Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T08:54:48.812Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sex differences in neural correlates of common psychopathological symptoms in early adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 March 2021

Francesca Biondo*
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Charlotte Nymberg Thunell
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare, Socialstyrelsen, Stockholm, Sweden
Bing Xu
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Congying Chu
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Tianye Jia
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK Institute of Science and Technology for Brain-Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai, China Key Laboratory of Computational Neuroscience and Brain-inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Ministry of Education, China
Alex Ing
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Erin Burke Quinlan
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Nicole Tay
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Tobias Banaschewski
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Arun L. W. Bokde
Affiliation:
Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
Christian Büchel
Affiliation:
University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
Sylvane Desrivières
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Herta Flor
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Mannheim, Germany
Vincent Frouin
Affiliation:
NeuroSpin, CEA, Université Paris-Saclay, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
Hugh Garavan
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, USA
Penny Gowland
Affiliation:
Sir Peter Mansfield Imaging Centre School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, UK
Andreas Heinz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité, Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany
Bernd Ittermann
Affiliation:
Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt, Braunschweig and Berlin, Germany
Jean-Luc Martinot
Affiliation:
Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, INSERM U A10 ‘Trajectoires développementales en psychiatrie’, Université Paris-Saclay, Ecole Normale supérieure Paris-Saclay, CNRS, Centre Borelli, Gif sur Yvette, France
Hervé Lemaitre
Affiliation:
Groupe d'Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle, Institut des Maladies Neurodégénératives, CNRS UMR 5293, Université de Bordeaux, Centre Broca Nouvelle-Aquitaine, Bordeaux, France
Frauke Nees
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Medical Center Schleswig Holstein, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany
Dimitri Papadopoulos Orfanos
Affiliation:
NeuroSpin, CEA, Université Paris-Saclay, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
Luise Poustka
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Göttingen, Germany
Sabina Millenet
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Juliane H. Fröhner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany
Michael N. Smolka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany
Henrik Walter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité, Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany
Robert Whelan
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Global Brain Health Institute, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Edward D. Barker
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Gunter Schumann
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS) and Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK PONS Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charite Mitte, Humboldt University, Berlin and Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany, and Institute for Science and Technology of Brain-inspired Intelligence (ISTBI), Fudan University, Shanghai, P.R. China
*
Author for correspondence: Francesca Biondo, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Sex-related differences in psychopathology are known phenomena, with externalizing and internalizing symptoms typically more common in boys and girls, respectively. However, the neural correlates of these sex-by-psychopathology interactions are underinvestigated, particularly in adolescence.

Methods

Participants were 14 years of age and part of the IMAGEN study, a large (N = 1526) community-based sample. To test for sex-by-psychopathology interactions in structural grey matter volume (GMV), we used whole-brain, voxel-wise neuroimaging analyses based on robust non-parametric methods. Psychopathological symptom data were derived from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ).

Results

We found a sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in four brain clusters: right temporoparietal-opercular region (p < 0.01, Cohen's d = −0.24), bilateral anterior and mid-cingulum (p < 0.05, Cohen's d = −0.18), right cerebellum and fusiform (p < 0.05, Cohen's d = −0.20) and left frontal superior and middle gyri (p < 0.05, Cohen's d = −0.26). Higher symptoms of hyperactivity/inattention were associated with lower GMV in all four brain clusters in boys, and with higher GMV in the temporoparietal-opercular and cerebellar-fusiform clusters in girls.

Conclusions

Using a large, sex-balanced and community-based sample, our study lends support to the idea that externalizing symptoms of hyperactivity/inattention may be associated with different neural structures in male and female adolescents. The brain regions we report have been associated with a myriad of important cognitive functions, in particular, attention, cognitive and motor control, and timing, that are potentially relevant to understand the behavioural manifestations of hyperactive and inattentive symptoms. This study highlights the importance of considering sex in our efforts to uncover mechanisms underlying psychopathology during adolescence.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Heterogeneity in mental disorders is a significant and largely unresolved problem that stands in the way of the development of predictors and targeted therapies. A clear example of this heterogeneity is sex-related, which is a ubiquitous phenomenon in psychopathology – many disorders show uneven sex distributions of prevalence, symptoms, age of onset and treatment response (Martel, Reference Martel2013; Paus, Wong, Syme, & Pausova, Reference Paus, Wong, Syme and Pausova2017). For example, externalizing disorders are more frequently observed in males than females, whilst the reverse pattern is true for internalizing disorders (Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Teesson, Sunderland, Newton, Krueger, Conrod and Slade2016; Martel, Reference Martel2013). Previous research suggests that brain differences in males and females may be related to differences in the expression of both externalizing and internalizing forms of psychopathology (Giedd, Castellanos, Rajapakse, Vaituzis, & Rapoport, Reference Giedd, Castellanos, Rajapakse, Vaituzis and Rapoport1997; Kaczkurkin, Raznahan, & Satterthwaite, Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019; Lenroot et al., Reference Lenroot, Gogtay, Greenstein, Wells, Wallace, Clasen and Giedd2007). This underscores the importance of identifying sex-by-psychopathology interaction patterns as manifested in the brain. The timing of the development of sex differences is important. Brain and behavioural sex differences are particularly noticeable during developmental stages such as childhood and adolescence (Paus et al., Reference Paus, Wong, Syme and Pausova2017). Moreover, although psychopathology can be observed early in childhood (Egger & Angold, Reference Egger and Angold2006), widespread vulnerability to psychopathology becomes most apparent in adolescence (Paus, Keshavan, & Giedd, Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008; Schumann et al., Reference Schumann, Loth, Banaschewski, Barbot, Barker, Büchel and Struve2010), with half of the lifetime psychopathological burden detectable by the mid-teens and 75% by the mid-20s (Gore et al., Reference Gore, Bloem, Patton, Ferguson, Joseph, Coffey and Mathers2011; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Amminger, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee and Üstün2007). Hence, adolescence is a time when both sex differences and psychopathology are marked, making adolescence an important window to investigate potential sex differences in the neural correlates of these disorders. Advancing the understanding of sex-bias in common psychopathology may aid in the development of effective screening, intervention strategies and health policy decisions.

Previous research attempts to uncover sex-by-psychopathology interactions in the brain exist. For example, depression and low positive attributes have been found to associate with lower grey matter (GM) in limbic regions in females (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Chen, Womer, Jiang, Luo, Driesen and Wang2013) and lower GM in striatal regions in males (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Chen, Womer, Jiang, Luo, Driesen and Wang2013). In conduct disorder, sex differences were reported in parietal and frontal regions with boys showing smaller cortical thickness, higher gyrification and higher surface area compared to controls, whilst girls showed the opposite pattern (Frere et al., Reference Frere, Vetter, Artiges, Filippi, Miranda, Vulser and Lemaître2020; Smaragdi et al., Reference Smaragdi, Cornwell, Toschi, Riccelli, Gonzalez-Madruga, Wells and Fairchild2017). However, these previous studies are sparse and have various limitations that leave the search for sex-by-psychopathology interactions in the brain elusive. Limitations include power-related issues driven by small sample sizes and age or sex-dependent confounds driven by wide age ranges and unbalanced sex groups (Abi-Dargham & Horga, Reference Abi-Dargham and Horga2016; Kaczkurkin et al., Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Onnink, van Donkelaar, Wolfers, Harich, Shi and Franke2017; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Chen, Womer, Jiang, Luo, Driesen and Wang2013; Lenroot et al., Reference Lenroot, Gogtay, Greenstein, Wells, Wallace, Clasen and Giedd2007; Rutter, Caspi, & Moffitt, Reference Rutter, Caspi and Moffitt2003; Smaragdi et al., Reference Smaragdi, Cornwell, Toschi, Riccelli, Gonzalez-Madruga, Wells and Fairchild2017; Valera et al., Reference Valera, Brown, Biederman, Faraone, Makris, Monuteaux and Seidman2010; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Llera, Hoogman, Cao, Zwiers, Bralten and Wang2019). Also, one of the dominant methodological approaches is region-of-interest (ROI) analyses, which whilst being informative, is less useful in generating new findings and hypotheses outside of the pre-selected ROIs. Voxel-based morphometry offers an alternative whole-brain approach that does not require the a priori selection of ROIs (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2000). Furthermore, these studies often rely on case–control designs, which assume a categorical distribution of psychopathology, an assumption that has been contested with data suggesting that symptoms may be spread over a spectrum instead (Kaczkurkin et al., Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Onnink, van Donkelaar, Wolfers, Harich, Shi and Franke2017; Marquand, Rezek, Buitelaar, & Beckmann, Reference Marquand, Rezek, Buitelaar and Beckmann2016).

This study aims to investigate sex-by-psychopathology interactions in the brain structure of young adolescents. To do so we investigate GMV changes given that this brain measure has been previously associated with a myriad of psychiatric diagnoses (Goodkind et al., Reference Goodkind, Eickhoff, Oathes, Jiang, Chang, Jones-Hagata and Etkin2015). As a measure of psychopathology, we look at common externalizing and internalizing symptoms as these have been previously reported to show sex differences (Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Teesson, Sunderland, Newton, Krueger, Conrod and Slade2016; Martel, Reference Martel2013). Importantly, this study overcomes the limitations of previous reports mentioned above by way of the sample characteristics and methodological approaches it employs. We utilise IMAGEN, a sex-balanced sample of approximately 2000 adolescents within a focused age range of around 14 years (Schumann et al., Reference Schumann, Loth, Banaschewski, Barbot, Barker, Büchel and Struve2010). IMAGEN is a community-based sample that allows us to examine the symptoms of psychopathology as traits, which are assumed to be continuously, rather than categorically, distributed and hence are a closer representation of the distribution of symptoms in the general population (Kaczkurkin et al., Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019; Marquand et al., Reference Marquand, Rezek, Buitelaar and Beckmann2016). Considering the time between the occurrence of first symptoms and the first contact with psychiatric services generally spans several years, investigating symptoms rather than fully-fledged clinical cases may offer important novel insights into their aetiology. Importantly, instead of testing hypotheses via an ROI approach, we use an exploratory whole-brain method to maximise the discoverability of associated brain areas. At the same time, we use robust and conservative methods to minimise false positives, for example, by using non-parametric statistical brain analyses, correcting for multiple comparisons and including various covariates. Finally, a secondary aim of this study is to confirm whether sex differences exist at the symptom-behavioural level and we hypothesise that externalizing and internalizing symptoms will be higher in boys and girls, respectively.

Methods

Participants

The IMAGEN sample was recruited and tested across eight European sites. Recruitment was achieved via high schools with two broad aims: (i) reduce genetic diversity by maximizing European descent homogeneity, and (ii) maximise socioeconomic status (SES), emotional and cognitive development diversity by sampling equally from private, state-funded and special units. Details on recruitment and assessment can be found elsewhere (Schumann et al., Reference Schumann, Loth, Banaschewski, Barbot, Barker, Büchel and Struve2010). At each site, the study was approved by the appropriate Ethics boards. Informed assent and consent were obtained from the participant and their legal guardian, respectively. The community-based sample comprised of adolescents aged 13.3–15.5 years (Fig. 1). Participants were removed if they had missing data for the following variables: psychopathological symptoms, puberty score, age, SES, if they displayed a verbal or reasoning IQ score below 75 on the WISC-IV or if neuroimaging data were either missing or failed quality control, reducing the original sample of N = 2090 to N = 1526 (Supplementary material). For the behavioural analyses alone, a larger subset of the data (N = 2046) was available (Supplementary material). The socioeconomic and housing subscale of the Family Stresses questionnaire (Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward, & Meltzer, Reference Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward and Meltzer2000) was used as an index of SES (Supplementary material). Sex was self-reported (male or female). A puberty score was obtained from the Puberty Development Scale that is based on the development of external primary and secondary sex characteristics (Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988).

Fig. 1. Distribution of covariates. The pyramid plots show the distribution of puberty score, TIV, SES and age split by sex (girls, N = 801 in grey; boys, N = 725 in black). Means, standard deviation (s.d.), median, observed minimum and maximum are presented for each raw (non-residualised) covariate. Handedness (not illustrated) was 89.4% right and 10.6% left/ambidextrous for girls and, 85.8% right and 14.2% left/ambidextrous for boys. Additional plots using the maximum available cases for behavioural data (N = 2046) can be found in online Supplementary Fig. S1.

Psychopathology

Psychopathological symptoms were assessed using the self-rated Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997), a brief behavioural screening tool with five subscales and five items per subscale (Supplementary material). We focused on the four subscales measuring difficult behaviour: hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, emotional problems and peer problems (Fig. 2). These can be split into two categories, externalizing symptoms (hyperactivity/inattention; conduct) and internalizing symptoms (emotional; peer) (Goodman, Lamping, & Ploubidis, Reference Goodman, Lamping and Ploubidis2010).

Fig. 2. Distributions of the four psychopathological symptoms subscales of the SDQ. Pyramid plots are split by sex (girls, N = 801 in grey; boys, N = 725 in black). Significant sex differences were found for conduct problems (higher in boys), emotional difficulties (higher in girls) and peer problems (higher in boys). The plots show raw (non-residualised) versions of the variables. Full GLM details can be found in online Supplementary Table S3a. Additional variants of these plots and analyses can be found in Supplementary materials (online Supplementary Fig. S2, online Supplementary Tables S3b–S3c).

Neuroimaging

The MRI scanners used across the eight sites were 3-Tesla and of various manufacturers (Siemens, Philips, GE, Bruker). To ensure comparability across sites, scan parameters were standardised to the highest degree possible and were optimised based on the ADNI1 protocol (http://adni.loni.usc.edu/methods/documents/mri-protocols/) to give good agreement in the final images despite scanner differences. T1-weighted images were spatially normalised and segmented into GM using a voxel-wise-based morphometry (VBM) pipeline involving unified segmentation (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2005), a diffeomorphic registration (Ashburner, Reference Ashburner2007) within the SPM software (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) and smoothing to 8 mm (see Supplementary materials, for neuroimaging data acquisition and preprocessing details).

Statistical analyses

To address the main research question of whether sex-by-psychopathology interactions are detected for GM outcomes, we ran four general linear models (GLM), one for each of the four psychopathological symptom subscale scores (hyperactivity/inattention, conduct, emotional and peer problems) with GMV at the voxel-level as the dependent variable. The sex-by-psychopathology interaction term was calculated by multiplying sex to the relevant subscale score and used as the covariate of interest. Sex, the relevant subscale score, site, handedness, age, puberty, SES and total intracranial volume (TIV) were added as covariates (Figs 1 and 2). Also, considering that the four psychopathological symptom subscales were significantly correlated with each other (online Supplementary Table S1), each one of the four GLMs included the remaining three subscales as covariates (design matrix: online Supplementary Fig. S3). A version of the analyses excluding the other subscales as covariates can be found in Supplementary materials. These GLMs were implemented across the whole brain, using voxel-wise analyses using a threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) approach (Smith & Nichols, Reference Smith and Nichols2009). Cluster-wise inference is known to have higher sensitivity compared to voxel-wise tests when the signal is spatially distributed (Woo, Krishnan, & Wager, Reference Woo, Krishnan and Wager2014). One common way to run cluster-wise analysis is via the SPM random-field approach, however, to avoid problems related to this method (Eklund, Nichols, & Knutsson, Reference Eklund, Nichols and Knutsson2016) and avoid the arbitrariness of setting a cluster-forming threshold as well as ensure appropriate correction for the family-wise error (FWE), a robust non-parametric TFCE approach was used and implemented via FSL software's (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/) randomise function (Winkler, Ridgway, Webster, Smith, & Nichols, Reference Winkler, Ridgway, Webster, Smith and Nichols2014). The null distribution was generated using 10 000 permutations of the experimental labelling with adjustment for the FWE using a threshold of p < 0.05. Given that our analysis was exploratory, we tested for both directed contrasts (negative and positive associations with GMV) and hence used two-tailed statistical significance values. In addition, we corrected for multiple comparisons experiment-wise (across the four behavioural symptom subscales) using Bonferroni correction by multiplying the p value by four (i.e. number of tests conducted).

To test the direction of any resulting interaction, a post-hoc test was run by splitting the sample by sex and running a GLM for each significant cluster, with GMV in each cluster as the dependent variable, the relevant psychopathology symptom subscale (hyperactivity/inattention) as the covariate of interest and site, handedness, age, puberty, SES, TIV and the remaining three psychopathology symptom subscales as covariates. To test if an interaction effect was also global, not just regional, a second post-hoc test used an analogous GLM on the whole sample using whole-brain GMV as the dependent variable and sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention as the covariate of interest. A third post-hoc test investigated whether the sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention GMV interaction within the significant clusters remained significant even when covarying for an additional measure of brain volume, normalised brain volume, corresponding to the sum of the volumes of grey and white matter, divided by TIV. See Supplementary materials for post-hoc test details.

Finally, a secondary analysis was carried out to investigate sex differences within each of the four subscale symptoms scores and test if, as expected from the literature, the externalizing symptoms subscale scores (hyperactivity/inattention, conduct) and internalizing symptoms subscale scores (emotional, peer problems) were higher in boys and girls, respectively. This was achieved using four, Bonferroni-corrected, GLMs with the relevant subscale score as the dependent variable, sex as the covariate of interest and site, handedness, age, puberty, SES, TIV and the remaining three subscale scores as covariates. GLMs that yielded null findings were supplemented with Bayesian linear regressions to quantify the probability of the null finding. A Bayesian framework is different from a frequentist (p value-oriented) approach. The former uses observed data to calculate probability estimates for different hypotheses, reflecting which of these has higher or lower credibility (Van Den Bergh et al., Reference Van Den Bergh, Van Doorn, Marsman, Draws, Van Kesteren, Derks and Wagenmakers2020). Bayes factors (BF) were calculated using the ‘lmBF’ function with default settings from the BayesFactor package (Morey & Rouder, Reference Morey and Rouder2018) in R (R Core Team, 2019). The Bayes quotient, which allows to make an inference on the relative importance of a covariate, was calculated by dividing the BF of a full model (all covariates) by the BF of a reduced model (all covariates minus sex).

Results

We report a sex-by-psychopathology interaction in GMV with hyperactivity/inattention problems in four brain clusters: (i) a right cluster including the superior temporal gyrus and extending to the supramarginal, inferior parietal Heschl gyrus, and rolandic operculum; (ii) a frontal medial cluster including the bilateral cingulum anterior and extending to the mid-cingulum; (iii) a right cluster involving the cerebellum, fusiform and the inferior temporal gyrus; (iv) a left frontal cluster including the superior and middle frontal gyri and extending to the precentral gyrus (Table 1, Fig. 3, see online Supplementary Fig. S5 for the unthresholded statistical map). We report two additional small, left clusters; however, these did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (experiment-wise) (Table 1). Excluding the other psychopathology symptom subscales as covariates in the analysis did not change the overall pattern of our findings (online Supplementary Tables S2, S3c, online Supplementary Fig. S4). The GLMs using the remaining three psychopathological symptoms subscales (conduct, emotion and peer problems) did not show any significant sex-by-psychopathology interactions in GMV.

Fig. 3. Sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in GMV. The image illustrates coronal (top row), axial (middle row) and sagittal slices (bottom row) with coloured maps representing binary maps for the four statistically significant clusters indicating a negative association between GMV and symptom scores of hyperactivity/inattention in boys, and a positive association in girls for the temporoparietal-opercular and fusiform and cerebellar clusters. Green = cluster 1, right temporoparietal-opercular region; pink = cluster 2, cingulum; blue = cluster 3, right fusiform and cerebellum; red = cluster 4, frontal superior. For detailed anatomical descriptions, refer to Table 2 and the ‘Results’ section. For an unthresholded map and non-significant clusters, see Supplementary materials (online Supplementary Figs S5–S6). Maps are overlaid on a T1-weighted brain image made from 500 T1-weighted scans from the study sample. Images were produced using MRIcron (www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron).

Table 1. Sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in voxel-wise GMV

This table shows the six brain clusters that resulted from a voxel-wise analysis with a TFCE approach, indicating sex-by-hyperactivity interactions in GMV which were statistically significant for the first four clusters. In boys, a negative association between hyperactivity and inattentive symptoms and GMV was found in all four brain regions. In girls, the association was positive and significantly so in the temporal superior and cerebellar-fusiform clusters (see Fig. 4 for post-hoc tests revealing the direction of these interactions). Unadjusted p values are corrected for the FWE within the brain statistical map at an α = 0.05, adjusted p values are additionally corrected for experiment-wise multiple comparisons across the four subscales using Bonferroni correction. The adjusted p values that are statistically significant are marked with ** (p < 0.01) and * (p < 0.05). Anatomical descriptions were achieved using the AAL (Anatomical Automatic Labelling) and BA (Brodmann Area) atlases provided in MRIcron v.2016 (www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron) (Rorden, Karnath, & Bonilha, Reference Rorden, Karnath and Bonilha2007). For additional details see the ‘Results’ section. k = cluster size; degrees of freedom (df) = 1507; p value (2-tailed) with α = 0.05.

Post-hoc tests indicated that the associations between hyperactivity/inattentive symptoms and GMV were negative for boys (negative t-statistics with a Cohen's d ranging from d = −0.20 to d = −0.27), positive for girls (positive t-statistics, d = 0.13 to d = 0.19) and the directions of these associations were significant for all clusters (p < 0.05) except for two clusters for girls, the fusiform-cerebellar and cingulum, which were at borderline significance (Fig. 4). The second post-hoc test revealed that sex-by-hyperactivity was significantly associated with whole-brain GMV (β = −0.86, p = 0.0025, d = −0.16). The third post-hoc test showed that sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in GMV within the significant clusters remained significant even when covarying for normalised brain volume (β = −6.22, p = 6.27 × 10−10, d = −0.32). The latter result suggests that the main findings at the voxel-level were sufficiently robust, surviving additional brain volume correction.

Fig. 4. Direction of the sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in voxel-wise GMV. The scatterplots illustrate the residual of GMV on the y-axis for each one of the four brain clusters showing a significant sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction, and hyperactivity/inattention symptoms score on the x-axis, stratified by sex (girls in filled red circles; boys in empty blue triangles). At the top and right side of each scatterplot are density plots illustrating the distribution of hyperactivity/inattention symptoms score and standardised residuals, respectively. For boys, there was a negative association between hyperactivity/inattention symptoms and GMV in all four brain regions (negative t-values, p < 0.05). For girls, this association was positive but statistically significant for only the temporoparietal-opercular and frontal superior clusters (positive t values, p < 0.05). The standardised residuals were calculated using a GLM with GMV at each cluster as the dependent variable and sex, site, handedness, age, puberty, SES, TIV, conduct, peer and emotion problems as covariates.

The secondary analysis, which tested sex differences in each psychopathological symptoms subscale, revealed that conduct problems (β = 0.13, r = 0.12, d = 0.24, p adj = 1.20 × 10−5) and peer problems were significantly higher in boys (β = 0.17, r = 0.15, d = 0.30, p adj = 2.21 × 10−8) compared to girls, whilst emotional problems were higher in girls (β = −0.36, r = −0.33, d = −0.69, p adj = 6.15 × 10−39) compared to boys (Fig. 2). The hyperactivity/inattention problems subscale did not show a significant sex difference and using the maximum number of complete cases available for behavioural data and/or excluding psychopathology subscales as covariates did not change the overall pattern of our findings (online Supplementary Tables S3a–S3c). A Bayesian version of the latter analysis revealed that the data are 5.73 times more likely under the reduced model (no sex), compared to the full model. This suggests that sex is not a meaningful predictor of hyperactivity/inattention whilst accounting for the other covariates.

Discussion

We investigated the neural correlates of sex differences of common psychopathological symptoms in 14-year-old adolescents. For the first time, using a whole-brain approach within a large-scale, sex-balanced, community-based sample, we found a sex-by-psychopathology interaction effect. This main finding indicated a negative association between GMV in four brain regions and hyperactivity/inattention symptoms in boys, and a positive association in two out of these four regions in girls. A secondary set of findings includes sex differences in symptoms of psychopathology, with higher scores for boys in conduct and peer problems and higher scores for girls in emotional problems. All results reflect associations with psychopathology symptoms subscales that have been covaried out of the other three subscales. Nonetheless, excluding these covariates in the analyses did not change the overall pattern of our findings. The differences in GMV that we report may reflect impaired processing in functions associated with the affected brain regions, including the manifestation of the observed hyperactive and inattentive symptoms. However, our study does not permit making any direct brain–behaviour links (Poldrack, Reference Poldrack2011) and these GMV differences may be due to other reasons, for example, as a consequence rather than a cause of these symptoms. What follows are descriptions of the behavioural relevance of our brain results and given the sparsity of sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention findings, we interpret these with respect to the wider literature on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

The first brain cluster involved a right temporoparietal-opercular region. Abnormalities in this region (amongst others) have been previously reported in both structural neuroimaging studies on ADHD showing lower GMV (Kobel et al., Reference Kobel, Bechtel, Specht, Klarhöfer, Weber, Scheffler and Penner2010; Villemonteix et al., Reference Villemonteix, De Brito, Kavec, Balériaux, Metens, Slama and Massat2015a), as well as functional neuroimaging studies on ADHD showing decreased activation during attentional tasks (Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013). One important function associated with the temporoparietal-opercular region is attentional processing (Corbetta & Shulman, Reference Corbetta and Shulman2002). Also, other studies have previously linked abnormalities in this region to a reduced ability to ignore distractors and detect salient stimuli (Kobel et al., Reference Kobel, Bechtel, Specht, Klarhöfer, Weber, Scheffler and Penner2010; Rubia, Smith, Brammer, & Taylor, Reference Rubia, Smith, Brammer and Taylor2007) hence fitting with symptoms of inattention. Another function associated with this region is secondary sensorimotor processing, whereby sensory (e.g. tactile) stimuli are integrated with cognitive control (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Babiloni, Ferretti, Perrucci, Romani, Rossini and Del Gratta2008), which are also disrupted in ADHD (McLeod, Langevin, Goodyear, & Dewey, Reference McLeod, Langevin, Goodyear and Dewey2014). Of note, this result corroborates findings from healthy brain development that indicate less GMV in this brain region (amongst others), in boys when compared to girls (Ruigrok et al., Reference Ruigrok, Salimi-Khorshidi, Lai, Baron-Cohen, Lombardo, Tait and Suckling2014). This may suggest the possibility of an ‘extreme male brain’ developmental trajectory playing a part in explaining our finding (Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Lombardo, Auyeung, Ashwin, Chakrabarti and Knickmeyer2011) such that biological ‘maleness’ (e.g. exposure to foetal testosterone, expression of the Y chromosome) may lead to structural differences (e.g. via decreased GMV in the temporoparietal-opercular region), which in turn increases the risk of exhibiting certain, in this case, inattentive and hyperactive behaviours. However, our findings are insufficient to add weight to this hypothesis and further investigations are warranted.

The second cluster involved the anterior and mid-cingulum. Core abnormalities in the cingulum have been previously reported for individuals with ADHD (Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Wilke, Flechtner, & Krauel, Reference Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Wilke, Flechtner and Krauel2018; Bralten et al., Reference Bralten, Greven, Franke, Mennes, Zwiers, Rommelse and Buitelaar2016; Bush et al., Reference Bush, Frazier, Rauch, Seidman, Whalen, Jenike and Biederman1999; Dirlikov et al., Reference Dirlikov, Shiels Rosch, Crocetti, Denckla, Mahone and Mostofsky2015; Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012; Liddle et al., Reference Liddle, Hollis, Batty, Groom, Totman, Liotti and Liddle2011; McLeod et al., Reference McLeod, Langevin, Goodyear and Dewey2014) including a sex-by-ADHD interaction in boys (Villemonteix et al., Reference Villemonteix, De Brito, Slama, Kavec, Balériaux, Metens and Massat2015b). Recent developments suggest that the cingulum is a key component in a distributed network in decision making and high-level control of action (Heilbronner & Hayden, Reference Heilbronner and Hayden2016; Hunt & Hayden, Reference Hunt and Hayden2017) including monitoring (e.g. error monitoring), controlling (e.g. response selection and motor control) and evaluating value. Hence, disruption of these high-level operations is consistent with the attentional and impulsive impairments of ADHD (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Frazier, Rauch, Seidman, Whalen, Jenike and Biederman1999). We further note that together, the anterior cingulum and the right temporoparietal-opercular region, in particular the insula, overlap with the salience network, which is associated with the ability to detect salient stimuli and to task engagement (Menon & Uddin, Reference Menon and Uddin2010). This network has been previously found to be abnormal in participants with ADHD (Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Smith, Brammer and Taylor2007, Reference Rubia, Halari, Cubillo, Mohammad, Brammer and Taylor2009).

The third cluster includes the right cerebellum and right fusiform gyrus. Abnormalities in these regions have been widely implicated in the impairments of motor and cognitive control, attention and timing in ADHD (Bonath et al., Reference Bonath, Tegelbeckers, Wilke, Flechtner and Krauel2018; Castellanos & Proal, Reference Castellanos and Proal2012; del Campo, Müller, & Sahakian, Reference del Campo, Müller, Sahakian, Carter and Dalley2012; Giedd, Blumenthal, Molloy, & Castellanos, Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Molloy and Castellanos2001; Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Noreika, Falter, & Rubia, Reference Noreika, Falter and Rubia2013; Valera, Faraone, Murray, & Seidman, Reference Valera, Faraone, Murray and Seidman2007). In particular, abnormal activities in the cerebellum and fusiform (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Halari, Cubillo, Mohammad, Brammer and Taylor2009; Valera et al., Reference Valera, Brown, Biederman, Faraone, Makris, Monuteaux and Seidman2010) have been linked to compensatory attentional activity in ADHD (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Halari, Cubillo, Mohammad, Brammer and Taylor2009). One possibility is that lower GMV in visual processing areas, such as the fusiform, may reflect deficient modulations of the visual system, for example, by decreased top-down influence on perceptual processes (Sigi Hale et al., Reference Sigi Hale, Kane, Kaminsky, Tung, Wiley, McGough and Kaplan2014; Yalachkov, Kaiser, & Naumer, Reference Yalachkov, Kaiser and Naumer2010).

The fourth cluster involved the left middle and superior parts of the frontal gyrus extending to the precentral gyrus. This region is important for higher-order functions, the so-called executive functions, particularly in controlling behaviour in the context of conflicting stimuli, such as the Stroop task (Egner & Hirsch, Reference Egner and Hirsch2005; Mansouri, Tanaka, & Buckley, Reference Mansouri, Tanaka and Buckley2009) but also for controlling more basic motor functions. In the ADHD literature, lower GM in frontal lobes, including lateral and premotor regions, has been previously reported (Castellanos & Proal, Reference Castellanos and Proal2012; Dirlikov et al., Reference Dirlikov, Shiels Rosch, Crocetti, Denckla, Mahone and Mostofsky2015; Jarczok, Haase, Bluschke, Thiemann, & Bender, Reference Jarczok, Haase, Bluschke, Thiemann and Bender2019; Kumar, Arya, & Agarwal, Reference Kumar, Arya and Agarwal2017; Mostofsky, Cooper, Kates, Denckla, & Kaufmann, Reference Mostofsky, Cooper, Kates, Denckla and Kaufmann2002; Seidman et al., Reference Seidman, Valera, Makris, Monuteaux, Boriel, Kelkar and Biederman2006). Both executive dysfunction and impairments in motor response inhibition are relevant to symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity and core deficits in ADHD (Castellanos & Proal, Reference Castellanos and Proal2012; Rubia, Smith, & Taylor, Reference Rubia, Smith and Taylor2007; Sergeant, Geurts, Huijbregts, Scheres, & Oosterlaan, Reference Sergeant, Geurts, Huijbregts, Scheres and Oosterlaan2003).

Our findings have both novel and common aspects of previous research. To the best of our knowledge, there are only three studies on sex-by-ADHD interactions in the brain that have used whole-brain approaches. The first study did not report any sex-by-ADHD interactions at all (Castellanos et al., Reference Castellanos, Lee, Sharp, Jeffries, Greenstein, Clasen and Rapoport2002). The second study was small (N = 33) (Villemonteix et al., Reference Villemonteix, De Brito, Slama, Kavec, Balériaux, Metens and Massat2015b) and is closest to our findings, reporting a sex-by-ADHD interaction indicating lower GMV in the cingulum for boys (which we replicate) and also higher GMV in the same region in girls (which emerged as a trend in our results). The third study (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Llera, Hoogman, Cao, Zwiers, Bralten and Wang2019) was a multimodal analysis whereby a complex sex-by-ADHD interaction was reported showing, among other neuroimaging measures, higher GMV in frontal areas and lower GMV in posterior brain regions, for girls with ADHD compared to controls (girls), but the reverse for boys with ADHD compared to controls (boys). However, this study was limited given it only had nine female participants with ADHD. ROI approaches to sex-by-ADHD interactions are also sparse and show limited convergence with our results. For example, Dirlikov et al. (Reference Dirlikov, Shiels Rosch, Crocetti, Denckla, Mahone and Mostofsky2015) report lower surface area in precentral regions for boys (like us, although we looked at volume, not surface area), but not for girls for whom they found lower surface area in the prefrontal cortex (whilst we found higher GMV in the left frontal gyrus). However, unlike some other studies, we do not find any subcortical abnormalities (Qiu et al., Reference Qiu, Crocetti, Adler, Mahone, Denckla, Miller and Mostofsky2009; Seymour et al., Reference Seymour, Tang, Crocetti, Mostofsky, Miller and Rosch2017; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Xu, Li, Li, Zuo, Liao and Joshi2018). Nor did we detect any significant negative sex-by-ADHD interactions for girls (Dirlikov et al., Reference Dirlikov, Shiels Rosch, Crocetti, Denckla, Mahone and Mostofsky2015; Mahone et al., Reference Mahone, Ranta, Crocetti, O'Brien, Kaufmann, Denckla and Mostofsky2011). Overall, the four brain clusters fall within a subset of brain regions that have been previously associated with ADHD studies, which did not test for sex differences possibly because most participants in these studies were male (Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012; Kaczkurkin et al., Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019; Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Arya and Agarwal2017; Valera et al., Reference Valera, Brown, Biederman, Faraone, Makris, Monuteaux and Seidman2010). The effect sizes we report are small (Cohen's d of −0.18 to −0.26) but comparable and nominally larger than mega-studies on structural brain differences in ADHD (Boedhoe et al., Reference Boedhoe, van Rooij, Hoogman, Twisk, Schmaal, Abe and van den Heuvel2020; Hoogman et al., Reference Hoogman, Bralten, Hibar, Mennes, Zwiers, Schweren and Franke2017).

Our secondary analyses revealed sex differences at the phenotypic-symptom level. Conduct problems (externalizing) were higher in boys and emotional difficulties (internalizing) higher in girls. These results are congruent with the existing literature indicating that externalizing disorders are more frequent in boys and internalizing disorders are more frequent in girls (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Wang, Kunze, Otto, Schlack, Hölling and Rothenberger2018; Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Teesson, Sunderland, Newton, Krueger, Conrod and Slade2016; Martel, Reference Martel2013; Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Caspi and Moffitt2003). However, the remaining two psychopathology subscale scores did not replicate in a similar way. We expected peer relationship problems (internalizing) to be either the same across sexes (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Wang, Kunze, Otto, Schlack, Hölling and Rothenberger2018) or higher in girls (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Lamping and Ploubidis2010) but instead found they were higher in boys. Furthermore, we found no sex differences for hyperactivity/inattention symptoms (externalizing). One possibility is that these unexpected results may be due to specific study characteristics (e.g. sample type, measures used). Whilst null findings do not imply an absence of effect and should be interpreted with caution, a Bayesian version of this analysis supported this null finding and, other studies using similar study characteristics also failed to detect sex differences in behavioural hyperactivity/inattention symptoms (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Wang, Kunze, Otto, Schlack, Hölling and Rothenberger2018; Lundh, Wangby-Lundh, & Bjärehed, Reference Lundh, Wangby-Lundh and Bjärehed2008). Similar to these two studies, we used the self-rated version of the SDQ hyperactivity/inattention measure, which does not correlate highly with the parent-rated version (Du Rietz et al., Reference Du Rietz, Kuja-Halkola, Brikell, Jangmo, Sariaslan, Lichtenstein and Larsson2017). In addition, our results show a normalised (rather than a right-skewed) distribution for hyperactivity/inattention symptoms. The Lundh et al. (Reference Lundh, Wangby-Lundh and Bjärehed2008) study, which like ours is also a community-based sample, reports almost identical statistics to ours (means and standard deviations) for the hyperactivity/inattention subscale (although we cannot comment on the shape of the distribution as it is not included in their report). Considering the unexpected direction of the peer problems result and failure to detect any difference in the hyperactivity/inattention symptoms similar to the Lundh et al. (Reference Lundh, Wangby-Lundh and Bjärehed2008) and Becker et al. (Reference Becker, Wang, Kunze, Otto, Schlack, Hölling and Rothenberger2018) studies, this may suggest that a sex-related externalizing–internalizing dichotomy may not be so clear-cut.

Clinical implications

Clinically, our findings have two impacts. First, they inform the clinician that there are biological differences across the sexes in terms of hyperactivity/inattention symptoms. Second, they provide a lead to translational research. This includes research aimed at intervention practices, for example, pharmacological treatment as well as research on biomarkers of hyperactivity/inattention specific to boys and girls. A biomarker would aid the stratification of inattentive and hyperactive type of disorders by including a more objective biological-based approach instead of one only based on behavioural symptoms (Abi-Dargham & Horga, Reference Abi-Dargham and Horga2016). In addition, our findings are key to research aimed at uncovering mechanistic accounts of how psychopathological symptoms differ across sex and, in particular, as they do so along a continuum of psychopathology. Given that the assumptions for case–control designs are imperfect (e.g. well-defined grouping of cases v. controls, Marquand et al., Reference Marquand, Rezek, Buitelaar and Beckmann2016), investigating symptoms using a dimensional approach that spans from healthy to unhealthy is important as it is more representative of the manifestation of symptoms in the general population (Kaczkurkin et al., Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019).

Limitations

Sex information was limited to biological sex and did not include any gender information, potentially limiting the interpretability of the results (Krieger, Reference Krieger2003). Also, our non-clinical sample, having sparser psychopathology than a clinical sample, may not show sex-by-psychopathology interactions that other studies report (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Chen, Womer, Jiang, Luo, Driesen and Wang2013; Smaragdi et al., Reference Smaragdi, Cornwell, Toschi, Riccelli, Gonzalez-Madruga, Wells and Fairchild2017). In turn, sparser psychopathology makes it reasonable to assume that the presence of current treatment is minimal, which is advantageous, as it minimises additional confounds. However, data on medication were unavailable, hence this study cannot exclude any treatment effects on observed results (Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012; Villemonteix et al., Reference Villemonteix, De Brito, Kavec, Balériaux, Metens, Slama and Massat2015a). Furthermore, the failure to detect other sex-by-internalizing symptoms interaction effects in GMV may be, in part, due to these appearing later in adolescence. Finally, the brain regions reported in this study are likely important for a large number of behavioural and cognitive functions that go beyond the ones discussed and that may vary in degree of relevance to hyperactivity and inattention.

Conclusion

The brain regions we have identified via a sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction effect are the first to be reported from a large, sex-balanced and community-based sample, using an exploratory whole-brain VBM approach. These study characteristics make our study not only powered to confirm previous findings but also to explore new ones and, within a sample that is more representative of the distribution of psychopathological symptoms in the general population. By addressing these common issues of previous studies, our results are valuable as they offer a reliable reference on sex-by-psychopathology interactions in GMV. Overall, these findings provide important leads for future research on mechanistic accounts of psychopathology and their sex-biases that are crucial to the advancement of improved targeted early interventions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720005140.

Acknowledgements

This work received support from the following sources: the European Union-funded FP6 Integrated Project IMAGEN (Reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology) (LSHM-CT-2007-037286), the Horizon 2020 funded ERC Advanced Grant ‘STRATIFY’ (Brain network-based stratification of reinforcement-related disorders) (695313), ERANID (Understanding the Interplay between Cultural, Biological and Subjective Factors in Drug Use Pathways) (PR-ST-0416-10004), BRIDGET (JPND: BRain Imaging, cognition Dementia and next generation GEnomics) (MR/N027558/1), Human Brain Project (HBP SGA 2, 785907), the FP7 project MATRICS (603016), the Medical Research Council Grant ‘c-VEDA’ (Consortium on Vulnerability to Externalizing Disorders and Addictions) (MR/N000390/1), the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London, the Bundesministeriumfür Bildung und Forschung (BMBF grants 01GS08152; 01EV0711; Forschungsnetz AERIAL 01EE1406A, 01EE1406B), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG grants SM 80/7-2, SFB 940, TRR 265, NE 1383/14-1), the Medical Research Foundation and Medical Research Council (grants MR/R00465X/1 and MR/S020306/1), the National Institutes of Health (NIH) funded ENIGMA (grants 5U54EB020403-05 and 1R56AG058854-01). Further support was provided by grants from: the ANR (ANR-12-SAMA-0004, AAPG2019 – GeBra), the Eranet Neuron (AF12-NEUR0008-01 – WM2NA; and ANR-18-NEUR00002-01 – ADORe), the Fondation de France (00081242), the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (DPA20140629802), the Mission Interministérielle de Lutte-contre-les-Drogues-et-les-Conduites-Addictives (MILDECA), the Assistance-Publique-Hôpitaux-de-Paris and INSERM (interface grant), Paris Sud University IDEX 2012, the Fondation de l'Avenir (grant AP-RM-17-013), the Fédération pour la Recherche sur le Cerveau; the National Institutes of Health, Science Foundation Ireland (16/ERCD/3797), USA (Axon, Testosterone and Mental Health during Adolescence; RO1 MH085772-01A1), and by NIH Consortium grant U54 EB020403, supported by a cross-NIH alliance that funds Big Data to Knowledge Centres of Excellence.

Conflict of interest

Dr Banaschewski has served as an advisor to Lundbeck, Medice, Neurim Pharmaceuticals, Oberberg GmbH, Shire and Infectopharm. He received conference support or speaker's fee from Lilly, Medice and Shire; he received royalties from Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, CIP Medien, Oxford University Press; the present work is unrelated to these relationships. Dr Walter received a speaker honorarium from Servier (2014). The other authors report no potential conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

*

Equally contributing authors.

References

Abi-Dargham, A., & Horga, G. (2016). The search for imaging biomarkers in psychiatric disorders. Nature Medicine, 22(11), 12481255. doi: 10.1038/nm.4190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage, 38(1), 95113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry – the methods. NeuroImage, 11(6), 805821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26(3), 839851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M. V., Auyeung, B., Ashwin, E., Chakrabarti, B., & Knickmeyer, R. (2011). Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? PLoS Biology, 9(6), 110. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, A., Wang, B., Kunze, B., Otto, C., Schlack, R., Hölling, H., … Rothenberger, A. (2018). Normative data of the self-report version of the German strengths and difficulties questionnaire in an epidemiological setting. Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 46(6), 523533. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917/a000589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boedhoe, P. S. W., van Rooij, D., Hoogman, M., Twisk, J. W. R., Schmaal, L., Abe, Y., … van den Heuvel, O. A. (2020). Subcortical brain volume, regional cortical thickness, and cortical surface area across disorders: Findings from the ENIGMA ADHD, ASD, and OCD working groups. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(9), 834843. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19030331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonath, B., Tegelbeckers, J., Wilke, M., Flechtner, H. H., & Krauel, K. (2018). Regional gray matter volume differences between adolescents with ADHD and typically developing controls: Further evidence for anterior cingulate involvement. Journal of Attention Disorders, 22(7), 627638. doi: 10.1177/1087054715619682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bralten, J., Greven, C. U., Franke, B., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., Rommelse, N. N. J., … Buitelaar, J. K. (2016). Voxel-based morphometry analysis reveals frontal brain differences in participants with ADHD and their unaffected siblings. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 41(4), 272279. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G., Frazier, J. A., Rauch, S. L., Seidman, L. J., Whalen, P. J., Jenike, M. A., … Biederman, J. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biological Psychiatry, 45(12), 15421552. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00083-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carragher, N., Teesson, M., Sunderland, M., Newton, N. C., Krueger, R. F., Conrod, P. J., … Slade, T. (2016). The structure of adolescent psychopathology: A symptom-level analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(5), 981994. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castellanos, F. X., Lee, P. P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O., Greenstein, D., Clasen, L. S., … Rapoport, J. L. (2002). Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA, 288(14), 17401748. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.14.1740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castellanos, F. X., & Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: Beyond the prefrontal–striatal model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 1726. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, T. L., Babiloni, C., Ferretti, A., Perrucci, M. G., Romani, G. L., Rossini, P. M., … Del Gratta, C. (2008). Human secondary somatosensory cortex is involved in the processing of somatosensory rare stimuli: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 40(4), 17651771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
del Campo, N., Müller, U., & Sahakian, B. J. (2012). Neural and behavioral endophenotypes in ADHD. In Carter, C. S. & Dalley, J. W. (Eds.), Brain imaging in behavioral neuroscience (pp. 6591). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. doi:10.1007/7854_2012_200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dirlikov, B., Shiels Rosch, K., Crocetti, D., Denckla, M. B., Mahone, E. M., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Distinct frontal lobe morphology in girls and boys with ADHD. NeuroImage: Clinical, 7, 222229. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.12.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Rietz, E., Kuja-Halkola, R., Brikell, I., Jangmo, A., Sariaslan, A., Lichtenstein, P., … Larsson, H. (2017). Predictive validity of parent- and self-rated ADHD symptoms in adolescence on adverse socioeconomic and health outcomes. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(7), 857867. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0957-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 47(3–4), 313337. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task. NeuroImage, 24(2), 539547. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eklund, A., Nichols, T., & Knutsson, H. (2016). Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 113(28), 79007905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frere, P. B., Vetter, N. C., Artiges, E., Filippi, I., Miranda, R., Vulser, H., … Lemaître, H. (2020). Sex effects on structural maturation of the limbic system and outcomes on emotional regulation during adolescence. NeuroImage, 210, 116441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., & Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(2), 114126. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Molloy, E., & Castellanos, F. X. (2001). Brain imaging of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 33. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05772.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Rajapakse, J. C., Vaituzis, A. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1997). Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 21(8), 11851201. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(97)00158-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodkind, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Oathes, D. J., Jiang, Y., Chang, A., Jones-Hagata, L. B., … Etkin, A. (2015). Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 305315. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 38(5), 581586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R., Ford, T., Richards, H., Gatward, R., & Meltzer, H. (2000). The development and well-being assessment: Description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(5), 645655. doi: 10.1017/S0021963099005909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2010). When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(8), 11791191. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gore, F. M., Bloem, P. J. N., Patton, G. C., Ferguson, J., Joseph, V., Coffey, C., … Mathers, C. D. (2011). Global burden of disease in young people aged 10–24 years: A systematic analysis. The Lancet, 377(9783), 20932102. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60512-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2013). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(2), 185198. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilbronner, S. R., & Hayden, B. Y. (2016). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A bottom-up view. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 39, 149170. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoogman, M., Bralten, J., Hibar, D. P., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M. P., Schweren, L. S. J., … Franke, B. (2017). Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: A cross-sectional mega-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4), 310319. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, L. T., & Hayden, B. Y. (2017). A distributed, hierarchical and recurrent framework for reward-based choice. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(3), 172182. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarczok, T. A., Haase, R., Bluschke, A., Thiemann, U., & Bender, S. (2019). Bereitschaftspotential and lateralized readiness potential in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Altered motor system activation and effects of methylphenidate. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(8), 960970. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.05.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaczkurkin, A. N., Raznahan, A., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2019). Sex differences in the developing brain: Insights from multimodal neuroimaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(1), 7185. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0111-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Üstün, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: A review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20(4), 359364. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, M., Onnink, M., van Donkelaar, M., Wolfers, T., Harich, B., Shi, Y., … Franke, B. (2017). Brain imaging genetics in ADHD and beyond – mapping pathways from gene to disorder at different levels of complexity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 115155. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kobel, M., Bechtel, N., Specht, K., Klarhöfer, M., Weber, P., Scheffler, K., … Penner, I. K. (2010). Structural and functional imaging approaches in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Does the temporal lobe play a key role? Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 183(3), 230236. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.03.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, L., Chen, K., Womer, F., Jiang, W., Luo, X., Driesen, N., … Wang, F. (2013). Sex differences of gray matter morphology in cortico-limbic-striatal neural system in major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(6), 733739. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.02.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krieger, N. (2003). Genders, sexes, and health: What are the connections and why does it matter? International Journal of Epidemiology, 32(4), 652657. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyg156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumar, U., Arya, A., & Agarwal, V. (2017). Neural alterations in ADHD children as indicated by voxel-based cortical thickness and morphometry analysis. Brain and Development, 39(5), 403410. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2016.12.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenroot, R. K., Gogtay, N., Greenstein, D. K., Wells, E. M., Wallace, G. L., Clasen, L. S., … Giedd, J. N. (2007). Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. NeuroImage, 36(4), 10651073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddle, E. B., Hollis, C., Batty, M. J., Groom, M. J., Totman, J. J., Liotti, M., … Liddle, P. F. (2011). Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: Effects of motivation and methylphenidate. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52(7), 761771. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02333.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundh, L. G., Wangby-Lundh, M., & Bjärehed, J. (2008). Self-reported emotional and behavioral problems in Swedish 14 to 15-year-old adolescents: A study with the self-report version of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49(6), 523532. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00668.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahone, E. M., Ranta, M. E., Crocetti, D., O'Brien, J., Kaufmann, W. E., Denckla, M. B., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2011). Comprehensive examination of frontal regions in boys and girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 10471057. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansouri, F. A., Tanaka, K., & Buckley, M. J. (2009). Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: A clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(2), 141152. doi: 10.1038/nrn2538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marquand, A. F., Rezek, I., Buitelaar, J., & Beckmann, C. F. (2016). Understanding heterogeneity in clinical cohorts using normative models: Beyond case-control studies. Biological Psychiatry, 80(7), 552561. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martel, M. M. (2013). Sexual selection and sex differences in the prevalence of childhood externalizing and adolescent internalizing disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 12211259. doi: 10.1037/a0032247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, K. R., Langevin, L. M., Goodyear, B. G., & Dewey, D. (2014). Functional connectivity of neural motor networks is disrupted in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 566575. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure & Function, 214(5–6), 655667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2018). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12–4.2. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor.Google Scholar
Mostofsky, S. H., Cooper, K. L., Kates, W. R., Denckla, M. B., & Kaufmann, W. E. (2002). Smaller prefrontal and premotor volumes in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52(8), 785794. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01412-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noreika, V., Falter, C. M., & Rubia, K. (2013). Timing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 235266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T., Keshavan, M. S., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 947957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T., Wong, A. P., Syme, C., & Pausova, Z. (2017). Sex differences in the adolescent brain and body: Findings from the Saguenay youth study. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 95, 362370. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. doi: 10.1007/bf01537962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qiu, A., Crocetti, D., Adler, M., Mahone, E. M., Denckla, M. B., Miller, M. I., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Basal ganglia volume and shape in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(1), 7482. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Core Team. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Rorden, C., Karnath, H. O., & Bonilha, L. (2007). Improving lesion-symptom mapping. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(7), 10811088. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Halari, R., Cubillo, A., Mohammad, A. M., Brammer, M., & Taylor, E. (2009). Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naïve children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance task. Neuropharmacology, 57(7–8), 640652. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.08.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Brammer, M. J., & Taylor, E. (2007). Temporal lobe dysfunction in medication-naïve boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during attention allocation and its relation to response variability. Biological Psychiatry, 62(9), 9991006. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Smith, A., & Taylor, E. (2007). Performance of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on a test battery of impulsiveness. Child Neuropsychology, 13(3), 276304. doi: 10.1080/09297040600770761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruigrok, A. N. V., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Lai, M. C., Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M. V., Tait, R. J., & Suckling, J. (2014). A meta-analysis of sex differences in human brain structure. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 3450. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: Unifying issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(8), 10921115. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schumann, G., Loth, E., Banaschewski, T., Barbot, A., Barker, G., Büchel, C., … Struve, M. (2010). The IMAGEN study: Reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology. Molecular Psychiatry, 15(12), 11281139. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., Makris, N., Monuteaux, M. C., Boriel, D. L., Kelkar, K., … Biederman, J. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 60(10), 10711080. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., Huijbregts, S., Scheres, A., & Oosterlaan, J. (2003). The top and the bottom of ADHD: A neuropsychological perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7), 583592. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seymour, K. E., Tang, X., Crocetti, D., Mostofsky, S. H., Miller, M. I., & Rosch, K. S. (2017). Anomalous subcortical morphology in boys, but not girls, with ADHD compared to typically developing controls and correlates with emotion dysregulation. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 261, 2028. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.01.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigi Hale, T., Kane, A. M., Kaminsky, O., Tung, K. L., Wiley, J. F., McGough, J. J., … Kaplan, J. T. (2014). Visual network asymmetry and default mode network function in ADHD: An fMRI study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5(Jul), 120. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00081.Google Scholar
Smaragdi, A., Cornwell, H., Toschi, N., Riccelli, R., Gonzalez-Madruga, K., Wells, A., … Fairchild, G. (2017). Sex differences in the relationship between conduct disorder and cortical structure in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(8), 703712. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., & Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage, 44(1), 8398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valera, E. M., Brown, A., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Makris, N., Monuteaux, M. C., … Seidman, L. J. (2010). Sex differences in the functional neuroanatomy of working memory in adults with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1), 8694. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valera, E. M., Faraone, S. V., Murray, K. E., & Seidman, L. J. (2007). Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61(12), 13611369. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Den Bergh, D., Van Doorn, J., Marsman, M., Draws, T., Van Kesteren, E. J., Derks, K., … Wagenmakers, E. J. (2020). A tutorial on conducting and interpreting a Bayesian ANOVA in JASP. Annee Psychologique, 120(1), 7396. doi: 10.3917/anpsy1.201.0073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villemonteix, T., De Brito, S. A., Kavec, M., Balériaux, D., Metens, T., Slama, H., … Massat, I. (2015a). Grey matter volumes in treatment naïve vs. chronically treated children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A combined approach. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(8), 11181127. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.04.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villemonteix, T., De Brito, S. A., Slama, H., Kavec, M., Balériaux, D., Metens, T., … Massat, I. (2015b). Grey matter volume differences associated with gender in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A voxel-based morphometry study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 3237. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Xu, Q., Li, S., Li, G., Zuo, C., Liao, S., … Joshi, R. M. (2018). Gender differences in anomalous subcortical morphology for children with ADHD. Neuroscience Letters, 665, 176181. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.12.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winkler, A. M., Ridgway, G. R., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M., & Nichols, T. E. (2014). Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage, 92, 381397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woo, C. W., Krishnan, A., & Wager, T. D. (2014). Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: Pitfalls and recommendations. NeuroImage, 91, 412419. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Z. M., Llera, A., Hoogman, M., Cao, Q. J., Zwiers, M. P., Bralten, J., … Wang, Y. F. (2019). Linked anatomical and functional brain alterations in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23, 18. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101851.Google ScholarPubMed
Yalachkov, Y., Kaiser, J., & Naumer, M. J. (2010). Sensory and motor aspects of addiction. Behavioural Brain Research, 207(2), 215222. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Distribution of covariates. The pyramid plots show the distribution of puberty score, TIV, SES and age split by sex (girls, N = 801 in grey; boys, N = 725 in black). Means, standard deviation (s.d.), median, observed minimum and maximum are presented for each raw (non-residualised) covariate. Handedness (not illustrated) was 89.4% right and 10.6% left/ambidextrous for girls and, 85.8% right and 14.2% left/ambidextrous for boys. Additional plots using the maximum available cases for behavioural data (N = 2046) can be found in online Supplementary Fig. S1.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Distributions of the four psychopathological symptoms subscales of the SDQ. Pyramid plots are split by sex (girls, N = 801 in grey; boys, N = 725 in black). Significant sex differences were found for conduct problems (higher in boys), emotional difficulties (higher in girls) and peer problems (higher in boys). The plots show raw (non-residualised) versions of the variables. Full GLM details can be found in online Supplementary Table S3a. Additional variants of these plots and analyses can be found in Supplementary materials (online Supplementary Fig. S2, online Supplementary Tables S3b–S3c).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in GMV. The image illustrates coronal (top row), axial (middle row) and sagittal slices (bottom row) with coloured maps representing binary maps for the four statistically significant clusters indicating a negative association between GMV and symptom scores of hyperactivity/inattention in boys, and a positive association in girls for the temporoparietal-opercular and fusiform and cerebellar clusters. Green = cluster 1, right temporoparietal-opercular region; pink = cluster 2, cingulum; blue = cluster 3, right fusiform and cerebellum; red = cluster 4, frontal superior. For detailed anatomical descriptions, refer to Table 2 and the ‘Results’ section. For an unthresholded map and non-significant clusters, see Supplementary materials (online Supplementary Figs S5–S6). Maps are overlaid on a T1-weighted brain image made from 500 T1-weighted scans from the study sample. Images were produced using MRIcron (www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron).

Figure 3

Table 1. Sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in voxel-wise GMV

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Direction of the sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction in voxel-wise GMV. The scatterplots illustrate the residual of GMV on the y-axis for each one of the four brain clusters showing a significant sex-by-hyperactivity/inattention interaction, and hyperactivity/inattention symptoms score on the x-axis, stratified by sex (girls in filled red circles; boys in empty blue triangles). At the top and right side of each scatterplot are density plots illustrating the distribution of hyperactivity/inattention symptoms score and standardised residuals, respectively. For boys, there was a negative association between hyperactivity/inattention symptoms and GMV in all four brain regions (negative t-values, p < 0.05). For girls, this association was positive but statistically significant for only the temporoparietal-opercular and frontal superior clusters (positive t values, p < 0.05). The standardised residuals were calculated using a GLM with GMV at each cluster as the dependent variable and sex, site, handedness, age, puberty, SES, TIV, conduct, peer and emotion problems as covariates.

Supplementary material: File

Biondo et al. supplementary material

Biondo et al. supplementary material

Download Biondo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.9 MB