Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T09:33:46.332Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuropsychological differences between treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive schizophrenia: a meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 November 2021

Edward Millgate*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Olga Hide
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Stephen M Lawrie
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
Robin M Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
James H MacCabe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Eugenia Kravariti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Eugenia Kravariti, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Antipsychotic treatment resistance affects up to a third of individuals with schizophrenia. Of those affected, 70–84% are reported to be treatment resistant from the outset. This raises the possibility that the neurobiological mechanisms of treatment resistance emerge before the onset of psychosis and have a neurodevelopmental origin. Neuropsychological investigations can offer important insights into the nature, origin and pathophysiology of treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS), but methodological limitations in a still emergent field of research have obscured the neuropsychological discriminability of TRS. We report on the first systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate neuropsychological differences between TRS patients and treatment-responsive controls across 17 published studies (1864 participants). Five meta-analyses were performed in relation to (1) executive function, (2) general cognitive function, (3) attention, working memory and processing speed, (4) verbal memory and learning, and (5) visual−spatial memory and learning. Small-to-moderate effect sizes emerged for all domains. Similarly to previous comparisons between unselected, drug-naïve and first-episode schizophrenia samples v. healthy controls in the literature, the largest effect size was observed in verbal memory and learning [dl = −0.53; 95% confidence interval (CI) −0.29 to −0.76; z = 4.42; p < 0.001]. A sub-analysis of language-related functions, extracted from across the primary domains, yielded a comparable effect size (dl = −0.53, 95% CI −0.82 to −0.23; z = 3.45; p < 0.001). Manipulating our sampling strategy to include or exclude samples selected for clozapine response did not affect the pattern of findings. Our findings are discussed in relation to possible aetiological contributions to TRS.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Up to a third of individuals with schizophrenia show resistance to antipsychotic treatment (Elkis & Buckley, Reference Elkis and Buckley2016; Lally, Gaughran, Timms, & Curran, Reference Lally, Gaughran, Timms and Curran2016b; Mørup, Kymes, & Oudin Åström, Reference Mørup, Kymes and Oudin Åström2020; Stokes et al., Reference Stokes, Griffiths, Jones, Everard, Jones, Fowler, Hodgekins and Upthegrove2020), i.e. they do not respond adequately to two or more trials of antipsychotic medication, each lasting 4–6 weeks, at doses in at least the mid-point of the licensed therapeutic range (NICE guidelines; National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2014). In comparison to treatment responsive patients, those with treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS) tend to experience multiple symptomatic relapses, are exposed to higher doses of antipsychotic medication, and show poor functional recovery (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Chan, Honer, Bastiampillai, Suen, Yeung and Chang2021; Iasevoli et al., Reference Iasevoli, Giordano, Balletta, Latte, Formato, Prinzivalli and de Bartolomeis2016).

Evidence of glutamatergic rather than dopaminergic abnormalities in TRS (Demjaha et al., Reference Demjaha, Egerton, Murray, Kapur, Howes, Stone and McGuire2014; Gillespie, Samanaite, Mill, Egerton, & MacCabe, Reference Gillespie, Samanaite, Mill, Egerton and MacCabe2017; Goldstein, Anderson, Pillai, Kydd, & Russell, Reference Goldstein, Anderson, Pillai, Kydd and Russell2015; Mouchlianitis et al., Reference Mouchlianitis, Bloomfield, Law, Beck, Selvaraj, Rasquinha, Waldman and Howes2016) raises the possibility that the disorder is categorically distinct from treatment-responsive schizophrenia (Gillespie et al., Reference Gillespie, Samanaite, Mill, Egerton and MacCabe2017). Running counter to this possibility, clozapine surpasses other antipsychotics in improving total and positive symptoms in both TRS and treatment-responsive patients (Mizuno, McCutcheon, Brugger, & Howes, Reference Mizuno, McCutcheon, Brugger and Howes2020), supporting arguments against an illness subtype that responds specifically to clozapine (Mizuno et al., Reference Mizuno, McCutcheon, Brugger and Howes2020). A possibility that merits exploration is that TRS is aetiologically continuous with treatment-responsive schizophrenia but occupies a more extreme position in a continuum of neurodevelopmental impairment. In support of this hypothesis, 70–84% of patients with treatment-resistant psychosis are reported to be resistant from the first episode (Demjaha et al., Reference Demjaha, Lappin, Stahl, Patel, MacCabe, Howes, Heslin and Murray2017; Lally et al., Reference Lally, Ajnakina, Di Forti, Trotta, Demjaha, Kolliakou, Mondelli and Shergil2016a). In addition, some of the strongest predictors of poor therapeutic response in schizophrenia are the same as the defining features of what has been termed ‘neurodevelopmental’ schizophrenia: male sex, younger age at disease onset, poor premorbid adjustment, and longer duration of untreated illness (Carbon & Correll, Reference Carbon and Correll2014; Murray, O'Callaghan, Castle, & Lewis, Reference Murray, O'Callaghan, Castle and Lewis1992).

Understanding the aetiological and neurobiological mechanisms of TRS is important for developing personalised medicine, for ensuring early detection, and for initiating timely and appropriate treatment. The gold standard treatment for TRS is clozapine (Kane, Honigfeld, Singer, & Meltzer, Reference Kane, Honigfeld, Singer and Meltzer1988), with early pharmacological intervention improving functional outcomes in ~80% of those treated (John, Ko, & Dominic, Reference John, Ko and Dominic2018; Üçok et al., Reference Üçok, Çikrikçili, Karabulut, Salaj, Öztürk, Tabak and Durak2015; Yoshimura, Yada, So, Takaki, & Yamada, Reference Yoshimura, Yada, So, Takaki and Yamada2017). In contrast, a 3-year delay in commencing clozapine reduces response rates to only ~30% (Yoshimura et al., Reference Yoshimura, Yada, So, Takaki and Yamada2017). Notwithstanding this evidence and treatment guidelines, antipsychotic polypharmacy and high doses are commonly used prior to clozapine, which is initiated with a mean delay of 4 years (Howes et al., Reference Howes, Vergunst, Gee, McGuire, Kapur and Taylor2012).

Neuropsychological investigations can offer important insights into the nature, origin and pathophysiology of TRS. To date, a number of studies have reported deficits in verbal intelligence and memory, attention, working memory, visuospatial processing, and sensorimotor function in TRS patients compared to treatment-responsive controls (Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd, & Russell, Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015; Bourque et al. Reference Bourque, Lakis, Champagne, Stip, Lalonde, Lipp and Mendrek2013; de Bartolomeis et al. Reference de Bartolomeis, Balletta, Giordano, Buonaguro, Latte and Iasevoli2013; Frydecka, Beszłej, Gościmski, Kiejna, & Misiak, Reference Frydecka, Beszłej, Gościmski, Kiejna and Misiak2016; Huang et al. Reference Huang, Zhu, Fan, Chen, Hong, Cui, Luo and Yuan2020; Joober et al., Reference Joober, Rouleau, Lal, Dixon, O'Driscoll, Palmour, Annablem and Benkelfat2002; Lin, Chan, Peng, & Chen, Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019). However, inconsistent findings, as well as methodological variability and limitations in a largely emergent field of research make it difficult to elaborate on the neuropsychological profile of TRS and to discern its discriminability compared to schizophrenia at large. The only neuropsychological investigation to date to directly compare longitudinally characterised treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive patients at their first episode of psychosis found relative deficits in language functions in the former group (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Demjaha, Zanelli, Ibrahim, Wise, MacCabe, Reichenberg and Morgan2018). As such functions are largely reflective of premorbid ability, the authors concluded that treatment-resistant psychosis is likely to represent a severe variant of psychosis, embedded in aberrant neurodevelopmental processes (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Demjaha, Zanelli, Ibrahim, Wise, MacCabe, Reichenberg and Morgan2018).

We report on the first systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate and quantify differences in neuropsychological performance between patients with TRS and those responsive to antipsychotic treatment. Based on previous findings, we predicted that TRS patients would perform worse than treatment-responsive patients, and that verbal functions would yield the largest effect sizes.

Methods

Search strategy

Records were accessed from PsycINFO (1806 to October Week 3 2020), Ovid MEDLINE(R) (1946 to October 22nd 2020) and Web of Science on 24th October 2020. Search terms were selected using a PICO framework. Search terms which were exploded in MEDLINE and PsychINFO are indicated with ‘a’ in the description below, with asterisks (*) indicating a wildcard search term. This meta-analysis was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42019147035).

The following search terms were used: (Treatment-resistant schizophrenia OR TRS OR Treatment-refractory schizophrenia OR Antipsychotic-resistant OR Antipsychotic-refractory) AND (Cognit* OR Neuropsy* OR Executive function (a) OR Memory (a) OR Intelligence (a) OR Attention (a) OR Awareness (a) OR Learning (a). Additional publications (N = 3) were sourced from Google Scholar, PsycINFO, and Web of Science through searching ‘Treatment-resistance.Figure 1 provides a summary of the literature search strategy, using the PRISMA Group's guidelines for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (Liberati et al., Reference Liberati, Altman, Tetzlaff, Mulrow, Gøtzsche, Ioannidis, Clarke and Moher2009; Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, & Group, Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman and Group2009). The PRISMA checklist is presented in online Supplementary Table S1.

Fig. 1. A PRISMA based flow diagram of the literature search strategy and study selection.

Inclusion/exclusion criteria

Studies were included in the systematic review and meta-analysis, if they:

  1. 1. Reported neuropsychological findings from individuals with treatment-resistant, and controls with treatment-responsive, schizophrenia-spectrum disorders.

  2. 2. Defined schizophrenia-spectrum disorders according to the diagnostic criteria of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) or the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) classification systems (online Supplementary Table S2).

  3. 3. Adopted one of two definitions of treatment resistance: i. not responding adequately to antipsychotic treatment, despite the use of two or more different antipsychotics, each lasting 4–6 weeks, at doses in at least the mid-point of the licensed therapeutic range (NICE guidelines; National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2014), or ii. Currently treated with clozapine (online Supplementary Table S2).

With the exception of two studies (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019), all publications which met inclusion criteria comprised participants unselected for clozapine response. Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015) and Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019) included discrete subgroups of clozapine-responsive and clozapine-resistant (termed ultra-resistant) subgroups. To address the theoretical risk of inflated (ultra-resistant) or deflated (clozapine-responsive) effect sizes of cognitive deficits compared to unselected TRS samples, we adopted the conservative strategy of excluding the two ultra-treatment resistant subgroups from our main analyses. This approach maximised the analytic sample (the clozapine-responsive subgroups from both studies were retained in the main analysis), whilst cautiously biasing effect sizes towards conservative rather than inflated estimates (by excluding ultra-resistant samples). To examine if manipulating our sampling strategy would have any impact on our findings, the analysis was performed before (main analysis) and after (sensitivity analysis) (a) excluding clozapine-responsive samples (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019); (b) adding clozapine-resistant samples (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019).

As shown in Fig. 1, reviews, opinions, proceeding papers, meeting abstracts, letters, proof of concept studies, and case studies were not included in the study. Of published studies with overlapping participant samples, the largest study, or the one reporting on the largest set of neuropsychological findings, was included. This approach resulted in the exclusion of three publications (de Bartolomeis et al., Reference de Bartolomeis, Prinzivalli, Callovini, D'Ambrosio, Altavilla, Avagliano and Iasevoli2018; Iasevoli et al., Reference Iasevoli, Fagiolini, Formato, Prinzivalli, Giordano, Balletta and de Bartolomeis2017, Reference Iasevoli, D'Ambrosio, Francesco, Razzino, Buonaguro, Giordano and de Bartolomeis2018b). In cases of partial reporting on essential findings (e.g. means and standard deviations of composite scores rather than individual variables), two attempts were made to obtain data from corresponding authors before excluding the paper. Data were received for four studies (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Demjaha, Zanelli, Ibrahim, Wise, MacCabe, Reichenberg and Morgan2018; Lawrie et al., Reference Lawrie, Ingle, Santosh, Rogers, Rimmington, Naidu, Best and Johnstone1995; Legge et al., Reference Legge, Dennison, Pardinas, Rees, Lynham, Hopkins, Bates and Walters2019; Vanes, Mouchlianitis, Collier, Averbeck, & Shergill, Reference Vanes, Mouchlianitis, Collier, Averbeck and Shergill2018a).

Data extraction

For our main analysis (see below), 41 cognitive tasks were grouped into five primary cognitive domains by E.M. & E.K. based on each task's underpinning theoretical construct and earlier groupings in the literature (Fatouros-Bergman, Cervenka, Flyckt, Edman, & Farde, Reference Fatouros-Bergman, Cervenka, Flyckt, Edman and Farde2014; Fett, Viechtbauer, Penn, van Os, & Krabbendam, Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011; Fioravanti, Carlone, Vitale, Cinti, & Clare, Reference Fioravanti, Carlone, Vitale, Cinti and Clare2005): (1) executive function, (2) general cognitive function, (3) attention, working memory and processing speed, (4) verbal memory and learning, and (5) visual−spatial memory and learning (Table 1). Three of the 41 tasks (National Adult Reading Test, Phonological Verbal Fluency, Semantic Verbal Fluency) were also included in our sub-analysis of language-related functions (see below); the latter further included Vocabulary (which did not feature in the main analysis) (Table 1). The following information was extracted from each publication by two independent investigators (E.M. & O.H.): author names, publication year, diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, the definition of TRS, and, for each of the TRS and treatment-responsive groups, number of cases, mean age, number of males, mean age of illness onset, mean duration of illness, mean chlorpromazine equivalents, mean years of education, mean positive and negative symptom scale scores, as well as means and standard deviations of cognitive tasks (online Supplementary Table S4).

Table 1. Cognitive tasks contributing to the main analysis of five primary cognitive domains and the sub-analysis of language-related functions from across primary cognitive domains

Abbreviations: BACS, Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia; BRCCB, Brain Resource Centre Cognitive Battery; CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery; MCCB, MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery; WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third edition; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; WMS-R, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised.

aExtracted from the ‘General cognitive domain’ of the main analysis; bextracted from the ‘Executive function’ domain of the main analysis; cwas not included in the main analysis.

1 The main analysis included all treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS) samples across publications, except for the clozapine-resistant samples in Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015 and Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019. Both clozapine-resistant samples were added to the main analytic sample as part of our sensitivity analysis (see online Supplementary Fig. S3).

2 The sub-analysis focused selectively on language-related functions that were extracted from across the primary cognitive domains of the main analysis, in addition to Wechsler Vocabulary; the latter task was only included in the sub-analysis and did not feature in the main analysis.

Quality assessments for each publication were made using the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies and Quality Assessment of Controlled Intervention Studies tools from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute (NIH, 2014a). Each publication was independently rated by E.M and O.H, providing a ‘yes’, ‘no,’ ‘not applicable,’ ‘cannot determine,’ or ‘not reported’ response to each of fourteen statements. An overall quality rating (good, fair or poor) was derived based on these responses (online Supplementary Table S3).

Main analysis: Meta-analysis of five primary cognitive domains

Data were analysed in relation to five cognitive domains using the metan (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Bradburn, Deeks, Altman, Harbord, Steichen and Sterne2008), metaan (Kontopantelis & Reeves, Reference Kontopantelis and Reeves2010), metabias (Harbord, Harris, Sterne, & Steichen, Reference Harbord, Harris, Sterne and Steichen2009) and metafunnel (Sterne, Reference Sterne2003) commands in STATA/SE Version 15. The metaan command runs meta-analyses off the saved estimates from metan using a restricted maximum likelihood model (REML) and providing I 2 and Cochrane Q estimates for heterogeneity. The REML method reduces the likelihood of both positive and negative biases and has been recommended over eight other methods in a recent comparison of nine different heterogeneity variance estimators using simulated meta-analysis data (Langan et al., Reference Langan, Higgins, Jackson, Bowden, Veroniki, Kontopantelis and Simmonds2019). Z statistics, p values and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were estimated for the effect sizes following Altman & Bland's (Reference Altman and Bland2011) recommendations. Where two or more tasks from the same study contributed to the same cognitive domain, estimates from the metan command were used to create a within-study weighted average prior to metaan. This step was undertaken to preserve the independence of participant samples within each cognitive domain. Findings of significant heterogeneity in any cognitive domain were followed by meta-regressions using the metareg command (Harbord & Higgins, Reference Harbord and Higgins2008) to examine potential demographic (age, sex, and years of education), clinical (duration of illness, age at illness onset, positive and negative symptom ratings) and medication (chlorpromazine equivalents) sources of heterogeneity. Differences between groups in these variables were included as individual predictors in the meta-regression models.

Sub-analysis: Meta-analysis of language-related functions from across primary cognitive domains

Earlier findings from our research group (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Demjaha, Zanelli, Ibrahim, Wise, MacCabe, Reichenberg and Morgan2018) suggested that patients with a first episode of psychosis, who were later found to be treatment resistant, were impaired in verbal intelligence and fluency, but in no other composite scores, relative to their treatment-responsive counterparts. To examine the salience of language-related functions in the neuropsychological profile of TRS, in a second step, we selectively extracted tasks with a prominent language processing component from across the five primary cognitive domains (General cognitive function: National Adult Reading Test; Executive function: Phonological Verbal Fluency; Semantic Verbal Fluency), further adding Wechsler Vocabulary (which did not feature in the main analysis) to conduct a separate, language-focused meta-analysis (Table 1).

Sensitivity analysis: Clozapine response

Forty to 70% of TRS patients respond partially or poorly even to clozapine (Farooq, Choudry, Cohen, Naeem, & Ayub, Reference Farooq, Choudry, Cohen, Naeem and Ayub2019; Potkin et al., Reference Potkin, Kane, Correll, Lindenmayer, Agid, Marder and Howes2020; Siskind, Siskind, & Kisely, Reference Siskind, Siskind and Kisely2017) and are termed ultra-resistant. For the latter group, non-pharmacological augmentation strategies, such as electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) and transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) are shown to hold considerable promise (Lindenmayer et al., Reference Lindenmayer, Kulsa, Sultana, Kaur, Yang, Ljuri and Khan2019; Moulier, Krir, Dalmont, Guillin, & Rothärmel, Reference Moulier, Krir, Dalmont, Guillin and Rothärmel2021). These critical differences in treatment response have been proposed to correspond to three sub-types of schizophrenia: antipsychotic-responsive, clozapine-responsive and clozapine-resistant (Farooq et al., Reference Farooq, Choudry, Cohen, Naeem and Ayub2019).

A direct comparison of neuropsychological performance across the three subtypes would critically enhance the resolution and impact of our analysis. However, all but two studies (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019) included participants unselected for clozapine response, preventing such comparison. We instead examined the sensitivity of our analysis to (a) excluding the clozapine-responsive subgroups of both studies (these were included in the main analysis for the reasons outlined in ‘Inclusion/exclusion criteria’); (b) adding the ultra-treatment resistant subgroups from both studies. To approximate the predominant (undifferentiated) sampling strategy in the literature (and to preserve sample independence), the latter addition was performed by averaging the cognitive scores across the clozapine-responsive and the clozapine-resistant subgroups from each study (rather than including two discrete TRS samples from each study).

Results

Study characteristics

A total of 17 studies (16 observational, one experimental) made up our analytic sample (Fig. 1, Table 2). Table 1 lists the cognitive tasks employed across publications in relation to each of the five primary cognitive domains (main analysis) and in relation to the language-related functions (sub-analysis). The descriptive characteristics of the study samples are presented in Table 2. The 17 publications contributed 1864 participants (939 TRS) and 77 discrete comparisons in cognitive performance between TRS and treatment-responsive participants to the main analysis (online Supplementary Table S4), and 1129 participants (584 TRS) and 10 discrete comparisons in cognitive performance between TRS and treatment-responsive participants to the language sub-analysis (online Supplementary Table S4). The sensitivity analyses included 1678–1933 participants (857–994 TRS) (online Supplementary Figs S2 and S3). Only baseline / pre-intervention data were included from the experimental study by Rakitzi and Georgila, Reference Rakitzi and Georgila2019.

Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive schizophrenia samples in the 17 publications

Abbreviations: CPZEs, chlorpromazine equivalents; DOI, duration of illness; TR, treatment responder; TRS, treatment-resistant schizophrenia.

Note: All values reported are mean values unless indicated otherwise.

The specific list and number of publications that contributed analytic data (i.e. discrete comparisons in cognitive performance between treatment-responsive and TRS participants) to the main analysis and sub-analysis is listed in online Supplementary Table S4, and included 10 publications (24 comparisons) for executive function, nine publications (12 comparisons) for general cognitive function, 10 publications (24 comparisons) for attention, working memory and processing speed, eight publications (12 comparisons) for verbal memory and learning and five publications (five comparisons) for visual−spatial memory and learning (online Supplementary Table S4). Six publications contributed analytic data (10 comparisons) to the sub-analysis of language-related functions (online Supplementary Table S4).

Main analysis: Meta-analysis of five primary cognitive domains

Table 3 illustrates the REML findings and heterogeneity estimates for the main analysis (five primary cognitive domains) and the sub-analysis (language-related functions). TRS patients scored lower than treatment-responsive patients, with effect sizes ranging from small to moderate (based on Cohen's thresholds: 0.2 = small, 0.5 = medium, 0.8 = large; Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). Effect sizes were statistically significantly different from 0, except for executive function and visual−spatial memory and learning (Table 3). The largest effect size (moderate) emerged for verbal memory and learning (dl = −0.53). The remaining cognitive domains gave rise to small effects (dl = −0.27 to −0.38) (Table 3).

Table 3. Meta-analyses of performance differences between treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive schizophrenia samples across publications

Abbreviations and notes: I 2 (%), proportion of observed variance of effect sizes; LCI, lower confidence interval; REML, Restricted maximum likelihood model; T 2, measure of dispersion from true effect size; UCI, upper confidence interval.

1 The main analysis included all treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS) samples across publications, except for the clozapine-resistant samples in Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015 and Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019. Both clozapine-resistant samples were added to the main analytic sample as part of our sensitivity analysis (see online Supplementary Fig. S3).

2 The sub-analysis focused selectively on language-related functions that were extracted from across the primary cognitive domains of the main analysis, in addition to Wechsler Vocabulary; the latter task was only included in the sub-analysis and did not feature in the main analysis.

Figure 2 shows the forest plots for the five primary meta-analyses. Negative effect sizes (left) indicate worse cognitive performance in treatment-resistant cases. The diamond line shows the overall effect size for each meta-analysis.

Fig. 2. Main analysis: Forest plots of effect sizes of performance differences between treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant patients in 1. executive function, 2. general cognitive function, 3. attention, working memory and processing speed, 4. verbal memory and learning and 5. visual−spatial memory and learning.

Heterogeneity

Cochrane's Q statistic was significant for ‘Executive function’, indicating some degree of heterogeneity (Table 3). Using the I 2 statistic, there was no evidence of heterogeneity (I 2 = 0%) for ‘Attention, working memory and processing speed’ and ‘Visual−spatial memory’, and small heterogeneity (I 2 = 17.59%) for ‘General cognitive function’ (Table 3). However, there was moderate heterogeneity (defined as ‘30–60%’; Ryan, Reference Ryan2016) for ‘Executive function’ and ‘Verbal memory and learning’ (Table 3). Meta-regressions showed no significant effects of demographic, clinical and medication variables on the overall effect sizes of ‘Executive function’ and ‘Verbal memory and learning’ (online Supplementary Table S5).

Publication bias

To assess the potential of publication bias, a funnel plot for all datapoints across cognitive domains was generated and examined using visual inspection and Egger's test (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder, Reference Egger, Smith, Schneider and Minder1997). The latter can help detect bias with a smaller number of publications (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Smith, Schneider and Minder1997). The funnel plot was symmetrical (Fig. 3) and the Egger's test indicated no presence of publication bias (t(10) = −1.80, p = 0.109).

Fig. 3. A funnel plot for all 17 publications was included in the main analysis, with 95% confidence interval limits. Data points at the top of the funnel originate from larger sampled investigations.

Quality of studies

All publications received ‘good’ (N = 12) or ‘fair’ (N = 5) ratings following quality assessments (online Supplementary Table S3), indicating no bias due to flaws in study design or implementation, or some bias, but not sufficient to invalidate the study results (NIH, 2014b).

Sub-analysis: Meta-analysis of language-related functions from across primary cognitive domains

Table 3 illustrates the REML findings and heterogeneity estimates for the main analysis (five primary cognitive domains) and sub-analysis (language-related functions). The forest plot of the sub-analysis is further presented in online Supplementary Fig. S1. The meta-analysis of language-related functions gave rise to a moderate effect size, closely comparable to the effect size for verbal memory and learning (dl = −0.53, 95% CI −0.82 to −0.23; z = 3.45; p < 0.001) (Table 3, online Supplementary Fig. S1). The Cochrane's Q test (Q = 13.96; p = 0.016) and the I 2 index (64.17%) suggested substantial heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies (Ryan, Reference Ryan2016). Meta-regressions showed no significant effects of demographic, clinical or medication variables on effect sizes (online Supplementary Table S5).

Sensitivity analysis: Clozapine response

Online Supplementary Figs S2 and S3 present the results of the meta-analyses for the five cognitive domains after excluding the clozapine-responsive TRS samples from Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015) and Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019) (online Supplementary Fig. S2) and after adding the clozapine-resistant TRS samples from both studies (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, McIlwain, Kydd and Russell2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Chan, Peng and Chen2019) (online Supplementary Fig. S3). Manipulating the sampling strategy in this way did not alter the pattern of findings from that reported for the main analysis (online Supplementary Figs S2 and S3).

Discussion

This is the first systematic review and meta-analysis to compare neuropsychological performance between treatment-resistant (n = 925) and treatment-responsive (n = 939) patients with predominantly chronic schizophrenia across eligible published studies (n = 17). Meta-analyses were performed in relation to five cognitive domains, including executive function, general cognitive function, attention, working memory and processing speed, verbal memory and learning, and visual−spatial memory and learning. As part of a focused sub-analysis, we further meta-analysed findings in relation to variables with a prominent language processing component from across the primary cognitive domains. We finally performed separate sensitivity analyses to examine the effect of clozapine response on the main findings.

Confirming our hypotheses, all meta-analyses generated small to moderate effect sizes, which were statistically significant for all but two domains (executive function; visual−spatial memory and learning) and most pronounced for verbal memory and learning and language-related functions. These results suggest that chronic patients with TRS show wide-ranging neuropsychological deficits compared to those with treatment-responsive schizophrenia, which are most salient in verbal functions. Manipulating our sampling strategy to include or exclude samples selected for clozapine response did not affect the main pattern of findings.

Salience of verbal memory deficits in TRS

Verbal memory and learning consistently show the largest effect sizes in meta-analyses of neuropsychological deficits in first-episode (Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone, & Seidman, Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman2009), drug-naïve (Fatouros-Bergman et al., Reference Fatouros-Bergman, Cervenka, Flyckt, Edman and Farde2014) and chronic (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998) schizophrenia patients compared to healthy controls. The impairment is not secondary to IQ deficits (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Morgan, Fearon, Zanelli, Lappin, Dazzan, Morgan and Murray2009) and is also seen in an attenuated form in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients (Bora, Akdede, & Alptekin, Reference Bora, Akdede and Alptekin2017; Snitz, MacDonald, & Carter, Reference Snitz, MacDonald and Carter2006). This empirical research suggests that verbal memory impairment is an endophenotype for schizophrenia (McCarthy et al., Reference McCarthy, Badcock, Clark, Knowles, Cadby, Melton, Morgan and Jablensky2018) and taps into core pathophysiological processes in the disorder (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Morgan, Fearon, Zanelli, Lappin, Dazzan, Morgan and Murray2009).

Confirming our hypothesis and extending earlier findings (Fatouros-Bergman et al., Reference Fatouros-Bergman, Cervenka, Flyckt, Edman and Farde2014; Heinrichs & Zakzanis, Reference Heinrichs and Zakzanis1998; Mesholam-Gately et al., Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman2009), verbal memory and learning emerged as one of two cognitive aspects best discriminating between treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive patients in the present meta-analysis.

Salience of language function deficits in TRS

Language functions have been reported to distinguish between TRS patients and treatment-responsive controls already at the first episode (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Demjaha, Zanelli, Ibrahim, Wise, MacCabe, Reichenberg and Morgan2018). Our sub-analysis of language-related functions gave rise to an effect size comparable to that detected for verbal memory and learning in the main analysis. Early emergence of language-related deficits in TRS patients compared to treatment responders might reflect a greater contribution of neurodevelopmental impairment in the former group.

Interestingly, verbal intelligence and language deficits are among the less distinctive features of the neuropsychological profile of schizophrenia at large (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Morgan, Fearon, Zanelli, Lappin, Dazzan, Morgan and Murray2009; Mesholam-Gately et al., Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman2009). For example, language functions were only the fifth most impaired domain in a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in first-episode schizophrenia patients compared to healthy controls (Mesholam-Gately et al., Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman2009), while premorbid verbal and non-verbal intelligence are equally impaired in population-based studies (Khandaker, Barnett, White, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Barnett, White and Jones2011). A differential salience of language function deficits in the comparative neuropsychological profiles of TRS individuals and of schizophrenia patients at large might be underpinned by a qualitative neuropsychological difference between treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive schizophrenia. If confirmed, such distinction will be of great theoretical and practical interest, for example, in developing aetiological models and personalised medicine in TRS.

Origins of neuropsychological deficits in TRS

The effect sizes that emerged in our meta-analyses (0.27–0.53) are of similar magnitude to those reported in meta-analyses of neuropsychological findings from unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients relative to healthy controls (0.20–0.66) (Bora et al., Reference Bora, Akdede and Alptekin2017; Snitz et al., Reference Snitz, MacDonald and Carter2006). Combined, the above findings raise the possibility of a genetic and cognitive continuum of schizophrenia risk, which increases from undiagnosed community controls to unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients to treatment-responsive schizophrenia patients to TRS patients.

A broader hypothesis is that TRS is aetiologically continuous with treatment-responsive schizophrenia but occupies a more extreme position in a continuum of neurodevelopmental liability. This hypothesis is in keeping with findings relating to predictors of poor therapeutic response, which largely coincide with the defining features of neurodevelopmental schizophrenia (Carbon & Correll, Reference Carbon and Correll2014; Murray et al., Reference Murray, O'Callaghan, Castle and Lewis1992).

The dearth of neuropsychological investigations into the first psychotic episode of patients who develop TRS limits inferences on the origin and stability of the neuropsychological gradient between TRS and treatment-responsive schizophrenia. Based on limited findings to date, this differential is likely to predate clinical onset in relation to language functions (Kravariti et al., Reference Kravariti, Demjaha, Zanelli, Ibrahim, Wise, MacCabe, Reichenberg and Morgan2018).

Methodological considerations

This is the first systematic review and meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in treatment-resistant, relative to treatment-responsive, schizophrenia. The originality of our research undertaking, our systematic methodological approach, and the fair/good quality of the original studies are strengths of the present investigation.

Integrated with earlier research, our findings offer new insights into possible aetiological contributions to TRS, but they need to be viewed in the light of some limitations: Even though the largest effects sizes for verbal memory and learning and for language-related functions were moderate, thus distinguishable from the remaining (small) effect sizes, there was substantial overlap in 95% CIs. This might suggest that the true mean differences between the schizophrenia populations of interest might be less pronounced than our estimates suggest. Our conservative sampling and analytic strategies (biasing estimates towards conservative rather than inflated estimates) are likely to have mitigated this risk.

Our heterogeneity analyses suggested moderate-to-substantial inconsistency of effect sizes for executive function, verbal memory and learning, and language-related functions. This inconsistency was statistically addressed by employing random-effects models (Langan et al., Reference Langan, Higgins, Jackson, Bowden, Veroniki, Kontopantelis and Simmonds2019; Tanriver-Ayder, Faes, van de Casteele, McCann, & Macleod, Reference Tanriver-Ayder, Faes, van de Casteele, McCann and Macleod2021; Veroniki et al., Reference Veroniki, Jackson, Bender, Kuss, Langan, Higgins and Salanti2019), and, where appropriate, by performing meta-regressions (Ryan, Reference Ryan2016). However, the relatively small number of studies (n = 5–10) prevented subgroup analyses and is likely to also explain the lack of statistically significant findings in our meta-regressions.

Traditional conceptualisations of verbal fluency (VF) see VF as primarily an ‘executive function’ in the literature (Henry & Crawford, Reference Henry and Crawford2005; Joyce, Collinson, & Crichton, Reference Joyce, Collinson and Crichton1996). However, more recent factor-analytic evidence suggests that both letter (phonemic) and category (semantic) fluency are more closely related to language than to executive function (Whiteside et al., Reference Whiteside, Kealey, Semla, Luu, Rice, Basso and Roper2016). We addressed this duality by classifying verbal fluency as an executive function in our main analysis and as a language-related function in our sub-analysis.

Most neuropsychological studies of TRS to date have been cross-sectional and included chronic patient samples, which has limited the scope of the present meta-analysis. In the absence of first-episode studies and longitudinal designs, it is difficult to distinguish between neuropsychological deficits that tap into the core pathophysiology of TRS from those secondary to the combined effects of chronicity and persistent poor regulation of clinical symptoms.

A noteworthy limitation of our meta-analysis is the scarcity of publications that differentiated between clozapine-responsive and clozapine-resistant TRS subgroups. The dearth of relevant studies prevented us from addressing the critical importance of clozapine response (Farooq et al., Reference Farooq, Choudry, Cohen, Naeem and Ayub2019; Lindenmayer et al., Reference Lindenmayer, Kulsa, Sultana, Kaur, Yang, Ljuri and Khan2019; Moulier et al., Reference Moulier, Krir, Dalmont, Guillin and Rothärmel2021; Potkin et al., Reference Potkin, Kane, Correll, Lindenmayer, Agid, Marder and Howes2020; Siskind et al., Reference Siskind, Siskind and Kisely2017) in subgroup analyses. Although our sensitivity analysis produced nearly identical results to those of the main analysis, this is a likely reflection of the small number of studies underpinning it.

Conclusions and future directions

Patients with TRS show wide-ranging deficits of small to moderate effect sizes compared to treatment responders, which are most salient in verbal memory and learning and in language functions. The latter is of particular interest to theoretical and research explorations of treatment resistance, as they are likely markers of neurodevelopmental vulnerability to TRS.

A hypothesis that merits exploration in future research is that core deficits in language functions, a neurodevelopmental aetiology, and a primary glutamatergic dysfunction converge into a single model of TRS. In support of a model which helps to bridge glutamate and neurodevelopmental hypotheses of TRS, glutamate is associated with schizophrenia in genetic association analyses (Spangaro et al., Reference Spangaro, Bosia, Zanoletti, Bechi, Cocchi, Pirovano, Lorenzi and Cavallaro2012), with verbal fluency deficits in high-risk individuals (Allen et al., Reference Allen, Chaddock, Egerton, Howes, Barker, Bonoldi and McGuire2015) and, critically, with several language-related neurodevelopmental processes (Lebel, MacMaster, & Dewey, Reference Lebel, MacMaster and Dewey2016; Takenouchi et al., Reference Takenouchi, Hashida, Torii, Kosaki, Takahashi and Kosaki2014).

Future studies should employ longitudinal controlled designs extending from the high-risk to the first episode and onto the chronic stages of TRS, as well as undertake incisive comparisons across treatment-responsive, clozapine-responsive and clozapine-resistant subgroups of schizophrenia patients. Digit Symbol would be an interesting focus of future meta-analyses in TRS. The task taps into a distinct, marked and neurobiologically significant impairment, which exceeds that of other traditional neuropsychological tasks (Dickinson, Ramsey, & Gold, Reference Dickinson, Ramsey and Gold2007). The ultimate goal of the neuropsychological characterisation of TRS is to help advance the translational scope of research into TRS, particularly in relation to personalised medicine. Recent findings have stirred optimism in this direction. For example, a reported significant alteration in cognitive flexibility in TRS is believed to tap into a distinct underlying neurobiological mechanism and may inform future treatment strategies (e.g. glutamatergic targets and giving clozapine earlier in resistant patients) (Horne et al., Reference Horne, Vanes, Verneuil, Mouchlianitis, Szentgyorgyi, Averbeck and Shergill2021). Our findings suggest that language-related tasks, and potentially verbal memory and learning tasks, have likely applications in multidisciplinary strategies to elucidating the pathophysiology of TRS and to developing predictive models and personalised medical approaches.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004128

Acknowledgements

EM's PhD is funded by the MRC-doctoral training partnership studentship in Biomedical Sciences at King's College London. JHM, RMM & EK are part funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. In the past 3 years, S.M.L. has received personal support from Sunovion. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the MRC, the NIHR, Sunovion, or the Department of Health.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Allen, P., Chaddock, C. A., Egerton, A., Howes, O. D., Barker, G., Bonoldi, I., … McGuire, P. (2015). Functional outcome in people at high risk for psychosis predicted by thalamic glutamate levels and prefronto-striatal activation. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(2), 429439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G., & Bland, J. M. (2011). How to obtain the p value from a confidence interval. BMJ, 343, d2304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, V. M., McIlwain, M. E., Kydd, R. R., & Russell, B. R. (2015). Does cognitive impairment in treatment-resistant and ultra-treatment-resistant schizophrenia differ from that in treatment responders? Psychiatry Research, 230(3), 811818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., Akdede, B. B., & Alptekin, K. (2017). Neurocognitive impairment in deficit and non-deficit schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 47(14), 2401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourque, J., Lakis, N., Champagne, J., Stip, E., Lalonde, P., Lipp, O., & Mendrek, A. (2013). Clozapine and visuospatial processing in treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18(6), 615630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbon, M., & Correll, C. U. (2014). Clinical predictors of therapeutic response to antipsychotics in schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 16(4), 505.Google Scholar
Chan, S. K. W., Chan, H. Y. V., Honer, W. G., Bastiampillai, T., Suen, Y. N., Yeung, W. S., … Chang, W. C. (2021). Predictors of treatment-resistant and clozapine-resistant schizophrenia: A 12-year follow-up study of first-episode schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(2), 485494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Technometrics, 31(4), 499500.Google Scholar
de Bartolomeis, A., Balletta, R., Giordano, S., Buonaguro, E. F., Latte, G., & Iasevoli, F. (2013). Differential cognitive performances between schizophrenic responders and non-responders to antipsychotics: Correlation with course of the illness, psychopathology, attitude to the treatment and antipsychotics doses. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 387395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Bartolomeis, A., Prinzivalli, E., Callovini, G., D'Ambrosio, L., Altavilla, B., Avagliano, C., & Iasevoli, F. (2018). Treatment-resistant schizophrenia and neurological soft signs may converge on the same pathology: Evidence from explanatory analysis on clinical, psychopathological, and cognitive variables. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 81, 356366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demjaha, A., Egerton, A., Murray, R. M., Kapur, S., Howes, O. D., Stone, J. M., & McGuire, P. K. (2014). Antipsychotic treatment resistance in schizophrenia associated with elevated glutamate levels but normal dopamine function. Biological Psychiatry, 75(5), e11e13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demjaha, A., Lappin, J. M., Stahl, D., Patel, M. X., MacCabe, J. H., Howes, O. D., Heslin, M., … Murray, R. M. (2017). Antipsychotic treatment resistance in first-episode psychosis: Prevalence, subtypes and predictors. Psychological Medicine, 47(11), 19811989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D., Ramsey, M. E., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Overlooking the obvious: A meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(5), 532542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elkis, H., & Buckley, P. F. (2016). Treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Psychiatric Clinics, 39(2), 239265.Google ScholarPubMed
Farooq, S., Choudry, A., Cohen, D., Naeem, F., & Ayub, M. (2019). Barriers to using clozapine in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: Systematic review. BJPsych Bulletin, 43(1), 816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fatouros-Bergman, H., Cervenka, S., Flyckt, L., Edman, G., & Farde, L. (2014). Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in drug-naïve patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 158(1–3), 156162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A. K. J., Viechtbauer, W., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fioravanti, M., Carlone, O., Vitale, B., Cinti, M. E., & Clare, L. (2005). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review, 15(2), 7395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frydecka, D., Beszłej, J. A., Gościmski, P., Kiejna, A., & Misiak, B. (2016). Profiling cognitive impairment in treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research, 235, 133138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillespie, A. L., Samanaite, R., Mill, J., Egerton, A., & MacCabe, J. H. (2017). Is treatment-resistant schizophrenia categorically distinct from treatment-responsive schizophrenia? A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 17(1), 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M. E., Anderson, V. M., Pillai, A., Kydd, R. R., & Russell, B. R. (2015). Glutamatergic neurometabolites in clozapine-responsive and-resistant schizophrenia. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 18(6), pyu117. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyu117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gong, J., Cui, L. B., Xi, Y. B., Zhao, Y. S., Yang, X. J., Xu, Z. L., … Qin, W. (2020). Predicting response to electroconvulsive therapy combined with antipsychotics in schizophrenia using multi-parametric magnetic resonance imaging. Schizophrenia Research, 216, 262271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harbord, R., Harris, R. J., Sterne, J. A., & Steichen, T. (2009). METABIAS: Stata module to test for small-study effects in meta-analysis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harbord, R. M., & Higgins, J. P. (2008). Meta-regression in stata. The Stata Journal, 8(4), 493519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, R., Bradburn, M., Deeks, J., Altman, D., Harbord, R., Steichen, T., & Sterne, J. (2008). METAN: Stata module for fixed and random effects meta-analysis. Statistical Software Components S456798, Boston College Department of Economics, revised 19 Feb 2007. ideasrepecorg/c/boc/bocode/s456798html (22 July 2010, date last accessed).Google Scholar
Heinrichs, R. W., & Zakzanis, K. K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology, 12(3), 426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, J., & Crawford, J. (2005). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits in schizophrenia relative to other neurocognitive deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(1), 133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horne, C. M., Vanes, L. D., Verneuil, T., Mouchlianitis, E., Szentgyorgyi, T., Averbeck, B., … Shergill, S. S. (2021). Cognitive control network connectivity differentially disrupted in treatment-resistant schizophrenia. NeuroImage: Clinical, 30, 102631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, O. D., Vergunst, F., Gee, S., McGuire, P., Kapur, S., & Taylor, D. (2012). Adherence to treatment guidelines in clinical practice: Study of antipsychotic treatment prior to clozapine initiation. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 201(6), 481485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, J., Zhu, Y., Fan, F., Chen, S., Hong, Y., Cui, Y., Luo, X., … Yuan, Y. (2020). Hippocampus and cognitive domain deficits in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: A comparison with matched treatment-responsive patients and healthy controls✰,✰✰,★,★★. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 297, 111043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iasevoli, F., Avagliano, C., Altavilla, B., Barone, A., Ciccarelli, M., D'Ambrosio, L., Francesco, D. N., … de Bartolomeis, A. (2018a). Evaluation of a few discrete clinical markers may predict categorization of actively symptomatic non-acute schizophrenia patients as treatment resistant or responders: A study by ROC curve analysis and multivariate analyses. Psychiatry Research, 269, 481493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iasevoli, F., D'Ambrosio, L., Francesco, D. N., Razzino, E., Buonaguro, E. F., Giordano, S., … de Bartolomeis, A. (2018b). Clinical evaluation of functional capacity in treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients: Comparison and differences with non-resistant schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research, 202, 217225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iasevoli, F., Fagiolini, A., Formato, M. V., Prinzivalli, E., Giordano, S., Balletta, R., … de Bartolomeis, A. (2017). Assessing patient-rated vs. Clinician-rated adherence to the therapy in treatment-resistant schizophrenia, schizophrenia responders, and non-schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research, 249, 159166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iasevoli, F, Giordano, S., Balletta, R., Latte, G., Formato, M. V., Prinzivalli, E., ... de Bartolomeis, A. (2016). Treatment resistant schizophrenia is associated with the worst community functioning among severely-ill highly-disabling psychiatric conditions and is the most relevant predictor of poorer achievements in functional milestones. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 65, 3448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
John, A. P., Ko, E. K. F., & Dominic, A. (2018). Delayed initiation of clozapine continues to be a substantial clinical concern. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 63(8), 526531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joober, R., Rouleau, G. A., Lal, S., Dixon, M., O'Driscoll, G., Palmour, R., Annablem, L., … Benkelfat, C. (2002). Neuropsychological impairments in neuroleptic-responder vs.-nonresponder schizophrenic patients and healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Research, 53(3), 229238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyce, E. M., Collinson, S. L., & Crichton, P. (1996). Verbal fluency in schizophrenia: Relationship with executive function, semantic memory and clinical alogia. Psychological Medicine, 26(1), 3949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, J., Honigfeld, G., Singer, J., & Meltzer, H. (1988). Clozapine for the treatment-resistant schizophrenic: A double-blind comparison with chlorpromazine. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(9), 789796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Barnett, J. H., White, I. R., & Jones, P. B. (2011). A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 132(2–3), 220227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kontopantelis, E., & Reeves, D. (2010). Metaan: Random-effects meta-analysis. The Stata Journal, 10(3), 395407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kravariti, E., Demjaha, A., Zanelli, J., Ibrahim, F., Wise, C., MacCabe, J. H., Reichenberg, A., … Morgan, C. (2018). Neuropsychological function at first episode in treatment-resistant psychosis: Findings from the ÆSOP-10 study. Psychological Medicine, 49(12), 21002110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kravariti, E., Morgan, K., Fearon, P., Zanelli, J. W., Lappin, J. M., Dazzan, P., Morgan, C., … Murray, R. M. (2009). Neuropsychological functioning in first-episode schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(4), 336345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lally, J., Ajnakina, O., Di Forti, M., Trotta, A., Demjaha, A., Kolliakou, A., Mondelli, V., … Shergil, S. S. (2016a). Two distinct patterns of treatment resistance: Clinical predictors of treatment resistance in first-episode schizophrenia spectrum psychoses. Psychological Medicine, 46(15), 32313240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lally, J., Gaughran, F., Timms, P., & Curran, S. R. (2016b). Treatment-resistant schizophrenia: Current insights on the pharmacogenomics of antipsychotics. Pharmacogenomics and Personalized Medicine, 9, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langan, D., Higgins, J. P., Jackson, D., Bowden, J., Veroniki, A. A., Kontopantelis, E., … Simmonds, M. (2019). A comparison of heterogeneity variance estimators in simulated random-effects meta-analyses. Research Synthesis Methods, 10(1), 8398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrie, S. M., Ingle, G. T., Santosh, C. G., Rogers, A. C., Rimmington, J. E., Naidu, K. P., Best, J. J., … Johnstone, E. C. (1995). Magnetic resonance imaging and single-photon emission tomography in treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 167(2), 202210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebel, C., MacMaster, F. P., & Dewey, D. (2016). Brain metabolite levels and language abilities in preschool children. Brain and Behavior, 6(10), e00547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legge, S., Dennison, C., Pardinas, A., Rees, E., Lynham, A., Hopkins, L., Bates, L., … Walters, J. (2019). Clinical indicators of treatment-resistant psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 216(5), 259266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P., Clarke, M., … Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 62(10), e1e34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, A. S., Chan, H. Y., Peng, Y. C., & Chen, W. J. (2019). Severity in sustained attention impairment and clozapine-resistant schizophrenia: A retrospective study. BMC Psychiatry, 19(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenmayer, J. P., Kulsa, M. K. C., Sultana, T., Kaur, A., Yang, R., Ljuri, I., … Khan, A. (2019). Transcranial direct-current stimulation in ultra-treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Brain Stimulation, 12(1), 5461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, N. S., Badcock, J. C., Clark, M. L., Knowles, E. E., Cadby, G., Melton, P. E., Morgan, V. A., … Jablensky, A. (2018). Assessment of cognition and personality as potential endophenotypes in the western Australian family study of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(4), 908921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Giuliano, A. J., Goff, K. P., Faraone, S. V., & Seidman, L. J. (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 23(3), 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mizuno, Y., McCutcheon, R. A., Brugger, S. P., & Howes, O. D. (2020). Heterogeneity and efficacy of antipsychotic treatment for schizophrenia with or without treatment resistance: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45(4), 622631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & Group, T. P. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed1000097)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mørup, M. F., Kymes, S. M., & Oudin Åström, D. (2020). A modelling approach to estimate the prevalence of treatment-resistant schizophrenia in the United States. PloS one, 15(6), e0234121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mouchlianitis, E., Bloomfield, M. A., Law, V., Beck, K., Selvaraj, S., Rasquinha, N., Waldman, A., … Howes, O. D. (2016). Treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients show elevated anterior cingulate cortex glutamate compared to treatment-responsive. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(3), 744752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulier, V., Krir, M. W., Dalmont, M., Guillin, O., & Rothärmel, M. (2021). A prospective multicenter assessor-blinded randomized controlled study to compare the efficacy of short versus long protocols of electroconvulsive therapy as an augmentation strategy to clozapine in patients with ultra-resistant schizophrenia (SURECT study). Trials, 22(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, R. M., O'Callaghan, E., Castle, D. J., & Lewis, S. W. (1992). A neurodevelopmental approach to the classification of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 18(2), 319332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institutes of Health. (2014a). Study quality assessment tools. National Heart Lung and Blood Institute. Retrieved from https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-pro/guidelines/in-develop/cardiovascular-risk-reduction/tools/cohort (Accessed 2 May 2020).Google Scholar
National Institutes of Health. (2014b). Background: Development and use of study quality assessment tools. National Heart Lung and Blood Institute. Retrieved from https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/node/80102 (Accessed 3 March 2021).Google Scholar
Potkin, S. G., Kane, J. M., Correll, C. U., Lindenmayer, J. P., Agid, O., Marder, S. R., … Howes, O. D. (2020). The neurobiology of treatment-resistant schizophrenia: Paths to antipsychotic resistance and a roadmap for future research. NPJ Schizophrenia, 6(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rakitzi, S., & Georgila, P. (2019). Integrated psychological therapy and treatment-resistant schizophrenia: Initial findings. Psychiatry, 82(4), 354367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R. (2016). Heterogeneity and subgroup analyses in Cochrane consumers and communication group reviews: Planning the analysis at protocol stage. Cochrane Consumers and Communication Review Group, 2016, 29.Google Scholar
Siskind, D., Siskind, V., & Kisely, S. (2017). Clozapine response rates among people with treatment-resistant schizophrenia: Data from a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62(11), 772777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, R. C., Kadewari, R. P., Rosenberger, J. R., & Bhattacharyya, A. (1999). Nonresponding schizophrenia: Differentiation by neurological soft signs and neuropsychological tests. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(4), 813825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snitz, B. E., MacDonald, A. W. III, & Carter, C. S. (2006). Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangaro, M., Bosia, M., Zanoletti, A., Bechi, M., Cocchi, F., Pirovano, A., Lorenzi, C., … Cavallaro, R. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction and glutamate reuptake: Effect of EAAT2 polymorphism in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Letters, 522(2), 151155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterne, J. (2003). METAFUNNEL: Stata module to produce funnel plots for meta-analysis.Google Scholar
Stokes, I., Griffiths, S. L., Jones, R., Everard, L., Jones, P. B., Fowler, D., Hodgekins, J., … Upthegrove, R. (2020). Prevalence of treatment resistance and clozapine use in early intervention services. BJPsych Open, 6(5), E107. doi:10.1192/bjo.2020.89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takenouchi, T., Hashida, N., Torii, C., Kosaki, R., Takahashi, T., & Kosaki, K. (2014). 1p34. 3 deletion involving GRIK3: Further clinical implication of GRIK family glutamate receptors in the pathogenesis of developmental delay. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 164(2), 456460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanriver-Ayder, E., Faes, C., van de Casteele, T., McCann, S. K., & Macleod, M. R. (2021). Comparison of commonly used methods in random-effects meta-analysis: Application to preclinical data in drug discovery research. BMJ Open Science, 5(1), e100074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Üçok, A., Çikrikçili, U., Karabulut, S., Salaj, A., Öztürk, M., Tabak, Ö, & Durak, R. (2015). Delayed initiation of clozapine may be related to poor response in treatment-resistant schizophrenia. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 30(5), 290295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanes, L. D., Mouchlianitis, E., Collier, T., Averbeck, B. B., & Shergill, S. S. (2018a). Differential neural reward mechanisms in treatment responsive and treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 48(14), 2418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanes, L. D., Mouchlianitis, E., Wood, T. C., & Shergill, S. S. (2018b). White matter changes in treatment-refractory schizophrenia: Does cognitive control and myelination matter? NeuroImage: Clinical, 18, 186191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veroniki, A. A., Jackson, D., Bender, R., Kuss, O., Langan, D., Higgins, J. P., … Salanti, G. (2019). Methods to calculate uncertainty in the estimated overall effect size from a random-effects meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 10(1), 2343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, T. P., Wigton, R., Joyce, D. W., Collier, T., Fornito, A., & Shergill, S. S. (2016). Dysfunctional striatal systems in treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(5), 12741285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiteside, D. M., Kealey, T., Semla, M., Luu, H., Rice, L., Basso, M. R., & Roper, B. (2016). Verbal fluency: Language or executive function measure? Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 23(1), 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshimura, B., Yada, Y., So, R., Takaki, M., & Yamada, N. (2017). The critical treatment window of clozapine in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: secondary analysis of an observational study.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. A PRISMA based flow diagram of the literature search strategy and study selection.

Figure 1

Table 1. Cognitive tasks contributing to the main analysis of five primary cognitive domains and the sub-analysis of language-related functions from across primary cognitive domains

Figure 2

Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive schizophrenia samples in the 17 publications

Figure 3

Table 3. Meta-analyses of performance differences between treatment-resistant and treatment-responsive schizophrenia samples across publications

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Main analysis: Forest plots of effect sizes of performance differences between treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant patients in 1. executive function, 2. general cognitive function, 3. attention, working memory and processing speed, 4. verbal memory and learning and 5. visual−spatial memory and learning.

Figure 5

Fig. 3. A funnel plot for all 17 publications was included in the main analysis, with 95% confidence interval limits. Data points at the top of the funnel originate from larger sampled investigations.

Supplementary material: File

Millgate et al. supplementary material

Millgate et al. supplementary material

Download Millgate et al. supplementary material(File)
File 986 KB