, p Ebejer = 4.4 × 10−3), rs7992643 in association with ADHD total score (p Lasky-Su = 5.5 × 10−6, p Ebejer = 9.4 × 10−3), and rs10767942 in association with hyperactivity–impulsivity (p Lasky-Su = 7.9 × 10−6, p Ebejer = 8.4 × 10−3). Of interest, rs7992643 was uniquely associated with combined symptoms of ADHD. Within our sample, the top 25 SNPs for the combined symptoms phenotype were associated with either symptoms of inattention or hyperactive–impulsivity and not unique to the combined subtype, but overall approximately two-fifths of the strongest 100 SNP effects were unique to combined symptoms. There has been evidence to suggest there may be a genetically distinct form of combined ADHD (Christiansen et al., Reference Christiansen, Chen, Oades, Asherson, Taylor, Lasky-Su and Franke2008) but this has not been conclusively proven, and given the heterogeneous results we see across GWAS of ADHD, it remains speculation. Teasing apart the genes contributing to the subtypes and latent classes of ADHD will provide important information about the etiology of symptoms and the distinction of phenotypes.

The primary limitation of this study was the small size of our replication sample. In addition, we were unable to exclude possible cases of autism or phenotypes that could mimic ADHD and this could have influenced our findings. There are arguments for and against the exclusion of specific disorders when measuring ADHD. Doing so may exclude natural variation in symptoms and important etiological information, both genetic and phenotypic; a more restrictive analysis of symptoms allows a more specific focus on one dimension of behavior. An additional limitation was the use of retrospective parental report, possibly leading to a reinterpretation of behaviors that can occur with time.

In conclusion, there was suggested overlap in the genetic effects found for diagnosed cases of ADHD and ADHD measured as a quantitative trait. Questions remain regarding the genetic overlap between combined symptoms of ADHD, inattention, and hyperactivity–impulsivity. Within a population sample such as ours, genetic effects are likely to be more heterogeneous than a clinical sample due to the specificity of diagnosis in a clinical study. Paradoxically, it may be a community-based sample that provides the greatest insight into the true variation and comorbidity associated with ADHD symptoms, given the impairment reported by adults with subthreshold symptoms. Increasing the sample size of GWAS for quantitative measures of ADHD symptoms and examining the overlap between ADHD subtypes and comorbid disorders in conjunction with molecular genetics will provide information necessary for development of appropriate treatments.

Acknowledgments

We thank the twins and their families for their ongoing interest and participation in our studies. We also thank David Smyth and Scott Gordon for IT support and our research nurses Ann Eldridge and Marlene Grace. We thank Kerrie McAloney for project coordination and all staff at QIMR involved in data collection and administration without whom this work would not be possible. Our work was supported by several grants: Jane L. Ebejer is supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award and a Strategic Doctoral Scholarship from University of New England; Sarah E. Medland is supported by an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship 110100548; Nicholas G. Martin and Ian B. Hickie are supported by US National Institute on Drug Abuse (R00DA023549); and partial support was given by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council grant 1009839. Statistical analyses were carried out on the GenEpi Cluster, which is financially supported by contributions from grants from the NHMRC (389892; 496682; 496688; 496739; 613672) and ARC (FT0991022; FT0991360).

Appendix A

FIGURE A1 qq-plots for combined symptoms, inattention, and hyperactive–impulsive ADHD symptoms generated separately for data collected within the Melanocytic Naevi (MN) and Nineteen-Up (NU) studies.

FIGURE A2 Manhattan plot of GWAS results indicating the strength of genetic associations with SWAN data collected within the Melanocytic Naevi study.

FIGURE A3 Manhattan plot of GWAS results indicating the strength of genetic associations with SWAN data collected within the Nineteen-Up study.

TABLE A1 Descriptive Statistics of GWAS Indicating the Strongest 25 SNP Associations with SWAN-Measured ADHD Subtypes in Study of Melanocytic Naevi

TABLE A2 Descriptive Statistics of GWAS Indicating the Strongest 25 SNP Associations with SWAN-Measured ADHD Subtypes in Nineteen-Up Study

Appendix B

FIGURE B1 Combined results of this study with the meta-analysis of Neale et al. (Reference Neale, Medland, Ripke, Asherson, Franke, Lesch and Holmans2010) indicated the association between the SNP located in genomic position 96038981–96438981 on chromosome 7 of build 18 and SWAN-measured ADHD near genome wide significance (9.9×10−8) with combined symptoms and (7.4×10−8) with hyperactivity-impulsivity

References

Abecasis, G., & Willer, C. (2007). Metal: Meta analysis helper. Accessed on 16 November, 2012. Retrieved from http://www.sph.umich.edu/csg/abecasis/metal/Google Scholar
Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Friend, A., Willcutt, E. G., Byrne, B., Samuelsson, S., & Olson, R. K. (2013). The SWAN captures variance at the negative and positive ends of the ADHD symptom dimension. Journal of Attention Disorders, 17, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banaschewski, T., Becker, K., Scherag, S., Franke, B., & Coghill, D. (2010). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An overview. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19, 237257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Evans, M., Small, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2010). How persistent is ADHD? A controlled 10-year follow-up study of boys with ADHD. Psychiatry Research, 177, 299304.Google Scholar
Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Metah, P., Kenny, S., Bates, T., . . . Spiegel, M. (2010). The OpenMx Statistical Modeling Package. Accessed on 25 January, 2011. Retrieved from http://openmx.psyc.virginia.eduGoogle Scholar
Chen, W. M., & Abecasis, G. R. (2007). Family-based association tests for genomewide association scans. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, 913926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, H., Chen, W., Oades, R. D., Asherson, P., Taylor, E. A., Lasky-Su, J., . . . Franke, B. (2008). Co-transmission of conduct problems with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Familial evidence for a distinct disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 163175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K. J., & Easteal, S. (2012). A population-based study of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and associated impairment in middle-aged adults. PLoS One, 7, e31500.Google Scholar
Derks, E. M., Hudziak, J. J., Dolan, C. V., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M., Verhulst, F. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on the relation between attention problems and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Genetics, 38, 1123.Google Scholar
Ebejer, J. L., Medland, S. E., van der Werf, J., Gondro, C., Henders, A. K., Lynskey, M., . . . Duffy, D. L. (2012). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Australian adults: Prevalence, persistence, conduct problems and disadvantage. PLoS One, 7, e47404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (2005). What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 9, 384391.Google Scholar
Gillespie, N. A., Henders, A. K., Davenport, T. A., Hermens, D. F., Wright, M. J., Martin, N. G., & Hickie, I. B. (2012). The Brisbane Longitudinal Twin Study: Pathways to Cannabis Use, Abuse, and Dependence Project — Current status, preliminary results, and future directions. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 1, 113.Google Scholar
Gottesman, I. I., & Shields, J. (1967). A polygenic theory of schizophrenia. Proceedings of the Natural Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 58, 199205.Google Scholar
Hudziak, J. J., Derks, E. M., Althoff, R. R., Rettew, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2005). The genetic and environmental contributions to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as measured by the Conners’ rating scales — Revised. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 16141620.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Barkley, R., Biederman, J., Conners, C. K., Demler, O., . . . Secnik, K. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 716723.Google Scholar
Kooij, J. J., Buitelaar, J. K., van den Oord, E. J., Furer, J. W., Th. Rijnders, C. A., & Hodiamont, P. P. G. (2005). Internal and external validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of adults. Psychological Medicine, 35, 817827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lasky-Su, J., Neale, B. M., Franke, B., Anney, R. J. L., Zhou, K., Maller, J. B., . . . Buitelaar, J. (2008). Genome‐wide association scan of quantitative traits for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder identifies novel associations and confirms candidate gene associations. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147, 13451354.Google Scholar
Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 737744.Google Scholar
Li, Y., & Abecasis, G. R. (2006). Mach 1.0: Rapid haplotype reconstruction and missing genotype inference. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 79, 2290.Google Scholar
Li, D., Sham, P. C., Owen, M. J., & He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Human Molecular Genetics, 15, 22762284.Google Scholar
Liu, J. Z., Mcrae, A. F., Nyholt, D. R., Medland, S. E., Wray, N. R., & Brown, K. M. (2010). A versatile gene-based test for genome-wide association studies. American Journal of Human Genetics, 87, 139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, N., Scourfield, J., & McGuffin, P. (2002). Observer effects and heritability of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 260265.Google Scholar
Medland, S. E., Nyholt, D. R., Painter, J. N., McEvoy, B. P., McRae, A. F., Zhu, G., . . . Henders, A. K. (2009). Common variants in the trichohyalin gene are associated with straight hair in Europeans. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 750755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mick, E., McGough, J., Loo, S., Doyle, A. E., Wozniak, J., Wilens, T. E., . . . Faraone, S. V. (2011). Genome-wide association study of the child behavior checklist dysregulation profile. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 807817.Google Scholar
Neale, B. M., Lasky-Su, J., Anney, R., Franke, B., Zhou, K., Maller, J. B., . . . Faraone, S. V. (2008). Genome-wide association scan of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 5, 13371344.Google Scholar
Neale, B. M., Medland, S. E., Ripke, S., Asherson, P., Franke, B., Lesch, K. P., . . . Holmans, P. (2010). Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 884897.Google Scholar
Swanson, J., Schuck, S., Mann, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, K., Sergeant, J., . . . McCleary, R. (2005). Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: The SNAP and the SWAN Ratings Scales [draft]. Retrieved 24 February 2012, from http://www.adhd.net/SNAP_SWAN.pdfGoogle Scholar
Thapar, A., Langley, K., Asherson, P., & Gill, M. (2007). Gene-environment interplay in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the importance of a developmental perspective. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (1992). ICD-10 Classifications of Mental and Behavioural Disorder: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: Author.Google Scholar
Wright, M. J., & Martin, N. G. (2004). Brisbane adolescent twin study: Outline of study methods and research projects. Australian Journal of Psychology, 56, 6578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

FIGURE 1 Recruitment process leading to collection of SWAN measured ADHD data used in this study.

Figure 1

TABLE 1 Endorsement Probabilities for Listed SWAN Scale Items Within the Melanocytic Naevi (MN) and Nineteen-Up (NU) Studies

Figure 2

TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics for SWAN Scale Data Collected During the Twin Study of Melanocytic Naevi (MN) and Nineteen-Up (NU) Studies

Figure 3

TABLE 3 Top 25 Markers for MN and NU Data Meta-Analysis of Combined Symptoms, Inattention, and Hyperactivity–Impulsivity

Figure 4

FIGURE 2 Manhattan plot for MN and NU data meta-analysis indicating the strongest associations between the 22 autosomes and combined symptoms, inattention, and hyperactive–impulsive behaviors.

Figure 5

TABLE 4 Gene-Based Test Results of SNP Effects From Meta-Analysis of MN and NU Combined, Inattentive, and Hyperactive–Impulsive Symptoms

Figure 6

TABLE 5 Top SNPs Identified by Neale et al. (2010) and SNP Effects Within This Study Combined Using Metal

Figure 7

FIGURE A1 qq-plots for combined symptoms, inattention, and hyperactive–impulsive ADHD symptoms generated separately for data collected within the Melanocytic Naevi (MN) and Nineteen-Up (NU) studies.

Figure 8

FIGURE A2 Manhattan plot of GWAS results indicating the strength of genetic associations with SWAN data collected within the Melanocytic Naevi study.

Figure 9

FIGURE A3 Manhattan plot of GWAS results indicating the strength of genetic associations with SWAN data collected within the Nineteen-Up study.

Figure 10

TABLE A1 Descriptive Statistics of GWAS Indicating the Strongest 25 SNP Associations with SWAN-Measured ADHD Subtypes in Study of Melanocytic Naevi

Figure 11

TABLE A2 Descriptive Statistics of GWAS Indicating the Strongest 25 SNP Associations with SWAN-Measured ADHD Subtypes in Nineteen-Up Study

Figure 12

FIGURE B1 Combined results of this study with the meta-analysis of Neale et al. (2010) indicated the association between the SNP located in genomic position 96038981–96438981 on chromosome 7 of build 18 and SWAN-measured ADHD near genome wide significance (9.9×10−8) with combined symptoms and (7.4×10−8) with hyperactivity-impulsivity