Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T02:57:57.212Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food-induced brain responses and eating behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2012

Paul A.M. Smeets*
Affiliation:
Image Sciences Institute, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX, Utrecht, The Netherlands Division of Human Nutrition, Wageningen University and Research Centre, Bomenweg 2, 6703 HD, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Lisette Charbonnier
Affiliation:
Image Sciences Institute, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Floor van Meer
Affiliation:
Image Sciences Institute, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Laura N. van der Laan
Affiliation:
Image Sciences Institute, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Maartje S. Spetter
Affiliation:
Image Sciences Institute, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX, Utrecht, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Paul Smeets, fax+31 302513399, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The brain governs food intake behaviour by integrating many different internal and external state and trait-related signals. Understanding how the decisions to start and to stop eating are made is crucial to our understanding of (maladaptive patterns of) eating behaviour. Here, we aim to (1) review the current state of the field of ‘nutritional neuroscience’ with a focus on the interplay between food-induced brain responses and eating behaviour and (2) highlight research needs and techniques that could be used to address these. The brain responses associated with sensory stimulation (sight, olfaction and taste), gastric distension, gut hormone administration and food consumption are the subject of increasing investigation. Nevertheless, only few studies have examined relations between brain responses and eating behaviour. However, the neural circuits underlying eating behaviour are to a large extent generic, including reward, self-control, learning and decision-making circuitry. These limbic and prefrontal circuits interact with the hypothalamus, a key homeostatic area. Target areas for further elucidating the regulation of food intake are: (eating) habit and food preference formation and modification, the neural correlates of self-control, nutrient sensing and dietary learning, and the regulation of body adiposity. Moreover, to foster significant progress, data from multiple studies need to be integrated. This requires standardisation of (neuroimaging) measures, data sharing and the application and development of existing advanced analysis and modelling techniques to nutritional neuroscience data. In the next 20 years, nutritional neuroscience will have to prove its potential for providing insights that can be used to tackle detrimental eating behaviour.

Type
70th Anniversary Conference on ‘Body weight regulation – food, gut and brain signalling’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Abbreviations:
ALE

activation-likelihood estimation

fMRI

functional MRI

MVPA

multi variate pattern analysis

Food is required for survival and therefore is a primary reward. Abundance of food has become a greater problem than food shortage: the number of overweight and obese people exceeds that of those suffering from undernutrition( 1 ). Obesity is driven by rapid changes in our food environment( Reference Eaton and Eaton 2 , Reference Speakman 3 ) which promote overeating. Such eating behaviour is maladaptive in the longer term. This review addresses the interactions between foods, the gut and the brain, which give rise to eating behaviour (Fig. 1). With ‘eating behaviour’ we refer to food choice, meal frequency and meal size (where a ‘meal’ includes eating occasions such as eating a snack food or drinking something energetic). Eating behaviour is determined by eating decisions, namely what to eat, when to start and when to stop eating. These decisions are taken in the brain, which integrates a multitude of neural and hormonal signals reflecting internal state and the environment. They determine diet nutrient composition, eating frequency and portion size, i.e., a person's diet. Understanding how eating decisions come about is crucial for understanding maladaptive patterns of eating behaviour and improvement of their prevention and remediation. Note that eating decisions are usually not made (entirely) consciously, nor are they necessarily the result of ‘free will’ (see( Reference Levitsky and Pacanowski 4 )).

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of food–gut–brain interactions in relation to eating behaviour together with modulators of brain structure and function. Note that both brain state and brain responses can be affected by trait as well as state factors. Sensory charact., Sensory characteristics.

Relatively recently, neuroimaging techniques such as positron-emission tomography and MRI have enabled studying the brain in vivo. The beauty of MRI is its versatility: many different types of measurements can be obtained with the same machine. In brain research, the most commonly used types of MRI scans are: anatomical scans, showing e.g. grey and white matter; functional (f) MRI scans, either obtained during a task such as looking at food images or during ‘rest’ (resting state fMRI, see( Reference Biswal 5 )), or perfusion scans, which use arterial spin labelling techniques to obtain a semi-quantitative measure of cerebral blood flow; and diffusion-tensor imaging scans, which yield images of the white matter tracts. The most widely used fMRI technique is blood-oxygen level dependent fMRI( Reference Smith 6 ). This form of fMRI exploits the fact that at a site of increased neuronal firing (brain activation), increased local blood flow leads to a decreased concentration of deoxygenated Hb. This in turn attenuates the local distortion of the magnetic field by deoxygenated Hb, which is paramagnetic, and leads to a small increase in the fMRI signal (about 0·5–4%). Only a small percentage of all fMRI studies involve food-induced brain responses; however, a great deal of neuroscientific work has addressed brain systems intimately involved in, or relevant for, eating behaviour. This includes the neurophysiology and functional neuroanatomy of e.g. sensory perception, reward, emotion and decision-making.

Neuroimaging techniques can be used to measure (changes in) brain state (anatomy, resting state) as well as food cue-induced brain responses. These brain characteristics are affected by multiple trait as well as state factors, such as sex, age and BMI, which in turn modulate eating behaviour (Fig. 1). Understanding how eating behaviour is produced requires the integration of neuroimaging data with physiological, psychological and behavioural measures. In particular, linking brain measures to eating behaviour is necessary to be able to interpret neuroimaging findings and assess their real life relevance. Later, we will review the human neuroimaging literature on the interactions between food, gut, brain and eating behaviour and highlight research needs and techniques that could be used to foster progress.

Food–brain interaction

All senses are involved in the perception of foods and the regulation of food intake. Food perception induces innate and learned autonomic anticipatory physiological responses, which are referred to as cephalic phase responses (for reviews see( Reference Zafra, Molina and Puerto 7 , Reference Smeets, Erkner and De Graaf 8 )). In addition to cephalic phase responses, the sensory perception of a food before and during consumption induces numerous brain responses governing food choice and food intake behaviour. Eventually, this results in meal termination and possibly satiety. Of course, once ingestion has started, gastrointestinal neural and hormonal signals also start to contribute. Summaries and detail on the basic processing of food stimuli in the brain can be found in several reviews and meta-analyses addressing visual food stimuli (food images)( Reference van der Laan, de Ridder and Viergever 9 ), odour( Reference Wilson and Sullivan 10 ), taste( Reference Small 11 , Reference Veldhuizen, Albrecht and Zelano 12 ) and flavour (taste, odour and somatosensory stimulation)( Reference Lundstrom, Boesveldt and Albrecht 13 , Reference Small 14 ).

A well-known phenomenon driven by cephalic stimulation is sensory-specific satiety( Reference Rolls, Rolls and Rowe 15 ). Sensory-specific satiety has been demonstrated in the human orbitofrontal cortex for ad libitum consumption( Reference O'Doherty, Rolls and Francis 16 , Reference Kringelbach, O'Doherty and Rolls 17 ). However, further investigation is needed in order to disentangle liking and wanting effects (see e.g.( Reference Spetter, de and Viergever 18 )), and assess correlations with eating behaviour (rather than subjective ratings) when there is no ad libitum consumption. Under such conditions it has proven hard to observe sensory-specific satiety effects in the brain( Reference Smeets, Weijzen and de 19 ).

fMRI studies have shown that anticipation of consumption (food reward) and subsequent consumption (reward receipt) recruit partially different brain areas( Reference Bohon, Stice and Spoor 20 Reference O'Doherty 23 ). The distinction between reward anticipation and reward receipt processing in the brain is particularly relevant for a deeper understanding of aberrant responses to food cues since these could be driven by abnormalities in either one of these processes. For example, neuroimaging studies have shown not only diminished striatal responsivity to reward receipt in obese subjects( Reference Wang, Volkow and Logan 24 , Reference Stice, Spoor and Bohon 25 ) but also hyper-responsivity to both anticipation and consumption of palatable food in somatosensory, gustatory and reward valuation regions( Reference Ng, Stice and Yokum 26 ). Interestingly, a recent study found that the association between brain reward responsivity to imagined consumption and weight gain is modulated by genotype( Reference Stice, Yokum and Bohon 27 ).

There can be strong cognitive effects, so-called top-down effects, on food perception and eating behaviour, and this is also studied with functional neuroimaging. A growing number of studies has addressed effects of (selective) attention to specific product characteristics, such as taste and healthiness( Reference Hare, Malmaud and Rangel 28 , Reference Grabenhorst, Rolls and Bilderbeck 29 ). Other studies involve neuroimaging of the effects of product appearance or product labels( Reference Ng, Stice and Yokum 26 , Reference Plassmann, O'Doherty and Shiv 30 ). These topics are addressed further under section ‘Cognitive effects’.

Gut–brain interaction

The brain receives input from the viscera, including the gastrointestinal tract, and adipose tissue by way of multiple neural and hormonal signals. Most neural information is transmitted by the afferent part of the vagus nerve, which projects to the brain stem where vagal input from each visceral organ is directed to particular subnuclei of the nucleus of the solitary tract as well as integrated with input from other brain regions which regulate autonomic functions and homoeostasis( Reference Travagli, Hermann and Browning 31 , Reference Browning and Travagli 32 ). In addition, many gut peptides and other hormones such as leptin act on vagal afferents, brainstem nuclei and higher brain regions, in particular the hypothalamus, exerting both acute and long-term effects on the regulation of food intake and body weight( Reference Schwartz 33 , Reference Berthoud 34 ). Later, work on the relations between stomach distension and hormones and the human brain is reviewed (for detailed reviews of the latter, see( Reference Gibson, Carnell and Ochner 35 , Reference Schloegl, Percik and Horstmann 36 )).

Stomach distension

An important determinant of meal termination and satiety is stomach distension by the volume (and weight) of food( Reference Rolls and Roe 37 , Reference Powley and Phillips 38 ). To date, surprisingly little studies have investigated the neural correlates of non-painful stomach distension. In neuroimaging studies in which the stomach was distended with a gastric balloon, activation was observed in the brainstem, insula, amygdala, posterior insula, left inferior frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex( Reference Stephan, Pardo and Faris 39 , Reference Wang, Tomasi and Backus 40 ). Moreover, the response in the left amygdala and insula correlated negatively with changes in fullness and positively with changes in plasma ghrelin( Reference Wang, Tomasi and Backus 40 ). In addition, Wang et al.( Reference Wang, Tomasi and Backus 40 ) found that subjects with a higher BMI had a diminished responsivity to stomach distension in the right amygdala and insula. They interpret this as a greater insensitivity to stomach fullness. However, it is hard to rule out differences in stomach volume, which would affect the degree of stretch induced in the stomach wall. Also, a recent animal study has suggested that effects of food-like stomach distension on brain activity may in part be attributable to concomitant transient increases in blood pressure( Reference Min, Tuor and Chelikani 41 ). Future studies should ideally combine fMRI measures of brain activation with MRI measures of gastric volume and data on gastric pressure and stomach emptying, in the case of gastric loads (see e.g.( Reference Kwiatek, Menne and Steingoetter 42 )).

Hormones

For studying hormone–brain interaction, there are several options that are only beginning to be explored. The first is to assess which brain areas respond to a particular hormone by infusing it intravenously and assessing effects on baseline brain activity as well as on task-induced activation, e.g., tasting, smelling, or looking at foods. The same can be done by correlating such brain responses to baseline serum concentrations of hormones or to hormone responses induced by a nutrient load. Such experiments have been done for Peptide YY( Reference Batterham, ffytche and Rosenthal 43 ), cholecystokinin( Reference Dockray 44 , Reference Lassman, McKie and Gregory 45 ), insulin( Reference Guthoff, Grichisch and Canova 46 Reference Wallner-Liebmann, Koschutnig and Reishofer 48 ), glucagon-like peptide 1( Reference De Silva, Salem and Long 49 ), ghrelin( Reference Malik, McGlone and Bedrossian 50 , Reference Jones, McKie and Astbury 51 ) and leptin( Reference Frank, Heni and Moss 52 Reference Rosenbaum, Sy and Pavlovich 54 ). Most of these studies do not link brain (and hormone) responses to actual eating behaviour and this constitutes an important research gap that needs to be addressed if we are to understand the complex interaction between the gut and the brain in relation to (aberrant) eating behaviour. Similarly, the neural effects of ‘anti-obesity’ drugs can and should be assessed not only in animals( Reference Adan, Vanderschuren and Fleur 55 ) but as far as possible also in human subjects (see e.g.( Reference Fletcher, Napolitano and Skeggs 56 )).

Modulating factors

Evidently, there are many, often interrelated, factors which affect the brain response to food cues and ensuing eating behaviour (Fig. 1). Very basic ones are age, sex, BMI and internal state (hunger/satiety). Both age and sex affect brain structure and function, as well as eating behaviour. More specifically, there are many studies showing sex effects on the responses to food, e.g.( Reference Frank, Kim and Krzemien 57 Reference Uher, Treasure and Heining 61 ), and interactions between sex and internal state( Reference Frank, Laharnar and Kullmann 58 , Reference Uher, Treasure and Heining 61 ). Of course, internal state modulates the (brain) response to food, see e.g.( Reference van der Laan, de Ridder and Viergever 9 , Reference Spetter, de and Viergever 18 , Reference Haase, Cerf-Ducastel and Murphy 62 , Reference Bragulat, Dzemidzic and Bruno 63 ), as well as ‘resting’ brain activity (brain perfusion)( Reference Tataranni, Gautier and Chen 64 , Reference Small, Zatorre and Dagher 65 ).

Many functional studies have shown differences between lean and obese subjects (e.g.( Reference Wang, Volkow and Logan 24 Reference Ng, Stice and Yokum 26 )) or correlations between brain responses and BMI (e.g.( Reference Wang, Tomasi and Backus 40 , Reference Yokum, Ng and Stice 66 )). In addition, studies looking at grey and white matter volumes (voxel-based morphometry studies) have reported effects of BMI( Reference Raji, Ho and Parikshak 67 Reference Walther, Birdsill and Glisky 70 ). For example, in obese adults, lower grey matter density was found in brain areas involved in taste perception, reward and behavioural control( Reference Pannacciulli, Del and Chen 69 ). Moreover, several studies have linked brain morphology, in particular impairments in the orbitofrontal cortex, with cognitive performance and eating behaviour( Reference Cohen, Yates and Duong 71 , Reference Maayan, Hoogendoorn and Sweat 72 ). To a large extent, it remains to be determined in how far structural and functional differences between lean and obese subjects are cause or effect, and in how far they are reversible. For example, one study demonstrated that brain measures show partial structural recovery with weight loss( Reference Haltia, Viljanen and Parkkola 73 ); however, a functional study has suggested that aberrant responses to food cues persist after weight loss( Reference DelParigi, Chen and Salbe 74 ). In line with this, hormone levels continue to deviate in post-obese subjects up to 1 year after weight loss( Reference Sumithran, Prendergast and Delbridge 75 ).

Genetic effects

Although functional neuroimaging appears to be a powerful tool to investigate the relations between genes, the brain and behaviour, and many polymorphisms have been implicated in obesity, only few neuroimaging studies have addressed specific polymorphisms implicated in body weight control( Reference Stice, Spoor and Bohon 25 , Reference Stice, Yokum and Bohon 27 ) (both on dopamine receptor polymorphisms). A primary reason for this may be that it is hard to obtain enough suitable subjects. An indirect way of selecting for a particular genotype (usually involving multiple genes) may be to select extreme phenotypes( Reference Nebert 76 ). This is in fact what one does, albeit in a crude way, by selecting on extreme BMI. Thus, it may be worthwhile to select on more specific or additional (endo)phenotypic characteristics, such as personality characteristics or measures of eating behaviour. For example, it was shown that cholecystokinin and leptin (receptor) polymorphisms are associated with meal size and snacking frequency( Reference De Krom, van der Schouw and Hendriks 77 ), which makes these behavioural measures potential selection criteria for a genotype associated with ‘overeating’.

Personality characteristics

Evidence is mounting that variation in brain responses can be explained by differences in personality characteristics( Reference Miller, Donovan and Bennett 78 , Reference Diekhof, Nerenberg and Falkai 79 ). Nevertheless, in spite of neural differences the observed behaviour can be the same( Reference Smeets, Weijzen and de 19 , Reference Diekhof, Nerenberg and Falkai 79 ). For example, Diekhof et al.( Reference Diekhof, Nerenberg and Falkai 79 ) found that the same behavioural performance was subserved by different neural responses in highly impulsive and highly controlled individuals during a task in which subjects were required to decline immediate rewards.

There are several personality characteristics relevant for eating behaviour. These include reward sensitivity, impulsivity and inhibitory potential. How such personality characteristics affect food-induced brain responses has hardly been investigated (one exception is( Reference Beaver, Lawrence and van 80 ) on reward sensitivity). All of them have a bearing on self-regulation capacity and specifically on the ability to resist immediate reward in favour of a longer-term benefit, i.e., on the ability to delay gratification. The neural correlates of self-control, in particular in the food domain, have only been started to be addressed recently( Reference Hare, Camerer and Rangel 81 , Reference Kishinevsky, Cox and Murdaugh 82 ) and the brain mechanisms underlying conscious as well as unconscious self-control and inter-individual differences therein need to be explored in more detail. This constitutes an important research area since knowledge on these mechanisms, in particular those underlying effortless self-control( Reference Fujita 83 ), could be used to develop more effective ways of improving self-control so as to empower individuals to make healthier food choices.

Cognitive effects

Numerous studies show that food characteristics, such as appearance, packaging characteristics (e.g. labelling) and price, can strongly affect expectations, sensory perception and (eating) behaviour (e.g.( Reference Plassmann, O'Doherty and Shiv 30 , Reference Provencher, Polivy and Herman 84 , Reference Wansink and Chandon 85 )). Not surprisingly, this is also apparent in the brain. For example, changing the price label of a wine alters perceived pleasantness as well as taste activation in the medial orbitofrontal cortex( Reference Plassmann, O'Doherty and Shiv 30 ). The nature and extent of such cognitive effects may depend on subject characteristics such as BMI or dietary restraint. For example, a low-fat label increased snack intake more in overweight than in lean subjects( Reference Wansink and Chandon 85 ). Product features affecting perceived healthiness and thereby eating behaviour are of great interest because of their bearing on healthier eating patterns. Nevertheless, such features have only begun to be investigated explicitly on the brain level( Reference Ng, Stice and Yokum 26 , Reference Hare, Malmaud and Rangel 28 , Reference Hare, Camerer and Rangel 81 ).

Another type of cognitive effect is that of (selective) attention (and distraction) on food-induced brain responses and eating behaviour. Many fMRI studies have shown that selective attention increases brain activation in specific areas for taste( Reference Veldhuizen, Bender and Constable 86 Reference Veldhuizen and Small 88 ), odour( Reference Veldhuizen and Small 88 , Reference Rolls, Grabenhorst and Margot 89 ) and food images( Reference Siep, Roefs and Roebroeck 60 , Reference Yokum, Ng and Stice 66 ). So far, attention effects on brain responses remain to be correlated with eating behaviour (but see e.g.( Reference Yokum, Ng and Stice 66 ) on BMI), although evidence from non-imaging studies suggests that such correlations should exist. At the same time, these findings underscore that task design and task instruction can have strong attention-related effects on fMRI results (see also( Reference Bender, Veldhuizen and Meltzer 90 )).

Do brain measures predict eating behaviour?

Neuroimaging studies in which brain responses are correlated with eating behaviour are fairly scarce. Apart from the fact that such correlations may be beyond the scope of the study and increase study complexity, there are several interrelated factors that hamper their study. A principal one is sample size: behavioural studies tend to require more subjects than brain studies and scaling up a brain study can be costly. Furthermore, not only may behavioural measures show considerable variability, the same is true for imaging measures, which makes their combination prone to low statistical power. This can be ameliorated by stringent selection criteria, good fMRI task design and use of dedicated scan sequences. Note, however, that accurate power calculations in fMRI are far from straightforward, among others because many factors are in play, pertaining to hardware specifications, the scan sequence used, experimental design and subject characteristics.

Food choice

Of the eating behaviours defined earlier, food choice, although perhaps not entirely realistic in an MRI environment (or any other laboratory environment), is relatively easy to investigate provided that visual stimuli are used. Basically, there are three types of choice paradigms that have been employed in the food domain: choice (yes/no) for single food items( Reference Hare, Malmaud and Rangel 28 , Reference Hare, Camerer and Rangel 81 , Reference Born, Lemmens and Martens 91 ), forced choice from two options( Reference McClure, Ericson and Laibson 92 ) (using liquids), and choice from a set of options (a menu)( Reference Born, Lemmens and Rutters 93 , Reference Piech, Lewis and Parkinson 94 ). In addition to the relatively small number of studies specifically addressing food choices there is much brain research on decision making and the trade-off between different rewards and types of reward (see e.g.( Reference Peters and Buchel 95 )). In decision neuroscience, the idea that there is a common currency in the brain for different types of reward has received much empirical support( Reference Peters and Buchel 95 ); however, there is evidence that in addition to common areas reflecting stimulus reward value (ventromedial prefrontal cortex and striatum), there are specific brain areas involved in encoding the reward value of primary (food: hypothalamus) and secondary (money: posterior cingulate) rewards( Reference Levy and Glimcher 96 ).

Meal onset

Meal onset is hard to measure in an fMRI paradigm, thus, to our knowledge there are no studies addressing this. There is a fair amount of work though on anticipation and subsequent consumption of foods, as mentioned earlier.

Food intake

To date, only a few studies with different types of stimuli have correlated brain responses with subsequent food intake( Reference Spetter, de and Viergever 18 , Reference Batterham, ffytche and Rosenthal 43 , Reference Born, Lemmens and Martens 91 ). An example is shown in Fig. 2. It seems logical that it is harder to predict intake from anticipatory responses, e.g., responses to food images than from consummatory responses (i.e., tasting, such as in( Reference Spetter, de and Viergever 18 ), Fig. 2). In any case, such studies should be replicated and extended in order to establish whether there are neural markers for general and food-specific appetite. Note that single meals are fairly irrelevant for long-term weight and health outcomes( Reference Booth and Nouwen 97 , Reference Smeets and van der Laan 98 ). Therefore, such laboratory studies need to be complemented with data on actual dietary behaviour. Indeed, it is of great interest to find neural markers for dietary behaviour. Recent studies indicate that this is feasible on the level of BMI: brain reactivity to high-energy foods was found to predict 3-month and 9-month outcome in a weight-loss programme( Reference Murdaugh, Cox and Cook 99 ) and less activation in prefrontal areas during a monetary delayed discounting task predicted a greater rate of weight gain over the next 1–3 years( Reference Kishinevsky, Cox and Murdaugh 82 ). This shows the potential of neuroimaging measures for subject profiling. The next step would be to use this information to improve preventive strategies or treatment.

Fig. 2. Example of a correlation between food-induced brain responses and eating behaviour. The scattergram shows the association between taste-induced brain responses in the anterior cingulate after a 350-ml juice preload and subsequent ad libitum juice intake. Sweet: fruit juice (solid line), r 0·78; Savoury: tomato juice (dashed line), r 0·70. Adapted version of a figure from( Reference Spetter, de and Viergever 18 ).

Better value for research money

Ultimately, one wants to be able to model eating behaviour of an individual in specific contexts, i.e., be able to predict behaviour resulting from the integration of a great array of input signals with a particular set of state and trait characteristics in a brain model. Such a model system would enable improved design of prevention strategies, interventions and treatment of maladaptive patterns of eating behaviour. This requires combining data from multiple studies, which is something that can increase the scientific yield from research in general and neuroimaging studies in particular. For this one needs the following.

Standardisation of measures

In order to facilitate pooling of data, meta-analysis and reduced variability between studies, standardisation of measures is required. Specifically, one could agree on a basic set of measures that is acquired from every subject. In addition, one can then add custom measures tuned to the specific experiment at hand. Basic measures could be, e.g., relevant subject characteristics, beyond already common ones such as sex, age, and BMI. Furthermore, for neuroimaging experiments standardisation of data acquisition, pre-processing and analysis would help to increase consistency( Reference Bennett and Miller 100 ). In addition, it would be helpful if the same (validated) fMRI tasks or the same food stimuli were used for measuring responses to food stimuli or other relevant brain responses such as those related to reward sensitivity or impulsivity. A long-standing example is the International Affective Picture System, a database of >700 pictures( Reference Lang, Greenwald and Bradley 101 , Reference Lang, Bradley and Cuthberth 102 ). Similarly, we are striving to set up a food picture database.

Data sharing

Existing datasets can be exploited further by pooling them, such that variation attributable to measures of interest can be separated from noise and variance resulting from differences in methodology and factors of no interest. The value of a database, whether it contains brain scans or other data, depends on the associated meta-data, such as relevant subject characteristics. Some types of MRI data are easier to share than others; it is relatively easy to share resting state and anatomical data, and this is already fairly well established. The idea to share neuroimaging data (and link databases) is already more than 10 years old( Reference Roland, Svensson and Lindeberg 103 , Reference Amari, Beltrame and Bjaalie 104 ). Nowadays, there are more and more data sharing initiatives and databases are growing week by week. Examples are the International Neuroimaging Data-Sharing Initiative 1000 Functional Connectomes Project (holding resting state data), Open Access Series of Imaging Studies (holding structural MRI data). Proponents of data sharing have demonstrated the benefits( Reference Biswal 5 , Reference Van Horn and Ishai 105 ) and there is growing interest in data sharing in the neuroimaging community. Concurrently, tools for data sharing and management such as COINS (Collaborative Informatics and Neuroimaging Suite)( Reference Scott, Courtney and Wood 106 ) and XNAT (Extensible Neuroimaging Archive Toolkit)( Reference Marcus, Olsen and Ramaratnam 107 ) are under intense development. Unfortunately, for nutritional neuroscientists, these databases are unlikely to contain food-specific meta-data, although they will contain meta-data relevant for food scientists and neuroscientists alike, such as age and sex.

Application and development of (meta)analysis and modelling techniques

Many, if not all, of the meta-analysis and modelling techniques employed or to be employed to neuroimaging and food science data build on existing algorithms or techniques used in other fields. Several techniques of interest are as follows.

Activation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis

Activation-likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis is a technique used to assess common ground between neuroimaging studies on the same task, process or anatomic region. It is a so-called voxel-based meta-analysis technique that determines concurrence in reported peak coordinates between studies( Reference Turkeltaub, Eden and Jones 108 , Reference Eickhoff, Laird and Grefkes 109 ). It is considered to be much more accurate than previous methods, such as counting anatomical labels. However, the concurrence measure (ALE-value) is based on peak proximity and does not take statistical significance of the reported peak voxel into account (e.g., the Z-score) and/or size of the cluster of significant voxels surrounding the peak voxel, or the number of contributing studies. Unfortunately, many ALE meta-analyses fail to report the number of studies/foci contributing to significant clusters. This makes it harder to assess the generalisability of reported clusters, e.g. as in( Reference Veldhuizen, Albrecht and Zelano 12 ). When more studies contribute to an ALE-cluster, more credible or at least generic it is. We addressed this limitation in a meta-analysis of the brain response to food pictures( Reference van der Laan, de Ridder and Viergever 9 ), by ranking clusters on the number of contributing studies. Surprisingly, few clusters (only four) survived a criterion of >33% contributing studies. We anticipate that ALE-meta-analysis will be further developed in the near future so as to overcome its current limitations.

Multivariate pattern analysis

Classic neuroimaging analysis is ‘mass univariate’: every voxel is tested separately, creating the need to correct for multiple comparisons (there are approximately 25,000 brain voxels in a brain sampled in 4 × 4×4 mm voxels). In the 2000s, neuroscientists began to employ multivariate classification algorithms to fMRI data( Reference Haxby, Gobbini and Furey 110 Reference Mourao-Miranda, Bokde and Born 112 ). The main characteristic of multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) is that patterns of activation are analysed instead of single-voxel responses. More specifically, a mathematical model that incorporates the associations between voxel values is used to differentiate between conditions. Several algorithms can be used as the basis of such a model, e.g., support vector machines or simple linear regression. After training the model with a subset of the data, the model can be used to predict responses in other trials. MVPA has been used for ‘brain reading’ or decoding of patterns of activation, such as predicting what a person is looking at from the pattern of activation in the visual cortex( Reference Haxby, Gobbini and Furey 110 , Reference Cox and Savoy 111 ). Another application is the attempt to try and predict subsequent (choice) behaviour from fMRI data obtained during exposure to certain cues( Reference Tusche, Bode and Haynes 113 ), i.e., choice prediction as is commonly done in neuromarketing( Reference Ariely and Berns 114 ). As far as we know, only one study applied MVPA to food choices, so as to predict choice from brain responses to viewing products differing in packaging characteristics( Reference van der Laan, de Ridder and Viergever 115 ). However, the use of MVPA will undoubtedly continue to grow, in particular because MVPA analysis tools have become much more accessible.

Bayesian network modelling

Bayesian network modelling is a modelling technique complementary to classical statistical analysis. A Bayesian network is a probabilistic graphical model, also referred to as a ‘belief network’ or a ‘causal probabilistic network’. Expert knowledge can be used to create network structure and, subsequently, data can be used to calculate the parameters describing the strength of the relations between network nodes. Bayesian networks can learn by the addition of new data. Moreover, by changing specific parameters in the network possible outcomes can be assessed. Alternatively, based on new data the most likely outcomes can be inferred( Reference Hoffer, Feldman and Kim 116 ). Despite a wide application of Bayesian networks in other fields( 117 ), the application to food science is new. A layman's introduction to Bayesian networks and an outline of potential applications in the food domain have been provided recently by Phan et al.( Reference Phan, Garczarek, Dekker, Jaeger and MacFie 118 ). Particularly interesting is the potential to be able to assess product characteristics such as pleasantness, while manipulating other characteristics such as taste or odour intensity or physical product characteristics affecting such characteristics. For example, Phan et al. demonstrated how a combined network model of two different studies measuring determinants of the ad libitum intake of a tomato soup can provide additional insights into the process of satiation( Reference Phan, Ramaekers and Bolhuis 119 ). However, as they indicate there are still significant methodological obstacles that need to be addressed, in particular to facilitate the combination of data from different studies into one model. Ideally, studies should be designed with the integration of data already in mind, i.e., within the broader scope of a wider encompassing model. This will involve obtaining information beyond the scope of single studies, such that the same measures are available from all studies whose data are to be merged, i.e., standardisation. If this is not done, systematic missing data in the combined database will lead to unreliable parameter estimation( Reference Phan, Ramaekers and Bolhuis 119 ).

The incorporation of neuroimaging data seems to be quite a step further, although at present it would seem feasible to employ summary measures of imaging data, e.g. responses in a particular region of interest the brain in a Bayesian network model, in addition to or instead of e.g. subjective ratings. The next step would be to incorporate spatial and/or temporal patterns of brain activation, perhaps by using classifier algorithms or independent component analysis to summarise subject's brain responses.

Future directions

To progress more efficiently in food research in general, and neuroimaging in particular, needs standardisation of measurements, data sharing (neuroimaging data + meta-data), and the application and further development of analysis and modelling techniques to exploit large heterogeneous datasets. While single studies are certainly necessary, data sharing holds great promise for elucidating the effects of multiple factors on eating behaviour. The integration of a wide array of data, ranging from genetic and hormone data to psychological characterisations is needed to be able to assess the effects of manipulations aimed at e.g. changing eating behaviour within a complex of myriad interactions. A first step in this direction would be to create a test battery for profiling individuals so as to steer personalised interventions aimed at changing eating habits.

As yet ill-explored neuroscience research topics that hold great potential for enabling healthier patterns of eating behaviour are (eating) habit and food preference formation, (eating) habit modification, and the neural mechanisms underlying self-control (in particular delayed gratification). Related relevant research topics that have received relatively little or no attention in human nutritional neuroscience are nutrient sensing, dietary learning and the regulation of body adiposity (see( Reference Speakman, Levitsky and Allison 120 )). In the next 20 years, nutritional neuroscience will have to prove its potential for providing insights that can be used to tackle detrimental eating behaviour.

Acknowledgements

This work was partially supported by governmental research grants from the Dutch Technology Foundation STW (grant no. 07438) and NL Agency (grant no. FND08009) and received funding from the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement no. 266408. The authors declare no conflict of interest. P. A. M. S. wrote most of the paper. All other authors contributed to shaping the paper, provided relevant material and improved late versions of the manuscript.

References

1. Reworking the Global Food System (2011). World Disasters Report 2011 – Focus on Hunger and Malnutrition. International Federation of Red Cross and Red Crescent Societies, pp. 1035.Google Scholar
2. Eaton, SB & Eaton, SB, III (2000) Paleolithic vs. modern diets–selected pathophysiological implications. Eur J Nutr 39, 6770.Google Scholar
3. Speakman, JR (2004) Obesity: the integrated roles of environment and genetics. J Nutr 134, 8 Suppl., 2090S2105S.Google Scholar
4. Levitsky, DA & Pacanowski, CR (2012) Free will and the obesity epidemic. Public Health Nutr 15, 126141.Google Scholar
5. Biswal, BB (2012) Resting state fMRI: a personal history. Neuroimage 62, 638644.Google Scholar
6. Smith, K (2012) Brain imaging: fMRI 2·0. Nature 484, 2426.Google Scholar
7. Zafra, MA, Molina, F & Puerto, A (2006) The neural/cephalic phase reflexes in the physiology of nutrition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30, 10321044.Google Scholar
8. Smeets, PA, Erkner, A & De Graaf, C (2010) Cephalic phase responses and appetite. Nutr Rev 68, 643655.Google Scholar
9. van der Laan, LN, de Ridder, DT, Viergever, MA et al. (2010) The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage 55, 296303.Google Scholar
10. Wilson, DA & Sullivan, RM (2011) Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron 72, 506519.Google Scholar
11. Small, DM (2006) Central gustatory processing in humans. Adv Otorhinolaryngol 63, 191220.Google Scholar
12. Veldhuizen, MG, Albrecht, J, Zelano, C et al. (2011) Identification of human gustatory cortex by activation likelihood estimation. Hum Brain Mapp 32, 22562266.Google Scholar
13. Lundstrom, JN, Boesveldt, S & Albrecht, J (2011) Central processing of the chemical senses: an overview. ACS Chem Neurosci 2, 5–16.Google Scholar
14. Small, DM (2012) Flavor is in the brain. Physiol Behav (In the Press).Google Scholar
15. Rolls, BJ, Rolls, ET, Rowe, EA et al. (1981) Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol Behav 27, 137142.Google Scholar
16. O'Doherty, J, Rolls, ET, Francis, S et al. (2000) Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11, 893897.Google Scholar
17. Kringelbach, ML, O'Doherty, J, Rolls, ET et al. (2003) Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb Cortex 13, 10641071.Google Scholar
18. Spetter, MS, de, GC, Viergever, MA et al. (2012) Anterior cingulate taste activation predicts ad libitum intake of sweet and savory drinks in healthy, normal-weight men. J Nutr 142, 795802.Google Scholar
19. Smeets, PA, Weijzen, P, de, GC et al. (2011) Consumption of caloric and non-caloric versions of a soft drink differentially affects brain activation during tasting. Neuroimage 54, 13671374.Google Scholar
20. Bohon, C, Stice, E & Spoor, S (2009) Female emotional eaters show abnormalities in consummatory and anticipatory food reward: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Int J Eat Disord 42, 210221.Google Scholar
21. Small, DM, Veldhuizen, MG, Felsted, J et al. (2008) Separable substrates for anticipatory and consummatory food chemosensation. Neuron 57, 786797.Google Scholar
22. O'Doherty, JP, Deichmann, R, Critchley, HD et al. (2002) Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron 33, 815826.Google Scholar
23. O'Doherty, JP (2004) Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14, 769776.Google Scholar
24. Wang, GJ, Volkow, ND, Logan, J et al. (2001) Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet 357, 354357.Google Scholar
25. Stice, E, Spoor, S, Bohon, C et al. (2008) Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science 322, 449452.Google Scholar
26. Ng, J, Stice, E, Yokum, S et al. (2011) An fMRI study of obesity, food reward, and perceived caloric density. Does a low-fat label make food less appealing? Appetite 57, 6572.Google Scholar
27. Stice, E, Yokum, S, Bohon, C et al. (2010). Reward circuitry responsivity to food predicts future increases in body mass: moderating effects of DRD2 and DRD4. Neuroimage 50, 16181625.Google Scholar
28. Hare, TA, Malmaud, J & Rangel, A (2011) Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. J Neurosci 31, 1107711087.Google Scholar
29. Grabenhorst, F, Rolls, ET & Bilderbeck, A (2008) How cognition modulates affective responses to taste and flavor: top-down influences on the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortices. Cereb Cortex 18, 15491559.Google Scholar
30. Plassmann, H, O'Doherty, J, Shiv, B et al. (2008) Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105, 10501054.Google Scholar
31. Travagli, RA, Hermann, GE, Browning, KN et al. (2006) Brainstem circuits regulating gastric function. Annu Rev Physiol 68, 279305.Google Scholar
32. Browning, KN & Travagli, RA (2011) Plasticity of vagal brainstem circuits in the control of gastrointestinal function. Auton Neurosci 161, 6–13.Google Scholar
33. Schwartz, MW (2006) Central nervous system regulation of food intake. Obesity (Silver Spring) 14, Suppl. 1, 1S8S.Google Scholar
34. Berthoud, HR (2002) Multiple neural systems controlling food intake and body weight. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26, 393428.Google Scholar
35. Gibson, CD, Carnell, S, Ochner, CN et al. (2010) Neuroimaging, gut peptides and obesity: novel studies of the neurobiology of appetite. J Neuroendocrinol 22, 833845.Google Scholar
36. Schloegl, H, Percik, R, Horstmann, A et al. (2011) Peptide hormones regulating appetite-focus on neuroimaging studies in humans. Diabetes Metab Res Rev 27, 104112.Google Scholar
37. Rolls, BJ & Roe, LS (2002) Effect of the volume of liquid food infused intragastrically on satiety in women. Physiol Behav 76, 623631.Google Scholar
38. Powley, TL & Phillips, RJ (2004) Gastric satiation is volumetric, intestinal satiation is nutritive. Physiol Behav 82, 6974.Google Scholar
39. Stephan, E, Pardo, JV, Faris, PL et al. (2003) Functional neuroimaging of gastric distention. J Gastrointest Surg 7, 740749.Google Scholar
40. Wang, GJ, Tomasi, D, Backus, W et al. (2008) Gastric distention activates satiety circuitry in the human brain. Neuroimage 39, 18241831.Google Scholar
41. Min, DK, Tuor, UI & Chelikani, PK (2011) Gastric distention induced functional magnetic resonance signal changes in the rodent brain. Neuroscience 179, 151158.Google Scholar
42. Kwiatek, MA, Menne, D, Steingoetter, A et al. (2009) Effect of meal volume and calorie load on postprandial gastric function and emptying: studies under physiological conditions by combined fiber-optic pressure measurement and MRI. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 297, G894G901.Google Scholar
43. Batterham, RL, ffytche, DH, Rosenthal, JM et al. (2007) PYY modulation of cortical and hypothalamic brain areas predicts feeding behaviour in humans. Nature 450, 106109.Google Scholar
44. Dockray, GJ (2012) Cholecystokinin. Curr Opin Endocrinol Diabetes Obes 19, 8–12.Google Scholar
45. Lassman, DJ, McKie, S, Gregory, LJ et al. (2010) Defining the role of cholecystokinin in the lipid-induced human brain activation matrix. Gastroenterology 138, 15141524.Google Scholar
46. Guthoff, M, Grichisch, Y, Canova, C et al. (2010) Insulin modulates food-related activity in the central nervous system. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 95, 748755.Google Scholar
47. Kroemer, NB, Krebs, L, Kobiella, A et al. . (2012) (Still) longing for food: Insulin reactivity modulates response to food pictures. Hum Brain Mapp (In the Press).Google Scholar
48. Wallner-Liebmann, S, Koschutnig, K, Reishofer, G et al. (2010) Insulin and hippocampus activation in response to images of high-calorie food in normal weight and obese adolescents. Obesity (Silver Spring) 18, 15521557.Google Scholar
49. De Silva, A, Salem, V, Long, CJ et al. (2011) The gut hormones PYY 3–36 and GLP-1 7–36 amide reduce food intake and modulate brain activity in appetite centers in humans. Cell Metab 14, 700706.Google Scholar
50. Malik, S, McGlone, F, Bedrossian, D et al. (2008) Ghrelin modulates brain activity in areas that control appetitive behavior. Cell Metab 7, 400409.Google Scholar
51. Jones, RB, McKie, S, Astbury, N et al. . (2012) Functional neuroimaging demonstrates that ghrelin inhibits the central nervous system response to ingested lipid. Gut (In the Press).Google Scholar
52. Frank, S, Heni, M, Moss, A et al. (2011) Leptin therapy in a congenital leptin-deficient patient leads to acute and long-term changes in homeostatic, reward, and food-related brain areas. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 96, E1283E1287.Google Scholar
53. Farooqi, IS, Bullmore, E, Keogh, J et al. (2007) Leptin regulates striatal regions and human eating behavior. Science 317, 1355.Google Scholar
54. Rosenbaum, M, Sy, M, Pavlovich, K et al. (2008) Leptin reverses weight loss-induced changes in regional neural activity responses to visual food stimuli. J Clin Invest 118, 25832591.Google Scholar
55. Adan, RA, Vanderschuren, LJ & Fleur, El (2008) Anti-obesity drugs and neural circuits of feeding. Trends Pharmacol Sci 29, 208217.Google Scholar
56. Fletcher, PC, Napolitano, A, Skeggs, A et al. (2010) Distinct modulatory effects of satiety and sibutramine on brain responses to food images in humans: a double dissociation across hypothalamus, amygdala, and ventral striatum. J Neurosci 30, 1434614355.Google Scholar
57. Frank, TC, Kim, GL, Krzemien, A et al. (2010) Effect of menstrual cycle phase on corticolimbic brain activation by visual food cues. Brain Res 1363, 8192.Google Scholar
58. Frank, S, Laharnar, N, Kullmann, S et al. (2010) Processing of food pictures: influence of hunger, gender and calorie content. Brain Res 1350, 159166.Google Scholar
59. Smeets, PA, de Graaf, C, Stafleu, A et al. (2006) Effect of satiety on brain activation during chocolate tasting in men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 12971305.Google Scholar
60. Siep, N, Roefs, A, Roebroeck, A et al. (2009) Hunger is the best spice: an fMRI study of the effects of attention, hunger and calorie content on food reward processing in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Behav Brain Res 198, 149158.Google Scholar
61. Uher, R, Treasure, J, Heining, M et al. (2006) Cerebral processing of food-related stimuli: effects of fasting and gender. Behav Brain Res 169, 111119.Google Scholar
62. Haase, L, Cerf-Ducastel, B & Murphy, C (2009) Cortical activation in response to pure taste stimuli during the physiological states of hunger and satiety. Neuroimage 44, 10081021.Google Scholar
63. Bragulat, V, Dzemidzic, M, Bruno, C et al. (2010) Food-related odor probes of brain reward circuits during hunger: a pilot FMRI study. Obesity (Silver Spring) 18, 15661571.Google Scholar
64. Tataranni, PA, Gautier, JF, Chen, K et al. (1999) Neuroanatomical correlates of hunger and satiation in humans using positron emission tomography. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96, 45694574.Google Scholar
65. Small, DM, Zatorre, RJ, Dagher, A et al. (2001) Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain 124, 17201733.Google Scholar
66. Yokum, S, Ng, J & Stice, E (2011) Attentional bias to food images associated with elevated weight and future weight gain: an FMRI study. Obesity (Silver Spring) 19, 17751783.Google Scholar
67. Raji, CA, Ho, AJ, Parikshak, NN et al. (2010) Brain structure and obesity. Hum Brain Mapp 31, 353364.Google Scholar
68. Ho, AJ, Raji, CA, Becker, JT et al. (2011) The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain. Hum Brain Mapp 32, 13711382.Google Scholar
69. Pannacciulli, N, Del, PA, Chen, K et al. (2006) Brain abnormalities in human obesity: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage 31, 14191425.Google Scholar
70. Walther, K, Birdsill, AC, Glisky, EL et al. (2010) Structural brain differences and cognitive functioning related to body mass index in older females. Hum Brain Mapp 31, 10521064.Google Scholar
71. Cohen, JI, Yates, KF, Duong, M et al. (2011) Obesity, orbitofrontal structure and function are associated with food choice: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 1, e000175.Google Scholar
72. Maayan, L, Hoogendoorn, C, Sweat, V et al. (2011) Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity (Silver Spring) 19, 13821387.Google Scholar
73. Haltia, LT, Viljanen, A, Parkkola, R et al. (2007) Brain white matter expansion in human obesity and the recovering effect of dieting. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 92, 32783284.Google Scholar
74. DelParigi, A, Chen, K, Salbe, AD et al. (2004) Persistence of abnormal neural responses to a meal in postobese individuals. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 28, 370377.Google Scholar
75. Sumithran, P, Prendergast, LA, Delbridge, E et al. (2011) Long-term persistence of hormonal adaptations to weight loss. N Engl J Med 365, 15971604.Google Scholar
76. Nebert, DW (2000) Extreme discordant phenotype methodology: an intuitive approach to clinical pharmacogenetics. Eur J Pharmacol 410, 107120.Google Scholar
77. De Krom, M, van der Schouw, YT, Hendriks, J et al. (2007) Common genetic variations in CCK, leptin, and leptin receptor genes are associated with specific human eating patterns. Diabetes 56, 276280.Google Scholar
78. Miller, MB, Donovan, CL, Bennett, CM et al. (2012) Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between individuals. Neuroimage 59, 8393.Google Scholar
79. Diekhof, EK, Nerenberg, L, Falkai, P et al. (2011) Impulsive personality and the ability to resist immediate reward: An fMRI study examining interindividual differences in the neural mechanisms underlying self-control. Hum Brain Mapp (In the Press).Google Scholar
80. Beaver, JD, Lawrence, AD, van, DJ et al. (2006) Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. J Neurosci 26, 51605166.Google Scholar
81. Hare, TA, Camerer, CF & Rangel, A (2009) Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646648.Google Scholar
82. Kishinevsky, FI, Cox, JE, Murdaugh, DL et al. (2012) fMRI reactivity on a delay discounting task predicts weight gain in obese women. Appetite 58, 582592.Google Scholar
83. Fujita, K (2011) On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 15, 352366.Google Scholar
84. Provencher, V, Polivy, J & Herman, CP (2009) Perceived healthiness of food. If it's healthy, you can eat more! Appetite 52, 340344.Google Scholar
85. Wansink, B & Chandon, P (2006) Can ‘low-fat. nutrition labels lead to obesity? J Mark Res 43, 605617.Google Scholar
86. Veldhuizen, MG, Bender, G, Constable, RT et al. (2007) Trying to detect taste in a tasteless solution: modulation of early gustatory cortex by attention to taste. Chem Senses 32, 569581.Google Scholar
87. Grabenhorst, F & Rolls, ET (2008) Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. Eur J Neurosci 27, 723729.Google Scholar
88. Veldhuizen, MG & Small, DM (2011) Modality-specific neural effects of selective attention to taste and odor. Chem Senses 36, 747760.Google Scholar
89. Rolls, ET, Grabenhorst, F, Margot, C et al. (2008) Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes olfactory stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci 20, 18181826.Google Scholar
90. Bender, G, Veldhuizen, MG, Meltzer, JA et al. (2009) Neural correlates of evaluative compared with passive tasting. Eur J Neurosci 30, 327338.Google Scholar
91. Born, JM, Lemmens, SG, Martens, MJ et al. (2011) Differences between liking and wanting signals in the human brain and relations with cognitive dietary restraint and body mass index. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 392403.Google Scholar
92. McClure, SM, Ericson, KM, Laibson, DI et al. (2007) Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci 27, 57965804.Google Scholar
93. Born, JM, Lemmens, SG, Rutters, F et al. (2010) Acute stress and food-related reward activation in the brain during food choice during eating in the absence of hunger. Int J Obes (Lond) 34, 172181.Google Scholar
94. Piech, RM, Lewis, J, Parkinson, CH et al. (2010) Neural correlates of affective influence on choice. Brain Cogn 72, 282288.Google Scholar
95. Peters, J & Buchel, C (2010) Neural representations of subjective reward value. Behav Brain Res 213, 135141.Google Scholar
96. Levy, DJ & Glimcher, PW (2011) Comparing apples and oranges: using reward-specific and reward-general subjective value representation in the brain. J Neurosci 31, 1469314707.Google Scholar
97. Booth, DA & Nouwen, A (2010) Satiety. No way to slim. Appetite 55, 718721.Google Scholar
98. Smeets, PA & van der Laan, LN (2011) Satiety. not the problem, nor a solution. Comment on ‘Satiety. No way to slim’. Appetite 57, 772773.Google Scholar
99. Murdaugh, DL, Cox, JE, Cook, EW, III et al. (2012) fMRI reactivity to high-calorie food pictures predicts short- and long-term outcome in a weight-loss program. Neuroimage 59, 27092721.Google Scholar
100. Bennett, CM & Miller, MB (2010) How reliable are the results from functional magnetic resonance imaging? Ann N Y Acad Sci 1191, 133155.Google Scholar
101. Lang, PJ, Greenwald, MK, Bradley, MM et al. (1993) Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology 30, 261273.Google Scholar
102. Lang, PJ, Bradley, MM & Cuthberth, BN (1999) International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
103. Roland, P, Svensson, G, Lindeberg, T et al. (2001) A database generator for human brain imaging. Trends Neurosci 24, 562564.Google Scholar
104. Amari, S, Beltrame, F, Bjaalie, JG et al. (2002) Neuroinformatics: the integration of shared databases and tools towards integrative neuroscience. J Integr Neurosci 1, 117128.Google Scholar
105. Van Horn, JD & Ishai, A (2007). Mapping the human brain: new insights from fMRI data sharing. Neuroinformatics 5, 146153.Google Scholar
106. Scott, A, Courtney, W, Wood, D et al. . (2011) COINS: an innovative informatics and neuroimaging tool suite built for large heterogeneous datasets. Front Neuroinform 5, 33.Google Scholar
107. Marcus, DS, Olsen, TR, Ramaratnam, M et al. (2007) The extensible neuroimaging archive toolkit: an informatics platform for managing, exploring, and sharing neuroimaging data. Neuroinformatics 5, 1134.Google Scholar
108. Turkeltaub, PE, Eden, GF, Jones, KM et al. (2002) Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage 16, 765780.Google Scholar
109. Eickhoff, SB, Laird, AR, Grefkes, C et al. (2009) Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 29072926.Google Scholar
110. Haxby, JV, Gobbini, MI, Furey, ML et al. (2001) Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293, 24252430.Google Scholar
111. Cox, DD & Savoy, RL (2003) Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) “brain reading”: detecting and classifying distributed patterns of fMRI activity in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 19, 261270.Google Scholar
112. Mourao-Miranda, J, Bokde, AL, Born, C et al. (2005) Classifying brain states and determining the discriminating activation patterns: support vector machine on functional MRI data. Neuroimage 28, 980995.Google Scholar
113. Tusche, A, Bode, S & Haynes, JD (2010) Neural responses to unattended products predict later consumer choices. J Neurosci 30, 80248031.Google Scholar
114. Ariely, D & Berns, GS (2010) Neuromarketing: the hope and hype of neuroimaging in business. Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 284292.Google Scholar
115. van der Laan, LN, de Ridder, DT, Viergever, MA et al. (2012) Appearance matters: Neural correlates of food choice and packaging aesthetics. PLoS One 7, e41738. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041738.Google Scholar
116. Hoffer, EP, Feldman, MJ, Kim, RJ et al. (2005) DXplain: Patterns of Use of a Mature Expert System. AMIA Annu Symp Proc 321325.Google Scholar
117. Bayesian Networks (2008) A Practical Guide to Applications. New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
118. Phan, VA, Garczarek, U, Dekker, M et al. (2010) Bayesian networks for food science: theoretical background and potential applications. In Consumer-driven Innovation in Food and Personal Care Products, pp. 488513. [Jaeger, SR, MacFie, HJ, editors]. Cambridge: Woodhead Publishing Limited.Google Scholar
119. Phan, VA, Ramaekers, MG, Bolhuis, DP et al. (2012) On the use of Bayesian networks to combine raw data from related studies on sensory satiation. Food Quality and Preference 26, 119127.Google Scholar
120. Speakman, JR, Levitsky, DA, Allison, DB et al. (2011) Set points, settling points and some alternative models: theoretical options to understand how genes and environments combine to regulate body adiposity. Dis Model Mech 4, 733745.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of food–gut–brain interactions in relation to eating behaviour together with modulators of brain structure and function. Note that both brain state and brain responses can be affected by trait as well as state factors. Sensory charact., Sensory characteristics.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Example of a correlation between food-induced brain responses and eating behaviour. The scattergram shows the association between taste-induced brain responses in the anterior cingulate after a 350-ml juice preload and subsequent ad libitum juice intake. Sweet: fruit juice (solid line), r 0·78; Savoury: tomato juice (dashed line), r 0·70. Adapted version of a figure from(18).